2017 Henry (2017)

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 18

15404781, 2017, 3, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/modl.12412 by University Of Glasgow, Wiley Online Library on [10/11/2022].

See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
L2 Motivation and Multilingual
Identities
ALASTAIR HENRY
University West, Department of Social and Behavioural Studies, Trollhättan, Sweden Email: al.henry@hv.se

By tradition, L2 motivation research has a monolingual bias – the motivational systems of a learner’s
different languages conceptualized as separate entities rather than as cognitively interconnected. At a
time when multilingualism has become a new world order (Douglas Fir Group, 2016) and where there
is evidence of powerful identity experiences connected to speaking several languages (Pavlenko, 2006)
this is unfortunate. In alignment with the multilingual and dynamic turns in SLA (de Bot, 2015; May,
2014), and adopting a complexity thought modeling approach (Larsen–Freeman & Cameron, 2008),
this article explores multilingual learners’ L2 motivation. It is suggested that the motivational systems of
a multilingual learner’s different languages can be understood as constituting a higher-level multilingual
motivational self system that is part of an ecology of interconnected and interpenetrating systems. This
system contains multilingual self guides, one of which is the ideal multilingual self. Drawing on construal-
level theory (Trope & Liberman, 2010), the manner and effects of mental representations of an ideal
multilingual self are assessed. Finally, it is suggested that motivation deriving from a broader identity that
encompasses but, in important ways, transcends a multilingual person’s language-specific identities has
a central role to play in multilingual education.
Keywords: L2 motivational self system; complex dynamic systems; multilingualism; multilingual turn; ideal
multilingual self; construal-level theory; multilingual education

AS IN SLA GENERALLY, WHERE A MONOLIN- in those that have been conducted, greater fo-
gual tradition substantially predates Chomskyan cus has been placed on comparisons than interac-
conceptualizations of linguistic competence, re- tions. Although understandable, the monolingual
search into language learners’ motivation has a bias has implications. As in other fields of SLA
strongly monolingual bias. Neither of the mod- where a monolingual mindset “has been blocking
els historically dominating the field, Gardner’s a holistic view of multilingualism” (Jessner, 2006,
(2001) Socio-Educational Model and Dörnyei’s p. 141), L2 motivation research has treated the
(2009) L2 Motivational Self System, take account motivational systems of different languages sepa-
of other languages the individual speaks or is rately, rather than as cognitively interconnected.
learning. While Gardner’s central concept of in- As a consequence, emergent properties of motiva-
tegrativeness is constructed in accordance with tion to learn more than one L2 are overlooked. In
a monolingual logic where (monolingual) native our globalized, multilingual world this blindspot
speakers are the reference, Dörnyei’s model has a is regrettable as sight is lost of a potentially im-
similarly monolingual premise, motivation con- portant source of L2 motivation: the desire to
ceptualized as deriving from the discrepancy achieve or improve multilingual competence (see
between a person’s current and ideal self- Ushioda, 2017). This is particularly unfortunate
perceptions as the speaker of a target language as there is growing evidence of powerful identity
(TL). A monolingual bias is similarly evident in experiences connected to speaking several lan-
empirical work. Studies examining motivation to guages, and that developing a multilingual iden-
learn more than one language are scarce, and tity unconstrained by a dominant language or lan-
guages can be deeply enriching (Dewaele, 2011;
Pavlenko, 2006).
The Modern Language Journal, 101, 3, (2017) In a world where globalization “has penetrated
DOI: 10.1111/modl.12412 all aspects of L2 learners’ lifeworlds” (Douglas
0026-7902/17/548–565 $1.50/0 Fir Group, 2016, p. 22) new ways of understand-

C 2017 The Modern Language Journal
ing people’s motivation to learn languages are
15404781, 2017, 3, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/modl.12412 by University Of Glasgow, Wiley Online Library on [10/11/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Alastair Henry 549
required. In this conceptual article I argue that For some of Pavlenko’s informants—particularly
when the aim of research is to understand lan- those for whom learning an additional language
guage learners’ motivation in situations where two involved a sense of loss—transitioning between
or more languages are learned/acquired, a sep- different and phenomenologically distinct life-
arationist approach does not yield adequate in- worlds constituted a source of discomfort. For
sights. Rather, in line with the multilingual (May, such people the identity experiences connected
2014) and dynamic (de Bot, 2015) turns in SLA, with being multilingual could involve feeling frag-
I propose that the motivational systems of the mented, incomplete, and inadequate.
learner’s different languages need to be concep- However, a majority of Pavlenko’s respondents
tualized as interrelated systems that are simultane- were enthusiastic about being multilingual, cel-
ously constituents within a higher-level multilin- ebrating the experience and mocking the idea
gual motivational system. Adopting a “complexity that being a speaker of different languages might
thought modeling” approach (Larsen–Freeman somehow entail a schizophrenic existence. Com-
& Cameron, 2008), where “aspects of a particular monly, people asserted the uniquely enjoyable
problem are described in terms of complex, dy- and empowering experiences of being multilin-
namic systems in order to develop hypotheses for gual, stating for example that they felt “very
research or plans for action” (p. 41), and drawing lucky,” and that it can be “a very pleasant feeling
on complexity-generated understandings of L2 because it gives me choices I wouldn’t have if I
motivation (Dörnyei, MacIntyre, & Henry, 2015), were monolingual.” These participants appeared
multilingualism (Jessner, 2008), and self-concept to thrive on the diversity created by moving be-
development (Nowak, Vallacher, & Zochowski, tween languages and to derive enjoyment from
2005), I suggest that in situations where bilin- the “hybridity and relativity of their existence”
guals/multilinguals learn additional languages, (p. 29). Being multilingual also involved a sense
or where monolinguals are learning two or more of feeling “linguistically integrated.” Pavlenko
L2s, focus needs to be directed to the dynamic in- observed that multilinguals with positive iden-
teractions of the Lx and Ly motivational systems tity experiences may not necessarily “perceive
and the emergent motivational properties arising such sharp differences between their linguistic
therefrom. Having previously suggested that mo- selves” (p. 27), demonstrating how, in their texts,
tivation can be generated when L3 learners are they “position themselves discursively as whole”
encouraged to focus on the multilingual person (p. 26).
they want to be (Henry, 2012) and should be sup- For the majority of Pavlenko’s respondents, the
ported in developing visions of a multilingual self identity emerging from experiences of speaking
(Henry, 2016), in the current article my aim is several languages was one of feeling empowered,
to explain how, through processes of emergence, enriched, and fortunate in having unique op-
multilingual self guides come into being and the portunities, choices, and perspectives. In other
effects they can have on L2 motivation. Because research, multilinguals report perceiving high lev-
the self-conception of being/becoming multilin- els of agency when making choices about lan-
gual is central in developing these ideas, the arti- guage use for personal expression and describe
cle begins by examining the identity experiences feelings of freedom and flexibility (Panayiotou,
of multilinguals.1 2006). Indeed, it is because being multilingual in-
volves selecting between languages that Kramsch
MULTILINGUALS’ IDENTITY EXPERIENCES and Huffmaster (2015) argue that multilinguals
can find themselves living “at the intersection” be-
Although the phenomenology of being a tween their languages (p. 132). Further, the same
speaker of several languages and the emotions researchers suggest that when making choices
attaching to self-identifications as being multilin- and exercising agency, multilinguals can experi-
gual have received little research attention, work ence being close to their different languages in
by Pavlenko (2006) shows how the identity experi- the sense of possessing a “multilingual self” that,
ences of people who switch between languages are akin to a “multilingual holy spirit,” can invade and
varied and multidimensional. Analysing over 1000 envelop the body (p. 133).
written responses to the question ‘Do you feel like What appears very clearly in this research is
a different person sometimes when you use your how, in addition to identities connected to the
different languages?’, she found that, while some different languages they speak, multilinguals can
multilinguals have ambivalent experiences of also develop an identity that transcends those
identity and belonging, others are proud of their that are language-specific. Whether character-
multilingualism and intercultural experiences. ized by diversity, hybridity, and integration, or by
15404781, 2017, 3, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/modl.12412 by University Of Glasgow, Wiley Online Library on [10/11/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
550 The Modern Language Journal 101 (2017)
discomfort, fragmentation, and loss, these multi- approaches to L1 and L2 acquisition ineffective”
lingual identities appear as phenomenologically (Herdina & Jessner, 2013, p. 755).
distinct. If, then, people who speak several lan- Embracing complexity perspectives requires a
guages develop an identity that is more than a sim- restructuring of the way that language acquisi-
ple conglomeration of discrete L2 identities, in- tion processes are conceptualized (de Bot, 2012,
teresting questions arise for motivation research. 2015). A challenge common for researchers in
What effects might a multilingual identity have both fields where focal phenomena continually
on motivation, and how, in relation to current “wax and wane depending on environmental de-
theorizing, can this identity be conceptualized? mands” (Herdina & Jessner, 2013, p. 754) is to de-
Addressing these questions, in the sections that velop designs and methodologies that can capture
follow I introduce a systemic model of multilin- the constant variation characteristic of the sys-
gual motivation that extends Dörnyei’s (2009) L2 tems they investigate (Hiver & Al-Hoorie, 2016).
