Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 18

Journal of Intellectual & Developmental Disability

ISSN: 1366-8250 (Print) 1469-9532 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/cjid20

Teaching play skills to young children with autism

Sunhwa Jung & Diane M. Sainato

To cite this article: Sunhwa Jung & Diane M. Sainato (2013) Teaching play skills to young children
with autism, Journal of Intellectual & Developmental Disability, 38:1, 74-90

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.3109/13668250.2012.732220

Published online: 16 Nov 2012.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 1656

View related articles

Citing articles: 16 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=cjid20

Download by: [Thomas Jefferson University] Date: 07 December 2017, At: 19:53
Journal of Intellectual & Developmental Disability, March 2013; 38(1): 74–90

LITERATURE REVIEW

Teaching play skills to young children with autism

SUNHWA JUNG1 & DIANE M. SAINATO2


1Otterbein University, USA, and 2The Ohio State University, USA
Downloaded by [Thomas Jefferson University] at 19:53 07 December 2017

Abstract
Background Play is critical for the development of young children and is an important part of their daily routine.
However, children with autism often exhibit deficits in play skills and engage in stereotypic behaviour. We reviewed
studies to identify effective instructional strategies for teaching play skills to young children with autism.
Method Empirical studies on teaching play skills to young children with autism published from 1990 to 2011 were located.
These studies included single subject and group designs.
Results Twenty-six studies were reviewed. The majority of studies on teaching play skills used combined interventions.
Children with autism improved their play skills, with direct intervention embedding their interests during play. Improve-
ments in play skills increased positive social interactions and decreased inappropriate behaviour as collateral effects.
Conclusions Further research is needed to develop more effective play skill interventions that assess the functional use of
play and are implemented in the natural environment.

Keywords: autism, play skills, instructional strategies

Introduction
facilitate social and communicative interactions
Most young children naturally engage in play follow- with their peers (McConnell, 2002) and encourage
ing a typical developmental sequence and acquire engagement in appropriate behaviour and coopera-
critical developmental skills while interacting with tive play while decreasing inappropriate behaviour
play materials and peers (Boucher & Wolfberg, 2003; (Baker, 2000; Koegel, Koegel, Hurley, & Frea, 1992;
Boutot, Guenther, & Crozier, 2005; Pierce-Jordan & MacDonald et al., 2009; Machalicek et al., 2009;
Lifter, 2005; Stahmer, 1999; Thomas & Smith, Stahmer & Schreibman, 1992). These skills may also
2004). However, the play of children with autism help children with autism to be viewed as members
may be often skewed by restricted interests and of their class and to build friendships with their peers
stereotypic or repetitive patterns of behaviour (Blanc, (Jordan, 2003).
Adrien, Roux, & Barthélémy, 2005; Boutot et al., Since deficits in social and communicative beha-
2005; Holmes & Willoughby, 2005; Libby, Powell, viour are two of the defining characteristics of autism
Messer, & Jordan, 1998; Lydon, Healy, & Leader, (American Psychiatric Association, 2000), play is
2011; MacDonald, Sacramone, Mansfield, Wiltz, & often used as the context to improve these beha-
Ahearn, 2009; Machalicek et al., 2009; Stahmer, viours. The literature is replete with intervention
1999; Thomas & Smith, 2004). As a result, children studies designed to improve and enhance children’s
with autism may have limited opportunities for peer social and communicative behaviour; however, there
interaction, the acquisition of appropriate play, and are fewer studies focusing on directly teaching and
other critical developmental skills. assessing play skills. Several researchers reporting
Engaging appropriately with play materials and outcomes of play skill intervention studies classi-
peers in classrooms allows young children with fied targeted play skills as functional, symbolic, or
autism to acquire critical developmental skills via sociodramatic play, while others have focused on
naturally occurring learning opportunities. This may appropriate play with the chosen materials without

Correspondence: Sunhwa Jung, Otterbein University, Westerville, OH 43081, USA. E-mail: sjung@otterbein.edu

ISSN 1366-8250 print/ISSN 1469-9532 online © 2013 Australasian Society for Intellectual Disability, Inc.
DOI: 10.3109/13668250.2012.732220
Teaching play skills 75

specification of type of play (e.g., Barry & Burlew, disorders and found that nonverbal cognitive ability
2004; Morrison, Sainato, Benchaaban, & Endo, and expressive language are significantly related
2002). More attention has been paid to teaching to symbolic play. Since children with autism show
symbolic play, although there are inconsistencies deficits in symbolic play and the development
across studies in the classification of types of play of social interactions (Stahmer, 1995), studies
targeted. Research focusing on functional play (i.e., have focused on explicitly teaching symbolic play
functionally using an object such as pushing a toy skills (Colozzi et al., 2008; Hine & Wolery, 2006;
car; Libby, Powell, Messer, & Jordan, 1997) suggests Kasari, Freeman, & Paparella, 2006; MacDonald
that children with autism exhibit no significant dif- et al., 2005, 2009; Palechka & MacDonald, 2010;
ferences in functional play when compared to devel- Stahmer, 1995; Wong, Kasari, Freeman, & Paparella,
opmentally matched children with other disabilities 2007; Zercher, Hunt, Schuler, & Webster, 2001).
and typically developing children (Blanc et al., 2005; Results of these studies suggest that children with
Holmes & Willoughby, 2005; Libby et al., 1998; autism were able to produce symbolic play with
Williams, Reddy, & Costall, 2001). appropriate intervention.
However, other researchers note that while chil- Sociodramatic play is defined as an advanced
Downloaded by [Thomas Jefferson University] at 19:53 07 December 2017

dren with autism may not demonstrate deficits in form of symbolic play involving engagement in
functional play, this play is qualitatively different role-playing and cooperative dramatisations around
(Williams et al., 2001). They suggest that the func- a particular theme (Goldstein & Cisar, 1992;
tional play of children with autism is often relatively Stahmer, 1999; Thorp, Stahmer, & Schreibman,
simple and less elaborate than that of developmen- 1995). Studies suggest that children with autism
tally matched children. While some intervention were able to engage in sociodramatic play and dem-
studies focusing on play may not identify targeted onstrate positive collateral changes in their social
skills as functional play, other studies subsume and communicative interactions after intervention
functional and symbolic play under pretend play (Dauphin, Kinney, & Stromer, 2004; Goldstein &
(Colozzi, Ward, & Crotty, 2008; Lydon et al., 2011; Cisar, 1992; Thorp et al., 1995). However, Thorp
MacDonald, Clark, Garrigan, & Vangala, 2005; and colleagues (1995) noted that although children
MacDonald et al., 2009; Palechka & MacDonald, with autism learned to engage in sociodramatic play
2010; Sancho, Sidener, Reeve, & Sidener, 2010) with intervention, the quality of their play may be
or the appropriate or functional use of toys or play different from that of children with other develop-
with materials (Barry & Burlew, 2004; D’Ateno, mental disabilities.
Mangiapanello, & Taylor, 2003; Hine & Wolery, With regard to another type of play, games may
2006; Jahr, Eldevik, & Eikeseth, 2000; Morrison provide a unique context for the remediation of
et al., 2002; Paterson & Arco, 2007). Increases in various skills for young children with autism. In
the appropriate and functional use of toys, engage- two studies, children with autism were able to
ment with play materials, and amount of time play games with their typically developing peers or
spent in play after conducting play interventions siblings after being taught games in which their
are reported outcomes of these investigations. obsessive interests were embedded (Baker, 2000;
Symbolic play is defined as a child’s ability to act Baker, Koegel, & Koegel, 1998). Teaching modified
on an object as if it is something else. This type games using the children’s interests increased recip-
of play involves three forms: object substitution rocal play and social interactions among children
(e.g., using a brick as some soap), the attribution with and without autism.
of false properties (e.g., pretending a doll is ill), Research on directly teaching play skills demon-
and the attribution of presence to imaginary objects strates that young children with autism can acquire
(e.g., driving a truck over an invisible bridge; Leslie, appropriate play behaviours. These children respond
1987). Research reports that children with autism well to the play skill interventions that include
have significant deficits in symbolic play when com- modelling, prompting with contingent reinforce-
pared to typically developing children at the same ment, and naturalistic instruction (Lang et al.,
developmental age (Blanc et al., 2005; Holmes & 2009). Acquisition of play skills helps children with
Willoughby, 2005; Libby et al., 1998; Rutherford, autism interact positively with their typically deve-
Young, Hepburn, & Rogers, 2007). Results from loping peers (Goldstein & Cisar, 1992; Stahmer,
these studies also suggest symbolic play deficits in 1995; Thomas & Smith, 2004), and may reduce
children with autism appear to be highly associated children’s engagement in inappropriate behaviour
with their limited language skills. In particular, as a collateral effect (Baker, 2000; Nuzzolo-Gomez,
Stanley and Konstantareas (2007) examined the Leonard, Ortiz, Rivera, & Greer, 2002; Stahmer &
assessment files of 101 children with autism spectrum Schreibman, 1992).
76 S. Jung & D. M. Sainato

