Emotion Suppression, Emotional Eating, and Eating Behavior Among Parent-Adolescent Dyads

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 15

Emotion

Emotion Suppression, Emotional Eating, and Eating


Behavior Among Parent–Adolescent Dyads
Rebecca A. Ferrer, Paige A. Green, April Y. Oh, Erin Hennessy, and Laura A. Dwyer
Online First Publication, April 3, 2017. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/emo0000295

CITATION
Ferrer, R. A., Green, P. A., Oh, A. Y., Hennessy, E., & Dwyer, L. A. (2017, April 3). Emotion
Suppression, Emotional Eating, and Eating Behavior Among Parent–Adolescent Dyads. Emotion.
Advance online publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/emo0000295
Emotion In the public domain
2017, Vol. 0, No. 999, 000 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/emo0000295

Emotion Suppression, Emotional Eating, and Eating Behavior Among


Parent–Adolescent Dyads

Rebecca A. Ferrer, Paige A. Green, and April Y. Oh Erin Hennessy


National Institutes of Health Tufts University

Laura A. Dwyer
Cape Fox Facilities Services, Manassas, Virginia

Emotion suppression may lead to ironic increases in emotional experience. More important, suppression
is a transactional process, creating stress and disrupting interactions for the suppressor and those in social
interactions with individuals who are suppressing emotion. However, no research has examined the
behavioral consequences of emotion suppression in close relationships. We examine the possibility that
emotion suppression will predict eating behaviors as a secondary emotion regulatory strategy among
1,556 parent–adolescent dyads (N ⫽ 3,112), consistent with evidence suggesting that suppression
influences eating at the individual-level. Actor-partner interdependence models and structural equation
modeling demonstrate that one’s own emotion suppression was associated with emotional eating; greater
consumption of hedonic—low nutrient, high energy dense—foods; and lower consumption of fruits and
vegetables (actor effects). One’s partner’s emotion suppression was also independently associated with
one’s own emotional eating; lower consumption of fruits and vegetables; and greater consumption of
hedonic foods (partner effects), although this association was most consistent for adolescents’ suppres-
sion and parents’ eating (compared with the converse). These analyses suggest that dyadic emotion
regulatory processes have implications on eating behavior. Moreover, analyses suggest that emotion
suppression has potential implications on eating behaviors of others within close relationships with a
suppressor, consistent with the notion that emotion regulation is a transactional process. These findings
suggest that interventions to improve eating habits of parents and their adolescent children should
consider dyadic emotion regulatory processes.

Keywords: emotion regulation, emotion suppression, eating behavior

Social circumstances where it is necessary to engage in emotion children are taught not to convey disappointment when they re-
suppression— or purposely contain one’s emotional reaction and ceive a gift they do not like (e.g., Carlson & Wang, 2007). Indeed,
its accompanying nonverbal facial and bodily displays—are ubiq- even among children, inhibitory control is seen as a sign of social
uitous (Gross & John, 2003). Inappropriate social displays of and emotional competence (Rhoades, Greenberg, & Domitrovich,
emotion can have serious consequences, such as when an em- 2009). Accordingly, emotion suppression is relatively common in
ployee fails to suppress anger expressions in front of his supervisor social exchanges, and can have positive consequences for relation-
and colleagues (e.g., Tavris, 1984). Even relatively mild inappro- ships (see Butler & Gross, 2004). For example, suppression can
priate displays of emotion can seem consequential. For example, prevent escalation of negative emotions in relationships, contrib-

necessarily reflect the views or policies of the Department of Health and


Rebecca A. Ferrer and Paige A. Green, Basic Biobehavioral and Psy- Human Services, nor does mention of trade names, commercial prod-
chological Sciences Branch, Behavioral Research Program, Division of ucts, or organizations imply endorsement by the US Government.
Cancer Control and Population Sciences, National Cancer Institute, Na- Additional funding was provided through a Cancer Research Training
tional Institutes of Health; April Y. Oh, Health Communication and Infor- Award from the National Cancer Institute (LAD). At the time this study
matics Research Branch, Behavioral Research Program, Division of Can- was conducted, Hennessy was a Senior Behavioral Scientist (Contrac-
cer Control and Population Sciences, National Cancer Institute, National tor), Clinical Research Directorate/Clinical Monitoring Research Pro-
Institutes of Health; Erin Hennessy, Friedman School of Nutrition Science gram, Leidos Biomedical Research, Inc., NCI Campus at Frederick,
and Policy, Tufts University; Laura A. Dwyer, Cape Fox Facilities Ser- Frederick, MD 21702.
vices, Manassas, Virginia. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Rebecca
The FLASHE study was funded by the National Cancer Institute A. Ferrer, Health Scientist/Program Director, Basic Biobehavioral and
under contract HHSN261201200039I issued to Westat. This project has Psychological Sciences Branch, Behavioral Research Program, Division of
been funded in whole or in part with federal funds from the National Cancer Control and Population Sciences, National Cancer Institute, Na-
Cancer Institute, National Institutes of Health, under contract tional Institutes of Health, 9609 Medical Center Drive, Rockville, MD
HHSN261200800001E (EH). The content of this publication does not 20850. E-mail: ferrerra@mail.nih.gov

