The Social-Cognitive Basis of Personality Disorders: Review

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 5

REVIEW

CURRENT
OPINION The social-cognitive basis of personality disorders
Sabine C. Herpertz and Katja Bertsch

Purpose of review
The review summarizes recent results on abnormalities in social cognition in patients with personality
disorders that predispose them to develop dysfunctional interaction with others. The review starts with more
basic social cognition processes, such as emotion recognition and reactions to social exclusion that are
followed by more complex processes such as cognitive and affective empathy.
Recent findings
The deficits in social cognition depend on the particular function that is investigated and is strongly
associated with characteristic symptoms of particular personality disorders. Thus, patients with borderline
personality disorder are hypersensitive for social threat, they show deficits in cognitive empathy and high
emotion contagion, that is, they share emotions of others without properly discriminating between one’s
own feelings and those of others. Psychopaths are characterized by deficiency in facial fear recognition
and emotional empathy similar to patients with narcissistic personality disorder. Studies on social cognition
in cluster A and C personality disorders are sparse.
Summary
Research indicates deficits in social cognition in patients with personality disorders, but more research is
needed to investigate social cognition in cluster A and C personality disorders and to compare deficits in
social cognitive functions across personality disorders.
Keywords
empathy, facial emotion recognition, moral judgment, rejection sensitivity, trust appraisal

INTRODUCTION are generally subsumed under cognitive empathy or


During the past two decades, issues of affect regu- theory of mind. In contrast, emotional empathy
lation and impulse control have dominated research denotes emotion sharing and social executive con-
questions in the field of personality disorders. Mean- trol to regulate this experience and to keep track of
while, disturbed relationships are thought to be a the origins of self and other feelings [2]. In their
&

third prominent sector of psychopathology in indi- review on social cognition in BPD Roepke et al. [3 ]
viduals meeting the criteria of a personality dis- claim that BPD individuals are characterized by
order, borderline personality disorder (BPD), in systematic negative, malevolent biases in their inter-
particular. A fearful or highly reactive interpersonal actions with others, that they exhibit subtle deficits
style has been suggested to be a highly distinctive in appropriate emotional responses to others and
and pathogenic component of BPD psychopatho- that they show ambiguous, hard to read social sig-
&

logy, often designed as interpersonal hypersensi- nals in the interaction to others. Mak and Lamm [4 ]
tivity. In addition, dichotomous thinking as a conclude that a ‘hypermentalizing’ tendency may
prominent characteristic of BPD patients’ evalu- contribute to BPD individuals’ socially inappropri-
ations of others may contribute to interpersonal ate interaction with others, as well as a difficulty to
&
dysfunction in BPD [1 ]. In this review, we focus disengage attention from threatening cues.
on abnormalities in social cognition in patients with
personality disorders that predispose them to
Department of General Psychiatry, Center for Psychosocial Medicine,
develop dysfunctional interaction with others. University of Heidelberg, Heidelberg, Germany
Humans have to interpret other people’s Correspondence to Sabine C. Herpertz, MD, Department of General
internal states by analyzing external cues, particu- Psychiatry, Center for Psychosocial Medicine, University of Heidelberg,
larly facial expressions. They also apply learnt cog- Heidelberg, Vossstr. 2, 69115 Heidelberg, Germany. Tel: +49 6221
nitive models of how an individual is expected to 5622751; e-mail: sabine.herpertz@uni-heidelberg.de
experience specific situations in order to infer other Curr Opin Psychiatry 2014, 27:73–77
people’s goals and predict their actions. Both aspects DOI:10.1097/YCO.0000000000000026

0951-7367 ß 2013 Wolters Kluwer Health | Lippincott Williams & Wilkins www.co-psychiatry.com

