Perks of Blindness: Enhanced Verbal Memory Span in Blind Over Sighted Adults

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Brain Research 1789 (2022) 147943

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Brain Research
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/brainres

Perks of blindness: Enhanced verbal memory span in blind over


sighted adults
Karen Arcos a, *, Susanne M. Jaeggi a, b, Emily D. Grossman a
a
Department of Cognitive Science, University of California, Irvine, 3151 Social Sciences Plaza, Irvine, CA 92697-5100, United States
b
School of Education, University of California, Irvine, 3452 Education, Irvine, CA 92697-5500, United States

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Blind individuals commonly use verbal encoding (i.e. text-to-speech) and memory-based strategies (i.e. serial
Visual deprivation recall) for situations in which sighted individuals use vision (i.e. finding items). These strategies may serve to
Blindness train cognitive systems responsible for maintaining and manipulating verbal information. To test this hypothesis,
Socioeconomic status
we investigate whether early visual deprivation is linked to improved verbal short-term and working memory
Verbal short-term memory
Working memory
abilities, and thus might illustrate experience-dependent plasticity in memory systems. We also test whether the
sensory modality for encoding information influences performance. Our data show that blind adults recalled
more items on a verbal short-term memory span task than sighted participants. Furthermore, blind individuals
performed equally well on auditory forward and backward conditions despite the fact that recalling items in
reverse order is more difficult for the general population. However, the benefits of recalling items in reverse
order did not extend to the tactile domain, specifically, a braille version of the short-term memory digit span task
in blind individuals. Furthermore, we observed no differences between blind and sighted individuals on a more
demanding auditory n-back task evaluating more complex working memory processes. We conclude that the
memory benefits associated with blindness might be restricted to auditory-verbal short-term memory and likely
reflect strategy use and practice.

1. Introduction 2013). Likewise, adults with congenital or early onset blindness exhibit
improved short-term recall for letters, digits, and words (Amedi et al.,
Individuals who are blind must rely extensively on nonvisual mem­ 2003; Arcos et al., 2022; Occelli et al., 2017; Pasqualotto et al., 2013;
ory strategies to complete everyday tasks that sighted individuals typi­ Raz et al., 2007; Röder et al., 2001).
cally accomplish visually. When navigating, individuals who are blind A number of factors mediate the observed improved memory span in
utilize their working memory to orient using auditory cues while, for congenital or early blindness. For example, superior verbal memory
example, maintaining conversation (Raz et al., 2007). Those who are spans are not typically reported for individuals that experience late
blind also rely on memory for serial order and position to distinguish onset blindness or those with residual vision (Cohen et al., 2010; Dormal
items that otherwise can only be distinguished visually (e.g., different et al., 2016; Hull and Mason, 1995; Pasqualotto et al., 2013; Smits and
flavored yogurt in identical sealed containers differentiated solely by Mommers, 1976). Moreover, evidence indicates blind individuals’
labels; Raz et al., 2007). enhanced short-term memory is restricted to the auditory-verbal mo­
One hypothesis is that blind individuals’ memory reliance may dality for highly salient linguistic input. For example, blind and sighted
provide unique, practice-related benefits, illustrating experience- participants show equivalent performance on recognition memory for
dependent plasticity of memory systems. This has been most clearly nonverbalizable sounds (Arcos et al., 2022), and the improved span
demonstrated in the verbal short-term memory domain, particularly performance in congenitally blind participants disappears after ac­
among congenitally or early blind individuals. For example, children counting for perceptual encoding efficiency differences (Rokem and
who become blind at birth or early in life recall significantly more items Ahissar, 2009).
than sighted individuals, as demonstrated with auditory span tasks (Hull Furthermore, improvements in verbal short-term memory do not
and Mason, 1995; Swanson and Luxenberg, 2009; Withagen et al., extend to spatial short-term memory. Studies report no group

* Corresponding author.
E-mail addresses: karcos1@uci.edu (K. Arcos), smjaeggi@uci.edu (S.M. Jaeggi), grossman@uci.edu (E.D. Grossman).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2022.147943
Received 5 October 2021; Received in revised form 10 April 2022; Accepted 8 May 2022
Available online 21 May 2022
0006-8993/© 2022 The Author(s). Published by Elsevier B.V. This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
K. Arcos et al. Brain Research 1789 (2022) 147943