1
2 FERRER, GREEN, OH, HENNESSY, AND DWYER

uting to relationship maintenance (Kennedy-Moore & Watson, stress for both the suppressor and their interaction partner (Butler
2001). et al., 2003; although see Richards et al., 2003 for evidence that
However, emotion suppression has also been shown to have suppression does not have dyadic consequences for subjectively
negative consequences (Gross, 1998, 1999). Like other attempts to experienced negative affect among romantic couples). Negative
consciously suppress psychological processes and thoughts (e.g., social consequences of suppression are particularly common in
Wegner, Schneider, Carter, & White, 1987), experimental evi- western cultures (Butler, Lee, & Gross, 2007).
dence demonstrates emotion suppression often results in an ironic
increase in the subjective experience of emotion (and accompa-
Behavioral Consequences of Suppression
nying physiological processes; Gross, 1998; Gross & Levenson,
1997; Harris, 2001; Richards & Gross, 1999, 2000). For example, Research suggests that emotion suppression may have conse-
individuals instructed to suppress disgust when watching a video quences that extend beyond those in the social and affective
depicting a graphic surgical procedure report experiencing greater categories, resulting in behaviors that may not be in the actor’s best
subjective feelings of disgust than those who watch the video interest—including unhealthy eating behaviors. Indeed, experi-
without instruction (Gross & Levenson, 1993). Of note, some mental evidence shows that emotion suppression inductions lead to
individuals tend to engage in emotion suppression strategies more unhealthy eating behaviors (Hofmann, Rauch, & Gawronski, 2007;
than others, and the consequences of habitual suppression may Taut, Renner, & Baban, 2012; Vohs & Heatherton, 2000). For
extend beyond the laboratory (Gross & John, 2003; John & Gross, example, female dieters instructed to suppress in response to a sad
2004). Individuals who tend to suppress negative emotions expe- film ate more ice cream than those not instructed to suppress (Vohs
rience negative consequences, consistent with those observed in & Heatherton, 2000), and women instructed to suppress in re-
experiments, including ironic increases in subjective negative af- sponse to an emotional film ate more candy than those instructed
fect, depression, and anxiety, as well as decreased well-being to watch naturally (Hofmann et al., 2007). Moreover, and also
(Haga, Kraft, & Corby, 2009; Moore, Zoellner, & Mollenholt, consistent with experimental evidence, habitual emotion suppres-
2008). sion is associated with increased consumption of hedonic foods
after negative emotion induction (Evers, Marijn Stok, & de Ridder,
2010). Similarly, habitual suppression has been linked to overeat-
Social Consequences of Suppression
ing reported in an experience sampling study, such that overweight
Because emotion suppression unfolds in social contexts, the women are more likely to report overeating on days in which they
negative consequences of emotion suppression can be social in also tended to suppress emotion (Butler, Young, & Randall, 2010).
addition to affective (Butler & Gross, 2004, 2009). Research No known research to date has examined whether emotion sup-
suggests that both spontaneous daily suppression and experimen- pression leads to unhealthy eating behavior at a dyadic level,
tally induced suppression are associated with lower well-being and although previously described evidence on the social conse-
perceived quality of the parent– child bond among parents of quences of emotion suppression suggests the possibility that one’s
young children (Le & Impett, 2016). Moreover, experimentally suppression could have behavioral consequences for a dyadic
induced emotion suppression leads to greater memory for emo- partner.
tional conversation content (during interactions in the laboratory) The complex psychological processes through which emotion
and lower levels of rapport and affiliation among individuals regulation (including suppression) influences unhealthy eating re-
instructed to suppress before interacting with a romantic partner main undiscovered (Leith & Baumeister, 1996). One notable po-
(Richards, Butler, & Gross, 2003). Accordingly, habitual emotion tential mechanism involves ironic experiences of negative affect.
suppression has been linked to difficulties in forming close rela- As stated, individuals who tend to engage in emotion suppression
tionships (Srivastava, Tamir, McGonigal, John, & Gross, 2009). experience increased negative affect, and thus, may experience
Tendency to suppress emotion has also been linked to decreased behavioral consequences associated with this negative affect (in-
social support and weaker social connections (English, John, Sriv- cluding unhealthy eating; Gross & John, 2003; Haga, Kraft, &
astava, & Gross, 2012; Labott, Martin, Eason, & Berkey, 1991; Corby, 2009; John & Gross, 2004; Moore, Zoellner, & Mollenholt,
Srivastava et al., 2009). Emotion suppression interferes with inti- 2008). Negative affect has been linked (in both experimental and
macy, precludes self-disclosure, and can be perceived as lacking correlational studies) to overeating or excessive consumption of
integrity (Butler & Gross, 2004). Moreover, suppression has del- hedonic foods (Arnow, Kenardy, & Agras, 1995; Cools, Schotte,
eterious consequences for self-reported relationship satisfaction & McNally, 1992; Ganley, 1989; Gilboa-Schechtman, Avnon,
(English & John, 2013). In summary, engagement in this regula- Zubery, & Jeczmien, 2006; Greeno & Wing, 1994; Hofmann,
tory strategy has important implications for a number of outcomes Rauch, & Gawronski, 2007; Macht, 2008; Oliver, Wardle, &
related to relationships (Butler & Gross, 2004; Gross, 2002; Gross Gibson, 2000; Tomiyama, Dallman, & Epel, 2011; Taut, Renner,
& John, 2003; John & Gross, 2004). & Baban, 2012; Vohs & Heatherton, 2000). This association is
Emerging evidence also suggests the social consequences of consistent with the concept of emotional eating, or eating in the
suppression extend beyond the individual who is suppressing. absence of hunger when anxious or sad (Arnow, Kenardy, &
Individuals that engage with suppressors in an experimental setting Agras, 1995; Faith, Allison, Geliebter, & Dalton, 1997; Haedt-
report less desire to affiliate among previously unacquainted dyads Matt et al., 2014; VanStrien, Frijters, Bergers, & Defares, 1986).
(Tackman & Srivastava, 2016). Moreover, experimentally induced As such, researchers have speculated that ineffective emotion
emotion suppression has dyadic consequences, such that it can regulation strategies (such as suppression) may lead to maladap-
disrupt social exchanges among previously unacquainted dyads, tive behaviors engaged as secondary regulatory strategies to de-
influencing interpersonal communication and creating additional crease negative affect (including original negative affect that was
EMOTION SUPPRESSION IN PARENT–ADOLESCENT DYADS 3