Copyright © Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Personality disorders

could also explain impairments in, that is, slower and


KEY POINTS less correct, recognition of negative facial emotions
 Patients with borderline personality disorder are that are shown for longer time periods or in full
hypersensitive for social threat and they consistently intensity compared to healthy controls. Consistent
show deficits in cognitive empathy. with previous reviews (e.g. [9]), this meta-analysis
also revealed no general deficit in emotion recog-
 Patients with borderline personality disorder exhibit
nition, but rather a particular impairment in the
enhanced unconscious emotional simulation processes
resulting in high emotion contagion. recognition of fully displayed angry, but also neutral
and disgusting facial expressions in BPD patients.
 Psychopaths are characterized by deficiency in facial As manual reaction times only represent a rather
fear recognition and emotional empathy. late measure of conscious emotion recognition [10],
researchers have started to additionally measure eye
movements, electrophysiological correlates, and
Antisocial individuals are thought to show brain activations in response to emotional facial
deficits in the recognition of negative emotional expressions. Apart from an enhanced amygdala acti-
facial expressions, although they may be good vation for facial, and particularly angry facial,
at perceiving others’ intentions [5]. As antisocial
&& &
expressions [11 ,12,13 ], which might be related
&
personality disorder is rather a heterogeneous diag- to the patients’ enhanced trait anxiety [13 ] or
&&
nostic entity, this finding was found to be more history of early (emotional) maltreatment [14 ],
stringently associated with the psychopathy sub- BPD patients have been found to show more and
type, which, according to Hare [6], is characterized faster initial, reflexive eye movements towards the
by emotional detachment, callousness, and egocen- eyes, and thus the most threatening and arousing
tricity. Individuals with narcissistic personality dis-
&&
region of very briefly presented angry faces [11 ].
order (NPD) were shown to exhibit deficits in This latter study also revealed that a single dose of
affective empathy [7]. intranasally administered synthetic oxytocin –
The article reviews the recent data on social which, across species, exerts stress-reducing, anxio-
cognitive functions in personality disorders apply- lytic-like effects – may alter the hypersensitivity for
ing a Pubmed search using the following keywords: facial threat in BPD by reducing the speed and
‘borderline personality disorder’, ‘antisocial person- amount of initial eye movements towards angry
ality disorder’, ‘narcissistic personality disorder’, eyes as well as the amygdala’s responsiveness for
‘psychopathy’, ‘social cognition’, ‘empathy’, ‘theory angry faces. In addition to enhanced amygdala
of mind’, ‘facial emotion recognition’, ‘trust responses, recent findings also indicate alterations
(appraisal)’, ‘rejection sensitivity’, ‘moral judg- in the activity of frontal areas and the fronto-limbic
ment’, and ‘moral decision making’. connectivity for negative facial emotions in BPD
patients compared to healthy controls. Thus, BPD
patients showed increased activity in the dorsal and
FACIAL EMOTION RECOGNITION rostral anterior cingulate cortex for fearful versus
The recognition of facial emotions is a basic and
&
neutral facial distractors [13 ], and enhanced con-
immediate competence that is crucial for successful nectivity between bilateral amygdala and rostral
interpersonal interaction. From a clinical perspect- anterior cingulate cortex for faces displaying overt
ive, patients with BPD have been regarded as hyper- fear compared to healthy individuals [15].
sensitive for subtle cues of social threat and rejection. Regarding antisocial personality disorder, and
Experimental data on facial emotion recognition, particularly psychopathy, there is strong evidence
however, appear rather inconsistent and seem to for impairments in the recognition of several facial
heavily depend on task specificities, such as presen- emotions including fear and sadness in adults as
tation time, intensity of facial emotions, and type of well as children and adolescents (for review, see
instruction, as well as on the dependent variables
&&
[16 ]). This has been associated with blunted amyg-
(e.g. response latencies, error rates). On the basis of a dala responses (for review, see [17]) and a reciprocal
&&
systematic meta-analysis, Daros et al. [8 ] conclude reduction in the functional connectivity between
that BPD patients’ typical enhanced arousal may be the amygdala and visual as well as prefrontal corti-
responsible for their hypersensitivity to subtle facial ces, and hence deficits in the integration of
cues of negative emotions, that is, they recognize emotions into cognition in psychopaths [18].
negative emotions faster and more correct in faces Little is known about facial emotion recognition
that are presented briefly or display the emotion in a in NPD. First results from the study by Marissen et al.
low intensity. As the increased arousal may hinder
&
[19 ] indicate that although patients with NPD
the disentangling of attention from the emotion, this regard themselves as sensitive for the feelings of