Motivational Self System framework by addition- While an eclectic range of Complex Dynamic
ally accounting for the effects of identity experi- Systems (CDS) principles has been drawn upon,
ences connecting to a multilingual identification. some have been found to offer particularly “mean-
I begin by looking at the central role played by ingful and valid mappings on to the problem
the complexity principle of emergence in both spaces” of the respective fields (Larsen–Freeman
the L2 motivation and multilingualism research & Cameron, 2008, p. 25). While in motivation
paradigms. research attention is often paid to attractor states
(since they provide the closest CDS equivalents
COMPLEXITY THEORIES AND EMERGENCE to individual difference [ID] factors) (for empir-
ical work see, e.g., Dörnyei, 2014; Waninge et al.,
Although now some 20 years since Larsen– 2014; and for a conceptual account, Hiver, 2015),
Freeman (1997) first proposed that language de- in multilingual research focus is more frequently
velopment is best understood as a complex sys- directed to interactions between different language
tem, it is only recently that a ‘dynamic turn’ has systems (see, e.g., Lowie & Verspoor, 2011, 2015).
taken place in SLA, and only now that a criti- In both fields emergence is of central importance.
cal mass of researchers is engaging in complex- For example, changing degrees of observed moti-
ity conversations (de Bot, 2015; Larsen–Freeman, vational intensity (e.g., Yashima & Arano, 2015)
2013). With the recognition that complexity per- and the growth and attrition of linguistic phe-
spectives can serve “both as a useful heuristic nomena (e.g., de Leeuw, Opitz, & Lubinska, 2013;
and an integrative platform” (Vallacher & Nowak, Opitz, 2013) are both understood as the emer-
2007, p. 756), research in applied linguistics is gence of novel behaviors. Similarly, in the “com-
now moving beyond the purely metaphorical use plexity thought modeling” (Larsen–Freeman &
of complexity principles to the development and Cameron, 2008, p. 41) in the current article,
empirical application of complexity models (de emergence is the base concept upon which propo-
Bot, 2015; Verspoor, de Bot, & Lowie, 2011). sitions are constructed. Specifically, it is suggested
While interest in complexity conceptualizations that interactions between Lx and Ly self guides
of SLA phenomena is far from uniform, two ar- lead to the emergence of multilingual self guides,
eas where footholds are now firmly established and that these multilingual self guides impact on
are motivation (e.g., Dörnyei, 2009; Dörnyei et Lx and Ly motivation.
al., 2015) and multilingualism (e.g., Aronin & In complexity conceptualizations of psycholog-
Jessner, 2016; Jessner, 2006, 2008; Herdina & ical phenomena, the principles of emergence
Jessner, 2013). and self-organization—the way in which order
There are good reasons why people working and stability arise from the system’s intrinsic
with motivation and multilingualism are finding dynamics—provide root explanations of growth
complexity approaches appealing. While motiva- (Van Geert, 1991, 1994). Indeed, it is exactly be-
tion researchers are faced with the task of investi- cause system stability is an emergent property
gating a phenomenon that by nature is “dynamic that Byrne and Callaghan (2014) maintain that
and changeable” (Waninge, Dörnyei, & de Bot, it becomes necessary “to start from emergence
2014, p. 704), and where “the common experi- and develop a science that fits that crucial as-
ence would seem to be motivational flux rather pect of complex reality” (p. 7). Emergence can
than stability” (Dörnyei, 2001, p. 46), for those re- be understood as “the arising of novel and co-
searching multilingualism the level of complexity herent structures, patterns, and properties during
inherent in the multitude of interactions between the process of self-organization in complex sys-
different language systems “renders conventional tems” (Goldstein, 1999, p. 49), and an emergent
15404781, 2017, 3, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/modl.12412 by University Of Glasgow, Wiley Online Library on [10/11/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Alastair Henry 551
property as something that originates sponta- yses of self-concept development, Nowak et al.
neously from the system’s internal dynamics, (2005) explain that different self-concepts inter-
that is novel and not previously existing, and act to bring about the emergence of higher-order
that displays a distinct and identifiable wholeness properties and that, over time, these movements
(Byrne & Callaghan, 2014). As Larsen–Freeman “become coordinated to achieve a higher-order
& Cameron (2008) emphasize, emergent behav- action” (p. 352). In this framing, the self-concept
ior is something “more than the sum of its parts,” can therefore be understood as the product of
and a phenomenon that “cannot be explained re- constant interactions between different subsys-
ductively through the activity of the component tems (such as, e.g., self-efficacy and self-esteem).
parts” (p. 59). In CDS theory, systems are conceptualized as
chronically susceptible to the influence of other
THE SELF AS AN EMERGENT, HIGHER-LEVEL systems, and thus ‘open.’ Importantly, influence
SYSTEM between open systems does not only take place
between systems at the same level; rather, the di-
In complex dynamic systems, growth (devel- rection of the influence one system exerts on an-
opment) is a function of relationships between other can be vertical as well as horizontal. In this
the system’s different components, with inter- way the self-concept can be viewed as a ‘nested sys-
connections functioning to achieve system-level tem’ comprising multiple layers where change is
coherence (Vallacher & Nowak, 2007). Processes influenced by the “whole environment of other
of iterative and adaptive self-organization are relevant systems, including systems with which it
particularly transparent in psychological systems is interpenetrating and with which it shares sub-
such as personality or motivation, where inter- systems” (Byrne & Callaghan, 2014, p. 35). In-
actions and mutual adjustments of the system’s deed it is because complex dynamic systems have
different elements function to bring about dif- permeable boundaries and because interpenetra-
ferent states of higher-order coherence around tion between systems in a network is continu-
particular properties. This means that, over time, ous and bidirectional that, compared to hierar-
the system demonstrates stability and consistency chical metaphors, the notion of ‘nesting’ more
(Nowak et al., 2005; Vallacher & Nowak, 2007). In- effectively captures the complexity of system in-
deed, it is for this very reason that people’s belief, terrelations (Larsen–Freeman & Cameron, 2008;
attitude, and value systems, although not entirely Mercer, 2015).
fixed, tend to be robust and resilient to external
pressures or influences. It similarly accounts for RESEARCHING MULTILINGUALISM USING A
how complex systems such as the self-concept can SYSTEMS APPROACH
be characteristically stable, yet also responsive
to situational changes (Markus & Kunda, 1986), Researching multilinguals’ language develop-
and explains why a person who “consistently, over ment involves the investigation of related systems
a period of years, endorses a set of personality in common networks (Lowie & Verspoor, 2011).
attributes as a self-descriptive can demonstrate re- Any language system is likely to contain many
markable fluctuations in her self-view over the pe- subsystems nested one within another (Spoelman
riod of a few days” (Stein & Markus, 1996, p. 354). & Verspoor, 2010). In system ecologies, nesting
As previously discussed, in complex dynamic makes it possible to understand how different
systems higher-order cognitive states and accom- systems share a common structure, how interac-
panying patterns of behavior emerge without tions between systems at the same level lead to
higher-level supervision or control. However this the emergence of novel properties in those at
is rarely a single-step process. Rather, as Vallacher, a higher level, and how these higher-order sys-
Van Geert, and Nowak (2015) explain, it “typi- tems simultaneously exert an influence on lower-
cally involves many iterations of mutual adjust- level systems. In their systemic account of multilin-
ment among elements before they are sufficiently gual acquisition and development of the Dynamic
organized to promote a system-level property,” the Model of Multilingualism (DMM), Herdina and
same authors going on to point out that “once a Jessner (2002) argue that in situations of mul-
system-level state emerges, it stabilizes the system tiple learning/acquisition, languages cannot be
by constraining subsequent thought and behav- conceptualized as separate, autonomous systems.
ior” (p. 59). Thus, when structure and order ap- Rather, they need to be understood as interde-
pear at a higher level, stability is both contingent pendent, in constant interaction, and as consti-
upon and the outcome of the system’s internal tuting subsystems of an overall Multilingual System.
dynamical processes. Applying these ideas to anal- This point is similarly emphasized by Lowie and
15404781, 2017, 3, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/modl.12412 by University Of Glasgow, Wiley Online Library on [10/11/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
552 The Modern Language Journal 101 (2017)
Verspoor (2015) who offer the following instruc- and content to the identity subsystems of people
tive explanation: who speak or who are learning several languages.
Similarly to the M-factor/effect (Jessner, 2008),
Aronin (2016) conceptualizes multilinguality as
Similar to dynamic systems in the physical world, lan-
guages consist of many embedded subsystems. Lin-
an emergent property of multilingualism, ex-
guistic subsystems are for instance the phonology, plaining that it “appears from the use of two
the morphosyntax, and the semantics of a language, and more languages,” and needs to be seen as
which in turn consist of subsystems for different lan- “as a whole, not divided or separated into distinct
guages in a multilingual speaker (see Lowie & Ver- sub-identities” (p. 145). Arguing that multilingual
spoor, 2011). The language system itself is embedded identities are uniquely constituted by the combi-
as a subsystem in the larger cognitive system, which nation of languages a multilingual person speaks,
is embedded in a person’s body and mind. (p. 73) Aronin (2016) suggests that the linguistic, cogni-
tive, and emotional factors together have unique
In a multilingual language system, the behavior transformational effects, meaning that a multilin-
of individual language systems will depend on gual identity is fundamentally different from iden-
the behaviors of related systems. Consequently, tities typical of people who are either monolin-
it makes little sense to investigate language sys- gual or bilingual. As she explains, a multilingual
tems in isolation (Jessner, 2008), de Bot (2016) identity embraces “everything that results from us-
going so far as to question whether this is indeed ing and learning several languages, both in the
even possible. Nested together in a higher-level present and also potentially in the future,” and
structure, a multilingual’s different language includes “perceptions, attitudes and personal life
systems not only influence the development of scenarios, both real and possible” (Aronini 2016,
particular languages but also “the development p. 145). From a motivational perspective, these
of the overall multilingual system” (Jessner, possible life scenarios and the mental imagery in
2008, p. 274). In Herdina and Jessner’s (2002) which they are clothed can be understood as mo-
dynamic model the key feature is an emergent tivational self guides (Dörnyei, 2009)2 and con-
property; the ‘M(ultilingualism)-factor or ef- ceptualized as constituting part of a discrete iden-
fect.’ Manifested in communicative flexibility tity subsystem, the Multilingual Motivational Self
and divergent, creative, and original ways of System (see Figure 1). The existence of a multi-
thinking, the M-factor/effect encapsulates the lingual motivation system containing multilingual
skills theorized to be uniquely possessed by mul- self guides raises intriguing questions. How do
tilinguals. Importantly, in Herdina and Jessner’s these self guides come into being, what forms do
conceptualization, the M-factor/effect is both a they take, how are they cognitively represented,
precondition for and result of the emergence of and how can motivational power be harnessed in
multilingualism. As an emergent higher-order promoting multiple language learning? These are
property, the M-factor/effect can be understood questions that inform the rest of this article.
as the outcome of multiple bidirectional and
reciprocally causational interactions between the HOW DO MULTILINGUAL SELF GUIDES
multilingual system and its nested subsystems. COME INTO BEING?