differences on spatial memory tasks that require recalling object con­ also reflect observers’ ability to actively explore information visually at
figurations and paths (Occelli et al., 2017; Vecchi et al., 2004), and a self-guided pace, the tendency for visual information to persist for
sighted participants commit fewer errors than blind participants on longer time durations than auditory information, and differences in how
spatial imagery tasks (Cornoldi et al., 1991). Of note, these latter tasks readily information can be verbalized, which is faster for visual as
require maintaining and manipulating objects’ relative spatial positions, compared to auditory information in sighted individuals (Frick, 1984;
thus drawing upon executive processes such as working memory, Klatzky et al., 2006).
beyond short-term memory storage demands. Blind individuals’ preferred sensory modality for encoding verbal
Together these findings are consistent with neurophysiological evi­ information is not yet clear. Consistently, blind participants are more
dence that congenitally blind individuals’ “visual” cortex is recruited efficient when encoding auditory information as compared to the
during linguistic tasks, including verbal memory tasks (Amedi et al., sighted (Hötting and Röder, 2009; Muchnik et al., 1991; Pigeon and
2003; Bedny et al., 2011; Hamilton and Pascual-Leone, 1998; Pant et al., Marin-Lamellet, 2017; Rokem and Ahissar, 2009). Early blind in­
2020; Röder et al., 2002; Sadato et al., 1996). Whether this reorgani­ dividuals also process heard linguistic information more efficiently than
zation reflects compensatory mechanisms or inherent pluripotent ca­ sighted individuals (Röder et al., 2003). Moreover, improved linguistic
pacity of cortex traditionally characterized as unimodal is not yet clear processing may transfer to tactile encoding. Sighted individuals make
(Bavelier and Neville, 2002; Bedny, 2017). Age of visual impairment significantly more errors on verbal working memory n-back tasks when
onset, however, is believed to be a critical variable such that early onset using raised letters versus visual letters, while blind braille readers
provides dual opportunity for memory training, both through cortical perform equally well when using raised tactile or braille letters (Bliss
plasticity during a critical developmental window, and as a result of et al., 2004). When comparing auditory versus tactile encoding, blind
extensively practicing memory strategies daily to promote functional and braille literate children recalled more words encoded in braille as
independence. compared to when listening to words (Pring, 1988). This suggests that
Whether experience-dependent training and/or plasticity may also proficient braille readers may preferentially encode linguistic informa­
enhance other executive systems in early blindness has gained tion in braille.
increasing interest. For example, early blind individuals have improved
selective attention relative to sighted individuals as evaluated using 1.1. Considering socioeconomic status
speeded reaction times (Kujala et al., 1997). The blind also show
improved auditory and tactile selective attention, including divided or When measuring executive function, considering an individual’s
sustained attention over long intervals (Collignon et al., 2006; Pigeon economic and social opportunities is important. Executive function in­
and Marin-Lamellet, 2015). However, evidence of either impaired or volves coordinating one’s thoughts and actions to achieve goals (Miller
enhanced performance in more complex working memory domains is and Wallis, 2009). Low socioeconomic status (SES) is linked to poorer
much less clear, depending on how working memory processes are scores on working memory measures (Ursache and Noble, 2016); the
evaluated. For example, when assessing working memory using a disadvantages appear early in development and remain stable through
backwards verbal span task, some studies report significantly larger late adolescence, reflecting opportunity gaps. Importantly, those diag­
spans in early blind participants versus sighted (Hull and Mason, 1995; nosed with sensory impairments, such as blindness, are more likely to
Occelli et al., 2017; Withagen et al., 2013), while others report no group come from low SES backgrounds, in part a consequence of major health
differences (Bathelt et al., 2018; Pigeon and Marin-Lamellet, 2015; inequality (Dandona and Dandona, 2001). This creates the double
Rokem and Ahissar, 2009; Wyver and Markham, 1998). Nonetheless, disadvantage of managing a complex disability in an environment with
blind participants consistently outperform sighted ones in working fewer resources, yet, those who become blind early on have been shown
memory studies evaluating verbal span with interference tasks to sup­ to strengthen their memory abilities and partially compensate for these
press rehearsal (Arcos et al., 2022; Cohen et al., 2010; Dormal et al., barriers as a result.
2016). In early and late blind participants, some studies measuring The current study evaluates verbal short-term memory and working
working memory through n-back tasks report faster reaction times (Pi­ memory abilities in participants with and without impaired vision across
geon and Marin-Lamellet, 2015, 2017), while still others report no dif­ sensory modalities, while also matching for demographic characteristics
ferences in accuracy between the two samples (Park et al., 2011; Pigeon to account for potential SES differences. Using three established tasks,
and Marin-Lamellet, 2015). we assess the effects of blindness on verbal short-term memory perfor­
Each task’s differential loading on key aspects of working memory mance (forward digit span), maintaining and manipulating items in
processes may partly explain these discrepancies. While backward spans verbal working memory (backward digit span), as well as updating and
rely largely on maintaining and manipulating items, interference tasks inhibiting verbal items in working memory (n-back).
require participants to suppress task-irrelevant information, and n-back
tasks impose even stronger demands on manipulating information (Kane 2. Results
et al., 2007). Verbal short-term and working memory also rely on unique
brain systems (as demonstrated in sighted individuals). Verbal storage is 2.1. Digit span
associated with the left inferior frontal gyrus and parietal cortex, while
executive functions involved in verbal working memory are associated A one-way repeated measures ANOVA with recall order (forward vs.
with right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Jonides et al., 2008). Blind backward) as the within-subjects factor and group (blind vs sighted) as
individuals recruit similar brain regions during verbal working memory, the between-subjects factor revealed a main effect of group on auditory
and additionally recruit occipital cortex (Park et al., 2011). Thus span accuracy (F(1, 75) = 39.82, p <.001, η2p = 0.35) such that blind
experience-dependent plasticity may be most pronounced in brain net­ participants recalled significantly more items than sighted participants
works supporting verbal working memory maintenance processes. when collapsing across direction (forward and backward; cf. Fig. 1,
Consideration of the sensory modality used to encode experimental Table 3, Supplementary Table 2). We found no significant main effect of
information during verbal working memory tasks is important. Sighted recall order (F(1, 75) = 3.02, p =.09, η2p = 0.04) nor a recall order by
adults and children have demonstrated benefits for information encoded group interaction (F(1, 75) = 0.42, p =.52, η2p = 0.01).
in a preferred modality, as evidenced by improved verbal recall for items We also tested whether the item encoding modality (auditory, visual,
read visually as compared to auditorily (Frick, 1984). Presumably also, or tactile) impacted memory span performance. Trials correct were
encoding information through non-dominant modalities imposes higher analyzed separately in the two groups due to the unique modalities in
cognitive loads, i.e., increases task difficulty, which may impair subse­ which they participated, i.e., visual and auditory in sighted participants
quent recall (De Jong, 2010). Better recall when visually reading may and auditory and tactile in blind participants. Among the sighted

2
K. Arcos et al. Brain Research 1789 (2022) 147943

Table 3). Accuracy decreased as a function of n-back level, reflecting


increased working memory load. We found neither a main effect of
group (F(1, 71) = 0.17, p =.68, η2p = 0.002) nor an n-back level by group
interaction (F(3, 213) = 0.58, p =.63, η2p = 0.008).
Using reaction times (RTs) as an outcome measure, we found a main
effect of n-back level (F(3, 213) = 21.65, p <.001, η2p = 0.23), again
indicating increased load was associated with slower RTs. In addition,
we found no main effect of group on RTs (F(1, 71) = 2.52, p =.12, η2p =
0.03), such that sighted and blind participants showed no differences in
RTs. The group by n-back level interaction was not significant either (F
(3, 213) = 1.10, p =.35, η2p = 0.02).