suppressed as well as ironic increases negative affect resulting Gross, 2004; Le & Impett, 2016), and because negative affect is
from suppression; Agras & Telch, 1998; Arnow et al., 1995; associated with greater consumption of hedonic— but not nonhe-
Gagnon-Girouard et al., 2009; Ganley, 1989; Macht, 2008; Macht, donic—foods (Arnow et al., 1995; Cools et al., 1992; Garg et al.,
Haupt, & Ellgring, 2005; Macht & Simons, 2000; Wiser & Telch, 2007; Gilboa-Schechtman et al., 2006; Greeno & Wing, 1994;
1999). Moreover, because an individual interacting with someone Macht, 2008), we predict that emotion suppression contributes to
who is suppressing emotions experiences increased negative affect unhealthy eating behaviors among both suppressors and their
(Butler et al., 2003; Le & Impett, 2016), suppression could also interaction partners through its influence on their emotional eating.
increase unhealthy eating among both people in a dyad where one In addition to the proposed association of emotional eating with
or both is suppressing. reduced fruit and vegetable consumption articulated in this frame-
These speculations have been borne out in some empirical work, we note that fruit and vegetable consumption may be also be
evidence: maladaptive emotion regulation strategies have been negatively associated with hedonic food consumption because of
linked to emotional eating, particularly among adolescents and tendencies for individuals to “trade off” on their food choices (i.e.,
young adults (Crockett, Myhre, & Rokke, 2015). Indeed, emo- eating fewer fruits and vegetables when they are eating more
tional eating may effectively regulate negative emotional experi- hedonic foods; e.g., Larson et al., 2008; Neumark-Sztainer et al.,
ence by shifting the focus away from the self (Heatherton & 2003; Seghers & Rutten, 2010).
Baumeister, 1991), because hedonic foods are pleasing to the Despite the theoretical support for pathways in Figure 1, de-
senses (Lehman & Rodin, 1989) or otherwise pleasing because scribed above, no research has examined whether emotion sup-
they are taboo or forbidden (Fairburn & Cooper, 1982), through pression contributes to unhealthy eating behaviors within a dyadic
activation of neural circuitry associated with reward (Adam & context in which the relationship is preexisting (as opposed to
Epel, 2007), and/or because of positive physiological changes in among previously acquainted dyads), either directly or via emo-
response to consumption of hedonic foods (Heatherton & tional eating. Moreover, no research has examined the dyadic
Baumeister, 1991; Macht, 2008; Macht & Mueller, 2007; Spoor et consequences of habitual (as opposed to experimentally induced)
al., 2007). emotion suppression. Addressing this research gap is important,
An effect of emotion suppression on one’s own or one’s part- given that such a link may have consequences for the design of
ner’s unhealthy eating behaviors has important consequences. interventions to promote healthy eating. Current interventions tar-
Emotional eating, particularly among adolescents, has been linked geting emotional eating largely focus on the individual-level an-
to eating in the absence of hunger (Nguyen-Michel, Unger, & tecedents and consequences of emotional eating and related affec-
Spruijt-Metz, 2007), which results in consumption of more energy tive constructs (e.g., Armitage, 2015; Katterman et al., 2014), but
dense foods in response to stress (Oliver, Wardle, & Gibson, do not take into account social influences. Demonstrating that
2000). Indeed, stress-induced eating has been linked to weight gain emotion suppression has consequences for unhealthy eating be-
(Laitinen, Ek, & Sovio, 2002). Given mounting evidence that haviors at a dyadic level could change the way that interventions
unhealthy eating behaviors contribute to myriad health conse- targeting emotional eating are designed and developed, increasing
quences both independently and via obesity (Fisher et al., 2002; their efficacy.
Ford, Zhao, Tsai, & Li, 2011; Khaw et al., 2008; Klein et al., 2014;
Pearson et al., 2002; Stefanek et al., 2009), recent syntheses have
Moderation
underscored the importance of examining and targeting ineffective
emotion regulation strategies (including emotion suppression) to Although the framework presented in Figure 1 proposes main
facilitate healthier patterns of food choices (see DeSteno, Gross, & effects of suppression on unhealthy eating behaviors, it is possible
Kubzansky, 2013; Ferrer, Green, & Barrett, 2015). that these effects may be stronger among some individuals com-
pared to others. Specifically, given that women tend to engage in
emotional eating as a compensatory regulatory strategy more often
Theoretical Framework
than men (e.g., Tanofsky et al., 1997), and may also be more
Here, we extend findings linking emotion suppression to eating affected by suppression because they are better able to identify
behaviors by examining how individual differences in emotion nuances of emotional communication (Sonnby-Borgstrom, Jons-
suppression are associated with eating behaviors among parent– son, & Svensson, 2008), emotion suppression may be more con-
adolescent dyads, including emotional eating, consumption of he- sequential for unhealthy eating behaviors among women compared
donic foods (i.e., low-nutrient, high energy dense foods), and fruit to men. This is consistent with findings suggesting that suppres-
and vegetable consumption. More important, we examine not only sion may be linked to negative affective outcomes differently for
whether one’s own emotion suppression influences these outcomes men than for women (Flynn, Hollenstein, & Mackey, 2010).
(actor effects), but also whether one’s partner’s (i.e., parent’s Further underscoring the need for moderating tests, many studies
adolescent child, or adolescent’s parent) suppression influences examining suppression’s influence on unhealthy eating via sec-
one’s own eating behavior (partner effects). Given the evidence ondary regulatory processes have focused exclusively on women
outlined above, we propose that emotion suppression has conse- (Hofmann, Rauch, & Gawronski, 2007; Vohs & Heatherton,
quences for eating behaviors among both the suppressor and his or 2000).
her dyadic interaction partner (i.e., parent and/or adolescent child). Moreover, we examined whether partner effects for emotion
Because emotion regulation results in increases of subjectively suppression were stronger for parents than children. Previous
experienced negative affect among the suppressor (Gross, 1998; research has shown that negative communication styles are more
Gross & Levenson, 1997; Harris, 2001; Richards & Gross, 1999, likely to influence partner eating behaviors of parents, compared
2000) and individuals interacting with the suppressor (Butler & with children, in parent– child dyads (Baiocchi-Wagner & Talley,
4 FERRER, GREEN, OH, HENNESSY, AND DWYER