THE MULTILINGUAL SYSTEM AND THE In learning more than one language, a person
MULTILINGUAL IDENTITY SYSTEM can develop self guides for each language, for
example an ideal Lx self, and an ideal Ly self.
For multilinguals the overall multilingual sys- Because in multilingual language learning peo-
tem (Jessner, 2008) comprises networks of nested ple draw constantly on linguistic resources from
subsystems (Lowie & Verspoor, 2015). One of the range of languages they know (Hammarberg,
these subsystems is the multilingual identity 2009; Jessner, 2006), these self guides are also
system. Although unexplored from a systems likely to interact in working cognition. To explain,
perspective, in mapping the psychology of mul- in third language learning, even when there may
tilingualism, Aronin (2016) has developed the be psychotypological differences, previously
concept of multilinguality to capture the unique learned languages can play a role in processes
identity experiences, emotions, self-knowledge, of acquiring a new language, often functioning
and cognitive processes that comprise a multi- as a supply of linguistic support (De Angelis &
lingual identity (see also Aronin & Ó Laoire, Dewaele, 2011; Jessner, 2006, 2008; Sanchez,
2004). In an ecology of multilingual systems, 2011). When one L2 plays a supporting role in
multilinguality functions to provide substance the processes of learning another, it becomes
15404781, 2017, 3, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/modl.12412 by University Of Glasgow, Wiley Online Library on [10/11/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Alastair Henry 553
FIGURE 1
A Systemic Representation of Multilingual Identity

Note. The Multilingual Identity System encompasses the identity dimensions of multilinguality (Aronin, 2016) and is
a subsystem nested within the overall Multilingual System (Jessner, 2008). The Multilingual Motivational Self System
is conceptualized as a subsystem nested within the Multilingual Identity System.

cognitively activated. It is in the context of such depleting motivational energy. Over time, and as
situations that interactions between the respec- a consequence of a multitude of situations where
tive Lx and Ly self guides can occur. To take an competition of this sort arises, one of the two
empirical example, for Swedish students learn- ideal self guides is likely to become chronically
ing LOTEs, Henry (2010) showed how, in L3 dominant. This means that, whenever activated,
learning situations, their L2 English self-concept it automatically poses a threat to its counterpart.
was frequently active in cognition and how in- Eventually, a point is reached where it systemat-
teractions between L2 and L3 self-concepts took ically overrides the other self guide, effectively
place. It is from such interactions, together with nullifying its motivational power. This would be
the influences of attitudes and beliefs about characteristic of a situation where, for exam-
multilingualism, that multilingual self guides are ple, a student in a northern European country
suggested to emerge. learns two languages at school—e.g., English and
The types of self guides that emerge from Lx Spanish—and where differences in social pres-
and Ly interactions and the effects they have on tige and opportunities for use greatly favor one
motivation will be a function of the nature of (English) at the expense of the other (Spanish).
the interaction taking place and the influences However, it is not simply the case that dynam-
of other interpenetrating systems.3 While inter- ical interactions between the ideal Lx self and
actions between Lx and Ly self guides will be the ideal Ly self solely impact on their intrin-
complex and highly dependent on situational sic relations. As a consequence of processes of
contingencies (and because it is never possible to emergence, higher-level effects might also be
predict the types of higher-order properties that anticipated. In this northern European scenario
emerge when complex dynamic systems inter- a conceivable effect would be the emergence of a
penetrate one another), for the purpose of illus- higher-order self guide representing an identity
tration two scenarios common in third language as someone comfortable and confident in speak-
learning can be imagined. In both, a person is ing their native language (e.g., Swedish) and the
learning two foreign languages. While in the first currently dominant global language (English),
scenario interactions between the ideal Lx and but not perceiving any additional need or having
ideal Ly selves are antagonistic, in the second they any particular interest in speaking another lan-
function in a mutually complementary manner. guage (see Henry, 2011). Similar to individuals
We can first imagine a situation where the rela- wishing to remain “steadfastly monolingual,” it is
tionship between the ideal Lx self and the ideal Ly an identity founded on the person’s indifference
self is uneasy. When, during instances of crosslin- to multilingual competence and characterized
guistic interaction, both become activated in by a desire to “insulate themselves” from other
cognition, competition for the learner’s generally languages (Douglas Fir Group, 2016, p. 26).
limited cognitive resources is likely to take place This higher-order self guide can be understood
(see Henry, 2012, 2016, for a full discussion). as a contentedly bilingual self. The emergence of
When this happens, the learner can experience a contentedly bilingual self would likely have
discomfort, the need to address the conflict a systematic and enduring impact on learning
15404781, 2017, 3, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/modl.12412 by University Of Glasgow, Wiley Online Library on [10/11/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
554 The Modern Language Journal 101 (2017)
behavior in the Ly (Spanish) classroom. For cal causation, emergent properties interact with
example, its effects would likely be manifested the system components whose interactions first
in the routine approaches to learning and the led to their appearance. These processes give rise
passivity characteristic of demotivated learners to new behaviors, something Byrne and Callaghan
(Dörnyei & Ushioda, 2011) and those who, as (2014) make clear in their discussion of emer-
Kramsch (2009) has observed, “just sit out the gence and the bi-directionality of interactions
language class as a boring but necessary step and behaviors. As they point out, to understand
towards graduation [and who] find themselves processes of emergence, we need to concern
vindicated in their monolingual selves” (p. 5).4 ourselves “not only with interactions among com-
The second scenario involves an altogether dif- ponents at the same level but also with downward
ferent situation. Here the relationship between effects.” Continuing, they explain that this means
the ideal Lx and ideal Ly self guides is harmo- that “sometimes we have to explain parts in, at
nious. Our student learning English and Spanish least to some degree, terms of wholes” (p. 22).
may not only enjoy learning processes and be Revisiting the scenarios previously sketched
instrumentally motivated to develop skills in both out, we can understand then that the multilin-
languages, but may also view the opportunity to gual self guides also impact on the Lx and LY self
become proficient in a language not everyone can guides. While the contentedly bilingual self can have
speak (Spanish) as of significant personal impor- the effect of further weakening the power of the
tance (Henry, 2011). Here, when crosslinguistic ideal Ly self, the ideal multilingual self can have
interactions take place and the ideal English and the opposite effect, enhancing the strength of the
ideal Spanish self guides become active in cog- ideal Ly self. For students who develop a content-
nition, they will have a mutually complementary edly bilingual self, this downwards process can man-
relation. In this case, interactions lead to the ifest itself in notions that English is sufficient, that
emergence of a self guide reflecting the person’s knowledge of English will comfortably deliver all
aspirations to be/become multilingual, an ideal the opportunities needed in life, and that learn-
multilingual self (see Figure 2). This multilingual ing Spanish is simply not worth the effort. For
self guide would function as a powerful source of those who develop an ideal multilingual self, the
motivation to learn Spanish, generating energy ideal Spanish self is enhanced. As part of a project
additional to motivation derived from the ideal of personal development the goal of which is
Spanish-speaking self. to be/become multilingual, developing skills and
To summarize, in both scenarios the contentedly becoming closer to the Spanish-speaking person
bilingual self and the ideal multilingual self can be they aspire to be takes on another dimension (a
conceptualized as emergent properties of the point returned to later).
interactions between ideal Lx and ideal Ly selves We also need to note that the emergence
(Figure 2) and as components of a higher-level of a higher-level ideal multilingual self and the
multilingual motivational self system (Figure 1). concurrent strengthening of the ideal Ly self
Having a distinct motivational function, these has two additionally important consequences.
multilingual self guides operate additionally to First, as a higher-level self-concept, the ideal mul-
the Lx and Ly self guides. In the first scenario, tilingual self will create increased stability and
the contentedly bilingual self has a demotivating cohesion within the multilingual identity system.
effect. In the second, the effect is the opposite; This means that, whenever the Lx and Ly selves
here the ideal multilingual self functions to gener- are simultaneously active in working cognition,
ate motivational energy additional to that created they are likely to achieve greater alignment and
by the desire to become a proficient speaker of function in a more consistently complementary
English and of Spanish. manner. In such situations, alongside its lin-
guistically supportive function (Jessner, 2006,
THE IMPACT ON Lx AND Ly SELF GUIDES 2008), the activation of the Lx in Ly learning
can be conceptualized as having a motivationally
At this point it is important to recall that in supportive function.
complex dynamic systems emergent properties However, while a more frequent alignment
have a recognizable wholeness. The desire to and consistently supportive motivational func-
be/become multilingual is not simply the sum of tion can reduce the likelihood of antagonism,
the desire to speak Swedish, English, and Span- competition between the Lx and Ly selves may
ish, but is phenomenologically distinct. However, never be entirely eradicated. In situations of
by the same token, neither are emergent behav- affective strain, stress, or discomfort (Dewaele
iors purely holistic. Rather, in processes of recipro- & MacIntyre, 2016) it is possible for tension to
15404781, 2017, 3, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/modl.12412 by University Of Glasgow, Wiley Online Library on [10/11/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Alastair Henry 555
FIGURE 2
The Ideal Multilingual Self as an Emergent Property of Interactions Between the Ideal Selves of the Different
Languages Known and/or Being Learned

emerge within the otherwise harmonious rela- tion to learn additional languages, the ideal multi-
tionship between the Lx and Ly selves. In such lingual self can have a positive effect. Moving on,
situations, competition for cognitive resources is the question that next arises concerns the ways
still likely to occur. However, as a consequence in which these multilingual self guides might be
of the downward effects of the ideal multilingual represented in cognition. Because questions con-
self, when competition does occur the Ly self will cerning the manner in which the ideal L2 self
display greater resilience in the face of threat, is mentally represented have not previously been
any negative impact from Lx being less enduring. addressed in L2 motivation research, and because
It is exactly this effect that is highlighted of the importance of the issue in understanding
in Henry’s (2011) study of the motivation of the motivational potential of the ideal multilin-
Swedish secondary students learning English and gual self and for the design of appropriate inter-
a LOTE. One of the participants, a 16-year-old ventions, a closer insight into this fascinating area
boy fascinated by languages and the opportuni- of self theory is provided.