74 www.co-psychiatry.com Volume 27  Number 1  January 2014

Copyright © Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Social-cognitive basis of personality disorders Herpertz and Bertsch

others, they show general deficits in the recognition compared to healthy controls. It should, however, be
of facial emotions with greatest impairments in the noted that the performance in the RME test does not
recognition of fear and disgust compared to healthy require an explicit meta-representation that is the
controls and patients with cluster C personality representation of another person’s thought process,
disorders. Thus, very first experimental findings but can rather be resolved by strategies other than
point to a combination of subjective overestimation mentalizing, including simulating processes.
of and objective deficits in basic social cognitive Functional neuroimaging studies may deepen
abilities in patients with NPD. our understanding of theory of mind deficiencies
in BPD patients. Several studies report decreased
neuronal activity in the superior temporal gyrus,
TRUST APPRAISAL AND REJECTION the superior temporal sulcus, and the precuneus,
SENSITIVITY and thus central structures of the social brain
Social cognition not only relies on the recognition involved in representing and predicting the actions
of facial emotions but also on the appraisal of and social intentions of other people from an
personality traits, such as trustworthiness, that are analysis of biological-motion cues (for review, see
closely related to emotion recognition but also [31,32]). These abnormally low activations were
require more sophisticated social judgments. found while BPD patients performed the Multifac-
Patients with BPD have been found to be highly eted Empathy Test [33] and the Social–Cognitive task
&&
sensitive to social rejection (e.g. [20,21]). This hyper- [34 ], both of which challenge theory of mind pro-
sensitivity may help to avoid actual experiences of cesses. Interestingly, lower neuronal activation in the
social rejection and may thus reduce dysphoric and theory of mind circuit was also found during the RME
&&
painful social events, which are experienced more test [30 ], supporting the assumption that BPD
intensely by patients with BPD [22]. In line with patients – despite good performance in this task –
this, Fertuck et al. [23] recently found a bias to did not apply cognitive mentalizing strategies.
appraise neutral faces less trustworthy as well as In psychopaths, previous findings permit only
slower reaction times for faces with ambiguous a partial understanding of potential empathy
trustworthiness in patients with BPD. The patients deficiencies. On the basis of a precise and ecologically
did not differ either in measures of sensitivity or valuable laboratory measurement – the empathic
discriminability from healthy controls or in the accuracy paradigm – criminal psychopaths exhibited
&&
appraisal of facial fear, which may indicate that this impaired cognitive empathy [35 ]. Psychopathy
bias may be typical of BPD. So far, the results of two turned out to be negatively correlated with empathic
studies indicate that, at least in trust games, the accuracy, particularly with regard to negatively
intranasal administration of oxytocin does not valenced emotions. The authors suggest from the
improve trust and the experienced trustworthiness pattern of findings in psychopaths a failure of more
in patients with BPD [24,25]. The results of these complex, dynamic, and interactive processing of
studies and those of Fertuck et al. [23] additionally others’ emotions rather than of the processing of
emphasize the role of individual differences within simple static social stimuli.
groups of patients with personality disorders, such
as BPD. Thus, attachment style [24] and childhood
traumatization [23,25] have to be considered when AFFECTIVE EMPATHY
&
investigating trust, facial trust appraisal, and rejec- Similar to previous studies (for a review, see [36 ]),
tion sensitivity, at least in BPD. New et al. [27] found enhanced self-reported
emotional distress in social contexts in BPD patients
using the Interpersonal Reactivity Scale. In addition,
COGNITIVE EMPATHY the record of electromyographic activity during
Despite some inconsistencies in findings, data from facial emotion perception revealed increased acti-
experimental psychopathology suggest impaired vity of the musculus corrugator supercilii in BPD
cognitive theory of mind abilities in patients with patients when confronted with faces displaying
&
BPD (for review, see [26]). Weak perspective taking is anger, sadness, or disgust [37 ], but lower activity
already reflected in self-report measurements, such of the musculus levator labii in response to happy
as the Interpersonal Reactivity Index [27]. This and surprised faces. This may indicate a high auto-
is particularly true when patients are assessed by matic simulation in individuals with BPD while
more complex, more difficult, and ecologically valid viewing faces which reflect emotional states similar
measurements. Interestingly, in the Reading the but not different to one’s own.
Mind in the Eyes (RME) test, BPD patients show There is consistent evidence for enhanced amyg-
&&
equal [28] or even enhanced [29,30 ] performance dala activations during tasks requiring affective