Parent Hedonic
Food
Parent Parent Parent Consumption
Emotion Negative Emotional
Suppression Affect Eating

Parent F&V
Consumption

Adolescent
Hedonic Food
Consumption

Adolescent Adolescent Adolescent


Emotion Negative Emotional
Suppression Affect Eating

Adolescent
Junk
F&VF&V
Consumption

Figure 1. Conceptual framework.

2013). This suggests that maladaptive communication patterns are Method


more consequential for parents involved in the communication
than for children, at least with respect to eating behavior. Given
that evidence above suggests that suppression may be a negative Participants and Procedure
communication style, we predicted that partner linkages between
Data for this study were collected as part of the National Cancer
emotion suppression and eating behavior may be stronger for
Institute’s Family Life, Activity, Sun, Health, and Eating
adolescents’ suppression and parents’ eating behavior than the
(FLASHE) study (http://cancercontrol.cancer.gov/brp/hbrb/flashe
converse.
.html), a Web based, cross-sectional study of parent–adolescent
dyads administered through Westat, Inc. and fielded between
Hypotheses
April–October 2014. Adult participants were recruited to the study
through the Ipsos Consumer Opinion Panel. The sample was
Our analyses were driven by the following hypotheses:
selected using sample balancing methods (Oh et al., 2017; Westat,
Hypothesis 1: The emotion suppression of an actor will lead to 2015), with sample distributions matching the U.S. population as
greater emotional eating and consumption of hedonic foods, closely as possible on the following key demographic character-
and to less fruit and vegetable consumption among the indi- istics: gender of the panel member, census division, household
vidual suppressing emotions (i.e., actor effects). income, household size, and race/ethnicity. A panel member was
eligible if he or she was at least 18 years of age and lived with at
Hypothesis 2: The emotion suppression of an actor will lead to
least one child between the ages of 12 and 17 for at least 50% of
greater emotional eating and consumption of hedonic foods
the time.
and to lower fruit and vegetable consumption among the
Participants in the present study were 1,556 parent–adolescent
dyadic partner of the individual suppressing emotion (i.e.,
dyads (N ⫽ 3,112) who completed all measures necessary for
partner effects).
analyses, described below. Mean age was 43.66 (SD ⫽ 7.91)
We also conducted ancillary analyses to examine hypotheses among parents and 14.45 (SD ⫽ 1.62) among adolescents. The
based on more preliminary evidence and not included in the sample was 63% female (75% among parents and 50% among
overarching conceptual framework. These were: adolescents). Full sociodemographic characteristics for the sample,
and stratified by parent/adolescent, are contained in Table 1.
Moderation Hypothesis 1: The link between emotion suppres-
Web based surveys were emailed to participants and completed
sion and outcomes—particularly emotional eating—may be
online. Full study methodology and item development are reported
stronger among women than men.
elsewhere (Nebeling et al., 2017; Oh et al., 2017). The FLASHE
Moderation Hypothesis 2: The link between one’s own sup- study was reviewed and approved by the US Government’s Office
pression and one’s partner’s outcomes (partner effects) may of Management and Budget (OMB), NCIs Special Studies Insti-
be stronger for the adolescent-to-parent partner associations tutional Review Board (SSIRB), and Westat’s Institutional Review
than for the parent-to-adolescent partner associations. Board (IRB).
EMOTION SUPPRESSION IN PARENT–ADOLESCENT DYADS 5