ties they provide for communicating with people As previously made clear, the self-concept can
in different parts of the world, described occa- be conceptualized as a multidimensional, mul-
sional antagonism between his English and his tifaceted structure that researchers define vari-
Russian self-concepts. However, because being ously in terms of hierarchies, prototypes, spaces,
multilingual was an important part of his identity, and networks (Markus & Wurf, 1987). The self-
his determination to succeed with Russian was concept comprises collections of self-schemas
difficult to dent. Not only was his ideal Russian (generalizations about the self) generated from
self highly resilient to threats from the English previous experiences (Markus, 1977; Markus &
self-concept, when they did occur he was quickly Sentis, 1982). The role of these self-schemas is to
able to suppress the recognition of being commu- mediate intra- and interpersonal processes, and
nicatively more competent in English, evidenced they function in ways that represent the self as
in his steadfast commitment to remaining in both ‘the knower’ and ‘the known’ (Markus &
‘Russian-speaking mode’ (Dewaele, 2001). Wurf, 1987). Because self-conceptions are never
cognitively active in isolation, the self-concept is
best viewed as a “continually active, shifting ar-
ray of accessible self-knowledge” (Markus & Wurf,
HOW IS THE IDEAL MULTILINGUAL SELF
1987, p. 306). Research demonstrates that numer-
MENTALLY REPRESENTED?
ous representations of the self coexist in cogni-
To recap, I have so far suggested that, for peo- tion and that “considerable complexity exists in
ple learning two or more additional languages, the networks of self information held by each per-
processes of emergence produce multilingual self son” (Hinkley & Andersen, 1996, p. 1281). It is
guides. These self guides can be conceptualized as for this reason that Hinkley and Andersen sug-
constituents of a higher-level multilingual motiva- gest that the self “is perhaps best understood as a
tional self system. While the contentedly bilingual self ‘family of selves’ with various overlapping resem-
is suggested to have a negative impact on motiva- blances and with some selves more prominent,
15404781, 2017, 3, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/modl.12412 by University Of Glasgow, Wiley Online Library on [10/11/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
556 The Modern Language Journal 101 (2017)
elaborated, and accessible than others” (p. 1281, the essence of CLT—it is hypothesized that there
emphasis added). exists “a common mechanism that people use
While considerable work has been carried out to travel mentally over time, space, people, and
into the visionary dimensions of L2 motivational possibilities” (Fujita, Trope, & Liberman, 2015,
self guides (Dörnyei & Chan, 2013; Dörnyei & p. 405).
Kubanyiova, 2014; You & Chan, 2015), the way in The next thing that needs noting is that the
which ideal L2 selves are construed remains to be further away in psychological distance an event
fully explored. Construal refers to the manner in might be, the greater challenge there is in con-
which a self-concept is mentally represented, the structing an image. For proximal events the
nature of the image, and the level of abstraction conjuring of mental representations is facilitated
(Liberman & Trope, 1998). In a language learn- by drawing on the rich and detailed experiences
ing context, construals of self guides are likely to of the present. Not only this, but representa-
involve representations of L2 interaction. How- tions can be individualized and tailored to incor-
ever, the types of interaction featured in people’s porate uniquely idiosyncratic features prescient
mental representations of future language use are to the immediate environment. However, as the
likely to differ. While some people might con- psychological distance increases, opportunities to
jure representations that involve interaction and draw on direct experiences decrease. This means
communication taking place in the context of in- that we have to rely on processes of construal,
terpersonal relationships (e.g., sitting in a café, mentally constructing representations of distant
chatting with imaginary friends), for others rep- events from the sometimes limited knowledge
resentations may focus less on person-to-person currently available.
exchanges and be instead characterized by images Travelling across psychological distance de-
where productive activities (e.g., giving a speech), mands the use of cognitive abstraction. Cognitive ab-
or receptive activities (reading an email) are more straction is a reductive process that, as Fujita et al.
generally featured.5 (2015) explain, “entails extracting the essential,
A particularly important aspect of mental con- core (i.e. gist) aspects of objects or events while
strual concerns the psychological distance from the ignoring surface-level, or verbatim, features”
imagined event. Psychological distance, which (p. 405). By means of illustration, they provide
is the central concept in Trope and Liberman’s the example of making preparations for a seaside
(2010) construal-level theory (CLT), is the extent vacation:
to which an event is removed from the person’s
direct experience. Psychological distance is Consider the challenge of constructing a represen-
highly individual and is anchored in a person’s tation about a distant future event. Much about
perception of themselves in the here and now. distant future events is subject to change. In plan-
Events that are further from the here-and-now ning a beach vacation for next year, for example,
point of reference are considered to be psycho- it is unclear—and perhaps even unknowable—what
logically distant. Conversely, those that are closer clothes one might need. We cannot know exactly
are considered to be psychologically proximal. Psy- what the weather will be like, and our waistlines
chological distance exists across four dimensions: might change, constraining which bathing suits may
time, space, social distance, and hypotheticality. be suitable. There are some features, however, that
are likely to remain invariant irrespective of how that
While each of these distances is of itself distinct,
future event manifests. For example, for many of us, a
the psychological operations involved in travel- beach vacation means that we will be on a beach with
ling mentally across the different dimensions are a drink in hand, feeling the warmth of the sun on our
the same (Bar-Anan, Liberman, & Trope, 2006). faces. We may not know which beach, what drink, or
In addition to showing how the manner in how warm, but every beach vacation will have these
which people conceive of events that are tempo- elements. This schematic information about beach
rally distant is phenomenologically similar to the vacations is sufficient to create a representation with
ways in which they conceive of events that are which to make predictions about and construct pref-
physically distant, socially distant, or hypotheti- erences related to this event. As the event becomes
cal, Trope and Liberman (2010) also demonstrate more proximal we can update this schematic repre-
sentation with the idiosyncratic and unique features
that thinking about a distant event on one dimen-
that distinguish this particular manifestation of the
sion facilitates thinking about distant events on event from others like it. Thus, with greater proxim-
another. Thus, when invited to imagine an event ity, we can represent a beach vacation as enjoying this
that is distant on one dimension, people have stretch of Miami Beach, drinking this mojito made
a tendency to similarly imagine events that are at that drink stand, while basking in the 90-degree
distant on the others. Consequently—and this is weather with gentle breezes and no clouds. (p. 405)
15404781, 2017, 3, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/modl.12412 by University Of Glasgow, Wiley Online Library on [10/11/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Alastair Henry 557
In this example it is the process of abstraction— ideal multilingual self are construed. Because dur-
or high-level construal—that, when the vacation ing the course of learning the Ly students will
is first conceived of, allows the person to ex- have experienced TL cultures mediated in var-
tract invariant features from their ‘beach vacation ious multimodal forms and will have encoun-
schemata.’ tered TL speakers (if not in real life, in books,
videos, and on the Internet), these experiences
Psychologically Distant Events and Possibilities Are provide a body of phenomenological material
Difficult to Concretize from which an ideal Ly self could—potentially at
least—be constructed. While, in the absence of
When “distant times, places, perspectives of imagery training (Dörnyei & Kubanyiova, 2014),
other people, and possibilities” are envisaged, this may not always be easy, the mental image of
they are represented cognitively as high-level con- speaking a familiar language in known surround-
struals (Fujita et al., 2015, p. 406). In fact, the ings is nevertheless likely to be visually more de-
psychologically distant objects or states to which tailed and more concrete than an image of be-
connections are made may not be immediately ing/becoming multilingual. The experience of
obvious; nor are they necessarily linked to cur- being/becoming multilingual is hard to represent
rent situations or events. Moreover, they may not in mental imagery; not only are there more hy-
readily lend themselves to mental concretization, potheticals and various possibilities, but as demon-
and may even be abstruse. Generally, they will lack strated by Pavlenko (2006) and Kramsch and
the degree of detail characteristic of lower-level Huffmaster (2015), it is a highly subjective expe-
construals. To illustrate the difference between rience. This means that the level of construal of
a low- and a high-level construal, let us take the the ideal multilingual self will be much higher,
following example of an imagined future event. and that images of being/becoming multilingual
A student from Holland has been delegated to in the future will lack the degree of detail more
attend an international conference as part of a generally characteristic of the learner’s ideal Ly
school’s democracy program. Apart from making self.
a short presentation about the citizenship work
going on at her school, she knows that she will be Abstract Images of an Ideal Multilingual Self Can Be
spending 3 days at the venue, meeting fellow del- Highly Motivational
egates from all over the world. While preparing
for the conference she might in her mind’s eye To recap once again, I have suggested that
imagine herself giving a fluent and well-received for people who develop an ideal multilingual
presentation in English. Here the image of this self, motivation to learn the Ly can be greatly
5-minute event would likely contain a degree of enhanced in that developing TL competence be-
detail (the script of the speech may already be comes part of a larger identity project. However,
prepared and the clothes she will be wearing al- at the same time, it is also clear that the image
ready decided on). At the same conference, our of an ideal multilingual self is likely to lack detail
student might also imagine mingling at various and elaboration. How then, if the construal of
receptions and coffee breaks, talking with other an ideal multilingual self takes place at a higher
participants not only in English, but also perhaps level of abstraction, can it have positive effects
in other languages she knows. However, the man- on motivational energy? Indeed, the reasonable
ner in which these two types of activity are men- assumption would be that an abstract and loosely
tally construed may be very different. With regard defined image of being multilingual would be un-
to the mingling, there are more hypotheticals and likely to have any substantial motivational impact.