0951-7367 ß 2013 Wolters Kluwer Health | Lippincott Williams & Wilkins www.co-psychiatry.com 75

Copyright © Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Personality disorders

empathy in patients with BPD compared to healthy between psychopathy and moral judgment [43],
&& &&
volunteers [30 ,34 ]. In the Social–Cognitive task, others revealed impaired moral judgments in psy-
BPD patients additionally showed a task-independ- chopaths linked with hypoactivations in the dorso-
&& &
ent hyperactivity in the somatosensory cortex [34 ] lateral prefrontal cortex [45 ]. Apart from this,
that may indicate enhanced unconscious emotional psychopaths have been reported to show impair-
simulation processes. In the RME test, areas con- ments in identifying own behaviors that may cause
nected with the amygdala and prefrontal cortex, fear in others and in judging the moral acceptability
such as the left temporal pole, the middle temporal of these behaviors [46].
gyrus, and the medial frontal gyrus, showed
&&
increased activations in patients with BPD [30 ].
During the affective empathy condition of the CONCLUSION
Multifaceted Empathy Test (MET), BPD patients Social cognitive dysfunction is at the heart of the
were characterized by enhanced right mid-insular concept of personality disorder. However, research
activity [33], which is known to co-occur with self- is still at the initial stage. Future studies may aim at
oriented emotional reactions to others and developing experimental designs that are closer to
emotional distress in the observer (for review, see the complex, dynamic, and often equivocal nature
[38]), whereas in the RME test, anterior insular of social interactions in the real world, using multi-
activity, which is a key mediator of the other-ori- modal stimuli and the possibilities of virtual reality.
ented sharing of emotions (for a review, see [39]), In addition to dyadic interactions, social cognitive
&&
was lower in BPD patients than in controls [30 ]. functions have to be investigated in social groups
Psychopathy is conceptualized to reflect a lack with various protagonists covering different prob-
of empathy and remorse, and blunted responding to lem areas of individuals with personality disorders
emotional cues. Correspondingly, structural neuro- in the private and professional life, also consider-
imaging data found specific abnormalities in crim- ing issues of age and sex. Social cognition closely
inal offenders with psychopathy as opposed to those interacts with affect regulation and the function-
without psychopathy in cortical regions implicated ing of the self, leading to interesting questions
in empathic processing and moral reasoning, that for future research. Finally, a deeper understand-
is, in the anterior rostral prefrontal cortex and ing of social cognitive dysfunction and its inter-
&&
temporal poles [40 ] and in midline cortical areas actions with other personality domains will help to
&
[41]. However, Domes et al. [42 ] could not confirm further elaborate psychotherapeutic and pharma-
their hypothesis of defective emotional empathy to cological treatments for this significant group of
be specific for adult psychopathic offenders, but patients.
rather found low cognitive and emotional empathy
in criminal offenders in general, irrespective of the Acknowledgements
level of psychopathy. None.
Although lack of empathy is also a core symp-
tom of NPD, empathy has hardly been investigated Conflicts of interest
in this population. The only existing experimental This work was supported by two grants of the German
study revealed deficits in emotional empathy, but Research Foundation to S.C.H. on borderline personality
unimpaired cognitive empathy in patients with disorder (Clinical Research Group on Mechanisms of
NPD compared to healthy individuals and patients Disturbed Emotion Processing in Borderline Personality
with BPD [6]. Disorder: He 2660/12–1; He 2660/7–2).
Both authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.
MORAL JUDGMENT AND DECISION-
MAKING REFERENCES AND RECOMMENDED
So far, moral judgment and decision-making have READING
Papers of particular interest, published within the annual period of review, have
been mainly investigated in psychopaths with been highlighted as:
psychopathy, predicting utilitarian decisions in & of special interest
&& of outstanding interest
moral dilemma situations (e.g. [43]), possibly related
with reduced activity in the ventromedial prefrontal 1. Arntz A, ten Haaf J. Social cognition in borderline personality disorder:
cortex [44]. Thus, psychopathic individuals are & evidence for dichotomous thinking but no evidence for less complex attribu-
tions. Behav Res Ther 2012; 50:707–718.
more likely to choose effective options that may The authors of this study investigated different aspects of social cognition patients
cause harm for other individuals. Results, how- with borderline personality disorder in comparison to healthy controls and patients
with cluster C personality disorders.
ever, remain less consistent regarding moral judg- 2. Decety J, Lamm C. Human empathy through the lens of social neuroscience.
ment. Whereas some studies found no associations Scientific World J 2006; 20:1146–1163.