Table 1
Sociodemographic and Outcome Variables (n ⫽ 3,112)

Variable Full sample Parent Adolescent

Age 43.66 SD ⫽ 7.91 14.45 SD ⫽ 1.62


Body Mass Index 26.84 SD ⫽ 9.80 20.63 SD ⫽ 8.30
Percent female 2,022 (63%) 1,245 (75%) 777 (50%)
Education — —
Less than high school 21 (1%)
High school 276 (17%)
Some college 575 (35%)
College 779 (47%)
6th grade or less 103 (7%)
7th grade 245 (16%)
8th grade 268 (17%)
9th grade 255 (16%)
10th grade 303 (20%)
11th grade 251 (16%)
12th grade 78 (5%)
Marital status — —
Married 1,180 (72%)
Divorced 161 (10%)
Widowed 24 (1%)
Separated 29 (2%)
Never married 161 (10%)
Member of unmarried couple 91 (6%)
Race
American Indian/Alaskan 74 (2%) 29 (2%) 45 (1%)
Asian 136 (4%) 67 (4%) 69 (4%)
Black 651 (20%) 320 (19%) 331 (21%)
Pacific Islander/Hawaiian 14 (⬍1%) 4 (⬍1%) 10 (1%)
White 2,463 (77%) 1,275 (77%) 1,188 (77%)
Ethnicity
Hispanic/Latino(a) 282 (9%) 122 (7%) 160 (10%)
Not Hispanic/Latino(a) 2,829 (91%) 1,521 (93%) 1,369 (90%)
Employment status — —
Employed 967 (59%)
Self-employed 124 (8%)
Out of work 1⫹ year 75 (5%)
Out of work for ⬍1 year 38 (2%)
Homemaker 399 (24%)
Student 19 (1%)
Retired 28 (2%)
Underweight 24 (1%) 70 (4%)
Normal weight 645 (35%) 1,120 (65%)
Overweight 545 (30%) 248 (14%)
Obese 558 (30%) 203 (12%)
Emotional eating 2.29 SD ⫽ 1.00 2.39 SD ⫽ .99 2.18 SD ⫽ .99
Hedonic food frequency 4.30 SD ⫽ 3.76 3.69 SD ⫽ 3.44 4.94 SD ⫽ 2.77
Fruit and vegetable frequency 2.98 SD ⫽ 2.23 3.11 SD ⫽ 2.18 2.84 SD ⫽ 3.98

Measures etary Screener (http://epi.grants.cancer.gov/nhanes/dietscreen/) and


the 2010 National Youth Physical Activity and Nutrition Survey (20
All measures were similarly assessed in both parents and adoles-
items; Brener et al., 2011; http://www.cdc.gov/healthyYouth/yrbs/pdf/
cents. Emotion suppression was assessed with a four-item version of
nypans/2010nypans_questionnaire.pdf), while 7 items were new
the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (Gross & John, 2003). Items
(Smith et al., 2017). This screener has been validated in comparison
included: “I keep my emotions to myself;” “When I am feeling
to more in-depth food frequency questionnaires (Block, Gillespie,
POSITIVE emotions, I am careful not to express them;” “I control my
emotions by NOT EXPRESSING THEM;” and “When I am feeling Rosenbaum, & Jenson, 2000), objectively assessed diet records (An-
NEGATIVE emotions, I make sure not to express them.” Items had dersen, Johansson, & Solvoll, 2002), and intensive 24-hr recalls
answer options: 1 ⫽ strongly disagree to 5 ⫽ strongly agree. Scale (Nelson & Lytle, 2009). Additional details on the dietary screener are
reliability was acceptable, ␣ ⫽ .81 (parents ␣ ⫽ .81, adolescents ␣ ⫽ available elsewhere (Smith et al., 2017).
.80). The screener queried the frequency of foods eaten and bever-
Frequency of key foods, beverages, and food groups were assessed ages consumed. For example, participants were asked: “DURING
through a 27-item dietary screener. Items were adapted from the THE PAST 7 DAYS, how many times did you eat FRUIT like
National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey, 2009 –2010 Di- apples, bananas, melons, etc.?” Answer options were: 1: I did not
6 FERRER, GREEN, OH, HENNESSY, AND DWYER