various different possibilities; she has no idea in Counterintuitively, research indicates that ab-
what directions conversations might lead, where stract construals of future identities can indeed
they might take place, the type of interaction, her be highly motivational. To begin with, studies
mood and emotional state, or what languages she show that when people are prompted to think
might speak. Consequently, the images of casual about psychologically distal future selves, their
social interaction that she conjures up are likely construals tend to be abstract. However, because
to be more abstract, diffuse, and lacking the de- they are abstract, the focus of the image shifts
tail of the more concrete mental image of the to the essence of the experience (Wakslak et al.,
presentation. 2008). To best understand this point, it is use-
Turning to a language learner’s ideal selves, ful to return to the example of the beach va-
similar differences are likely to be observed in cation. As Fujita et al. (2015) explain, because
the manner in which the ideal Ly self and the an abstract, high-level construal of a vacation
15404781, 2017, 3, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/modl.12412 by University Of Glasgow, Wiley Online Library on [10/11/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
558 The Modern Language Journal 101 (2017)
in the sun might lack clarity and detail, it can & Houser–Marko, 2001) show how an ideal self
instead function in a way that forges valuable that is psychologically distant and construed at a
connections “to other remote life experiences high level of abstraction has the potential to have
such as seeing people whom we have not seen a substantial effect on motivation (Oyserman &
in a while, visiting exotic locations, and more James, 2011).
generally, highlighting our distant dreams and as-
pirations” (p. 406). Consequently, while represen- MENTAL REPRESENTATIONS OF IDEAL
tations of psychologically distant events might be MULTILINGUAL SELVES
abstract and diffuse in terms of detail, they are rich
and elaborated in terms of the aspirations that they While the previously cited studies by Pavlenko
embody. (2006) and Kramsch and Huffmaster (2015) sug-
Further, because construals of psychologically gest that people who speak several languages are
distant events are inherently abstract, they be- likely to possess some form of multilingual iden-
come freed from situational constraints. This tity, a question of particular interest for motiva-
means the focus of the image tends to shift at- tion research is whether students learning two
tention inwards towards the individual’s central, or more foreign languages—and who are in the
intrinsic, and more generally idealistic concerns process of being/becoming multilinguals—might
(Kivetz & Tyler, 2007). Specifically, research shows also develop a multilingual identity. Furthermore,
that future selves that are psychologically distant if they do, how might a multilingual identity
tend to be construed in the form of generic per- be mentally construed? Carrying out extensive
sonality traits and that this is particularly true in ethnographic research at a secondary school in
situations when people are asked to imagine ac- Sweden where language learning was actively pro-
tions that are related to a distal future self (Pronin moted by the school’s leaders and where large
& Ross, 2006). As Kivetz and Tyler (2007) explain, numbers of students were learning not just one
in the construal of future selves that are psycho- foreign language in addition to English but of-
logically distant, people are more likely to focus ten two, and sometimes even three, I had these
on aspects that are central to their notion of self; questions constantly in my mind.6 During focus
which align with their core values, principles, and group interviews I invited students learning two
beliefs; and which reflect “the person’s sense of or more LOTEs whether they identified them-
true self” (p. 193). selves as multilinguals and, if they did, to de-
The observed tendency of an increasing fo- scribe the images that came to mind when think-
cus on self-relevant content correspondent with ing about a multilingual identity.7 Similarly to
higher levels of construal is important. This is be- the multilinguals studied by Pavlenko (2006) and
cause, when people pursue goals that are person- Kramsch and Huffmaster (2015), the students
ally meaningful and which, in different ways, con- emphasized the sensory and perceptual aspects
nect with their inner sense of self, they experience of being/becoming multilingual. Rather than de-
greater satisfaction and demonstrate greater on- scribing images containing depictions of imag-
task perseverance (Sheldon & Elliot, 1998, 1999). ined events or situations (for example, switching
Providing a comprehensive theory of the mo- between languages during interaction), the stu-
tivational effects of goals that are more funda- dents instead described images using figurative
mentally connected to a person’s central identity language, the metaphors they employed indexing
concerns and to their inner or ‘core’ sense of openness, empathy, and discovery:
self, Sheldon and Elliot (1999) explain that self-
For me openness is something very important and
concordant goals can be conceptualized as a type of
being able to speak several languages opens more
self-concept. Further, given that a self-concordant doors. You are less limited. And so when I see myself
goal can “energize and direct so much of peo- in the future, I have an image that I am multilingual
ple’s behavior” (p. 485), it is a self-concept of and I have opened more doors. (Felicia, grade 8)
considerable importance, Dörnyei, Henry, and
Muir (2016) explaining how the possession of a Being able to speak lots of languages, it is a feeling.
self-concordant goal is centrally characteristic for That you are not like stuck in your little box. You are
language learners who experience periods of in- open to things and go out and discover, so to say. It
is a feeling, how should I put it, of expanding your
tense and sustained motivation. In combination,
awareness in some way. (Daniel, grade 9)
research where construal-level theory is applied
to possible selves (Kivetz & Tyler, 2007; Pronin & Sure, it would be fun to go there and speak to people.
Ross, 2006) and studies demonstrating the moti- But for me it is more like an interest. That there are
vational impact of self-concordant goals (Sheldon languages all around me. (Max, grade 9)
15404781, 2017, 3, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/modl.12412 by University Of Glasgow, Wiley Online Library on [10/11/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Alastair Henry 559
You can feel that you are a more social person. You plied disciplines show how visions of an imagined
can perhaps experience yourself as more social. But future can have powerful effects on current be-
at the same time, for me anyway, it is the feeling that haviors, but numerous ideal self-based interven-
I am another person. (Gino, grade 9) tions have been successfully carried out in areas as
Not only do these descriptions reflect a diverse as school achievement, psychosocial well-
deeper feeling and personal value attached to being, career counselling, and in various ther-
being/becoming multilingual, but the students apeutic contexts (Dunkel & Kerpelman, 2006).
also emphasize the social and intersubjective ben- Importantly, there are now a number of stud-
efits of a multilingual repertoire: ies showing how motivation is boosted when lan-
guage learners take part in imagery training de-
I don’t think that it really makes such a great dif- signed to stimulate projections of future L2 use
ference which languages I can speak. Sure, I want (e.g., Chan, 2014; Mackay, 2014; Magid, 2014;
to speak them well. But I want to speak several lan- Sampson, 2012). Given indications from a num-
guages. (Carmen, grade 8) ber of educational contexts of declining motiva-
It feels powerful. It feels good to be able to speak tion to learn LOTEs (see, e.g., Busse, this issue),
several. But it is not only the feeling of being able a self guide centered on the vision of be-
to speak several languages. But more the sense of ing/becoming multilingual could have a signifi-
a feeling of having things in common with others. cant role to play in generating motivation.
(Sophie, grade 9) While the notion of multilingual self guides
is novel,9 the tools teachers need to support
Downplaying the sense of a desire to become
students in generating and maintaining a posi-
proficient in a particular language, for these
tive ‘self-as-multilingual’ image are not. In recent
students—and for others like them who attend
years important work has taken place in develop-
schools where languages are valued and linguis-
ing innovative visualization techniques that can
tic diversity is celebrated—the abstract image of
be profitably used in language classrooms (see,
an ideal multilingual self may have a powerful
e.g., Arnold, Puchta, & Rinvolucri, 2007; Dörnyei
effect on motivation. Specifically, the desire to
& Kubanyiova, 2014; Hadfield & Dörnyei, 2013).
be/become multilingual can function to shift the
With relative ease, many of these programs and
ultimate goal of learning inwards, an idealized
activities can be adapted to focus on visions of be-
multilingual identity becoming part of the collec-
ing/becoming multilingual, thus providing gen-
tion of core self-knowledge in the person’s self-
uine opportunities for students to develop and
concept and aligned with their ‘true self’ (Kivetz
enhance positive multilingual identities. For stu-
& Tyler, 2007). Of course, students who perceive
dents with a cosmopolitan outlook, work can use-
of themselves as being/becoming multilingual
fully be directed to strengthening a multilingual
and who attune to the affordances in educational
vision, transforming it into action, and keeping it
and social environments are frequently outnum-
alive (Dörnyei & Kubanyiova, 2014). For students
bered by those who lack similar desires or for
lacking motivation to learn LOTEs, two interven-
whom the social/educational environment quite
tion types with a focus on multilingual self guides
simply does not provide such opportunities.8 Nev-
have the potential to increase motivated learning
ertheless, multilingual self guides can still have an
behavior.
important role to play in generating motivation to
learn languages other than English. It is to this as
yet untapped potential that I now turn in the final Interventions for Generating an Ideal Multilingual
sections of the article. Self

For students lacking a particular reason for


HOW CAN THE MOTIVATIONAL POTENTIAL learning LOTEs or who lack an affective attach-
OF MULTILINGUAL SELF GUIDES BE ment or cultural affiliation to the TL, the desire
HARNESSED IN PROMOTING MULTIPLE to perceive themselves as multilingual may have
LANGUAGE LEARNING? greater motivational potential than a desire to
The Transformative Potential of Possible Selves be proficient in the TL per se. Outside of soci-
etally bilingual contexts, opportunities to develop
It is the transformational potential of possi- a language-mediated identity and an ideal L2 self
ble selves that is the most compelling feature capable of generating motivation are often lim-
of Dörnyei’s (2009) reconceptualization of lan- ited for LOTEs. While TL experiences and expo-
guage learning motivation. Not only does an sure/use outside the classroom may not always be
abundance of studies in psychology’s various ap- extensive, inside the classroom, as Block (2007)
15404781, 2017, 3, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/modl.12412 by University Of Glasgow, Wiley Online Library on [10/11/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
560 The Modern Language Journal 101 (2017)
has suggested, there may “be too much first gual self guide. As a consequence of antagonistic
language-mediated baggage and interference for relations between Lx and Ly self guides and neg-
profound changes to occur in the individual’s ative attitudes and beliefs about the value of mul-
conceptual system and his/her sense of self in tilingualism, many students may develop a con-
the TL” (p. 144). At the same time, and ever in- tentedly bilingual self. As previously suggested,
creasingly so, school environments provide mul- this self guide will have a negative impact on L3
tilingual spaces within which identity work can motivation. However, it is important to note that
take place (Canagarajah, 2013). As a consequence possible selves are dynamic, malleable, and sus-
of migration and transnational family configura- ceptible to change (Henry, 2015). Not only do
tions, many classrooms today are populated by changes take place to the quality and manner of
students who speak different languages in differ- visualization of L2 self guides (You & Chan, 2015),
ent situations (i.e., at home, at school, and in in- but research shows that shifts from one possible
teraction with peers) (Duff, 2015). In the chang- self to another can also take place (see, e.g., Chan,
ing school and free-time environments shaped Dörnyei, & Henry, 2015). Consequently, for stu-
by global migration flows, not only is multilin- dents with a contentedly bilingual self, interven-
gualism the norm, but being a speaker of several tions designed to transform this into a feared self
languages constitutes a valuable social resource could be of real value.