Play skills may be the critical tools in a child’s and (d) studies directly focusing on teaching and
repertoire needed to engage with peers in early measuring play skills. Studies targeting other skills
childhood settings, as well as critical developmental (e.g., social interactions, communication) taught
skills in and of themselves. Teaching appropriate through play or using play as a context to improve
play skills to young children with autism at an other skills were excluded.
early age may enhance children’s acquisition of According to the criteria of this review, 26 stud-
age-appropriate developmental skills, build posi- ies were included for review while 23 studies were
tive relationships with peers, and enable children excluded. The 26 included studies used both group
to demonstrate desirable behaviours (van Berckelaer- and single subject designs, with 23 of these studies
Onnes, 2003). employing single subject research designs and three
Given the importance of play, practitioners in studies employing group designs. In their review of
early childhood/early childhood special educa- single subject design studies, Odom et al. (2003)
tion should pay more attention to play skills and suggest the multiple-baseline experimental design
incorporate play skills instruction effectively into most often yields a demonstration of effectiveness
young children’s daily activities and individualised through multiple replications of treatment effects.
Downloaded by [Thomas Jefferson University] at 19:53 07 December 2017

education programs (Malone & Langone, 1999; Of the 23 single subject studies reviewed, 21 stud-
Mastrangelo, 2009). Identifying effective instruc- ies implemented a multiple-baseline/multiple-probe
tional approaches for young children with autism design and two studies used other single subject
is a vital step toward promoting play skills within experimental designs such as a reversal or alternating
the context of natural play environments. The treatment design.
purpose of this review is to identify effective
instructional procedures for teaching play skills
Categorisation of studies
and to discuss the implications for practice and
for future research. Play skill studies were categorised by the primary
intervention as defined by the author of the study in
their title, review of literature, and method. Many
Method studies employed prompting strategies; however,
some studies were categorised under systematic
Search process and selection criteria
prompting strategies, whereas other studies were
In order to review the research on teaching play skills listed under other primary intervention categories
to young children with autism, a literature search (e.g., script training, Social Stories) based on the
using PsycINFO and ERIC (1990 to 2011) was con- researchers’ specifications and our own analysis.
ducted. The search was limited to English-language That is, if a study’s use of prompting procedures
publications, peer-reviewed journals, and empirical was systematically planned and implemented as a
studies. The studies were located using the following primary intervention (e.g., a least to most, most
search terms: autism and teaching play skills, autism to least, simultaneous prompting, time delay), the
and play intervention, autism and play, and autistic study was categorised under “Systematic prompt-
and play. The first search using the terms autism and ing strategy.” If the prompting procedure was
teaching play skills, and autism and play intervention implemented to scaffold students’ responses after
identified 24 studies. The search was then expanded the primary intervention strategy was employed,
using the broader terms autism and play and autistic the study was categorised under the researchers’
and play. In the initial search, 292 articles related to identified intervention strategy. Interrater agree-
play skills and autism were identified. After examin- ment on the categorisation of intervention strategies
ing the titles and abstracts of these articles, 49 stud- was assessed by the second author. Both authors
ies teaching play skills to children with autism were independently categorised the intervention strat-
identified. egies of all 26 articles. There was 100% agree-
The criteria for selecting studies after the search ment on the categorisation of the intervention
were as follows: (a) experimental studies dem- strategies.
onstrating functional relationships between the Interventions reviewed included video and live
independent and dependent variables; (b) stud- modelling, the use of systematic prompting strat-
ies including at least one participant with autism egies, and pivotal response training. In addition,
aged from 0 to 8 years old; (c) studies including studies using children’s restricted interests, activity
at least one participant who was identified as hav- schedules with correspondence training, the inte-
ing autism, autism spectrum disorder, or pervasive grated playgroup model, script training, and Social
developmental disorder not otherwise specified; Stories are included in this review.
Teaching play skills 77

Results newly acquired behaviours were maintained after


watching ICV for a play scenario and CAV for a
Researchers have employed a variety of instructional
different play scenario. Two participants learned the
strategies to teach play skills to young children with
scripted play sequence rapidly with ICV, and one
autism. Table 1 summarises the features of the 26
participant showed similar responses to both video
included studies.
models. The researchers suggest that the differential
effects occurred as the ICV provided a more exact
and specific model of the play behaviours with the
Video and live modelling
materials and toys that were more similar to those
Strategies emphasising observation and imitation available during free play sessions.
of a model have been used to facilitate the lear- Researchers have also combined VM with other
ning of a variety of skills by children with autism strategies such as reinforcement contingencies, verbal
(Ledford, Gast, Luscre, & Ayres, 2008; McDuffie instruction, and matrix training (Dauphin, Kinney,
et al., 2007). Video modelling (VM) encourages & Stromer, 2004; Hine & Wolery, 2006; Paterson &
children to acquire targeted skills through observa- Arco, 2007). In a multiple-probe design, Hine and
Downloaded by [Thomas Jefferson University] at 19:53 07 December 2017

tional learning. In this strategy children are shown a Wolery (2006) used a point-of-view VM procedure
videotaped demonstration of the desired play actions for playing with gardening and cooking play sets
and asked to imitate the model’s behaviour in the with two preschool children with autism. Children
video. Video models can be provided by adults, peers, were then asked to play with play sets identical to
self, “point-of-view” (i.e., showing only the viewpoint the models. In addition, verbal instruction to imi-
of the target behaviour without showing the entire tate the video and reinforcement for the appropri-
person), or by using a combined approach (McCoy ate play were provided to a child who was not able
& Hermansen, 2007). This intervention uses the to learn the play skills by only watching the video.
visual strengths and interests of children with autism. Paterson and Arco (2007) also conducted a study
It is also an efficient strategy for offering repeated using a multiple-baseline design across play beha-
practise of targeted skills and for providing mul- viours while implementing VM with verbal prompts
tiple exemplars (Hine & Wolery, 2006; McCoy & and praise. In their study, two boys aged 6 and 7 were
Hermansen, 2007). asked to watch the videotape demonstrating play
Eight studies implementing VM or VM combined with transportation toys. The children’s appropriate
with other strategies indicate that VM is an effective play with toys increased while repetitive behaviour
tool for teaching play skills. For example, in D’Ateno decreased. In another study, Dauphin et al. (2004)
et al.’s study (2003) employing a multiple-baseline implemented VM embedded in a computer activity
design across response categories, a girl with autism schedule, with matrix training and teacher prompt-
of 3 years 8 months was asked to watch video- ing evaluated in a multiple-baseline design across
taped play sequences of the selected play actions as responses. As a result, the 3-year-old participant in
modelled by adults (i.e., tea party, shopping, and their study showed increases in scripted play and
baking). She was given the same play materials untrained play activities.
that were shown in the videotaped demonstration. Sancho et al. (2010), using an adapted alternating
Following the intervention, the child was able to treatments design, compared the effectiveness of two
engage in appropriate play with the selected toys types of VM for teaching pretend play skills. That is,
without reinforcement or prompting. Other studies VM without prompts or reinforcement versus simul-
using VM to teach play skills report that children taneous VM with prompts and reinforcement for imi-
learned the sequences of scripted verbalisations and tation. Two children with autism aged 5 participated
play actions quickly, without reinforcement and in the study. During the simultaneous VM condition,
adult prompts, and maintained them over time the participants were shown the videotaped model
(MacDonald et al., 2005; MacDonald et al., 2009; and physically prompted and reinforced (i.e., with
Palechka & MacDonald, 2010). snacks) for prompted and independent imitation of
In another study, Palechka and MacDonald (2010) the play actions. In the video priming condition, the
attempted to compare the effects of instructor- participants were shown the video without access to
created video modelling (ICV) versus commercially the toys. Reinforcements were delivered for attend-
available video models (CAV) using a multi-element ing to the video. For one participant, both proce-
design within participant and across model types dures were similarly effective for imitating scripted
and a multiple-probe design across participants. play actions. However, the second participant showed
Three young children with autism of 4 to 5 years increases in scripted play actions more quickly in the
learned scripted play from watching ICVs. Their simultaneous VM than in the video priming.
Downloaded by [Thomas Jefferson University] at 19:53 07 December 2017

78

Table 1. Play skills intervention

Instructional strategies/
Authors Participants Play skills taught/settings experimental design Results Generalisation