eat [food] during the past 7 days; 2: 1–3 times in the past 7 days; et al., 1998), and as such it is appropriate to control for these to
3: 4 – 6 times in the past 7 days; 4: 1 time per day; 5: 2 times per reduce the potential for bias or confounding in analyses. Moreover,
day; and 6: 3 or more times per day. Original responses were controlling for these promotes consistency across FLASHE anal-
converted to daily frequencies (never ⫽ 0; 1 to 3 times during the yses (e.g., Nebeling et al., 2017). Analyses also controlled for
past 7 days ⫽ 0.29; 4 to 6 times during the past 7 days ⫽ 0.71; 1 BMI, given that overweight individuals are at risk for emotional
time per day ⫽ 1; etc.). eating (Ganley, 1989; Macht, 2008; Van Strien, Engels, Van
Fruit and vegetable frequency was the sum of 3 items that Leeuwe, & Snoek, 2005).
reported consumption of green salad, nonfried vegetables, and fruit We first examined each outcome independently, using actor-
over the past 7 days. Hedonic food frequency was the sum of 8 partner interdependence models (APIM; Campbell & Kashy, 2002;
items that reported consumption of sugary cereal, candy and choc- Cook & Kenny, 2005), which account for nonindependence among
olate, fried potatoes, chips, processed meat, cookies and cake, dyads in the data and simultaneously test the hypothesized actor
frozen desserts, and fast food over the past 7 days. and partner effects. We ran three separate sets of analyses (one for
Emotional eating was assessed with two items, which were each outcome) in SAS 9.3. First, APIM were performed with only
averaged, r ⫽ 0.76, p ⬍ .001. These two items were selected from main effects predictors (parent and adolescent emotion suppres-
the emotional eating subscale of a larger eating-in-the-absence-of- sion) and sociodemographic control variables. Then, two-way in-
hunger-scale, based on factor loadings in a sample of adolescents teractions were introduced to examine whether actor and partner
(Tanofsky-Kraff et al., 2008). These items were designed to assess effects differed by sex or dyad member (parent vs. adolescent), as
eating in response to anxiety and sadness (consistent with the hypothesized. Finally, each model was run with a three-way in-
concept of emotional eating; Faith et al., 1997). Eating when sad teraction, where actor suppression was crossed with actor gender
was assessed with a single item: “There are lots of reasons you and actor parent/adolescent factor.
might start eating or continue eating when you aren’t hungry. How We further examined a mediation model with structural equa-
often do YOU start or continue to eat when YOU’RE not hungry tion modeling techniques to examine whether these correlational
because . . . you feel sad or depressed?” (1 ⫽ never to 5 ⫽ always). data showed direct associations between emotion suppression and
Eating when anxious was assessed with a single item that em- eating behaviors, and indirect effects of emotion suppression via
ployed the same stem, and ended: “. . . you feel anxious or ner- emotional eating, consistent with the theoretical framework artic-
vous?” (1 ⫽ never to 5 ⫽ always). For consistency in assessment ulated in Figure 1, using Mplus 5. Mediational paths for parent and
to facilitate dyadic analyses, the same scale was administered to adolescent actor and partner effects were estimated separately.
both adolescents and parents. Only mediational pathways between variables measured in the
dataset (noted as solid, rather than dotted, arrows) were examined.
Nonsignificant paths were trimmed from the final model, unless
Control Variables
they were components of a significant mediational pathway (i.e.,
Race, parent marital status, and parent college education were where the main effect of a variable on an outcome was not
coded as dichotomous (i.e., dummy-coded), based on variability significant, but the indirect effect of the variable on the outcome,
on these variables in the sample. Race was coded as White (1)/ via a mediator, was significant). Note that indirect paths were only
non-White (0). Parental marital status was coded as married (1)/ evaluated when conditions for mediation were met (i.e., when the
nonmarried (0). Parent education was coded as college educated proposed mediator was associated with both the predictor and the
(1)/less than college educated (0). Gender was coded as female outcome). Bootstrapped confidence intervals (95% CI) for indirect
(1)/male (0). Analyses also controlled for whether the participant paths in the mediation model were also reported.
was a parent (0) or adolescent (1).
To calculate Body Mass Index (BMI), parents and adolescents
each self-reported their height (in feet and inches) and weight (in
Results
pounds), which were converted to centimeters and kilograms for Complete results for all APIM are located in Table 2.
calculations. BMI was chosen rather than BMI z-score for adoles-
cents to facilitate dyadic analyses. The Web based platform was Hypothesis 1: We predicted that an actor’s emotion suppres-
preprogrammed with soft system checks so that if a participant sion would be associated with all of the actor’s eating behav-
entered a value outside of a biologically plausible range for height iors (actor effects). In support of this hypothesis, we found
or weight, a pop-up box appeared on the screen, which the respon- emotion suppression was associated with more emotional eat-
dent had to confirm before being able to move to the next question. ing (B ⫽ 0.21, p ⬍ .001); lower consumption frequency for
Data were also screened to identify potential outliers (see Westat, fruits and vegetables (B ⫽ ⫺0.11, p ⫽ .009); and greater
2015). consumption frequency for hedonic foods (B ⫽ 0.31, p ⬍
.001; see Table 2).
Analyses Hypothesis 2: We predicted that an actor’s dyadic partner’s
Listwise deletion was used within each analysis for missing emotion suppression would be associated with all actor eating
data. All analyses controlled for standard sociodemographic fac- behaviors (partner effects). In support of this hypothesis, we
tors at both the individual-level (each individual’s age, gender, and
race),1 as well as dyadic-level (parent education and marital sta- 1
T The pattern and significance of results for all three models remained
tus). These covariates were selected because they are known to be unchanged when these standard sociodemographic factors were not in-
correlated with eating behaviors (e.g., Subar et al., 1995; Trudeau cluded as covariates.
EMOTION SUPPRESSION IN PARENT–ADOLESCENT DYADS 7