2.3. Groups matched on age and SES

Fig. 1. Digit span performance as a function of group (accuracy). Performance 2.3.1. Digit span
for sighted individuals is shown on the left (visual and auditory digit spans), In the matched sample, a two-way repeated measures ANOVA on the
and performance for blind individuals is shown on the right (auditory and auditory digit span accuracy scores revealed a main effect of group (F(1,
braille digit spans). Error bars denote +/− 1 standard errors of the mean. 32) = 30.43, p <.001, η2p = 0.49), which is consistent with the full
Significant differences between forward and backwards span performance sample’s results. We found neither an effect of recall order (forward vs.
(within-subjects) are indicated by asterisks (*p <.01; ***p <.001). backward; F(1, 32) = 2.297, p =.14, η2p = 0.07) nor a group by recall
order interaction (F(1, 32) = 0.40, p =.53, η2p = 0.01). The full corre­
participants, a two-way repeated measures ANOVA revealed a main lation relationship between age, SES factors, and the dependent mea­
effect of modality (F(1, 57) = 30.57, p <.001, η2p = 0.35) such that sures is shown in Supplementary Tables 5 and 6.
sighted individuals recalled more items after encoding the list visually
rather than when heard. A main effect of recall order was also found (F 2.3.2. N-back
(1, 57) = 18.46, p <.001, η2p = 0.25) such that sighted participants Replicating results from the full sample, when matched for age and
recalled significantly more digits in forward as compared to backward SES, we found no main effect of group on n-back accuracy (F(1, 32) =
order. Sighted participants’ recall order by modality interaction was not 2.10, p =.16, η2p = 0.06), though the effect size was slightly larger in the
significant (F(1, 57) = 1.49, p =.23, η2p = 0.03). matched as compared to the full sample. No group by n-back level
A two-way repeated measures ANOVA revealed a main effect of interaction was found (F(3, 96) = 0.70, p =.56, η2p = 0.02; cf. Fig. 2).
modality among the blind participants such that more items were For RTs, similar to what we observed in the full sample, we found no
recalled when heard versus read in braille (F(1, 18) = 20.15, p <.001, η2p main effect of group (F(1, 32) = 2.74, p =.11, η2p = 0.08), and the group
= 0.53). Furthermore, we found a main effect of recall order (F(1, 18) = by n-back level interaction was not significant either (F(3, 96) = 1.22, p
19.89, p <.001, η2p = 0.53) such that more items were recalled in forward =.31, η2p = 0.04).
vs. backward order. However, a significant recall order by modality
interaction in blind participants conditioned these main effects (F(1, 17)
2.4. Controlling for stimulus duration
= 8.46, p =.01, η2p = 0.32). Post-hoc tests indicated that blind partici­
pants recalled the same number of items in reverse and forward order
We further considered the possibility that the braille digit span’s
when listening to the lists (t(18) = 0.59, p =.56, d = 0.14) and fewer
extended stimulus duration (1 sec, with a 1 sec ISI) possibly allowed
digits in reverse order as compared to forward when reading in braille (t
more time for rehearsal and deeper encoding as compared to the rapid,
(18) = 8.50, p <.001, d = 1.95). Whereas the blind participants did not
visually presented items (500 msec, with a 1 sec ISI). Therefore, a new
differ in forward span whether digits were heard or read in braille (t(18)
= 0.78, p >.05, d = 0.18), backward span performance was significantly
better when hearing digits as compared to reading them in braille (t(18)
= 6.12, p <.001, d = 1.40).
When comparing only blind participants who learned braille before
age eight, similar results emerged as compared to the full sample of blind
participants. We found a main effect of modality such that more items
were recalled when heard versus read in braille (F(1, 15) = 14.73, p
<.01, η2p = 0.495). A main effect of recall order was also found (F(1, 15)
= 17.24, p =.001, η2p = 0.54) such that more items were recalled in
forward vs. backward order. However, unlike in the full sample of blind
participants, no recall order by modality interaction was found in those
literate in braille before age 8 (F(1, 15) = 3.83, p =.07, η2p = 0.20).
Among the full sample, self-reported braille proficiency and digit span
(forward and backward) were significantly correlated, and this corre­
lation persisted only in the forward span among individuals that learned
braille before age eight (Supplementary Table 3).

2.2. N-back

A two-way ANOVA with n-back level (one through four-back) as the Fig. 2. N-back performance as a function of level and group (matched partic­
within-subjects factor and group (blind and sighted) as the between- ipants only). Accuracy reflects PR scores (proportion hits – proportion false
subjects factor revealed a main effect of n-back level on accuracy (F(3, alarms). Error bars denote +/− 1 standard error of the mean. No group dif­
213) = 115.23, p <.001, η2p = 0.62; c.f. Table 3 and Supplementary ferences reached significance.