Video and live modelling


D’Ateno et al. (2003) A child with autism aged Scripted verbal statements and Video modelling of play Modelled verbal and Not generalised to novel
3 years 8 months modelled motor responses sequences motor play responses and not modelled
(tea party, shopping, and baking) Multiple-baseline design across increased responses
A conference room response categories
Dauphin et al. (2004) A child with autism Sociodramatic play Video-enhanced activity Sociodramatic play Generalised to untrained
spectrum disorder aged Child’s home schedules and matrix training activities increased play activities
S. Jung & D. M. Sainato

3 years 1 month Reversal design


Hine & Wolery (2006) 2 children with autism, Play actions with gardening and Video modelling with verbal Video-modelled play Generalised to untrained
aged 2 years 6 months cooking toys instructions and positive actions increased materials and the
and 3 years 7 months A preschool therapy room reinforcements classroom for one toy
Multiple-probe design across two set
behaviours
Jahr et al. (2000) 6 children with autism, Cooperative play (Lego, dolls, Modelling with verbal description Targeted play Play generalised to novel
aged from 4 to wooden blocks, etc.) Nonconcurrent multiple-baseline responses increased settings and play
12 years old Residential home or classrooms design across participants partners
MacDonald et al. 2 children with autism, Pretend play (town, ship, house Video modelling of play Scripted play actions Not generalised
(2005) aged 4 and 7 years old play set) sequences and verbalisations and verbalisations
Preschool classroom Multiple-probe design across increased
play sets
MacDonald et al. 2 children with autism, Pretend play (airport, zoo, grill Video modelling of scripted play Scripted play 1 child showed increases in
(2009) aged 5 and 7 years old play set) interactions behaviour increased unscripted verbalisation
A testing room Multiple-probe design across
play sets
Palechka & MacDonald 3 children with ASD, aged Pretend play (scripted vocalisations, Instructor-created video models Scripted play increased Not reported
(2010) from 4 to 5 years old scripted play actions), attending and commercially available in both conditions
to video, and attending to toys videos Differential effects
during video viewing Multi-element design within upon the
Therapy room participant and across model participants in two
types; a multiple-probe design procedures
across participants
Paterson & Arco (2007) 2 children with autism, Play with transport toys Video modelling with prompts, Appropriate toy play 1 child generalised to
aged 6 and 7 years old A play room in the special verbal instruction, praise increased and related toys
education centre Multiple-baseline design across repetitive behaviour
play behaviours decreased
Sancho et al. (2010) 2 children with autism, Pretend play (a house and a circus Simultaneous video modelling Play actions and Play actions generalised to
aged 5 years 4 months play set) (prompts and reinforcements) attending increased other play but limited
and 5 years 11 months 5 different locations and video priming similarly in both
(with no prompts) interventions
Adapted alternating treatments
design
Downloaded by [Thomas Jefferson University] at 19:53 07 December 2017

Systematic prompting strategies


Coe et al. (1990) 2 children, one with Initiating and playing a ball game Least-to-most intrusive prompts, Learned targeted Not reported
autism, aged 6 years Contrived play setting reinforcements, and time out nonverbal and verbal
old, and one with Down Multiple-baseline design across play responses
syndrome, aged 5 years children and behaviours
old
Colozzi et al. (2008) 3 children with autism and Pretend play expressive vocabulary Simultaneous prompting Learned targeted skills Variable effects in both
a child with Down skills and motor skills procedure, a multiple-probe but group conditions
syndrome aged from A separate preschool classroom design across training instruction required
3 years 7 months to responses and students, and a more trials than 1:1
4 years 4 months repeated acquisition design condition
across the two experimental
conditions of 1:1 versus small
group instruction
Kasari et al. (2006) 58 children with autism, Joint attention and symbolic play Prompting, reinforcement, Intervention groups Generalised to untrained
aged from 3 to 4 years One-on-one intervention session and naturalistic instruction showed caregivers
old F-statistics in the mixed-effect improvements in
(21 in the playgroup, 20 in regressions joint attention and
the joint attention, and symbolic play
17 in the control group)
Liber et al. (2008) 3 children with autism, Appropriate social play with toys Task analysis of play activity Social play activity 2 students generalised to
aged 6, 7, and (circus train and zoo keeper) and time delay increased different settings and a
9 years old Classrooms Multiple-baseline design across larger group of peers
subjects
Lifter et al. (2005) 3 children with PDD, Play skills assessed by play Least-to-most prompting Targeted play activities Not reported
aged 4 years 10 months, assessment for each participant followed by the child’s lead increased
5 years 2 months, and Child’s home Modified multiple-baseline
6 years 4 months old design across play activities
Wong et al. (2007) 41 children with autism, Joint attention and symbolic play Prompting, reinforcement, Reached performance Generalised to different
aged from 2 years One-on-one session and naturalistic instruction mastery criteria in settings
7 months to 4 years Randomised group experimental joint attention and
7 months old design symbolic play
Pivotal response training
Lydon et al. (2011) 5 children with autism, Pretend play (circus toys, Barney, Pivotal response training Play actions in both Play actions for PRT
aged from 3 years to zoo, etc.) and video modelling interventions generalised to settings
6 years old Room in the participants’ school Paired samples t-test significantly but play verbalisations
increased but no not generalised for both
significant increases interventions
in play verbalisations
in both interventions

(Continued )
Teaching play skills
79
Downloaded by [Thomas Jefferson University] at 19:53 07 December 2017

80

Table 1. (Continued )

Instructional strategies/
Authors Participants Play skills taught/settings experimental design Results Generalisation

Stahmer (1995) 7 children with autism, Symbolic play (a tea set, male doll, Pivotal response training Symbolic play and 6 children generalised to
aged from 4 years Barbie doll, etc.) Multiple-baseline design across complexity of play different toys and
3 months to 7 years Family room, bedroom, or empty subjects increased different people
2 months classroom
Thorp et al. (1995) 3 children with autism, Sociodramatic play (Cabbage-Patch Pivotal response training Sociodramatic play, Moderately generalised to
S. Jung & D. M. Sainato

aged 5 years 4 months, boy doll, toy bottle, toy baby Multiple-baseline probe design language skills, and different toys, settings,
9 years 9 months, and food, etc.) across subjects social behaviour and people
8 years 2 months Child’s home and a clinic setting increased
Restricted interests
Baker (2000) 3 children with autism, Bingo game Teaching modified game using Increases in social play Generalised into other
aged 5 years 8 months, Playrooms in clinic adult prompts to play with interactions and games, other settings
5 years 5 months, and siblings continued increases with peers
6 years 9 months Multiple-baseline design across during follow-up
participants phase
Baker et al. (1998) 3 children with autism, Games: tag, follow the leader Teaching modified game using Increased engagement Generalised to other
aged 7 years 7 months, Recess or lunch at school adult prompts to play with in appropriate social games
5 years 4 months, and peers play interactions
8 years 9 months Multiple-baseline design across
participants
Activity schedules with correspondence training
Machalicek et al. 3 children with autism, Engagement with playground Activity schedule and Play activities Not reported
(2009) aged 6, 7, and activities (swing, slide, etc.) correspondence training increased and
12 years old Playground Multiple-baseline design across challenging
participants behaviour decreased
Morrison et al. (2002) 4 children with autism, Engagement with play materials Activity schedule and Engagement with play Generalised to different
aged from 3 years Classroom correspondence training materials increased settings
6 months to 5 years Multiple-baseline design across
10 months participants
Integrated playgroup model
Wolfberg & Schuler 3 children with autism Play engagement (manipulation, Integrated playgroup Variable but play Reported by teacher
(1993) aged 7 years functional, symbolic play) Multiple-probe baseline design engagement and parent interview
Special classroom across participants improved
Zercher et al. (2001) Twins with autism, aged Joint attention, symbolic play, Peer-mediated integrated Joint attention, Not reported
6 years old and language playgroup symbolic play, and
Sunday school class Multiple-baseline design across verbal utterances
subjects increased
Teaching play skills 81

Generalised to other play Among eight studies on VM, only one study
targeted sociodramatic play (Dauphin et al., 2004),
whereas several studies taught functional and

Not reported but


anecdotal note
symbolic play skills. Three studies were conducted
in the child’s natural environment, such as the class-
partners

room or home (Dauphin et al., 2004; MacDonald


et al., 2005; Sancho et al., 2010), and five studies
were conducted in a separate place such as a clinic,
play, or therapy room (D’Ateno et al., 2003; Hine &
Increased independent