Table 2
Mixed Model Regression (Actor-Partner Independence Model)

Hedonic food
Emotional eating F&V frequency frequency
(n ⫽ 3,097) (n ⫽ 2,853) (n ⫽ 2,885)
Predictor B p B p B p

Self age ⫺.01 .437 .01 .094 ⫺.01 .406


Self White .26 <.001 ⴚ.27 .024 ⴚ.63 .001
Parent married ⫺.01 .934 ⫺.01 .932 .20 .298
Parent college educated .04 .310 .43 <.001 ⫺.18 .306
Self adolescent ⫺.01 .948 .07 .751 1.06 .001
Self Body Mass Index .29 <.001 ⴚ.14 .024 .03 .809
Self female .40 <.001 .10 .250 ⫺.26 .069
Partner female ⫺.01 .805 ⫺.15 .088 ⫺.17 .223
Self emotion suppression .21 <.001 ⴚ.11 .009 .31 <.001
Partner emotion suppression .05 .003 ⴚ.12 .004 .20 .005
Partner Suppression ⫻ Adolescent ⴚ.13 .003 .12 .269 ⫺.02 .901
Self Suppression ⫻ Self Female .06 .131 .11 .256 .09 .669
Partner Suppression ⫻ Partner female ⫺.04 .246 .12 .212 ⫺.01 .976
Note. F&V ⫽ fruit and vegetable. Bold lettering is significant at p ⬍ .01.

found that partner’s emotion suppression was associated with food (B ⫽ 0.04, 95% CI ⫽ 0.02, 0.05, p ⬍ .001) and fruit and
more emotional eating (B ⫽ 0.05, p ⫽ .003); lower consump- vegetable consumption (B ⫽ ⫺0.04, 95% CI ⫽ ⫺0.07, ⫺0.02,
tion frequency for fruits and vegetables (B ⫽ ⫺0.12, p ⫽ p ⬍ .001), via parent, emotional eating, were significant. Simi-
.004); and greater consumption frequency for hedonic food larly, actor pathways for adolescents were largely supported; ad-
(B ⫽ 0.20, p ⫽ .005; see Table 2). olescent suppression was associated with adolescent emotional
eating (B ⫽ 0.24, p ⬍ .001) and adolescent fruit and vegetable
Moderation Hypothesis 1 consumption (B ⫽ ⫺0.15, p ⬍ .001), although the main associa-
tion between adolescent suppression and adolescent hedonic food
We predicted that emotion suppression would be more strongly consumption was not significant. However, there was a significant
associated with eating behaviors for women than for men. Anal- indirect association of adolescent suppression and adolescent he-
yses did not support this hypothesis; although women reported donic food consumption, via adolescent emotional eating (B ⫽
greater emotional eating (B ⫽ 0.41, p ⬍ .001), the interaction 0.03, 95% CI ⫽ 0.01, 0.06, p ⫽ .004), as well as via parent
between own suppression and own sex was not significant in emotional eating (B ⫽ 0.01, 95% CI ⫽ 0.001, 0.02, p ⫽ .031).
predicting any outcome. Similarly, the association of adolescent suppression and adolescent
fruit and vegetable consumption, via adolescent emotional eating,
Moderation Hypothesis 2 was also significant (B ⫽ ⫺0.02, 95% CI ⫽ ⫺0.05, 0.009, p ⫽
We predicted that one’s adolescent’s emotion suppression .045; see Table 3).
would be more strongly associated with eating behaviors among Analyses of partner pathways were also largely supported (see
parents (partner effects; compared with one’s parent’s emotion Figure 2). Adolescent suppression was significantly associated
suppression’s association with eating behaviors among adoles- with parent emotional eating (B ⫽ 0.11, p ⬍ .001) and parent fruit
cents). Consistent with this hypothesis, we found a significant and vegetable consumption (B ⫽ ⫺0.20, p ⫽ .001), although the
interaction, whereby parents’ suppression was less strongly asso- main association of adolescent suppression and parent hedonic
ciated with adolescents’ emotional eating than the reverse food consumption was not significant. However, the association of
(B ⫽ ⫺0.12, p ⫽ .003; see Table 2), and in fact, that there was not adolescent suppression with parent hedonic food consumption, via
a significant association between parents’ suppression and adoles- parent emotional eating, was significant (B ⫽ 0.02, 95% CI ⫽
cents’ emotional eating (B ⫽ 0.01, p ⫽ .720), where the converse 0.01, 0.04, p ⬍ .001). Similarly, the association of adolescent
path was significant (B ⫽ 0.11, p ⬍ .001). suppression and parent fruit and vegetable consumption, via
parent emotional eating, was significant (B ⫽ ⫺0.03, 95%
CI ⫽ ⫺0.05, ⫺0.01, p ⫽ .004). There were no main associations
Mediation Model
between parent emotion suppression and any adolescent outcome.
As depicted in Figure 2, the direct pathways proposed in our However, the indirect associations of parent suppression and ad-
theoretical framework were largely supported for both actor and olescent hedonic food consumption, via parent query for blank
partner effects. Analyses of actor pathways showed a direct marking emotional eating, were significant (B ⫽ 0.02, 95% CI ⫽
association of parent emotion suppression with parent emo- 0.01, 0.04, p ⫽ .019). There were also positive associations be-
tional eating (B ⫽ 0.17, p ⬍ .001), parent hedonic food con- tween parents’ hedonic food consumption and their fruit and
sumption (B ⫽ 0.08, p ⫽ .041), and parent fruit and vegetable vegetable consumption (and similarly positive pathways for ado-
consumption (B ⫽ ⫺0.12, p ⫽ .049). Moreover, the indirect lescents between hedonic food and fruit and vegetable consump-
associations of parent emotion suppression with parent hedonic tion; see Table 3).
8 FERRER, GREEN, OH, HENNESSY, AND DWYER