3
K. Arcos et al. Brain Research 1789 (2022) 147943

group of sighted participants completed the visual digit span using both did not calibrate our stimuli’s salience to individual thresholds and thus
the standard (500 msec) and an extended (1 sec) stimulus duration, with the extent to which perceptual processing efficiencies contributed to the
the extended duration matching the braille presentation time. Partici­ group differences observed here remains unclear.
pants completed both conditions in randomized order. We further find that blind participants’ short-term memory advan­
A one-way repeated measures ANOVA using digit duration (fast vs. tage did not transfer across encoding modalities. In particular, blind
slow) as the within-subjects factor revealed a main effect of digit dura­ participants showed a clear benefit when recalling items encoded au­
tion (F(1, 48) = 26.88, p <.001, η2p = 0.53) such that the sighted par­ ditorily such that span did not differ between forward and backward
ticipants performed better on the shorter duration forward trials (M = recall, unlike on the braille digit span. To the extent that span capacity
11.80, SD = 3.46) as compared to the longer duration forward trials (M reflects cognitive loads imposed by encoding in the nonpreferred mo­
= 8.60, SD = 4.38). The sighted participants also performed better on dality (De Jong, 2010; Frick, 1984), our findings indicate a preference
the shorter duration backward trials (M = 9.00, SD = 2.96) as compared for auditory encoding over braille. The blind’s auditory backward digit
to the longer duration backward trials (M = 7.64, SD = 3.52). span performance is also notably different from sighted participants,
who recalled fewer items in reverse on both auditory and visual span
3. Discussion tasks. This finding is consistent with previous reports of improved
auditory encoding efficiency in blindness (Hötting and Röder, 2009;
This study evaluates how visual deprivation may impact verbal Muchnik et al., 1991; Rokem and Ahissar, 2009) and indicates that blind
short-term and working memory abilities using carefully controlled participants have sufficiently enhanced span capacity (or compensatory
environments and matching based on participant demographics. We strategies) such that reversing heard items during recall does not tax the
considered preferred sensory modalities during encoding and in­ cognitive system’s limit. Importantly, this advantage seems to be
dividuals’ SES. Consistent with previous studies (Occelli et al., 2017; Raz restricted to the auditory domain.
et al., 2007; Withagen et al., 2013), we find blind participants have a One inference from modality specificity in the backward recall is
clear advantage over sighted individuals on a verbal digit span task that, even for the experienced braille readers in this sample, encoding
(Occelli et al., 2017; Raz et al., 2007; Withagen et al., 2013). Gains were text processed through hearing is less cognitively demanding than
most evident on the auditory forward span task which drove the group reading in braille. Reasons for the difference in cognitive demand are not
effect. However, differences were also apparent on the backward audi­ entirely clear. Evidence suggests that decoding braille places demands
tory span task in that reversing digit order did not impact blind in­ on visuospatial processes, in addition to the verbal demands of main­
dividuals’ number of trials correct despite the additional cognitive load. taining the items in verbal short-term memory. In a previous study,
On the braille span task, digit order impacted recall: Backward recall recall on a braille letter span decreased among congenitally blind in­
was attenuated as compared to forward recall, consistent with sighted dividuals when completed in conjunction with a spatial interfering task
individuals’ lower backward recall on the visual task due to added load as compared to a verbal interfering task (Cohen et al., 2010). Given that
effects. We found no evidence of enhanced verbal working memory on congenitally blind individuals’ spatial memory processes are disadvan­
an auditory n-back task, even after controlling for age and SES. taged relative to sighted individuals (Cornoldi et al., 1991), encoding
verbal information in braille may more heavily tax cognitive systems
3.1. Digit span and render braille readers more susceptible to load effects. The differ­
ences may also reflect the dominance of accessibility tools such as
Our finding that adults who are blind outperform the sighted on spoken and text-to-speech technology in daily practice or inherent dif­
verbal span tasks replicates many previous studies showing congenitally ferences in demands of representing verbal information when encoding
blind populations recall more items as compared to sighted populations text through braille versus speech.
(Amedi et al., 2003; Hull and Mason, 1995; Occelli et al., 2017; Pas­ Reading braille is known to require more time than reading print,
qualotto et al., 2013; Röder et al., 2001; Swanson and Luxenberg, 2009; with even the most experienced braille readers having slower reading
Withagen et al., 2013). Our findings are consistent with the proposal rates as compared to print reading (Wetzel and Knowlton, 2000). In our
that the blind’s improved verbal short-term memory is likely a conse­ study, blind participants were exposed to braille digits for longer du­
quence of relying on verbal encoding strategies as an extensive form of rations in order to successfully encode each item. We do not believe,
training in ecological settings. however, that the longer trial durations on the braille digit span tasks
We know, for example, that superior verbal short-term memory ex­ would explain the blind’s verbal short-term memory advantage over
tends beyond item memory to include serial position recall (Raz et al., sighted participants. In a control experiment (completed by sighted
2007). Thus, performance on memory span tasks may reflect the use of participants only), the longer exposure duration did not facilitate recall
strategies that explicitly capitalize on ordinal processing, such as and instead appeared to promote item decay, potentially due to the
chaining, in which adjacent items are associated based on their spatial longer time between items (Ricker et al., 2014). Therefore, we do not
positions or on timing intervals between items. Sighted individuals attribute our results to increased rehearsal time.
trained to use chaining have increased recall on both short-term and In any memory study, considering age and SES as possible confounds
working memory tasks relative to non-trained controls, and those with is important. Cognitive sciences is increasingly recognizing the need to
higher memory capacity benefit more from memory strategies as include samples and researchers who more accurately represent diverse
compared to those with lower memory capacity (McNamara and Scott, populations in psychological findings (Hötting and Röder, 2009; Medin,
2001). Whether our results, and those of previous studies, reflect 2017; Roberts et al., 2020; Syed, 2017). Our study samples drew from a
increased verbal memory capacity or increased capacity facilitated highly diverse racial/ethnic population (cf. Tables 1 and 2). Blind par­
through chaining and ordinal positions is unclear. ticipants outperformed our full sample of sighted participants, despite
Our findings may also reflect improved speech processing efficiency. the fact that the blind participants included here represent a more so­
This improvement may, in turn, make available more cognitive re­ cioeconomically diverse and disadvantaged population as compared to
sources for engaging short-term memory functions. Studies suggest blind the sighted group. We take this as evidence that despite possibly facing
individuals may be more sensitive to speech sounds and selectively double disadvantages due to blindness and SES, blind individuals’ short-
attend to targeted speech more effectively (Hötting and Röder, 2009; term memory capacity is robust to the SES factors that may otherwise
Muchnik et al., 1991; Röder et al., 2003; Weaver and Stevens, 2007). adversely impact cognitive functions. To probe this further, an addi­
Moreover, when speech sounds’ perceptual salience are equated on an tional analysis of sighted and blind participants matched for age,
individual participant level, blind and sighted individuals show equiv­ household income, and maternal education returned similar results as
alent verbal short-term memory spans (Rokem and Ahissar, 2009). We the unmatched sample, with a notably larger effect size despite the

4
K. Arcos et al. Brain Research 1789 (2022) 147943

Table 1
Age, gender and demographic information of the full sample population.
Unmatched (Full) Sample Blind Sighted Sighted Timing Control t p Effect size

N 19 58 25

Age (range and mean) 22–48 (28.74) 17–49 (25.69) 18–45 (21.9)
− 1.46a’ 0.15 − 0.38a’

Age of VI onset (years, range and mean) 0–3 (0.66)

Gender (%)
Male 21 28 20
Female 79 66 80
Prefer not to disclose 0 7 0
0.52 0.47 0.08

Ethnicity (%)
African American/Black 11 0 8
Asian American/Pacific Islander 11 29 44
Latinx/Hispanic American 53 52 28
Native American/Alaska Native 11 0 0
White/Euro American 11 9 12
Nonhispanic 58 38 64
Prefer not to disclose 5 10 8
13.89 0.01 0.45

Maternal Education (%)


Less than high school 42 21 8
High School Diploma or GED 11 21 16
Some college/vocational trade 11 17 24
Associate’s Degree 5 7 8
Bachelor’s Degree 11 21 32
Postgraduate or Professional 11 5 8
Prefer not to disclose 11 9 4
5.31 0.38 0.28

Family Income (%)


$0–23,050 37 22 16
$23,050–32,500 11 10 12
$32,500–60,000 21 16 20
$60,000–100,000 5 21 12
$100,000–150,000 0 7 0
$150,000 or more 6 9 16
Prefer not to disclose 21 16 24
5.06 0.41 0.28

overall smaller sample size. We encourage future studies to include measures. Therefore, we do not attribute our findings to those factors.
broader SES factors such as household and material income, in addition Instead, our findings suggest that even with congenital or early blind­
to age and education. These could contribute to unmask structural fac­ ness promoting efficient verbal processing, this efficiency is insufficient
tors that otherwise obscure any cognitive advantages. to overcome the cognitive demands of manipulating and updating in­
formation in verbal working memory.