Wolery, 2006; MacDonald et al., 2009; Palechka &


behaviour increased

MacDonald, 2010; Paterson & Arco, 2007). Of the


appropriate play
All scripts and the

eight studies on VM, seven studies collected general-


related social

choices and

isation data. One of them documented generalisation


to untrained play activities (Dauphin et al., 2004),
and four studies indicated mixed or limited gener-
Downloaded by [Thomas Jefferson University] at 19:53 07 December 2017

alisation (Hine & Wolery, 2006; MacDonald et al.,


2009; Paterson & Arco, 2007; Sancho et al., 2010).
Two studies documented no generalisation effects
Multiple-baseline design across

Multiple-baseline design across


Script training using a least-to-

Teaching social stories using a

(D’Ateno et al., 2003; MacDonald et al., 2005). Six


studies reported that the effect of the intervention
prompting hierarchy

was maintained after removing the VM (Hine &


most prompting

Wolery, 2006; MacDonald et al., 2005; MacDonald


et al., 2009; Palechka & MacDonald, 2010; Paterson &
participants

Arco, 2007; Sancho et al., 2010). Only two studies


scripts

reported procedural integrity and social validity data


(Hine & Wolery, 2006; Sancho et al., 2010).
One additional study examined the effects of live
modelling on cooperative play using a non-concur-
Appropriate play (interacting with

rent multiple-baseline design across participants


(Jahr et al., 2000). The results indicated that the
Exceptional student education
and an unused classroom

modelling procedure alone is not sufficient but is


Participants’ classrooms

more effective when combined with a verbal descrip-


the materials/peers)

tion. Three children with autism aged 4–7-years-old


Sociodramatic play

and three children aged 9–12-years-old observed


models engaging in scripted play in two different
classroom

conditions: observation and imitation of modelled


play versus verbal description of modelled play and
imitation. The results indicated the children were not
able to learn the play skills with observation alone;
peers, aged from 3 years

however, they demonstrated the targeted play skills


Children with autism and

2 students with autism,

after a verbal description of the modelled play epi-


7 months to 5 years
typically developing

aged 7 and 8 years

sode was embedded in the intervention. Jahr et al.


(2000) also report that the learned play skills gen-
eralised across novel settings and with untrained play
partners and were maintained. They reported social
validity data but no procedural integrity data.
As indicated, several researchers suggest that
video or live modelling alone may not be effective
Barry & Burlew (2004)

and the intervention may need to be combined


with other strategies (Dauphin et al., 2004; Hine &
Goldstein & Cisar
Script training

Wolery, 2006; Jahr et al., 2000; Paterson & Arco,


Social Stories

2007). Further, the intervention may have a differ-


(1992)

ential effect on the play behaviour of individual par-


ticipants (Sancho et al., 2010). Critical prerequisite
skills for the use of modelling (e.g., attention and
82 S. Jung & D. M. Sainato

imitation skills, language, cognitive skills, recalling The children’s play was socially reinforced throughout
the auditory and visual stimulus) may be the key the intervention. The children in the joint attention
to the successful use of these types of interventions intervention group demonstrated improvements in
(Dauphin et al., 2004). responses to joint attention, and the children in the
play skill group demonstrated more diverse functional
and symbolic types of play. The participants were able
Use of systematic prompting strategies
to generalise their learned skills from the treatment
Systematic prompting strategies (e.g., least-to-most sessions with the experimenter to play with their
or most-to-least intrusive prompting procedures) caregivers (Kasari et al., 2006; Wong et al., 2007).
have been widely implemented to teach a variety of In addition, two studies implemented a sponta-
skills including play skills. Prompting strategies are neous prompting and time delay procedure. Using
often used in conjunction with other intervention a multiple-baseline design across training responses
tactics and have been successfully incorporated into and students, Colozzi et al. (2008) investigated the
script training (Goldstein & Cisar, 1992), Social effect of a simultaneous prompting procedure with
Stories (Barry & Burlew, 2004), games using the children in a one-on-one versus small group setting.
Downloaded by [Thomas Jefferson University] at 19:53 07 December 2017

children’s restricted interests (Baker, 2000; Baker The pretend play skills of three preschool children
et al., 1998), activity schedules (Machalicek et al., with autism and a child with Down syndrome were
2009; Morrison et al., 2002), or peer-mediated observed. During the sessions, the toys were pre-
interventions (Zercher et al., 2001). For the pur- sented to the students with physical assistance and a
poses of this paper, studies using systematic prompt- verbal model of play simultaneously. All participants
ing as a primary intervention strategy are reviewed learned and maintained the targeted skills in both
under this category. conditions. In addition, the participants demon-
Six studies primarily implemented a prompt- strated slightly higher generalisation during the small
ing procedure alone or with other instructional group instruction. In a study using a progressive time
strategies, such as certain components of natural- delay procedure, Liber et al. (2008) implemented a
istic instruction or reinforcement (Coe, Matson, progressive time delay procedure with task-analy-
Fee, Manikam, & Linarello, 1990; Colozzi et al., sed play activities to improve social play skills (i.e.,
2008; Kasari et al., 2006; Liber, Frea, & Symon, appropriate play with toys, initiations toward peers,
2008; Lifter, Ellis, Cannon, & Anderson, 2005; and requesting peer assistance) for three children
Wong et al., 2007). Coe and his colleagues (1990) aged 6, 7, and 9. The study employed a multiple-
attempted to teach two 5-year-old children with baseline design across subjects. All participants
autism and a 5-year-old child with Down syn- increased their targeted play skills while decreasing
drome ball play, including ball contact play initia- dependence on adult prompts. Two participants also
tion, cooperative ball play, and compliments to their generalised the play skills to different settings, with
play partner. A least-to-most intrusive prompting different materials and play partners.
procedure was used for each target play behaviour Six studies evaluated systematic prompting strat-
followed by reinforcement. Results indicated that egies and two of these studies focused on joint
each participant acquired the targeted verbal and attention and symbolic skills (Kasari et al., 2006;
nonverbal play skills with the intervention. Lifter Wong et al., 2007). One study taught pretend play
et al. (2005) also investigated the effects of a least- skills (Colozzi et al., 2008), and two other studies
to-most prompting procedure on targeted play investigated individual targeted play skills or appro-
skills in three preschool children with pervasive priate play with a ball or toys (Coe et al., 1990;
developmental disorders while employing a modi- Lifter et al., 2005). Two studies were conducted in
fied multiple-baseline design across play activities. the child’s classroom (Coe et al., 1990; Liber et al.,
The intervention was implemented by following the 2008). One study was implemented in a separate
child’s lead. Subsequently, the child’s performance preschool classroom (Colozzi et al., 2008), with an
of the targeted play skill was socially reinforced. additional study taking place in the child’s home
Their findings indicated that all participants learned (Lifter et al., 2005). Two studies were conducted
the targeted play activities. Kasari et al. (2006) and in one-on-one intervention settings (Kasari et al.,
Wong et al. (2007) also used a prompting hierarchy 2006; Wong et al., 2007). Four studies (Colozzi
combined with naturalistic instruction. Across two et al., 2008; Kasari et al., 2006; Liber et al., 2008;
studies, 58 and 41 preschool children with autism, Wong et al., 2007) reported generalisation data
respectively, were randomly assigned to one of two indicating the targeted play skills were generalised
intervention groups, joint attention or play skills, to other people and different settings (Kasari et al.,
and taught the targeted skills while being prompted. 2006; Wong et al., 2007) or variables (Colozzi et al.,
Teaching play skills 83

2008; Liber et al., 2008). Only one study (Colozzi the intensity of the intervention may differentially
et al., 2008) documented maintenance effects. Four affect outcomes in terms of generalisation and
studies reported the procedural integrity (Colozzi maintenance of skills.
et al., 2008; Kasari et al., 2006; Lifter et al., 2005; The studies on pivotal response training focused
Wong et al., 2007), with one study (Colozzi et al., on symbolic play skills and were conducted in a
2008) reporting social validity data. room in the participants’ school, the child’s home,
and/or a clinic setting. However, children’s play skills
were evaluated with peer play partners. All three
Pivotal response training
studies (i.e., Lydon et al., 2011; Stahmer, 1995;
Another strategy documenting effective outcomes Thorp et al., 1995) reported generalisation to dif-
for improving the language, social interactions, ferent toys, settings, and people, and demonstrated
and play skills of children with autism is that of that the learned play skills were maintained during
pivotal response training (PRT). The PRT model follow-up. No studies provided procedural integrity
is described as a naturalistic instructional strategy and social validity data.
incorporating a child’s interests into discrete trials
Downloaded by [Thomas Jefferson University] at 19:53 07 December 2017

