Inside The Mind of An Animal.

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 4

Feature

ILLUSTRATION BY KAROL BANACH

182 | Nature | Vol 584 | 13 August 2020


©
2
0
2
0
S
p
r
i
n
g
e
r
N
a
t
u
r
e
L
i
m
i
t
e
d
.
A
l
l
r
i
g
h
t
s
r
e
s
e
r
v
e
d
.
WHAT ANIMALS
brain regions. For example, earlier this
year2, neurobiologist David Anderson at the
California Institute of Technology (Caltech)
in Pasadena and his colleagues identified an

REALLY THINK
internal brain state — represented by a small
network of neurons — that prepares fruit flies
to engage in courtship or fighting behaviours.
Neuroscientists wanting to understand the
brain’s coding language have conventionally
studied how its networks of cells respond to
sensory information and how they generate
behaviour, such as movement or speech. But
they couldn’t look in detail at the important bit
in between — the vast quantities of neuronal

Neuroscientists are scrutinizing mountains of activity that conceal patterns representing


the animal’s mood or desires, and which help
data to uncover how brains create emotions it to calibrate its behaviour. Even just a few
years ago, measuring the activities of specific
and other internal states, such as aggression networks that underlie internal brain states
was impossible.
or desire. By Alison Abbott A slew of new techniques is starting to
change that. These methods allow scientists
to track electrical activity in the brain in
unprecedented detail, to quantify an animal’s
natural behaviour on millisecond timescales,
and to find patterns in the mountains of data
these experiments generate. These patterns
could be signatures of the innumerable inter-
nal states that a brain can adopt. Now the
challenge is to find out what these states mean.
Some neuroscientists are daring to wield
the technologies to probe one powerful group
of internal brain states: emotions. Others are
applying them to states such as motivation, or
existential drives such as thirst. Researchers
are even finding signatures of states in their
data for which they have no vocabulary.
The current trickle of research papers on

T
internal brain states is gaining momentum.
The work might even have potential clinical
applications. “Mental illness is essentially
wo years ago, Jennifer Li and Something else was strange. Looking in more disruption of internal states,” says Joshua
Drew Robson were trawling detail at the data, the researchers realized that Gordon, director of the US National Institute
through terabytes of data from a the ‘psychic’ cells were active for an unusually of Mental Health in Bethesda, Maryland. “They
zebrafish-brain experiment when long time — not seconds, as is typical for most need to be understood.”
they came across a handful of cells neurons, but many minutes. In fact, more or
that seemed to be psychic. less the duration of the larvae’s hunting bouts. Frames of mind
The two neuroscientists had “It was spooky,” says Li. “None of it made The brain of any animal is constantly
planned to map brain activity while sense.” bombarded with information about the
zebrafish larvae were hunting for food, and Li and Robson turned to the literature and creature’s environment from sensory organs
to see how the neural chatter changed. It slowly realized that the cells must be setting such as the eyes, ears, nose or skin. All of this
was their first major test of a technological an overall ‘brain state’ — a pattern of prolonged information is initially processed in the brain’s
platform they had built at Harvard Univer- brain activity that primed the larvae to engage sensory cortex. Then come more mysterious
sity in Cambridge, Massachusetts. The plat- with the food in front of them. The pair learnt processing steps, in which that information
form allowed them to view every cell in the that, in the past few years, other scientists is filtered through multiple internal brain
larvae’s brains while the creatures — barely using various approaches and different spe- states representing the creature’s constantly
the size of an eyelash — swam freely in a cies had also found internal brain states that changing moods and needs. That finally leads
35-millimetre-diameter dish of water, snack- alter how an animal behaves, even when noth- the motor cortex to generate movements that
ing on their microscopic prey. ing has changed in its external environment. are appropriate to the circumstances — to flap
Out of the scientists’ mountain of data Some, such as Li and Robson, had come to away a tickling fly, for example, or to move
emerged a handful of neurons that predicted the discovery serendipitously while trudging towards a tasty treat. Internal states can also be
when a larva was next going to catch and through their own brain-wide data. Others have generated entirely in the brain, without sensory
swallow a morsel. Some of these neurons even hypothesized that neurons coding for inter- input and without a behavioural output: think
became activated many seconds before the nal brain states must exist, and have actively of daydreaming, or replaying the events of the
larva fixed its eyes on the prey1. sought them in discrete and well-researched day in your mind.