3.2. N-back
3.3. Conclusions and future directions
N-back tasks rely more extensively on executive functions as
compared to the digit span (Kane et al., 2007). Our study adds to the Overall, our data provide evidence that participants who are blind
growing evidence that improved verbal short-term memory may not outperform sighted participants on verbal short-term memory digit span
imply enhanced executive function more generally. Previous work uti­ tasks. This superiority was most pronounced when encoding items au­
lizing the n-back in blind and sighted participants finds no impact of ditorily rather than through braille, evidence for heard speech being less
visual status on accuracy, although some studies report faster reaction cognitively taxing as compared to reading braille.
times in the blind populations (Gudi-Mindermann et al., 2018; Park Much remains to be understood of memory abilities, namely how
et al., 2011; Pigeon and Marin-Lamellet, 2015, 2017). When comparing visual deprivation contributes to developing these benefits. More
n-back accuracy across preferred modalities (visual for the sighted research is needed to understand how information modality and
sample, braille for a congenitally blind sample), no between-group dif­ encoding may interact, given the advantage we observed for auditory
ferences were found, and blind and sighted populations improved on n- over braille encoding. Based on the importance of SES on working
back tasks at equivalent training rates (Bliss et al., 2004; Park et al., memory development, we recommend future research addresses how
2011). susceptible these functions are to stressors, when the benefits emerge
Like studies before ours, we were careful to match on participants’ developmentally, and if these benefits may be associated with resilience.
age and education (Bliss et al., 2004; Park et al., 2011; Pigeon and Importantly, we conclude that blindness contributes to unique cognitive
Marin-Lamellet, 2017), both of which impact working memory strengths, such as verbal short term memory benefits in blind as

5
K. Arcos et al. Brain Research 1789 (2022) 147943

Table 2
Age, gender and demographic information for the matched sample.
Matched Sample

Unmatched Factors Blind Sighted t p Effect size (Cohen’s D)

N 17 17

Gender (%)
Male 24 41
Female 77 59
Prefer not to disclose 0 0
1.21 0.27 0.19

Ethnicity (%)
African American/Black 12 0
Asian American/Pacific Islander 6 6
Latinx/Hispanic American 53 82
Native American/Alaska Native 12 0
White/Euro American 12 12
Nonhispanic 41 18
Prefer not to disclose 6.00 0.00
5.06 0.28 0.39

Matched Factors
Age (range and mean) 22–48 (28.59) 21–49 (29.59)
0.39a 0.70 0.13a

Maternal Education (%)


Less than high school 47 24
High School Diploma or GED 6 18
Some college/vocational trade 12 24
Associate’s Degree 6 0
Bachelor’s Degree 6 12
Postgraduate or Professional 12 12
Prefer not to disclose 12 12
4.33 0.50 0.38

Family Income (%)


$0–23,050 41 24
$23,050–32,500 12 6
$32,500–60,000 18 24
$60,000–100,000 6 24
$100000–150,000 0 6
$150,000 or more 6 6
Prefer not to disclose 18 12
4.07 0.54 0.37

compared to sighted individuals. Casteren and Davis, 2007).


An additional group of sighted participants (N = 25, aged 18–45;
4. Experimental procedure mean age = 21.92 ± 5.18) participated in a control experiment using the
visual digit span task (described in further detail below).
4.1. Participants
Seventy-seven participants volunteered for this study (sighted: N = 4.2. Procedures
58, aged 17–49; mean age = 25.69 ± 8.16; blind: N = 19, aged 22–48; The University of California Irvine Human Subjects Institutional
mean age = 28.74 ± 6.98). To be eligible for the study, blind partici­ Review Board approved all study procedures, and all participants con­
pants could have no more than light perception in the better eye, and sented prior to participating. Study tasks were controlled using Mac
their preferred reading medium was braille. Sighted participants had computers running MATLAB (Mathworks, Inc.) with the PsychToolbox
self-reported normal, or corrected to normal vision. All participants (Brainard, 1997). Participants listened to items using the internal com­
were fluent English speakers. Participants were ineligible if they had puter speakers. Digits for the braille digit span were displayed on
additional neurological or developmental disabilities, as well as if they Freedom Scientific’s Focus 40 Braille Display. With the exception of the
had a recent history of illicit drug use. Participants were recruited using demographic questionnaire, participants inputted responses using an
snowball sampling and from throughout the university community. Two external numeric keypad.
of the 19 blind participants did not complete the n-back task but were Participants completed two tasks under blindfold: an auditory digit
included in the digit span analyses. Additional details on population span and an auditory n-back task. Sighted participants additionally
demographics are shown in Tables 1 and 2. completed a visual digit span task, while blind participants completed a
Both age and SES differed between our two groups’ full samples (cf. braille digit span task (also under blindfold). Participants completed
Table 1). Therefore, we selected a subset of blind (n = 17) and sighted (n digit span and n-back tasks in counterbalanced order.
= 17) participants from the larger group, matched on factors of age,
maternal education, and family income, for secondary analyses. SES was 4.2.1. Digit span. Participants were presented unique lists of single
held equivalent across both groups using the program ‘Match’ (Van digits (1–9) in pre-determined orders based on the Wechsler Memory

6
K. Arcos et al. Brain Research 1789 (2022) 147943

Table 3
Summary of participants, tasks, and results for the full and matched samples.
Unmatched (Full) Sample N F p η2p
Task Modality Blind Sighted