76 www.co-psychiatry.com Volume 27  Number 1  January 2014

Copyright © Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Social-cognitive basis of personality disorders Herpertz and Bertsch

3. Roepke S, Vater A, Preissler S, et al. Social cognition in borderline personality 26. Schmahl C, Herpertz SC. Clinical promise of translational research in border-
& disorder. Front Neurosci 2012; 6:195. line personality disorder. In: Oldham J, Skodol A, Bender DA, editors.
This is a comprehensive, clearly written review on social cognitive functioning in Textbook of personality disorders. Arlington, VA: The American Psychiatric
borderline personality disorder. Publishing; 2013.
4. Mak A, Lamm L. Neurocognitive profiles of people with borderline personality 27. New A S, Aan het Rot M, Ripoll L H, et al. Empathy and alexithymia in
& disorder. Curr Opin Psychiatry 2013; 26:90–96. borderline personality disorder: clinical and laboratory measures. J Pers
This review summarizes data from the broad field of neurocognitive research in Disord 2012; 26:660–675.
borderline personality disorder subsuming data on cognitive distortions, decision- 28. Preissler S, Dziobek I, Ritter K, et al. Social cognition in borderline personality
making, as well as emotion recognition and empathy. disorder: evidence for disturbed recognition of the emotions, thoughts, and
5. Decety J, Moriguchi Y. The empathic brain and its dysfunction in psychiatric intentions of others. Front Behav Neurosci 2010; 4:182.
populations: implications for intervention across different clinical conditions. 29. Fertuck EA, Jekal A, Song I, et al. Enhanced ‘Reading the Mind in the Eyes’ in
Biopsychosoc Med 2007; 1:22–65. borderline personality disorder compared to healthy controls. Psychol Med
6. Hare RD, Neumann CN. The PCL-R assessment of psychopathy: develop- 2009; 39:1979–1988.
ment, structural properties, and new directions. In: Patrick C, editor. Hand- 30. Frick C, Lang S, Kotchoubey B, et al. Hypersensitivity in borderline personality
book of psychopathy. New York: Guilford; 2013. pp. 58–88. && disorder during mind-reading. PLoS One 2012; 7:e41650.
7. Ritter K, Dziobek I, Preissler S, et al. Lack of empathy in patients with Using the ‘Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test’ the authors show stronger activation
narcissitic personality disorder. Psychiatry Res 2011; 187:241–247. of the amygdala and areas of the mirror neuron system during affective eye gazes,
8. Daros A, Zakzanis K, Ruocco A. Facial emotion recognition in borderline suggesting that they rather resonate intuitively with mental states of others than
&& personality disorder. Psychol Med 2012; 43:1954–1963. using cognitive mentalizing processes.
The authors not only summarize and discuss the results of several emotion 31. Pelphrey K, Carter E. Charting the typical and atypical development of the
recognition paradigms, but also describe a model that may explain these results. social brain. Dev Psychopathol 2008; 20:1081–1102.
9. Domes G, Schulze L, Herpertz S. Emotion recognition in borderline person- 32. Decety J, Meyer M. From emotion resonance to empathic understanding: a
ality disorder: a review of the literature. J Pers Disord 2009; 23:6–19. social developmental neuroscience account. Dev Psychopathol 2008;
10. Bannerman R, Milders M, de Gelder B, et al. Orienting to threat: faster 20:1053–1080.
localization of fearful facial expressions and body postures revealed 33. Dziobek I, Preissler S, Grozdanovic Z, et al. Neuronal correlates of altered
by saccadic eye movements. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 2009; 276:1635– empathy and social cognition in borderline personality disorder. Neuroimage