Nature | Vol 584 | 13 August 2020 | 183


©
2
0
2
0
S
p
r
i
n
g
e
r
N
a
t
u
r
e
L
i
m
i
t
e
d
.
A
l
l
r
i
g
h
t
s
r
e
s
e
r
v
e
d
.
Feature
Over the past few years, insights into the
nature of internal states are changing how
neuroscientists who study brain networks think
about animal behaviour. “We used to think of
animals as being kind of stimulus-response
machines,” says neuroscientist Anne
Churchland at Cold Spring Harbor Labora-
tory, New York. “Now we’re starting to realize
that all kinds of really interesting stuff is being
generated within their brains that changes the
way that sensory inputs are processed — and
so changes the animals’ behavioural output.”
Sketching out how to study this intriguing
middle ground has long been a preoccupation
for Anderson. Six years ago, he decided to
create a theoretical framework for research into
internal brain states that represent emotion. He

J. C. MARQUES ET AL./NATURE (REF. 1)


was irked by the view of some psychologists,
who think that because animals can’t express
their feelings in words, those feelings can’t
be studied at all. Together with his Caltech
colleague Ralph Adolphs, Anderson devel-
oped and published a hypothesis3 about the
characteristics that neural circuits associated
with internal brain states should have. Distinct groups of neurons control whether zebrafish larvae explore (right) or stay put (left).
Most importantly, they thought, an internal
brain state should outlast the original stimulus over many hours, and analyse every movement active as a consequence of P1 activation.
that triggered it. So a key feature of a neural in millisecond elements. The elements can Most of these ‘follower cells’ switched quickly
circuit underpinning such a state would be then be aligned with neural recordings, on and off, but a cluster called pCd neurons
its persistence, he says. “If you are hiking in matching moment-to-moment brain activity stayed active for many minutes. When the
mountains and see a snake, then you might with specific movements. researchers inserted a light-sensitive protein
jump in fear,” says Anderson. “Ten minutes Neuroscientists have capitalized on a surge into these cells and switched them off using
later, your brain’s internal state of fear is still in machine learning, artificial intelligence and a flash from a laser, the persistent effect of P1
active, so when you see a stick on your path new mathematical tools to make sense of the activation on behaviour disappeared. When
you might jump again.” gigabytes or terabytes of data that any exper- they activated them directly, bypassing P1,
Other characteristics of internal states iment with these technologies can generate, nothing happened: the pCd neurons needed
should include generalizability, meaning that and to coax out the neural activation patterns P1 as a trigger and, once sparked into action,
different stimuli should be able to prompt the that could represent internal brain states. they stayed on for much longer than the initial
same state, and scalability, in which different prompt2. If Anderson had to give the state a
stimuli can create states of different strength. Ready to engage name, he might call it the ‘ready-to-engage-in-
The paper became influential. Li says that it For his first study of an internal state, Anderson these-social-behaviours’ state, he says.
“was inspirational” as she and Robson were decided to build on his laboratory’s previous His team has conducted a similar exper-
trying to make sense of their psychic cells. interest in aggression in the fruit fly, which has iment in mice5, which have more complex
Anderson and Adolphs published their a tiny brain containing about 100,000 neu- brains containing about 100 million neurons.
paper in 2014, just as a raft of neurotechnol- rons. In many animal species, males start to The researchers found a particular group of
ogies was starting to make the necessary fight each other in the presence of females — neurons in the hypothalamus that, just like the
experiments feasible. It was already possible a well-established behaviour that Anderson pCd neurons, became persistently activated
to record from large numbers of individual calls the ‘Helen of Troy effect’, after the Greek in association with an innate drive — this time,
neurons at the same time, and since then the myth about a woman whose competing suitors fear. When the scientists placed a rat close to
technologies have improved and expanded started a war. Fruit flies are no exception: experimental mice for just a few seconds,
remarkably, allowing scientists to analyse indirect evidence suggests that exposure to the mice responded defensively by hugging
previously inaccessible activity. females causes males to engage in both court- the wall for several minutes and the group
Leading the pack is the Neuropixels probe, ship songs and aggressive behaviour towards of neurons remained active for all this time.
just 10 mm long, which can directly record other males for many minutes. “That’s a long When the team again used light to switch the
activity in hundreds of neurons across time in the short life of a fruit fly,” he says. neurons on and off, the wall-hugging behav-
different brain areas4. And special imaging He decided to search for neural activity iour came and went in tandem, even with no
techniques can indicate where as many as that correlated with the persistent courtship rat present.
tens of thousands of individual neurons are and fighting behaviours that are initiated by Neuroscientists are now discovering other
active across the brain. In calcium imaging, for neurons known as P1, found in a region that groups of neurons with persistent activity in
instance, animals are genetically engineered controls such social behaviours. These neu- different brain areas. Using calcium imaging in
to express a molecule in their cells that detects rons fire so quickly that they alone couldn’t mice, Andreas Lüthi at the Friedrich Miescher
calcium ions — when these pour into a neuron be responsible for maintaining an internal Institute for Biomedical Research in Basel, Swit-
as it fires, the molecule fluoresces. state. Using imaging techniques along with zerland, and Jan Gründemann at the University
New automatic behaviour monitors take automated behavioural analysis, his group of Basel searched in the amygdala, which is cen-
video recordings of freely behaving animals identified cells in other brain areas that become tral to the regulation of a range of emotions