(Blommaert, 2010). In these spaces, the sense of In addition to an ideal self, people will of-
being/becoming multilingual can have a differ- ten have a counterbalancing feared self. This is
ent meaning than in the foreign language class- particularly common in situations where possi-
room. Specifically, self-identification as a speaker bilities for different visions exist (Oyserman &
of several languages can provide access to partic- Markus, 1990). For students with a contentedly
ipation in language practices taking place within bilingual self, an important intervention would in-
the multilingual spaces of local contexts (Gynne volve transforming it into a feared bilingual self. This
& Bagga–Gupta, 2013). involves challenging the self-conception of being
Given the combination of difficulties stu- someone comfortable in only speaking their L1
dents can experience in developing LOTE iden- and English and triggering a sense of discom-
tities and the motivational potential of an fort associated with being a person who, in a
ideal multilingual self where mental imagery globalized world and (possibly) a local multilin-
is more inwardly oriented, an important strat- gual school context, only speaks their L1 and En-
egy involves orienting to local multilingual com- glish. As with the ideal multilingual self, motiva-
munities and language practices. Specifically, tion would derive not from a desire to speak a
interventions could valuably be directed to the ex- particular foreign language (although over time
ploration of different experiences of being mul- this would hopefully be a valuable side-effect),
tilingual within the wider school community. By but rather of becoming a participant in global
focusing on the unique and often positive ex- (and local) multilingual communities (Kramsch
periences of translanguaging (García, 2009) and & Whiteside, 2008). By working to change stu-
the use of language resources in critical and cre- dents’ beliefs and attitudes, not only might it be
ative ways (Li Wei, 2011), work on constructing an possible to transform a contentedly bilingual self
ideal multilingual self can be carried out. If pro- into a feared bilingual self, but also to create
vided with opportunities to explore multilingual- conditions under which an ideal multilingual self
ism in its broadest sense (i.e., not just in an FL con- might emerge.
text) and with self-enhancing interventions that
encourage students to begin to perceive of them- The Role of Motivation in a Holistic Pedagogy for
selves as being/becoming multilingual in local as Multilingualism
well as global contexts, the positive effects that an
ideal multilingual self can have on LOTE motiva- Shifting focus from language learners to lan-
tion can begin to take place. guage teachers, additional value may attach
to working with motivational strategies cen-
Interventions for Transforming the Contentedly tered around multilingual self-guides. As Cenoz
Bilingual Self and Gorter (2015) report, there is a current
trend in language education toward adopting a
While the ideal multilingual self may be an im- holistic approach to multilingualism aimed at
portant source of motivation in situations where “integrating the curricula of the different lan-
people learn more than one foreign language, it guages so as to activate the resources multilin-
is worth recalling that it is not the only multilin- gual speakers have” (p. 4). In a holistic approach,
15404781, 2017, 3, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/modl.12412 by University Of Glasgow, Wiley Online Library on [10/11/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Alastair Henry 561
students are encouraged to use their resources “predominant monolingual theories, constructs,
“cross-linguistically” and, in so doing, to be- and research practices” (Ortega, 2014, p. 33). In
come more efficient in their learning than would the context of a “rethought SLA,” where it has
be the case were languages to be taught sep- become necessary to reconsider and revise the
arately (Cenoz & Gorter, 2011). The impor- models used to research language development
tance of a multilingual pedagogical approach (Douglas Fir Group, 2016, p. 38) and in line
is similarly emphasized by other multilingual- with the dynamic turn in applied linguistics (de
ism researchers, notably De Angelis (2011) and Bot, 2015), Dörnyei’s reconceptualization of L2
Otwinowska (2014). While Blackledge and Creese motivation as a self system is propitious. With L2
(2010) identify a need to view all languages spo- motivation conceptualized as a self system, the
ken in any given classroom as a collective social re- motivational systems of different languages can
source, in their ‘translanguaging’ approach, Gar- be understood as functioning not only within
cía and Li Wei (2014) argue that language teach- ecologies of other motivational systems, but also
ers need to see the language practices of learners within other psychological, psycholinguistic, and
as a single linguistic repertoire containing “fea- social systems. Moreover, because the self guides
tures that have been societally constructed as be- forming the centerpieces of Dörnyei’s model are
longing to two separate languages” (p. 2). inherently dynamic (Henry, 2015; You & Chan,
In the development of a holistic pedagogy for 2015), the motivational self system approach
multilingualism, motivation has a central part to is ideally suited to studying the evolution and
play. Not only would a focus on multilingual self development of multilinguals’ language learning
guides facilitate processes where languages are motivation as a dynamic process (de Bot, 2012;
reframed as a collective linguistic and social re- de Bot & Jaensch, 2015).
source, but it could also have the structural effect The conceptual extension of Dörnyei’s self sys-
of providing concrete opportunities for enhanc- tem model generated in the “complexity thought
ing the integration of language teaching. Specifi- modelling” (Larsen–Freeman & Cameron, 2008)
cally, if the same interventions, the same visualiza- carried out in this article is intended to encourage
tion techniques, and the same activities designed a shift away from the monolingual mindset char-
to develop visions of being a competent Lx or Ly acteristic of much of the L2 motivation research
speaker were also used to promote visions of be- so far conducted. Not only might the multilin-
ing a competent multilingual speaker, and if this gual motivational framework proposed here facil-
uniform collection of strategies was systematically itate the construction of holistic designs to inves-
employed across language classrooms (including tigate the motivational effects of cross-linguistic
English, mother tongue, and heritage classes), interactions, but it also opens up possibilities
important added value would follow. Given that to explore motivation deriving from a broader
language teachers may embrace pedagogies of identity that encompasses, but in important ways
multilingualism but lack opportunities for con- transcends, the language-specific identities and
crete forms of collaboration (Haukås, 2016), a concerns in a multilingual person’s life. Finally,
cross-language motivational program based on vi- because Dörnyei’s self system conceptualization is
sualization techniques designed to nurture and accompanied by a toolbox of visualization tech-
develop multilingual identities could provide an niques for enhancing the motivational capacity
important space where teachers can work to of possible selves (Dörnyei & Kubanyiova, 2014),
construct school-wide pedagogies for operating the multilingual self guides identified in this ar-
between languages (Kramsch, 2013). ticle may have a value beyond that as psychologi-
cal constructs; incorporated within a multilingual
CONCLUSION curriculum (Kramsch, 2013), they constitute the
type of “innovative and sustainable lifeworld so-
A multilingual turn is taking place in second lutions” that can support language learners in
language acquisition research, the monolingual a multilingual world (Douglas Fir Group, 2016,
bias that has shaped the formulation of research p. 39).
problems and development of methodologies
no longer sustainable (May, 2014). With the
abandoning of monolingualism as an organizing ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
principle for the study of language acquisition
come demands for an “epistemic reorienta- In addition to the editors of this special edition and
tion” and for ways of investigating multilinguals’ the journal editor, I would like to thank the anony-
language experiences that offer alternatives to mous reviewers for their invaluable insights and advice.
15404781, 2017, 3, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/modl.12412 by University Of Glasgow, Wiley Online Library on [10/11/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
562 The Modern Language Journal 101 (2017)
I would also like to thank the students I interviewed ies in honour of Danuta Gabryś–Barker (pp. 23–35).
for sharing their experiences of being and becoming Berlin: Springer.
multilingual. Aronin, L., & Ó Laoire, M. (2004). Exploring mul-
tilingualism in cultural contexts: Towards a no-
tion of multilinguality. In C. Hoffmann & J. Ytsma
NOTES (Eds.), Trilingualism in family, school and community
(pp. 11–29). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.
1 While there is substantial debate around definitions Aronin, L., & Singleton, D. (2012). ‘Affordances the-
ory’ in multilingualism. Studies in Second Language
of ‘bilingualism’ and ‘multilingualism,’ my use of the
Learning and Teaching, 3, 311–331.
terms ‘multilingual’ and ‘multilingualism’ refers to sit-
Bar-Anan, Y., Liberman, N., & Trope, Y. (2006). The as-
uations involving either the learning/acquisition of two
sociation between psychological distance and con-
or more languages additional to the learner’s L1. Fur-
strual level: Evidence from an implicit association
ther, following the arguments of Cenoz and Gorter
test. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 135, 609–
(2015) and Block (2015), in this article the terms ‘being’
622.
and ‘becoming’ multilingual are understood as “distin-
Blackledge, A., & Creese, A. (2010). Multilingualism: A
guishable yet interrelated and interlinked phenomena”
critical perspective. London: Continuum.
(Block, 2015, p. 236).
2 Here it should be noted that, as Dörnyei (2009) Block, D. (2007). Second language identities. London:
Continuum.
points out, self guides do not exist a priori and not every-
Block, D. (2015). Becoming multilingual and being mul-
one can be assumed to develop self guides and certainly
tilingual. In J. Cenoz & D. Gorter (Eds.). Mul-
not in every domain of life.
3 For example, different social (i.e., family) and edu- tilingual education: Between language learning and
translanguaging (pp. 225–237). Cambridge: Cam-
cational (i.e., class, school) systems.
4 Here Kramsch is describing the case of U.S. stu- bridge University Press.
Blommaert, J. (2010). The sociolinguistics of globalization.
dents learning a single foreign language. See also Lan-
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
vers (2017, this issue) for a discussion of UK students
Busse, V. (2017). Plurilingualism in Europe: Exploring
who are contentedly monolingual.