in natural environments while following the child’s


Use of restricted interests
lead (Koegel & Koegel, 2006; Stahmer, Ingersoll, &
Carter, 2003). That is, the adult presents the child’s Researchers have attempted to use the restricted
favourite toys and models appropriate play. When interests of children with autism as an asset. Baker
the child engages in the appropriate play the child’s and colleagues (Baker, 2000; Baker et al., 1998)
response is reinforced and the adult provides exem- embedded the restricted interests of young children
plars while taking turns with the child. with autism into games (e.g., bingo, tag, follow the
Two studies investigated the effectiveness of piv- leader, etc.). Subsequently, these modified games were
otal response training using a multiple-baseline taught to children with autism and their typically
probe design across subjects (Stahmer, 1995; Thorp developing peers or siblings using adult prompting.
et al., 1995). For the young children aged 4 to 9, For example, one child was obsessed with writing
there were demonstrated increases in symbolic play consecutive numbers in a line. This behaviour was
(Stahmer, 1995) as well as in sociodramatic play embedded into a typical bingo game to match the
(Thorp et al., 1995). In addition, children’s play com- numbers on the play cards (Baker, 2000). Another
plexity increased, and they demonstrated improve- child was interested in U.S. geography, so he was
ment in social interactions. The newly acquired play asked to play a modified tag game using a map
skills also generalised into new settings, with new toys, of the US on the playground (Baker et al., 1998).
and with parents. Stahmer (1999) suggests that PRT The results for these studies, evaluated in a
is more effective for children who show some inter- multiple-baseline design across participants suggest
est in object manipulation, have some imitation skills, that teaching games increased appropriate social
and do not exhibit severe self-stimulatory behaviour. play. Further, these interactions were maintained
Lydon and colleagues (2011) attempted to com- without an adult’s cue and generalised to other
pare the effectiveness of PRT and VM in teaching non-obsession themed games or play activities and
pretend play skills to five preschoolers with autism. to other settings. The researchers concluded that
During the VM intervention, the participants were prompting children to engage in play may not be suf-
shown a video clip, but prompts or reinforcements ficient. However, after incorporating the children’s
were not used. During PRT, the experimenter pre- own interests into the games, the children engaged
sented the participant’s preferred toy and provided in reciprocal play with their peers or siblings. These
models of symbolic play actions and verbal statements two studies were conducted in playrooms in a clinic
while taking turns with the child. The participants or at recess time at school. These studies reported
showed significant increases in play actions with social validity data; however, no procedural integrity
both procedures; however, the increases in PRT data were provided.
were higher than in the VM condition. Play actions
generalised in PRT but not in the VM intervention.
Activity schedules with correspondence training
Play verbalisations did not significantly increase in
the training and generalisation settings for either Activity schedules are often used for children
intervention. This study also suggests students with with autism to facilitate independent engagement
sufficient language skills may increase pretend play in activities and encourage smooth transitions
skills with PRT and VM; implementation of pivotal from activity to activity. Two studies using activ-
response training required more time than VM; and ity schedules with correspondence training focused
84 S. Jung & D. M. Sainato

on improving engagement with functional and of three 7-year-old children with autism using a
symbolic play (Morrison et al., 2002), and play- multiple-probe baseline design across participants.
ground activities (Machalicek et al., 2009). They developed three playgroups consisting of two
Morrison and her colleagues (2002) imple- children with autism and three typically develop-
mented activity schedules combined with corres- ing children in each group. Children with autism in
pondence training using a multiple-baseline design the integrated playgroup were encouraged to play
across participants to increase the independent with the toys by their peers while adult guidance/
play of four preschool children with autism in an scaffolding was provided. Although the results were
inclusive classroom. Children with autism and variable, the participants engaged more in func-
their peers were taught to put photographs of their tional, symbolic, and social play with a decrease in
selected play activities on a clipboard and to fore- nonfunctional object manipulation and isolated play.
cast their selections. At the end of playtime, the Maintenance of the improved play engagement was
children were asked to report where they played. variable during the probe. For the generalisation and
Verbal feedback was provided after a review of the social validity measure, this study conducted parent
correspondence between where the children said and teacher interviews and individual symbolic
Downloaded by [Thomas Jefferson University] at 19:53 07 December 2017

they were going to play and where they actually play assessment before and after the intervention.
played. Results suggest that children’s engagement Procedural integrity measures were not reported.
with play materials, on-schedule behaviour, and Zercher and colleagues (2001) developed a play-
play correspondence behaviour increased with the group to increase the joint attention, symbolic
intervention and were maintained. The participants’ play, and language of the children with autism that
on-task behaviour during play also generalised into included two 6-year-old twin brothers with autism
an untrained setting. and three typically developing children. A multiple-
Machalicek et al. (2009) also conducted a study baseline design across subjects was implemented to
using photographic activity schedules and task examine the effects of the intervention. Typically
correspondence training with three children with developing children were taught to model targeted
autism 6, 7, and 12 years of age. The intervention play actions and to facilitate the play engagement of
was evaluated using a multiple-baseline design the children with autism. The experimenter reviewed
across participants. The teacher asked the partici- each training step with the peers prior to the session.
pants to name what they would like to play and Each participant showed increases in joint attention,
then reviewed the participants’ activity schedules symbolic play, and the number of verbal utterances.
with the participants during recess. The researchers In addition, the trained peers were able to conduct
indicate that all participants demonstrated increases play sessions without adult prompts during mainte-
in play on the playground; however, the teacher’s nance; however, the play behaviours of the children
verbal prompts were needed to remind children to with autism increased only with the peers’ prompts.
check their activity schedules in between planned This study documents data on procedural integrity
playground activities. No generalisation data were and social validity; however, generalisation effects
reported. Both studies were conducted in a natural were not reported.
environment with peers and reported procedural
integrity data. Only one study (Morrison et al.,
Script training
2002) documented social validity data.
Script training has been used to teach children
to follow specific sequences of play behaviour.
Integrated playgroup model
Goldstein and Cisar (1992) used script training to
Peer-mediated intervention studies are most often teach sociodramatic play to three preschool chil-
used to improve social interactions of children with dren with autism and six typical peers. Each script
autism. In these studies, play served as the con- consisted of the verbal and nonverbal behaviours
text to facilitate social interactions through which of three assigned roles for a sociodramatic theme
typically developing peers are taught and prompted (e.g., a salesperson, an animal caretaker, and a cus-
(Odom et al., 1999; Odom & Strain, 1986; Odom tomer in a pet shop). Three triads including a child
& Watts, 1991). Two studies on the peer-mediated with autism and two typical peers were taught scripts
integrated playgroup model were conducted with for their assigned roles using a most-to-least prompt-
a direct focus on facilitating the play behaviour of ing hierarchy. The study was conducted in classrooms
children with autism. and evaluated in a multiple-baseline design across
Wolfberg and Schuler (1993) investigated the scripts. Their findings indicated all children learned
effects of the integrated playgroup on the play the scripts and exhibited more theme-related social
Teaching play skills 85

interactions. In addition, all children maintained and et al., 2004; Hine & Wolery, 2006), modelling with
generalised the learned skills to different play part- verbal description (Jahr et al., 2000), use of restricted
ners. This study reported procedural integrity data interests (Baker, 2000; Baker et al., 1998), script
but no data on social validity. Script training of play training (Goldstein & Cisar, 1992), and Social
behaviour has also been used in conjunction with Stories (Barry & Burlew, 2004) with prompting
other intervention techniques such as VM (D’Ateno strategies. Studies also employed activity schedule
et al., 2003; MacDonald et al., 2009; Paterson & and correspondence training procedures (Morrison
Arco, 2007). et al., 2002), peer-mediated integrated playgroup
interventions (Wolfberg & Schuler, 1993; Zercher
Social Stories et al., 2001), or components of naturalistic instruc-
tion (Kasari et al., 2006; Lifter et al., 2005; Wong
Studies employing Social Stories include a descrip- et al., 2007).
tion of a challenging situation for the child and Although a variety of play skill instructional
the expected behaviours for that situation (Gray approaches have been implemented, VM has been
& Garand, 1993). Barry and Burlew (2004) used most widely implemented. VM utilises the visual
Downloaded by [Thomas Jefferson University] at 19:53 07 December 2017