184 | Nature | Vol 584 | 13 August 2020


©
2
0
2
0
S
p
r
i
n
g
e
r
N
a
t
u
r
e
L
i
m
i
t
e
d
.
A
l
l
r
i
g
h
t
s
r
e
s
e
r
v
e
d
.
and behaviours. The team found two different be inserted at once. basis to sing in staccato pulses or longer hums
populations of neurons that displayed sus- In a study published last year7, neuro­scientist depends in large part — but not totally — on
tained but opposing activation when the mice Karl Deisseroth at Stanford University in Cali- how the females respond to them. Murthy’s
switched between two distinct behaviours6 — fornia and his team used Neuropixels probes group found that three different internal brain
exploring the environment and performing to record the activity of 24,000 neurons across states also affected the male’s song choice.
defensive behaviours such as freezing. 34 cortical and subcortical brain regions in They dubbed the fly dispositions Close, Chas-
Gründemann acknowledges that the thirsty mice that were licking water from a ing and Whatever.
amygdala cells are unlikely to be working in spout. The scientists were able to tease out No matter the complexity of the model
isolation, and that cells across the whole brain signals related to the brain state of thirst organism that individual researchers have
are involved in maintaining the explorative or from signals related to licking behaviour. They adopted — worm, fish, fly or mouse — the
defensive states. “I’m sure it is just one node in found that these state-signalling neurons were question of how the entire brain coordinates
larger, brain-wide networks,” he says. activated throughout the brain — not just in internal states “is what we are all starting to
the hypothalamus, where dedicated thirst think about”, says Steve Flavell at the Massachu-
The whole picture neurons are located. setts Institute of Technology in Cambridge. In
Whereas many researchers have searched Using these extensive recording techniques, 2013, Flavell and his colleagues discovered that
particular brain areas for neurons that have even the brain of the Caenorhabditis elegans
enduring activity, Li and Robson, who moved worm, which has only 302 neurons, displays
to Germany last September to jointly run a properties of internal brain states that drive
lab at the Max Planck Institute for Biological particular behaviours, including two sets

MENTAL ILLNESS
Cybernetics in Tübingen, came across their of persistently active neurons controlling
persistently active neurons almost by chance. whether the animal lingers locally or moves