Digit Span
Auditory main effect of group 19 58 39.82 0.000 0.35
main effect of recall order 3.02 0.09 0.04
recall order × group 0.42 0.52 0.01
Visual (exp) main effect of modality – 58 30.57 0.000 0.35
main effect of recall order 18.46 0.000 0.25
recall order × modality 1.49 0.23 0.03
Braille main effect of modality 19 – 20.15 0.000 0.53
main effect of recall order 19.89 0.000 0.53
recall order × modality 8.46 0.01 0.32
Visual (timing control) – 25
main effect of digit duration 26.88 0.000 0.53

N-Back
Auditory 17 56
PR main effect of group 0.17 0.68 0.002
main effect of n-back 115.226 0.000 0.62
n-back level × group 0.58 0.63 0.008
RT main effect of group 2.52 0.12 0.03
main effect of n-back 21.65 0.000 0.23
n-back level × group 1.10 0.35 0.02

Matched (sub) Sample

Task Modality Blind Sighted F p Effect size

Digit Span
Auditory main effect of group 17 17
main effect of recall order 30.43 0.000 0.49
recall order × group 2.297 0.14 0.07
0.404 0.53 0.01

N-Back
Auditory PR main effect of group 17 17 2.10 0.16 0.06
n-back level × group 0.70 0.56 0.02

RT main effect of group 2.74 0.11 0.08


n-back level × group 1.22 0.31 0.04

Scale (Wechsler, 1945). Participants recalled the digits either in forward Working memory load increased as a function of n-back level, which
or reverse order, with the two tasks completed in separate blocks. To ranged from one to four in increasing difficulty across subsequent
familiarize themselves with the task, participants completed two prac­ blocks. The following consonants were selected for the n-back task to
tice trials per block, followed by the experimental trials. The lists ranged minimize confusion, maximize clarity, and for standardized letter
in length from two to twelve items, with two trials completed per list duration when heard: C, D, G, K, P, Q, T, and V (Jaeggi et al., 2010).
length. List lengths increased by one digit if the participant correctly Items were recorded using a female voice and were played back for 500
recalled all digits in order on the prior two trials. The task terminated msec with a 2.5 sec ISI.
when participants committed errors on both lists of the same length. Participants practiced the task for a list of 10 trials at each n-back
Trials correct, our dependent variable, was computed as the total level. Participants were required to achieve 70% accuracy (1-back) or
number of trials recalled correctly, summed over all list lengths. All 60% (2-back) to proceed to the main task. No minimum accuracy
participants completed the digit span using the same digit sequences; threshold was required for the 3- and 4-back practice trials.
however, lists of items were not repeated across either the visual, On the main task, each n-back level consisted of three lists with 20 +
auditory, or braille tasks. n trials per list. Each list included six target trials in which the letter
Digit duration and interstimulus intervals (ISIs) were adjusted for matched the n-back item. Two-, three-, and four-back lists included six
each digit span modality to facilitate sensory encoding. Digits on the lures (targets that matched the n-1 or n + 1 items). The remaining trials
auditory span task were recorded using a female speaker, standardized across all n-back lists consisted of filler items. Accuracy and reaction
to a 500 msec duration with a 1 sec ISI and played at a comfortable times were recorded. Accuracy was used to compute PR scores, i.e. the
volume. Digits on the visual span were displayed in black on a white difference between proportion hits (correctly identified targets) and
screen and were visible to sighted participants for 500 msec with a 1 sec proportion false alarms (fillers and lures incorrectly identified as tar­
ISI. Digit duration on the braille version was extended to 1 sec (1 sec ISI) gets). Average PR scores, averaged n + 1 and n-1 lure accuracy, and
to allow sufficient time for reading, which, even for the most advanced average median correct reaction times (RTs) were used as dependent
braille readers, is generally slower than print reading (Wetzel and variables.
Knowlton, 2000).
4.2.3. Digit span timing control experiment. An additional group of
4.2.2. Auditory n-back. Participants heard letters and identified sighted participants completed two versions of the visual digit span task:
whether or not the current letter matched the letter heard n positions an identical version as in the main experiment in which digits were
back on the list by key press (‘4′ for targets, and ‘6′ for non-targets). presented for a 500 msec duration with a 1 sec ISI, and an extended