Social Stories to teach choice making and appropri- strengths of children with autism; however, it shares
ate play skills to children with autism. In their study, common components with other play interventions
classroom teachers taught children to use Social Sto- such as task-analysed play actions and statements,
ries with photographs illustrating the components of and visual presentation of sequenced play actions
behaviours and skills. These stories included expec- with or without verbal instruction. The studies inves-
tations about choice making and appropriate play tigating play skills instruction indicate that a variety
with materials and peers. The Social Stories were of visual supports are included in the intervention
reviewed on a daily basis, and corrective feedback that consider the characteristics of children with
was provided during activities in a special educa- autism, such as pictures, icons, photos, video, or
tion classroom. The study, using a multiple-baseline other computer-based visual supports. Further,
design across participants, demonstrated that the the effective play skill interventions include common
children improved and maintained independent features such as motivational strategies (i.e., incor-
choice making and appropriate play with materi- porating the child’s interests into the play activity,
als. In addition, the children engaged in appropriate contingent reinforcement), selection of appropriate
interactive play with peers and independently played targeted play skills (developmentally appropriate),
with the materials. The findings also suggest that task analysis of play skills, teaching/modelling of
the way in which Social Stories are implemented, as each sequence of the skills, and providing systematic
well as their content, are critical components for the prompts.
effectiveness of the intervention (Lorimer, Simpson, The review of play skills intervention studies
Myles, & Ganz, 2002). That is, Social Stories need yields some implications for practice and future
to be reviewed with a child on a regular basis prior research. Some studies taught and assessed play
to the activity in which the child exhibits challeng- skills with peers in natural settings (e.g., Goldstein
ing behaviour. Research suggests corrective feedback & Cisar, 1992; Machalicek et al., 2009; Morrison
and prompts should be provided during and after et al., 2002). Children with autism have often been
the activity while providing sufficient opportunities taught play skills and assessed one on one with
to practise the skills (Barry & Burlew, 2004; Lorimer adults in clinical settings. Young children may play
et al., 2002). No generalisation, procedural integrity, alone or with adults; however, most children inter-
or social validity data were provided for the study. act with their peers in classrooms. Children with
autism do not spontaneously participate and take
a proactive role in play (Lifter, Mason, & Barton,
Discussion
2011). They may also have more difficulty interact-
Studies on teaching play skills indicate that children ing with peers versus adults since their peers are
with autism responded well to direct play inter- less likely to adjust to their atypical play behaviour
vention in the structured environment. Improve- (Oke & Schreibman, 1990).
ments in play skills also increased positive social Play with peers is closely related to context (Strain &
interactions and decreased inappropriate behav- Schwartz, 2001). Young children with autism may
iour as collateral effects. The majority of studies have more opportunities to engage in a variety of
on teaching play skills used combined approaches play contexts with their peers when play skills are
such as VM with reinforcement contingencies, taught directly and systematically during daily class-
verbal instruction, or matrix training (Dauphin room activities in natural environments. Providing
86 S. Jung & D. M. Sainato

intervention in the context of ongoing activities and materials and peers in appropriate ways. Children
routines facilitates generalisation of the learned skills with autism can learn more complex and varied
(Rogers, 2000). Teaching play skills to children with play within a structured context (Blanc et al.,
autism in natural environments with peers rather 2005). Games have rules, provide structure and are
than in one-on-one sessions with adults may enhance predictable, thus facilitating learning for children
children’s interactions, as well as promote mainte- with autism. With appropriate modifications, chil-
nance and generalisation of play skills (Liber et al., dren with autism may be able to learn games more
2008). Practitioners should consider the importance effectively and maintain and generalise game play-
of play skills as one of the critical developmental ing better than other play skills. Play or games that
skill areas rather than view play as merely a context both children with and without autism enjoy can
to teach other skills. Intervention should focus on be modified so that children with autism who have
incorporating play skills instruction into classroom limited language skills and their typically develop-
activities and individualised education programs ing peers engage in play together. Baker and col-
(Malone & Langone, 1999; Mastrangelo, 2009). leagues (1998) state that children can maintain
More studies need to be conducted in classrooms social play without adult prompts when both chil-
Downloaded by [Thomas Jefferson University] at 19:53 07 December 2017

focusing on embedding instruction of play skills into dren with and without autism have reciprocal rela-
daily routines and the impact of such instruction on tionships and can participate in play as comparable
children’s engagement in activities with peers. partners rather than in a helper–helpee role. Play
Acquisition and generalisation of play skills skills instruction may motivate children with autism
may be affected not only by how the play skills by using the behavioural characteristics of these
are taught, but also by which play skills are taught. children and incorporating their idiosyncratic and
The careful selection of play activities may further restricted interests into social play activities (e.g.,
encourage more spontaneous play among children popular games/play among typically developing
with autism and their peers. Researchers have paid children). As a result, children with autism may be
more attention to teaching symbolic play skills since more likely to engage spontaneously with play and
they are one of the reported deficits in children with peers and become comparable play partners, facili-
autism and are highly related to social development tating the play of both children with and without
(Stanley & Konstantareas, 2007). Research dem- autism without depending on an adult’s prompts.
onstrates that expressive language skills may be a Studies on play skills instruction often specify
predictor of symbolic play and should be taken into types of play skills (e.g., functional, symbolic, socio-
consideration when teaching these skills (Stahmer, dramatic play) or provide general descriptions of
1995; Stanley & Konstantareas, 2007). However, the targeted play skills with information about the
many young children with autism exhibit deficits in play materials (e.g., tea party, cooking toys, dolls,
language skills, indicating that they may not have blocks, cars) without specifying the type of play.
the prerequisite skills for learning symbolic play. Among the studies, there seems to be inconsisten-
Developmental readiness is an important consid- cies in categorising the types of play skills (e.g., the
eration for identifying the target play skills to help same play behaviours are often categorised under
children with autism learn the skills quickly, engage different types of play skills such as pretend, func-
spontaneously, and generalise these skills to new tional, or symbolic play). Future studies should
play materials (Lifter et al., 2005). Essential ele- attempt to provide clear definitions and measure-
ments of play also involve pleasurable and enjoy- ments of target play skills to better understand the
able engagement (Jordan, 2003). Lifter et al. (2011) play characteristics of children with autism and
emphasised that the key components of play involve their responses to play skills instruction. There may
spontaneous, naturally occurring activities with need to be more consistent definition and classifica-
objects that engage attention and interest. There- tion of play behaviours (see Barton & Wolery, 2008;
fore, play activities need to be chosen that focus on Lifter et al., 2011).
the strengths of children with autism, incorporate Future studies should investigate the differen-
their interests, and provide opportunities for expe- tial effects of play skills instruction by focusing on
riencing play in the same manner as their typically specific play skills, as well as outcomes for indi-
developing peers in a natural environment. vidual children to enhance the effectiveness and
Although just a few experimental studies investi- efficiency of interventions (Kasari et al., 2006).
gating games have been reported in the literature, That is, researchers should examine what play skills
teaching games to children with autism may be should be effectively and efficiently taught by which
an excellent tool to improve the play repertoire of instructional strategies. In addition, research should
these children and facilitate their engagement with identify what prerequisite skills are required to teach
Teaching play skills 87

certain play skills and implement specific instruc- studies should strive to measure the functional use of
tional strategies. Further, how the intensity of the play with peers and its maintenance. Future studies
play skills instruction can affect the pace of acquisi- should attend to the development of effective pro-
tion and maintenance and generalisation should be grams to facilitate maintenance and generalisation of
examined. For instance, a certain level of expressive play skills (Barton & Wolery, 2008).
language skill is a prerequisite for engaging in sym- To interpret the effectiveness of the intervention
bolic play (Stahmer, 1995; Stanley & Konstantar- and replicate the intervention, measurement of
eas, 2007). PRT may also be more effective with social validity and procedural integrity are critical.
children who have some imitation skills, who have The social validity assessment of the intervention
an interest in object play, and who do not show such as the feasibility, acceptability, and effective-
severe self-stimulatory behaviour (Stahmer, 1999). ness should be measured as important factors to
In addition, some instructional strategies (i.e., VM, design more effective and innovative instructional
live modelling) were found to be effective for some procedures that can be incorporated into typical
children with autism (D’Ateno et al., 2003; Mac- classroom activities (McConnell, 2002; Strain &
Donald et al., 2005; MacDonald et al., 2009) but Schwartz, 2001). However, few studies report
Downloaded by [Thomas Jefferson University] at 19:53 07 December 2017