IS ESSENTIALLY
Their zebrafish larvae are less complex than with purpose11. His group has since identified
fruit flies, having only 80,000 or so brain cells. the full circuitry involved in the two states and
Because these baby fish are transparent, the
activity of nearly all of their neurons can be DISRUPTION OF switching between them12.
Aside from their questions about the basic
monitored simultaneously using calcium
imaging. INTERNAL STATES. biology, researchers have an eye on the clini-
cal benefit of understanding how a particular
The pair has developed a method of
concurrently following both the movements THEY NEED TO BE state manifests in the brain. Those studying
pain in rodent models, for instance, rely on
and the neural activity as fish larvae swim
freely around a dish. They deploy a fluores- UNDERSTOOD.” standard tests such as observing when a rat
lifts its paw from a hot plate. “That movement
cent-microscope tracking system that moves reflects protective aspects of pain, but not the
on its imaging platform to keep the fish in neuroscientists are finding that there is a lot actual perception of pain,” says neurologist
constant view, and captures every flash of going on beneath the surface when an animal Clifford Woolf at the Boston Children’s Hos-
each neuron as the larvae move. The system performs a task — and not all of it seems rele- pital in Massachusetts. That makes it a poor
also films them — typically for 90 minutes, vant at first glance. In landmark papers last model for pain, he argues, because it is one
generating 4.5 terabytes of data — allowing year, groups led by Kenneth Harris at Uni- step removed from the actual sensation. He
the experimenters to align movement with versity College London and by Churchland has launched a research programme to try to
neuronal activity second by second. showed that when a mouse is engaged in a directly read brain signals that indicate the
Fish larvae might not seem to have the rich task, neurons activate throughout the brain, internal state of pain perception — potentially a
internal life enjoyed by mice, or even flies, but that a large proportion of the activation more timely and specific readout than waiting
but they have at least one robust behavioural is not correlated with the task at all8,9. Some for the animal’s response. “I’m extremely opti-
choice to make in their lives — whether to activity correlated instead with the animals’ mistic that we’re in one of those rare stages in
hunt locally, or to swim to unfamiliar waters fidgety movements. But around two-thirds science where this is going to be a transforma-
to search for new food sources. When Li and of the off-task activation didn’t tally with any tion of the way we do things,” he says.
Robson watched larvae making this choice, movement or action. “Part of this may be In this new field, even the basics are up for
they found three groups of neurons: one that related to internal brain states,” says Harris. grabs, says Li. “At this stage, we are still trying
was persistently active during local hunting, to understand what the questions are.”
another that stayed active during exploration Busy brain
and a third that flashed on briefly as the fish Many neuroscientists say that the sheer volume Alison Abbott is a writer based in Munich,
switched states1. Surprisingly, hunger didn’t of data pouring out of whole-brain experi- Germany.
seem to influence the states, which switched ments is also the field’s biggest bottleneck.
1. Marques, J. C., Li, M., Schaak, D., Robson, D. N. & Li, J. M.
automatically every few minutes — “just like our But they have been making progress in devel- Nature 577, 239–243 (2020).
own sleep–wake states switch automatically, oping techniques to sift through the flood of 2. Jung, Y. et al. Neuron 105, 322–333 (2020).
but on a much shorter timescale”, Robson says. measurements. One popular approach is to 3. Anderson, D. J. & Adolphs, R. Cell 157, 187–200 (2014).
4. Jun, J. J. et al. Nature 551, 232–236 (2017).
Neuroscientists working with more com- use a mathematical method called the hidden
5. Kennedy, A., Kunwar, P. S., Li, L., Wagenaar, D.
plex organisms can’t monitor the whole brain Markov model (HMM) to predict the probabil- & Anderson, D. J. Preprint at bioRxiv https://doi.
at once, but they have been able to find hints ity that a system will switch between different org/10.1101/805317 (2020).
of internal brain states with networks that are states at a particular time. 6. Gründemann, J. et al. Science 364, eaav8736 (2019).
7. Allen, W. E. et al. Science 364, eaav3932 (2019).
widely distributed in the brain. In technically Mala Murthy at Princeton University, New 8. Stringer, C. et al. Science 364, eaav7893 (2019).
challenging experiments in mice, they have Jersey, and her colleagues used the HMM to 9. Musall, S., Kaufman, M. T., Juavinett, A. L., Gluf, S. &
recorded the activity of thousands of neurons discover rhythms in the brains of male fruit Churchland, A. K. Nature Neurosci. 22, 1677–1686 (2019).
10. Calhoun, A. J., Pillow, J. W. & Murthy, M. Nature Neurosci.
throughout the brain using calcium imaging, flies10 that influenced their choice of song
22, 2040–2049 (2019).
and of hundreds of neurons using a single pattern when courting females. Whether 11. Flavell, S. W. et al. Cell 154, 1023–1035 (2013).
Neuropixels electrode, several of which can male flies choose on a moment-to-moment 12. Cermak, N. et al. eLife 9, e57093 (2020).

Nature | Vol 584 | 13 August 2020 | 185


©
2
0
2
0
S
p
r
i
n
g
e
r
N
a
t
u
r
e
L
i
m
i
t
e
d
.
A
l
l
r
i
g
h
t
s
r
e
s
e
r
v
e
d
.

You might also like