5 Here it is important to note that construals are attitudes towards English and other European lan-
guages among adolescents in Bulgaria, Germany,
highly personal; generally situated; and, as one of the
the Netherlands, and Spain. Modern Language Jour-
reviewers pointed out, as varied as the individuals who
nal, 101, 566–582.
‘construe’ them. For a discussion of differences in, e.g.,
Byrne, D., & Callaghan, G. (2014). Complexity theory and
males’ and females’ construals of ideal selves, see Henry
the social sciences: The state of the art. Abingdon, UK:
& Cliffordson (2013).
6 This research was carried out as part of the MO- Routledge.
Canagarajah, A. S. (Ed.). (2013). Literacy as translingual
TISSE (Motivational Teaching in Swedish Secondary En-
practice: Between communities and classrooms. New
glish) project.
7 Several of these students had L1s, such as, for exam- York: Routledge/Taylor &Francis.
Cenoz, J., & Gorter, D. (2011). Focus on multilingual-
ple, Arabic and Persian.
8 For work on the affordances of multilingualism see ism: A study of trilingual writing. Modern Language
Journal, 95, 356–369.
Aronin & Singleton (2012).
9 The idea of encouraging learners to focus on the Cenoz, J., & Gorter, D. (2015). Towards a holistic ap-
proach in the study of multilingual education.
multilingual person they wanted to be in the future was
In J. Cenoz & D. Gorter (Eds.), Multilingual edu-
first proposed in Henry (2012); similar proposals also
cation: Between language learning and translanguag-
made in Henry (2014, 2016).
ing (pp. 1–15). Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Chan, L. (2014). Effects of an imagery training strat-
REFERENCES
egy on Chinese university students’ possible sec-
ond language selves and learning experiences. In
Arnold, J., Puchta, H., & Rinvolucri, M. (2007). Imag- K. Csizér & M. Magid (Eds.), The impact of self-
ine that! Mental imagery in the EFL classroom. Cam- concept on language learning (pp. 357–376). Bristol,
bridge: Cambridge University Press. UK: Multilingual Matters.
Aronin, L. (2016). Multi-competence and dominant lan- Chan, L., Dörnyei, Z., & Henry A. (2015). Learner
guage constellation. In V. Cook & Li Wei (Eds.), archetypes and signature dynamics in the lan-
The Cambridge handbook of linguistic multicompetence guage classroom: A retrodictive qualitative mod-
(pp. 142–163). Cambridge: Cambridge University elling approach to studying L2 motivation. In Z.
Press. Dörnyei, P. D. MacIntyre, & A. Henry (Eds.), Mo-
Aronin, L., & Jessner, U. (2016). Spacetimes of mul- tivational dynamics in language learning (pp. 238–
tilingualism. In D. Gałajda, P. Zakrajewski, & M. 259). Bristol, UK: Multilingual Matters.
Pawlak, (Eds.), Researching second language learning De Angelis, G. (2011). Teachers’ beliefs about the role
and teaching from a psycholinguistic perspective: Stud- of prior language knowledge in learning and how
15404781, 2017, 3, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/modl.12412 by University Of Glasgow, Wiley Online Library on [10/11/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Alastair Henry 563
these influence teaching practices. International Dörnyei, Z., & Kubanyiova, M. (2014). Motivating learn-
Journal of Multilingualism, 8, 216–234. ers, motivating teachers: Building vision in the language
De Angelis, G., & Dewaele, J.–M. (Eds.) (2011). New classroom. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
trends in crosslinguistic influence and multilingualism Dörnyei. Z., MacIntyre, P. D., & Henry, A. (Eds.). (2015).
research. Bristol, UK: Multilingual Matters. Motivational dynamics in language learning. Bristol,
de Bot, K. (2012). Rethinking multilingual process- UK: Multilingual Matters.
ing: From a static to a dynamic approach. Dörnyei, Z., & Ushioda, E. (2011). Teaching and research-
In J. C. Amaro, S. Flynn, & J. Rothman ing motivation. Harlow, UK: Longman.
(Eds.), Third language acquisition in adulthood Douglas Fir Group. (2016). A transdisciplinary frame-
(pp. 79–94). Philadelphia/Amsterdam: John work for SLA in a multilingual world. Modern Lan-
Benjamins. guage Journal, 100 (Supplement 2016), 19–47.
de Bot, K. (2015). A history of applied linguistics: From Dunkel, C., & Kerpelman, J. (Eds.). (2006). Possible
1980 to the present. New York: Routledge/Taylor & selves: Theory, research and applications. New York:
Francis. Nova Science.
de Bot, K. (2016). Multi-competence and dy- Duff, P. A. (2015). Transnationalism, multilingualism,
namic/complex systems. In V. Cook & Li Wei and identity. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics,
(Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of linguistic multi- 35, 57–80.
competence (pp. 125–141). Cambridge: Cambridge Fujita, K., Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2015). On the psy-
University Press. chology of near and far: A construal level theoreti-
de Bot, K., & Jaensch, C. (2015). What is special about L3 cal approach. In G. Keren & G. Wu (Eds.), The Wi-
processing? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, ley Blackwell handbook of judgment and decision mak-
18, 130–144. ing (Vol. 1; pp. 404–429). Chichester, UK: Wiley
de Leeuw, E., Opitz, C., & Lubinska, D. (2013). Dy- Blackwell.
namics of first language attrition across the lifes- García, O. (2009). Bilingual education in the 21st century.
pan. International Journal of Bilingualism, 17, 667– Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
674. García, O., & Li Wei. (2014). Translanguaging: language,
Dewaele, J.–M. (2001). Activation or inhibition? The in- bilingualism, education. Basingstoke, UK: Palgrave
teraction of L1, L2 and L3 on the language mode Macmillan.
continuum. In J. Cenoz, B. Hufeisen, & U. Jess- Gardner, R. C. (2001). Integrative motivation and sec-
ner (Eds.), Cross-linguistic influence in third language ond language acquisition. In Z. Dörnyei & R.
acquisition: Psycholinguistic perspectives (pp. 69–89). Schmidt (Eds.), Motivation and second language ac-
Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters. quisition (Technical Report 23) (pp. 1–19). Hon-
Dewaele, J.–M. (2011). Reflections on the emotional olulu: University of Hawaii Second Language
and psychological aspects of foreign language Teaching and Curriculum Center.
learning and use. Anglistik: International Journal of Goldstein, J. (1999). Emergence as a construct: History
English Studies 22, 1, 23–42. and issues. Emergence, 1, 49–72.
Dewaele, J.–M., & MacIntyre, P. D. (2016). Foreign Gynne, A., & Bagga–Gupta, S. (2013). Young people’s
language enjoyment and foreign language lass- languaging and social positioning. Chaining in
room anxiety. The right and left feet of FL ‘bilingual’ educational settings in Sweden. Linguis-
learning? In P. D. MacIntyre, T. Gregersen, & S. tics and Education, 24, 479–496.
Mercer (Eds.), Positive psychology in SLA (pp. 215– Hadfield, J., & Dörnyei, Z. (2013). Motivation and the
236). Bristol, UK: Multilingual Matters. ideal self. London: Longman.
Dörnyei, Z. (2001). New themes and approaches in sec- Hammarberg, B. (2009). Processes in third language ac-
ond language motivation research. Annual Review quisition. Edinburgh, UK: Edinburgh University
of Applied Linguistics, 21, 43–59. Press.
Dörnyei, Z. (2009). The L2 motivational self system. In Haukås, Å (2016). Teachers’ beliefs about multilingual-
Z. Dörnyei & E. Ushioda (Eds.), Motivation, lan- ism and a multilingual pedagogical approach. In-
guage identity and the L2 self (pp. 9–42). Bristol, UK: ternational Journal of Multilingualism, 13, 1–18.
Multilingual Matters. Henry, A. (2010). Contexts of possibility in simultane-
Dörnyei, Z. (2014). Researching complex dynamic sys- ous language learning: Using the L2 Motivational
tems: ‘Retrodictive qualitative modelling’ in the Self System to assess the impact of global English.
language classroom. Language Teaching, 47, 80– Journal of Multicultural and Multilingual Develop-
91. ment, 31, 149–162.
Dörnyei, Z., & Chan, L. (2013). Motivation and vision: Henry, A. (2011). Examining the impact of L2 English
An analysis of future L2 self images, sensory styles, on L3 selves: A case study. International Journal of
and imagery capacity across two target languages. Multilingualism, 8, 235–255.
Language Learning, 63, 437–462. Henry, A. (2012). L3 motivation. Gothenburg, Sweden:
Dörnyei, Z., Henry, A., & Muir, C. (2016). Motiva- Gothenburg University Press.
tional currents in language learning: Frameworks for fo- Henry, A. (2014). The motivational effects of crosslin-
cused interventions. New York: Routledge/Taylor & guistic awareness: Developing third language ped-
Francis. agogies to address the negative impact of the L2
15404781, 2017, 3, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/modl.12412 by University Of Glasgow, Wiley Online Library on [10/11/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
564 The Modern Language Journal 101 (2017)
on the L3 self-concept. Innovation in Language scape in the United Kingdom. Modern Language
Teaching and Learning, 8, 1–19. Journal, 101, 517–532.
Henry, A. (2015). In Z. Dörnyei, P. D. MacIntyre, & Larsen–Freeman, D. (1997). Chaos/complexity science
A. Henry (Eds.), Motivational dynamics in language and second language acquisition. Applied Linguis-
learning (pp. 83–94). Bristol, UK: Multilingual tics, 18, 141–165.
Matters. Larsen–Freeman, D. (2013). Complexity theory: A new
Henry, A. (2016). Enablements and constraints: Inven- way to think. Revista Brasileira De Linguística Apli-
torying affordances associated with lingua franca cada, 13, 369–373.