had to be combined with additional strategies for the social validity measures of teaching play skills
other children with autism (Dauphin et al., 2004; (Baker, 2000; Colozzi et al., 2008; Hine & Wolery,
Hine & Wolery, 2006; Jahr et al., 2000; Paterson & 2006; Jahr et al., 2000; Morrison et al., 2002; San-
Arco, 2007). For example, in Sancho et al.’s study cho et al., 2010; Wolfberg & Schuler, 1993; Zercher
(2010), two participants responded differentially to et al., 2001). In addition, few studies have reported
two procedures, VM only and video modelling with the measurement of procedural integrity (Colozzi
prompts and instructions. Lydon and colleagues et al., 2008; Goldstein & Cisar, 1992; Hine &
(2011) also indicate that both PRT and VM were Wolery, 2006; Kasari et al., 2006; Lifter et al., 2005;
similarly effective in increasing play actions in the Machalicek et al., 2009; Sancho et al., 2010; Wong
training environment, but PRT resulted in greater et al., 2007; Zercher et al., 2001). Future studies
increases in behaviour. However, there were no sig- should assess the social validity and procedural
nificant increases in play verbalisation in either inter- integrity of play skills interventions.
vention. The play actions were generalised only with Research demonstrates that improvements in play
PRT. According to the results of these studies, it is can facilitate social interaction, language skills, and
necessary to examine how the duration and intensity decrease the self-stimulatory behaviour of children
of the intervention affect acquisition, maintenance, with autism (Baker, 2000; Stahmer, 1995; Thorp
and generalisation of the specific play skills. et al., 1995). Longitudinal studies investigated the
Promoting the generalisation of learned play skills relationship between not only preschool pretend
with materials and peers is critical to enhance the play behaviour and the early adolescent creativity of
spontaneous play of children with autism. Few stud- typically developing children (Mullineaux & Dilalla,
ies have demonstrated the generalised effects of play 2009) but also the play and adaptive behaviour of
skills across different settings, with different materi- children with developmental disability (Sigafoos,
als, or different people following intervention (Baker, Roberts-Pennell, & Graves, 1999). Although it is
2000; Baker et al., 1998; Dauphin et al., 2004; suggested that play is critical for children’s develop-
Goldstein & Cisar, 1992; Jahr et al., 2000; Kasari ment, there are few studies on the long-term effects
et al., 2006; Liber et al., 2008; Morrison et al., 2002; of play on the development of children with autism.
Stahmer, 1995; Thorp et al., 1995; Wong et al., Future studies are needed to investigate the long-term
2007). Some studies have documented only modest effects of developmentally appropriate play skills on
or variable generalisation effects (Colozzi et al., the development of academic, social, communication,
2008; Hine & Wolery, 2006; Liber et al., 2008; or functional skills in children with autism.
Lydon et al., 2011; MacDonald et al., 2009;
Paterson & Arco, 2007; Sancho et al., 2010, Stahmer, Declaration of interest: This manuscript was not
1995; Thorp et al., 1995). In addition, several studies funded by any organisation and there is no conflict
report the maintenance of play skills after withdraw- of interest.
ing intervention (Baker, 2000; Baker et al., 1998;
Colozzi et al., 2008; Hine & Wolery, 2006; Goldstein
& Cisar, 1992; Jahr et al., 2000; Lydon et al., 2011; References
MacDonald et al., 2005; MacDonald et al., 2009; American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical
Morrison et al., 2002; Paterson & Arco, 2007; manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC:
Sancho et al., 2010; Zercher et al., 2001). More Author.
88 S. Jung & D. M. Sainato

Baker, M.J. (2000). Incorporating the thematic ritualistic Jordan, R. (2003). Social play and autistic spectrum disorders:
behaviors of children with autism into games: Increasing social A perspective on theory, implications and educational approa-
play interactions with siblings. Journal of Positive Behavior ches. Autism, 7, 347–360. doi:10.1177/1362361303007004002
Interventions, 2, 66–84. doi:10.1177/109830070000200201 Kasari, C., Freeman, S., & Paparella, T. (2006). Joint attention
Baker, M.J., Koegel, R.L., & Koegel, L.K. (1998). Increasing the and symbolic play in young children with autism: A
social behavior of young children with autism using their randomized controlled intervention study. Journal of Child
obsessive behaviors. Journal of the Association for Persons with Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 611–620. doi:10.1111/j.
Severe Handicaps, 23, 300–308. doi:10.2511/rpsd.23.4.300 1469-7610.2005.01567.x
Barry, L.M., & Burlew, S.B. (2004). Using Social Stories to teach Koegel, R.L., & Koegel, L.K. (2006). Pivotal response treatments
choice and play skills to children with autism. Focus on Autism for autism: Communication, social, & academic development.
and Other Developmental Disabilities, 19, 45–51. doi:10.1177/1 Baltimore, MD: Brookes.
0883576040190010601 Koegel, L.K., Koegel, R.L., Hurley, C., & Frea, W.D. (1992).
Barton, E.E., & Wolery, M. (2008). Teaching pretend play Improving social skills and disruptive behavior in children with
to children with disabilities: A review of the literature. autism through self-management. Journal of Applied Behavior
Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 28, 109–125. Analysis, 25, 341–353. doi:10.1901/jaba.1992.25-341
doi:10.1177/0271121408318799 Lang, R., Machalicek, W., O’Reilly, M.F., Sigafoos, J.,
Blanc, R., Adrien, J.-L., Roux, S., & Barthélémy, C. (2005). Rispoli, M.J., Shogren, K., & Regester, A. (2009). Review
Dysregulation of pretend play and communication develop- of interventions to increase functional and symbolic play in
Downloaded by [Thomas Jefferson University] at 19:53 07 December 2017

ment in children with autism. Autism, 9, 229–245. doi:10.1177/ children with autism. Education and Training in Developmental
1362361305053253 Disabilities, 44, 481–492.
Boucher, J., & Wolfberg, P. (2003). Aims and design of the Ledford, J.R., Gast, D.L., Luscre, D., & Ayres, K.M. (2008).
special issue [Editorial]. Autism, 7, 339–346. doi:10.1177/ Observational and incidental learning by children with autism
1362361303007004001 during small group instruction. Journal of Autism and
Boutot, E.A., Guenther, T., & Crozier, S. (2005). Let’s play: Developmental Disorders, 38, 86–103. doi:10.1007/s10803-007-
Teaching play skills to young children with autism. Education 0363-7
and Training in Developmental Disabilities, 40, 285–292. Leslie, A.M. (1987). Pretense and representation: The origins of
“theory of mind.” Psychological Review, 94, 412–426.
Coe, D., Matson, J., Fee, V., Manikam, R., & Linarello, C. (1990).
doi:10.1037/0033-295X.94.4.412
Training nonverbal and verbal play skills to mentally retarded
Libby, S., Powell, S., Messer, D., & Jordan, R. (1997). Imitation
and autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental
of pretend play acts by children with autism and Down
Disorders, 20, 177–187. doi:10.1007/BF02284717
syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27,
Colozzi, G.A., Ward, L.W., & Crotty, K.E. (2008). Comparison
365–383. doi:10.1023/A:1025801304279
of simultaneous prompting procedure in 1:1 and small group
Libby, S., Powell, S., Messer, D., & Jordan, R. (1998). Spontane-
instruction to teach play skills to preschool students with
ous play in children with autism: A reappraisal. Journal of
pervasive developmental disorder and developmental disa-
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28, 487–497. doi:10.1023/
bilities. Education and Training in Developmental Disabilities, 43,
A:1026095910558
226–248.
Liber, D.B., Frea, W.D., & Symon, J.B.G. (2008). Using time-
D’Ateno, P., Mangiapanello, K., & Taylor, B.A. (2003). Using
delay to improve social play skills with peers for children with
video modeling to teach complex play sequences to a pre-
autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38,
schooler with autism. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions,
312–323. doi:10.1007/s10803-007-0395-z
5, 5–11. doi:10.1177/10983007030050010801 Lifter, K., Mason, E.J., & Barton, E.E. (2011). Children’s play:
Dauphin, M., Kinney, E.M., & Stromer, R. (2004). Using video- Where we have been and where we could go. Journal of Early
enhanced activity schedules and matrix training to teach Intervention, 33, 281–297. doi:10.1177/1053815111429465
sociodramatic play to a child with autism. Journal of Positive Lifter, K., Ellis, J., Cannon, B., & Anderson, S.R. (2005).
Behavior Interventions, 6, 238–250. doi:10.1177/10983007040 Developmental specificity in targeting and teaching play
060040501 activities to children with pervasive developmental disorders.
Goldstein, H., & Cisar, C.L. (1992). Prompting interaction dur- Journal of Early Intervention, 27, 247–267. doi:10.1177/
ing sociodramatic play: Teaching scripts to typical preschoolers 105381510502700405
and classmates with disabilities. Journal of Applied Behavior Lorimer, P.A., Simpson, R.L., Myles, B.S., & Ganz, J.B. (2002).
Analysis, 25, 265–280. doi:10.1901/jaba.1992.25-265 The use of Social Stories as a preventative behavioral inter-
Gray, C.A., & Garand, J.D. (1993). Social Stories: Improving vention in a home setting with children with autism. Journal
responses of students with autism with accurate social of Positive Behavior Interventions, 4, 53–60. doi:10.1177/
information. Focus on Autistic Behavior, 8, 1–10. doi:10.1177/ 109830070200400109
108835769300800101 Lydon, H., Healy, O., & Leader, G. (2011). A comparison of video
Hine, J.F., & Wolery, M. (2006). Using point-of-view video mod- modeling and pivotal response training to teach pretend play
eling to teach play to preschoolers with autism. Topics in Early skills to children with autism spectrum disorder. Research in
Childhood Special Education, 26, 83–93. doi:10.1177/0271121 Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5, 872–884. doi:10.1016/j.
4060260020301 rasd.2010.10.002
Holmes, E., & Willoughby, T. (2005). Play behaviour of MacDonald, R., Clark, M., Garrigan, E., & Vangala, M. (2005).
children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Intellec- Using video modeling to teach pretend play to children with
tual & Developmental Disability, 30, 156–164. doi:10.1080/ autism. Behavioral Interventions, 20, 225–238. doi:10.1002/
13668250500204034 bin.197
Jahr, E., Eldevik, S., & Eikeseth, S. (2000). Teaching children MacDonald, R., Sacramone, S., Mansfield, R., Wiltz, K., &
with autism to initiate and sustain cooperative play. Research Ahearn, W.H. (2009). Using video modeling to teach recipro-
in Developmental Disabilities, 21, 151–169. doi:10.1016/S0891- cal pretend play to children with autism. Journal of Applied
4222(00)00031-7 Behavior Analysis, 42, 43–55. doi:10.1901/jaba.2009.42-43
Teaching play skills 89