1641. 2011; 57:539–548.
11. Bertsch K, Gamer M, Schmidt B, et al. Oxytocin reduces social threat 34. Mier D, Lis S, Esslinger C, Sauer C, et al. Neuronal correlates of social
&& hypersensitivity in females with borderline personality disorder. Am J Psy- && cognition in borderline personality disorder. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2013;
chiatry 2013; 170:1169–1177. 8:531–537.
In this fMRI study, borderline patients show a hypersensitivity to social threat in In this complex study on social cognition in borderline personality disorder, the
early, reflexive stages of information processing that are compensated by the authors use three social cognition tasks of various complexity: the data confirm
intranasal administration of oxytocin. amygdala hyperactivation in BPD, whereas hypoactivation was found in mentaliz-
12. Donegan N, Sanislow C, Blumberg H, et al. Amygdala hyperreactivity in ing areas such as the superior temporal sulcus.
borderline personality disorder: implications for emotional dysregulation. Biol 35. Brook M, Kosson D. Impaired cognitive empathy in criminal psychopathy:
Psychiatry 2003; 54:1284–1293. && evidence from a laboratory measure of empathic accuracy. J Abnorm Psychol
13. Holtmann J, Herbot M, Wüstenberg T, et al. Trait anxiety modulates fronto- 2013; 122:156–166.
& limbic processing of emotional interference in borderline personality disorder. On the basis of the empathy accuracy paradigm, the authors report deficiencies of
Front Hum Neurosci 2013; 7:54. empathy for negatively valenced emotions in psychopaths.
This study nicely reveals the importance of interindividual differences in personality 36. Dinsdale N, Crespi BJ. The borderline empathy paradox: evidence and
traits within patients with personality disorders. & conceptual models for empathic enhancements in borderline personality
14. van Harmelen A, van Tol M, Demenescu L, et al. Enhanced amygdala reactivity disorder. J Pers Disord 2012; 27:172–195.
&& to emotional faces in adults reporting childhood emotional maltreatment. Soc This is a nice systematic review on facets of empathy in borderline personality
Cogn Affect Neurosci 2013; 8:362–369. disorder.
This interesting study reveals an amygdalar hyper-responsivity for emotional faces 37. Matzke B, Herpertz SC, Berger C, et al. Facial reactions to emotional
in adults with experiences of childhood emotional maltreatment independent of & expressions in borderline personality disorder: a facial EMG study. Psycho-
psychiatric disorders. pathology (in press).
15. Cullen K, Vizueta N, Thomas K, et al. Amygdala functional connectivity in The authors find patterns of increased facial reactions to negative facial stimuli and
young women with borderline personality disorder. Brain Connect 2011; decreased facial responding to positive faces, concluding that borderline patients
1:61–71. tend to perceive their social world with a negativity bias.
16. Dawel A, O’Kearney R, McKone E, et al. Not just fear and sadness: meta- 38. Lamm C, Nusbaum HC, Meltzoff AN, et al. What are you feeling? Using
&& analytic evidence of pervasive emotion recognition deficits for facial and vocal functional magnetic resonance imaging to assess the modulation of sensory
expressions in psychopathy. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2012; 36:2288– and affective responses during empathy for pain. PLoS One 2007; 2:e1292.
22304. 39. Fan Y, Duncan NW, de Greck M, et al. Is there a core neural network in
This excellent meta-analysis summarizes the results of 26 studies that investigated empathy? An fMRI based quantitative meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev
facial emotion recognition in psychopathic children/adolescents and adults. 2011; 35:903–911.
17. Blair R. Neuroimaging of psychopathy and antisocial behavior: a targeted 40. Gregory S, Ffytche D, Simmons A, et al. The antisocial brain: psychopathy
review. Curr Psychiatry Rep 2010; 12:76–82. && matters. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2012; 69:962–972.
18. Contreras-Rodrı́guez O, Pujol J, Batalla I, et al. Disrupted neural processing of In this structural neuroimaging study, psychopaths showed reduced GM volume
emotional faces in psychopathy. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2013. doi: 10. within areas mediating empathic processing, moral reasoning, processing of
1093/scan/nst014. prosocial emotions, such as guilt, shame.
19. Marissen MA, Deen ML, Franken IH. Disturbed emotion recognition in patients 41. Bertsch K, Grothe M, Prehn K, et al. Brain volumes differ between diagnostic
& with narcissistic personality disorder. Psychiatry Res 2012; 198:269–273. groups of violent criminal offenders. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2013;
This is one of the few experimental studies using a facial emotion recognition task 263:593–606.
to examine facets of empathy in narcissistic personality disorder. 42. Domes G, Hollerbach P, Vohs K, et al. Emotional empathy and psychopathy in
20. Miano A, Fertuck E, Arntz A, Stanley B. Rejection sensitivity is a mediator & offenders: an experimental study. J Pers Disord 2013; 27:67–84.
between borderline personality disorder features and facial trust appraisal. The authors compare different groups of criminal offenders and do not find
J Pers Disord 2013; 27:442–456. differences in emotional empathy in those with and without psychopathy.
21. Staebler K, Renneberg B, Stopsack M, et al. Facial emotional expression in 43. Tassy S, Deruelle C, Mancini J. High levels of psychopathic traits alter moral
reaction to social exclusion in borderline personality disorder. Psychol Med choice but not moral judgment. Front Hum Neurosci 2013; 7:229.
2011; 41:1929–1938. 44. Lotze M, Veit R, Anders S, et al. Evidence for a different role of the ventral and
22. Stiglmayr C, Grathwol T, Linehan M, et al. Aversive tension in patients with dorsal medial prefrontal cortex for social reactive aggression: an interactive
borderline personality disorder: a computer-based controlled field study. Acta fMRI study. Neuroimage 2007; 34:470–478.
Psychiatr Scand 2005; 111:372–379. 45. Pujol J, Batalla I, Contreras-Rodriguez O, et al. Breakdown in the brain
23. Fertuck E, Grinband J, Stanley B. Facial trust appraisal negatively biased in & network subserving moral judgment in criminal psychopathy. Soc Cogn Affect
borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Res 2013; 207:195–202. Neurosci 2012; 7:917–923.
24. Bartz J, Simeon D, Hamilton H, et al. Oxytocin can hinder trust and coopera- This study revealed alterations in a brain network involved in moral judgment in
tion in borderline personality disorder. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2010; criminal psychopath during moral judgment but also outside moral dilemma
6:556–563. situations.
25. Ebert A, Kolb M, Heller J, et al. Modulation of interpersonal trust in borderline 46. Marsh AA, Cardinale EM. When psychopathy impairs moral judgments: neural
personality disorder by intranasal oxytocin and childhood trauma. Soc Neu- responses during judgments about causing fear. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci
rosci 2013; 8:305–313. 2012. [Epub ahead of print]

0951-7367 ß 2013 Wolters Kluwer Health | Lippincott Williams & Wilkins www.co-psychiatry.com 77

Copyright © Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

You might also like