English. International Journal of Bilingual Education Larsen–Freeman, D., & Cameron, L. (2008). Complex sys-
and Bilingualism, 19, 488–510. tems and applied linguistics. Oxford: Oxford Univer-
Henry, A., & Cliffordson, C. (2013). Motivation, gender, sity Press.
and possible selves. Language Learning 63, 271– Li Wei. (2011). Moment analysis and translanguaging
295. space: Discursive construction of identities by mul-
Herdina, P., & Jessner, U. (2002). A dynamic model of mul- tilingual Chinese youth in Britain. Journal of Prag-
tilingualism: Changing the psycholinguistic perspective. matics, 43, 1222–1235.
Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters. Liberman, N., & Trope, Y. (1998). The role of feasibility
Herdina, P., & Jessner, U. (2013). The implications and desirability considerations in near and distant
of language attrition for dynamic systems theory: future decisions. A test of temporal construal the-
Next steps and consequences. International Journal ory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75,
of Bilingualism, 17, 752–756. 5–18.
Hinkley, K., & Andersen, S. M. (1996). The working self- Lowie, W., & Verspoor, M. (2011). The dynamics of mul-
concept in transference: Significant-other activa- tilingualism: Levelt’s speaking model revisited. In
tion and self change. Journal of Personality and So- M. S. Schmid & W. Lowie (Eds.), Modeling bilingual-
cial Psychology, 71, 1279–1295. ism: From structure to chaos (pp. 267–288). Philadel-
Hiver, P. (2015). Attractor states. In Z. Dörnyei, P. D. phia/Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
MacIntyre, & A. Henry (Eds.), Motivational dynam- Lowie, W., & Verspoor, M. (2015). Variability and varia-
ics in language learning (pp. 20–28). Bristol, UK: tion in second language acquisition orders: A dy-
Multilingual Matters. namic reevaluation. Language Learning, 65, 63–88.
Hiver, P., & Al-Hoorie, A. (2016). A dynamic ensemble Mackay, J. (2014). Applications and implications of the
for second language research: Putting complexity L2 Motivational Self System in a Catalan EFL con-
theory into practice. Modern Language Journal, 100, text. In K. Csizér & M. Magid (Eds.), The impact of
741–756. self-concept on language learning (pp. 377–402). Bris-
Jessner, U. (2006). Linguistic awareness in multilinguals: tol, UK: Multilingual Matters.
English as a third language. Edinburgh, UK: Edin- Magid, M. (2014). A motivational programme for learn-
burgh University Press. ers of English: An application of the L2 Moti-
Jessner, U. (2008). A DST model of multilingualism and vational Self System. In K. Csizér & M. Magid
the role of metalinguistic awareness. Modern Lan- (Eds.), The impact of self-concept on language learning
guage Journal, 92, 270–283. (pp. 333–356). Bristol, UK: Multilingual Matters.
Kivetz, Y., & Tyler, T. R. (2007). Tomorrow I’ll be me: Markus, H. R. (1977) Self-schemata and processing in-
The effect of time perspective on the activation formation about the self. Journal of Personality and
of idealistic versus pragmatic selves. Organizational Social Psychology, 35, 63–78.
Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 102, 193– Markus, H. R., & Kunda, Z. (1986). Stability and mal-
211. leability of the self-concept. Journal of Personality
Kramsch, C. (2009). The multilingual subject. Oxford: Ox- and Social Psychology 51, 858–866.
ford University Press. Markus, H. R., & Sentis, K. (1982). The self in social in-
Kramsch, C. (2013). The translingual and transcul- formation processing. In J. Suls (Ed.), Psychological
tural imagination. In J. L. Plews & B. Schmenk perspectives on the self (Vol. 1; pp. 41–70). Hillsdale,
(Eds.), Translations and transitions: Curricula for Ger- NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
man studies (pp. 21–36). Waterloo, Canada: Wilfrid Markus, H. R., & Wurf, E. (1987). The dynamic self-
Laurier University Press. concept: A social psychological perspective. An-
Kramsch, C., & Huffmaster, M. (2015). Multilingual nual Review of Psychology, 38, 299–337.
practices in foreign language study. In J. Cenoz May, S. (Ed). (2014). The multilingual turn: Implications
& D. Gorter (Eds.), Multilingual education: Between for SLA, TESOL, and bilingual education. New York:
language learning and translanguaging (pp. 114– Routledge/Taylor & Francis.
136). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Mercer, S. (2015). Social network analysis and complex
Kramsch, C., & Whiteside, A. (2008). Language ecol- dynamic systems. In Z. Dörnyei, P. D. MacIntyre,
ogy in multilingual settings: Towards a theory of & A. Henry (Eds.), Motivational dynamics in lan-
symbolic competence. Applied Linguistics, 29, 645– guage learning (pp. 73–82). Bristol, UK: Multilin-
671. gual Matters.
Lanvers, U. (2017). Contradictory others and the habitus Nowak, A., Vallacher, R. R., & Zochowski, M. (2005).
of languages: Surveying the L2 motivation land- The emergence of personality: Dynamic
15404781, 2017, 3, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/modl.12412 by University Of Glasgow, Wiley Online Library on [10/11/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Alastair Henry 565
foundations of individual variation. Developmental happiness: Can there be an upward spiral? Jour-
Review, 25, 351–385. nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 152–
Opitz, C. (2013). A dynamic perspective on late bilin- 165.
guals’ linguistic development in an L2 environ- Spoelman, M., & Verspoor, M. (2010). Dynamic patterns
ment. International Journal of Bilingualism 17, 701– in development of accuracy and complexity: A lon-
715. gitudinal case study in the acquisition of Finnish.
Ortega, L. (2014). Ways forward for a bi/multilingual Applied Linguistics, 31, 532–553.
turn in SLA. In S. May (Ed.), The multilingual turn: Stein, K. F., & Markus, H. R. (1996). The role of the self
Implications for SLA, TESOL and bilingual education in behavioral change. Journal of Psychotherapy Inte-
(pp. 32–53). New York: Routledge/Taylor & Fran- gration, 6, 349–384.
cis. Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2010). Construal-level theory
Otwinowska, A. (2014). Does multilingualism influence of psychological distance. Psychological Review, 117,
plurilingual awareness of Polish teachers of En- 440–463.
glish? International Journal of Multilingualism, 11, Ushioda, E. (2017). The impact of global English on
97–119. motivation to learn other languages: Towards an
Oyserman, D., & James, L. (2011). Possible identities. In ideal multilingual self. Modern Language Journal,
S. J. Schwartz, K. Luyckx, & V. L. Vignoles (Eds.), 101, 469–482.
Handbook of identity theory and research (pp. 117– Vallacher, R. R., & Nowak, A. (2007). Dynamical social
145). New York: Springer. psychology: Finding order in the flow of human
Oyserman, D., & Markus, H. R. (1990). Possible selves experience. In A. W. Kruglanski & E. T. Higgins
and delinquency. Journal of Personality and Social (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles
Psychology, 59, 112–125. (pp. 734–758). New York: Guilford.
Panayiotou, A. (2006). Translating guilt: An endeavour Vallacher, R. R., Van Geert, P., & Nowak, A. (2015). The
of shame in the Mediterranean? In A. Pavlenko intrinsic dynamics of psychological process. Cur-
(Ed.), Bilingual minds: Emotional experience, expres- rent Directions in Psychological Science, 24, 58–64.
sion and representation (pp. 183–208). Clevedon, Van Geert, P. (1991). A dynamic systems model of cogni-
UK: Multilingual Matters. tive and language growth. Psychological Review, 98,
Pavlenko, A. (2006). Bilingual selves. In A. Pavlenko 3–53.
(Ed), Bilingual minds: Emotional experience, expres- Van Geert, P. (1994). Dynamic systems of development:
sion and representation (pp. 1–33). Clevedon, UK: Change between complexity and chaos. New York: Pren-
Multilingual Matters. tice Hall/Harvester Wheatsheaf.
Pronin, E., & Ross, L. (2006). Temporal differences in Verspoor, M. H., de Bot, L., & Lowie, W. (Eds.). (2011).
trait self-ascription: When the self is seen as an A dynamic approach to second language development:
other. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, Methods and techniques. Philadelphia/Amsterdam:
197–209. John Benjamins.
Sampson, R. (2012). The language-learning self, Wakslak, C. J., Nussbaum, S., Liberman N., & Trope Y.
self-enhancement activities, and self-perceptual (2008). Representations of the self in the near and
change. Language Teaching Research, 16, 317–335. distant future. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
Sanchez, L. (2011). ‘Luisa and Pedrito’s dog will the chology, 95, 757–773.
breakfast eat’: Interlanguage transfer and the role Waninge, F., Dörnyei, Z., & de Bot, K. (2014). Motiva-
of the second language factor. In G. De Angelis, tional dynamics in language learning: Change, sta-
& J. M. Dewaele (Eds.), New trends in crosslinguistic bility, and context. Modern Language Journal, 98,
influence and multilingualism research (pp. 86–104). 704–723.
Bristol, UK: Multilingual Matters. Yashima, T., & Arano, K. (2015) Understanding EFL
Sheldon, K. M., & Elliot, A. J. (1998). Not all personal learners’ motivational dynamics: A three-level
goals are personal: Comparing autonomous and model from a dynamic systems and sociocultural
controlled reasons for goals as predictors of ef- perspective. In Z. Dörnyei, P. D. MacIntyre, & A.
fort and attainment. Personality and Social Psychol- Henry (Eds.), Motivational dynamics in language
ogy Bulletin, 24, 546–557. learning (pp. 285–314). Bristol, UK: Multilingual
Sheldon, K. M., & Elliot, A.J. (1999). Goal striving, need Matters.
satisfaction, and longitudinal well-being: The self- You, C., & Chan, L. (2015). The dynamics of L2 imagery
concordance model. Journal of Personality and So- in future motivational self-guides. In Z. Dörnyei, P.
cial Psychology, 76, 482–497. D. MacIntyre, & A. Henry (Eds.), Motivational dy-
Sheldon, K. M., & Houser–Marko, L. (2001). Self- namics in language learning (pp. 397–418). Bristol,
concordance, goal attainment, and the pursuit of UK: Multilingual Matters.

You might also like