Machalicek, W., Shogren, K., Lang , R., Rispoli, M., Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 479–497. doi:10.1007/
O’Reilly, M.F., Franco, J.H., & Sigafoos, J. (2009). Increasing BF02216054
play and decreasing the challenging behavior of children with Palechka, G., & MacDonald, R. (2010). A comparison of the
autism during recess with activity schedules and task corre- acquisition of play skills using instructor-created video models
spondence training. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 3, and commercially available videos. Education and Treatment of
547–555. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2008.11.003 Children, 33, 457–474. doi:10.1353/etc.0.0100
Malone, D.M., & Langone, J. (1999). Teaching object-related play Paterson, C.R., & Arco, L. (2007). Using video modeling for
skills to preschool children with developmental concerns. generalizing toy play in children with autism. Behavior
International Journal of Disability, Development and Education, Modification, 31, 660–681. doi:10.1177/0145445507301651
46, 325–336. doi:10.1080/103491299100533 Pierce-Jordan, S., & Lifter, K. (2005). Interaction of social and
Mastrangelo, S. (2009). Harnessing the power of play: Opportu- play behaviors in preschoolers with and without pervasive
nities for children with autism spectrum disorders. Teaching developmental disorder. Topics in Early Childhood Special
Exceptional Children, 42, 34–44. Education, 25, 34–47. doi:10.1177/02711214050250010401
McConnell, S.R. (2002). Interventions to facilitate social Rogers, S.J. (2000). Interventions that facilitate socialization
interaction for young children with autism: Review of in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
available research and recommendations for educational Disorders, 30, 399–409. doi:10.1023/A:1005543321840
intervention and future research. Journal of Autism Rutherford, M.D., Young, G.S., Hepburn, S., & Rogers, S.J.
and Developmental Disorders, 32, 351–372. doi:10.1023/ (2007). A longitudinal study of pretend play in autism. Journal
Downloaded by [Thomas Jefferson University] at 19:53 07 December 2017

A:1020537805154 of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 1024–1039.


McCoy, K., & Hermansen, E. (2007). Video modeling for doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0240-9
individuals with autism: A review of model types and effects. Sancho, K. Sidener, T.M., Reeve, S.A., & Sidener, D.W. (2010).
Education and Treatment of Children, 30, 183–213. doi:10.1353/ Two variations of video modeling interventions for teaching
etc.2007.0029 play skills to children with autism. Education and Treatment of
McDuffie, A., Turner, L., Stone, W., Yoder, P., Wolery, M., & Children, 33, 421–442. doi:10.1353/etc.0.0097
Ulman, T. (2007). Developmental correlates of different types Sigafoos, J., Roberts-Pennell, D., & Graves, D. (1999). Longitu-
of motor imitation in young children with autism spectrum dinal assessment of play and adaptive behavior in young
disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, children with developmental disabilities. Research in Develop-
401–412. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0175-1 mental Disabilities, 20, 147–161. doi:10.1016/S0891-4222(98)
Morrison, R.S., Sainato, D.M., Benchaaban, D., & Endo, S. 00038-9
(2002). Increasing play skills of children with autism Stahmer, A.C. (1995). Teaching symbolic play skills to children
using activity schedules and correspondence training. Journal with autism using pivotal response training. Journal of Autism
of Early Intervention, 25, 58–72. doi:10.1177/10538151020 and Developmental Disorders, 25, 123–141. doi:10.1007/
2500106 BF02178500
Mullineaux, P.Y., & Dilalla, L.F. (2009). Preschool pretend Stahmer, A.C. (1999). Using pivotal response training to facilitate
play behaviors and early adolescent creativity. Journal of appropriate play in children with autistic spectrum disorders.
Creative Behavior, 43, 41–57. doi:10.1002/j.2162-6057.2009. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 15, 29–40.
tb01305.x Stahmer, A.C., Ingersoll, B., & Carter, C. (2003). Behavioral
Nuzzolo-Gomez, R., Leonard, M.A., Ortiz, E., Rivera, C.M., & approaches to promoting play. Autism, 7, 401–413. doi:10.
Greer, R.D. (2002). Teaching children with autism to 1177/1362361303007004006
prefer books or toys over stereotypy or passivity. Journal Stahmer, A.C., & Schreibman, L. (1992). Teaching children
of Positive Behavior Interventions, 4, 80–87. doi:10.1177/ with autism appropriate play in unsupervised environments
109830070200400203 using a self-management treatment package. Journal of
Odom, S.L. , Brown, W.H., Frey, T., Karasu, N. , Applied Behavior Analysis, 25, 447–459. doi:10.1901/jaba.
Smith-Canter, L.L. , & Strain , P.S . (2003). Evidence-based 1992.25-447
practices for young children with autism: Contributions Stanley, G.C., & Konstantareas, M.M. (2007). Symbolic play
for single-subject design research. Focus on Autism and in children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism
Other Developmental Disabilities, 18 , 166– 175. doi:10.1177/ and Developmental Disorders, 37, 1215–1223. doi:10.1007/
10883576030180030401 s10803-006-0263-2
Odom, S.L., McConnell, S.R., McEvoy, M.A., Peterson, C., Strain, P.S., & Schwartz, I. (2001). ABA and the development of
Ostrosky, M., Chandler, L.K., … Favazza, P.C. (1999). meaningful social relations for young children with autism.
Relative effects of interventions supporting the social com- Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 16,
petence of young children with disabilities. Topics in Early 120–128. doi:10.1177/108835760101600208
Childhood Special Education, 19, 75–91. doi:10.1177/ Thomas, N., & Smith, C. (2004). Developing play skills in
027112149901900202 children with autistic spectrum disorders. Educational Psy-
Odom, S.L., & Strain, P.S. (1986). A comparison of peer- chology in Practice, 20, 195–206. doi:10.1080/026673604
initiation and teacher-antecedent interventions for promoting 2000251781
reciprocal social interaction of autistic preschoolers. Journal Thorp, D.M., Stahmer, A.C., & Schreibman, L. (1995). Effects
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 19, 59–71. doi:10.1901/jaba. of sociodramatic play training on children with autism. Journal
1986.19-59 of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 25, 265–282. doi:10.1007/
Odom, S.L., & Watts, E. (1991). Reducing teacher prompts BF02179288
in peer-mediated interventions for young children with van Berckelaer-Onnes, I.A. (2003). Promoting early play. Autism,
autism. Journal of Special Education, 25, 26–43. doi:10.1177/ 7, 415–423. doi:10.1177/1362361303007004007
002246699102500103 Williams, E., Reddy, V., & Costall, A. (2001). Taking a closer
Oke, N.J., & Schreibman, L. (1990). Training social initiations to look at functional play in children with autism. Journal of
a high-functioning autistic child: Assessment of collateral Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 67–77. doi:10.1023/
behavior change and generalization in a case study. Journal of A:1005665714197
90 S. Jung & D. M. Sainato

Wolfberg, P.J., & Schuler, A.L. (1993). Integrated play groups: play skills in young children with autism. Research & Practice
A model for promoting the social and cognitive dimensions for Persons with Severe Disabilities, 32, 101–109
of play in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Zercher, C., Hunt, P., Schuler, A., & Webster, J. (2001).
Developmental Disorders, 23, 407–489. Increasing joint attention, play and language through peer
Wong, C.S., Kasari, C., Freeman, S., & Paparella, T. (2007). The supported play. Autism, 5, 374–398. doi:10.1177/136236130
acquisition and generalization of joint attention and symbolic 1005004004
Downloaded by [Thomas Jefferson University] at 19:53 07 December 2017

You might also like