7
K. Arcos et al. Brain Research 1789 (2022) 147943

version in which digits were presented for 1 sec with a 1 sec ISI References
(matching the braille span’s duration and timing). Lists and visual digit
span versions were counterbalanced across participants. Amedi, A., Raz, N., Pianka, P., Malach, R., Zohary, E., 2003. Early ’visual’ cortex
activation correlates with superior verbal memory performance in the blind. Nat.
Neurosci. 6 (7), 758–766. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1072.
4.3. Demographic questionnaire Arcos, K., Harhen, N., Loiotile, R., Bedny, M., 2022. Superior verbal but not nonverbal
All participants completed a demographic questionnaire. Partici­ memory in congenital blindness. Exp. Brain Res. 240, 897–908. https://doi.org/
10.1007/s00221-021-06304-4.
pants provided information about their gender, age, socioeconomic Bathelt, J., de Haan, M., Salt, A., Dale, N.J., 2018. Executive abilities in children with
status (income, education level of participants and both of their par­ congenital visual impairment in mid-childhood. Child Neuropsychol. 24 (2),
ents), race/ethnicity, and whether they were diagnosed with neurolog­ 184–202. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2016.1240158.
Bavelier, D., Neville, H.J., 2002. Cross-modal plasticity: where and how? Nat. Rev.
ical disorders or cognitive or psychiatric disabilities (Supplementary Neurosci. 3 (6), 443–452. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn848.
Table 1). In addition, participants who are blind answered questions Bedny, M., 2017. Evidence from blindness for a cognitively pluripotent cortex. Trends
about their blindness severity, rated their braille proficiency level, Cogn. Sci. 21 (9), 637–648. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.06.003.
Bedny, M., Pascual-Leone, A., Dodell-Feder, D., Fedorenko, E., Saxe, R., 2011. Language
braille reading frequency, and reported their preferred reading medium.
processing in the occipital cortex of congenitally blind adults. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
Fifteen participants who were blind answered additional questions 108 (11), 4429–4434. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1014818108.
about their blindness, providing details on whether or not they could see Bliss, I., Kujala, T., Hämäläinen, H., 2004. Comparison of blind and sighted participants’
shapes, color, or motion, as well as their visual diagnosis, current and performance in a letter recognition working memory task. Cogn. Brain Res. 18 (3),
273–277. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2003.10.012.
maximum visual acuity, and whether or not they perceived light. Par­ Brainard, D.H., 1997. The psychophysics Toolbox. Spat. Vis. 10 (4), 433–436. https://
ticipants also answered questions about their blindness onset, including doi.org/10.1163/156856897X00357.
the ages (in years) at which they stopped having functional vision and Cohen, H., Voss, P., Lepore, F., Scherzer, P., Gilbert, S., 2010. The nature of working
memory for Braille. PLoS ONE 5 (5), e10833.
reading print, as well as when they became totally blind for those whose Collignon, O., Renier, L., Bruyer, R., Tranduy, D., Veraart, C., 2006. Improved selective
vision loss was progressive. and divided spatial attention in early blind subjects. Brain Res. 1075 (1), 175–182.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2005.12.079.
Cornoldi, C., Cortesi, A., Preti, D., 1991. Individual differences in the capacity limitations
CRediT authorship contribution statement of visuospatial short-term memory: Research on sighted and totally congenitally
blind people. Mem. Cogn. 19 (5), 459–468. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03199569.
Karen Arcos: Conceptualization, Methodology, Software, Formal Dandona, R., Dandona, L., 2001. Socioeconomic status and blindness. Br. J. Ophthalmol.
85 (12), 1484–1488. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjo.85.12.1484.
analysis, Investigation, Data curation, Writing – original draft, Project De Jong, T., 2010. Cognitive load theory, educational research, and instructional design:
administration, Funding acquisition. Susanne M. Jaeggi: Conceptuali­ some food for thought. Instr. Sci. 38 (2), 105–134. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-
zation, Methodology, Validation, Formal analysis, Resources, Writing – 009-9110-0.
Dormal, V., Crollen, V., Baumans, C., Lepore, F., Collignon, O., 2016. Early but not late
review & editing, Visualization, Supervision, Project administration. blindness leads to enhanced arithmetic and working memory abilities. Cortex 83,
Emily D. Grossman: Conceptualization, Methodology, Software, Re­ 212–221. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2016.07.016.
sources, Writing – review & editing, Visualization, Supervision, Project Frick, R.W., 1984. Using both an auditory and a visual short-term store to increase digit
span. Mem. Cogn. 12 (5), 507–514. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03198313.
administration, Funding acquisition.
Gudi-Mindermann, H., Rimmele, J.M., Nolte, G., Bruns, P., Engel, A.K., Röder, B., 2018.
Working memory training in congenitally blind individuals results in an integration
of occipital cortex in functional networks. Behav. Brain Res. 348, 31–41. https://doi.
Declaration of Competing Interest org/10.1016/j.bbr.2018.04.002.
Hamilton, R.H., Pascual-Leone, A., 1998. Cortical plasticity associated with Braille
The authors declare that they have no known competing financial learning. Retrieved from Trends Cogn. Sci. 2 (5), 168–174. https://www.cell.com/t
rends/cognitive-sciences/home.
interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence
Hötting, K., Röder, B., 2009. Auditory and auditory-tactile processing in congenitally
the work reported in this paper. blind humans. Hear. Res. 258 (1–2), 165–174. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
heares.2009.07.012.
Hull, T., Mason, H., 1995. Performance of blind children on digit-span tests. J. Visual
Acknowledgments Impair. Blind. 89 (2), 166–169. https://doi.org/10.1177/0145482X9508900213.
Jaeggi, S.M., Buschkuehl, M., Perrig, W.J., Meier, B., 2010. The concurrent validity of the
N-back task as a working memory measure. Memory 18 (4), 394–412. https://doi.
We gratefully acknowledge study participants, as well as Braille
org/10.1080/09658211003702171.
Institute of America in Los Angeles, National Federation of the Blind, Jonides, J., Lewis, R.L., Nee, D.E., Lustig, C.A., Berman, M.G., Moore, K.S., 2008. The
Blind Children’s Center, and Wayfinder Family Services for recruitment mind and brain of short-term memory. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 59 (1), 193–224. https://
assistance. We thank the following individuals for collecting and doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093615.
Kane, M.J., Conway, A.R., Miura, T.K., Colflesh, G.J., 2007. Working memory, attention
analyzing data, as well as for preparing and offering graphics, refer­ control, and the N-back task: a question of construct validity. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn.
ences, equipment, and stimuli: Deena E. Afana, Jacky Wo Hay Au, Mem. Cogn. 33 (3), 615. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.33.3.615.
Jennifer Chau, Louis Herrera, Nicholas T. Hilberg, Oscar O. Huete, Klatzky, R.L., Marston, J.R., Giudice, N.A., Golledge, R.G., Loomis, J.M., 2006. Cognitive
load of navigating without vision when guided by virtual sound versus spatial
Patrick T. Hwu, Tania Reyes, Sharmin Shanur, and Kirstyn Tara. language. J. Exp. Psychol.: Appl. 12 (4), 223–232. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-
898X.12.4.223.
Kujala, T., Lehtokoski, A., Alho, K., Kekoni, J., Näätänen, R., 1997. Faster reaction times
Funding in the blind than sighted during bimodal divided attention. Retrieved from Acta
Psychol. 96 (1–2), 75–82. https://www.journals.elsevier.com/acta-psychologica.
Two National Science Foundation, United States fellowships and McNamara, D.S., Scott, J.L., 2001. Working memory capacity and strategy use. Mem.
Cogn. 29 (1), 10–17. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03195736.
grants supported this research (DGE-1321846 and BCS-1658560). It also Medin, D.L., 2017. Psychological science as a complex system: Report card. Perspect.
received support from the University of California Irvine and the Uni­ Psychol. Sci. 12 (4), 669–674. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691616687746.
versity of California President’s Postdoctoral Fellowship. Funding Miller, E.K., Wallis, J.D., 2009. Executive function and higher-order cognition: definition
and neural substrates. In: Squire, L.R. (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Neuroscience. Elsevier,
sources were not involved in study design; in the collection, analysis and
pp. 99–104.
interpretation of data; in writing the report; or in deciding to submit the Muchnik, C., Efrati, M., Nemeth, E., Malin, M., Hildesheimer, M., 1991. Central auditory
article for publication. The authors are solely responsible for the skills in blind and sighted subjects. Scand. Audiol. 20 (1), 19–23. https://doi.org/
10.3109/01050399109070785.
content.
Occelli, V., Lacey, S., Stephens, C., Merabet, L.B., Sathian, K., 2017. Enhanced verbal
abilities in the congenitally blind. Exp. Brain Res. 235 (6), 1709–1718. https://doi.
Appendix A. Supplementary data org/10.1007/s00221-017-4931-6.
Pant, R., Kanjlia, S., Bedny, M., 2020. A sensitive period in the neural phenotype of
language in blind individuals. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 41, 100744 https://doi.org/
Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi. 10.1016/j.dcn.2019.100744.
org/10.1016/j.brainres.2022.147943.

8
K. Arcos et al. Brain Research 1789 (2022) 147943

Park, H.J., Chun, J.W., Park, B., Park, H., Kim, J.I., Lee, J.D., Kim, J.J., 2011. Activation Sadato, N., Pascual-Leone, A., Grafman, J., Ibañez, V., Deiber, M.-P., Dold, G.,
of the occipital cortex and deactivation of the default mode network during working Hallett, M., 1996. Activation of the primary visual cortex by Braille reading in blind
memory in the early blind. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 17 (3), 407–422. https://doi. subjects. Nature 380 (6574), 526–528. https://doi.org/10.1038/380526a0.
org/10.1017/S1355617711000051. Smits, B., Mommers, M., 1976. Differences between blind and sighted children on WISC
Pasqualotto, A., Lam, J.S., Proulx, M.J., 2013. Congenital blindness improves semantic verbal subtests. J. Visual Impair. Blind. 70 (6), 240–246. https://doi.org/10.1177/
and episodic memory. Behav. Brain Res. 244, 162–165. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. 0145482X7607000604.
bbr.2013.02.005. Swanson, H.L., Luxenberg, D., 2009. Short-term memory and working memory in
Pigeon, C., Marin-Lamellet, C., 2015. Evaluation of the attentional capacities and children with blindness: support for a domain general or domain specific system?
working memory of early and late blind persons. Acta Psychol. 155, 1–7. https://doi. Child Neuropsychol. 15 (3), 280–294. https://doi.org/10.1080/
org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.11.010. 09297040802524206.
Pigeon, C., Marin-Lamellet, C., 2017. Ageing effects on the attentional capacities and Syed, M., 2017. Why traditional metrics may not adequately represent ethnic minority
working memory of people who are blind. Disabil. Rehabil. 39 (24), 2492–2498. psychology. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 12 (6), 1162–1165. https://doi.org/10.1177/
https://doi.org/10.1080/09638288.2016.1236407. 1745691617709590.
Pring, L., 1988. The ‘reverse-generation’ effect: A comparison of memory performance Ursache, A., Noble, K.G., 2016. Neurocognitive development in socioeconomic context:
between blind and sighted children. Br. J. Psychol. 79 (3), 387–400. https://doi.org/ Multiple mechanisms and implications for measuring socioeconomic status.
10.1111/j.2044-8295.1988.tb02297.x. Psychophysiology 53 (1), 71–82. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp. 12547.
Raz, N., Striem, E., Pundak, G., Orlov, T., Zohary, E., 2007. Superior serial memory in the Van Casteren, M., Davis, M.H., 2007. Match: A program to assist in matching the
blind: a case of cognitive compensatory adjustment. Curr. Biol. 17 (13), 1129–1133. conditions of factorial experiments. Behav. Res. Methods 39 (4), 973–978. https://
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2007.05.060. doi.org/10.3758/BF03192992.
Ricker, T.J., Spiegel, L.R., Cowan, N., 2014. Time-based loss in visual short-term memory Vecchi, T., Tinti, C., Cornoldi, C., 2004. Spatial memory and integration processes in
is from trace decay, not temporal distinctiveness. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. congenital blindness. Retrieved from NeuroReport 15 (18), 2787–2790. https:
40 (6), 1510. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000018. //journals.lww.com/neuroreport/pages/default.aspx.
Roberts, S.O., Bareket-Shavit, C., Dollins, F.A., Goldie, P.D., Mortenson, E., 2020. Racial Weaver, K.E., Stevens, A.A., 2007. Attention and sensory interactions within the occipital
inequality in psychological research: Trends of the past and recommendations for the cortex in the early blind: an fMRI study. J. Cognit. Neurosci. 19 (2), 315–330.
future. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 15 (6), 1295–1309. https://doi.org/10.1177/ https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.2.315.
1745691620927709. Wechsler, D., 1945. Wechsler Memory Scale. Psychological Corporation.
Röder, B., Demuth, L., Streb, J., Rösler, F., 2003. Semantic and morpho-syntactic priming Wetzel, R., Knowlton, M., 2000. A comparison of print and Braille reading rates on three
in auditory word recognition in congenitally blind adults. Lang. Cogn. Process. 18 reading tasks. J. Vis. Impair. Blind. 94 (3), 146–154. https://doi.org/10.1177/
(1), 1–20. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960143000407. 0145482X0009400303.
Röder, B., Rösler, F., Neville, H.J., 2001. Auditory memory in congenitally blind adults: a Withagen, A., Kappers, A.M., Vervloed, M.P., Knoors, H., Verhoeven, L., 2013. Short term
behavioral-electrophysiological investigation. Cogn. Brain Res. 11 (2), 289–303. memory and working memory in blind versus sighted children. Res. Dev. Disabil. 34
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0926-6410(01)00002-7. (7), 2161–2172. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2013.03.028.
Röder, B., Stock, O., Bien, S., Neville, H., Rösler, F., 2002. Speech processing activates Wyver, S.R., Markham, R., 1998. Do children with visual impairments demonstrate
visual cortex in congenitally blind humans. Eur. J. Neurosci. 16 (5), 930–936. superior short-term memory, memory strategies, and metamemory? J. Vis. Impair.
https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1460-9568.2002.02147.x. Blind. 92 (11), 799–811. https://doi.org/10.1177/0145482X9809201107.
Rokem, A., Ahissar, M., 2009. Interactions of cognitive and auditory abilities in
congenitally blind individuals. Neuropsychologia 47 (3), 843–848. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.12.017.

You might also like