Figure 2. Structural equation model examining associations of parent and adolescent suppression with parent
and adolescent hedonic food and fruit and vegetable consumption (independently and via parent and adolescent
emotional eating). ⴱ p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱⴱ p ⬍ .001.

Discussion sistent with the hypothesis that suppression has negative conse-
quences for oneself and one’s partner.
This study is the first known study to examine potential dyadic
consequences of emotion suppression among parent–adolescent Specifically, in the main analyses examining each outcome
dyads, and one of few that focuses on suppression as a transactive separately, we found that both one’s own and one’s partner’s
process in close relationships in a large national sample. These emotion suppression was associated with more emotional eating,
data suggest that, despite its necessity in some social interactions, greater consumption of hedonic foods, and less fruit and vegetable
emotion suppression is associated with negative behavioral con- consumption, even when controlling for a variety of sociodemo-
sequences in close relationships. As is the case in previously graphic factors and BMI. The relationships observed in these
unacquainted dyads (Butler et al., 2003, 2007) and among parents cross-sectional findings are in parallel with laboratory experiments
of young children (Le & Impett, 2016), we found evidence con- demonstrating that induced negative emotions (Garg et al., 2007)

Table 3
Indirect Paths in Mediation Model

Parent hedonic food consumption Parent F&V consumption


(via) Parent (via) Adolescent (via) Parent (via) Adolescent
emotional eating emotional eating emotional eating emotional eating
Model predictors B p B p B p B p

Parent suppression .04 ⬍.001 — — ⫺.04 .001 — —


Adolescent suppression .03 ⬍.001 — — ⫺.03 .004 — —

Adolescent hedonic food consumption Adolescent F&V consumption


(via) Parent (via) Adolescent (via) Parent (via) Adolescent
emotional eating emotional eating emotional eating emotional eating
B p B p B p B p

Parent suppression .02 .019 — — ⫺.02 .080 — —


Adolescent suppression .01 .031 .03 .004 ⫺.01 .096 ⫺.03 .045
Note. F&V ⫽ fruit and vegetable.
EMOTION SUPPRESSION IN PARENT–ADOLESCENT DYADS 9

and instructed emotion suppression (Evers et al., 2010; Taut et al., Although these pathways were not explicitly predicted (and are at
2012) lead to unhealthy eating behaviors, suggesting that naturally odds with hypotheses that adolescent-parent partner pathways
occurring emotion suppression may be consequential in real-world would be stronger than parent–adolescent partner pathways), they
settings. Findings are also consistent with laboratory research are consistent with the literature on social modeling and nutrition
demonstrating that emotion suppression has implications for com- behaviors in families. Specifically, research suggests that adoles-
munication within dyads (Butler et al., 2003). cent children are more likely to eat healthy foods and avoid
The pathways in the mediation model were also generally con- unhealthy foods to the extent that their parents model this these
sistent with predictions. There were both direct and indirect actor behaviors (Lopez et al., 2012; Rhee, 2008; Young et al., 2004), and
pathways among parent suppression and parent outcomes, and research suggests the same might be true for emotional eating
actor pathways generally emerged for adolescents as well. Nota- (Morrison, Power, Nicklas, & Hughes, 2013). Thus, it seems likely
bly, the paths from hedonic food consumption to fruit and vege- that the indirect association of parent suppression and adolescent
table consumption were positive, rather than negative, among both hedonic food consumption, via parent emotional eating, could be a
parents and adolescents, suggesting that the mechanism underlying result of the effects of parents modeling eating in the absence of
the association between emotion suppression and reduced fruit and hunger, when they are anxious or sad.
vegetable consumption is not adjustment to accommodate greater Our moderation hypotheses regarding the strength of associa-
consumption of hedonic foods. Rather, it seems possible that tions for women were largely not supported. Although women
another unmeasured psychological mechanism may independently were more likely to report emotional eating, suppression did not
underlie the association between suppression and fruit and vege- interact with participant sex to predict any outcome. We also found
table consumption. One possibility is social support. Suppression no evidence that one’s partner’s suppression had stronger links to
can weaken relationships (Butler & Gross, 2004; Le & Impett, outcomes for female, compared with male, partners. Moreover,
2016; Richards et al., 2003) and be a barrier to forming close there was no support for the notion that one’s partner’s suppression
relationships (Srivastava et al., 2009), leading to reduced social would predict outcomes more when the actor was female. These
support and weaker social connections (English et al., 2012; Labott findings stand in contrast to exploratory predictions that suppres-
et al., 1991; Srivastava et al., 2009). Research in health behavior sion would be more strongly associated with outcomes for women
change suggests that social support is critical to facilitate healthy (and when women were suppressors), based on previous research
behaviors, including healthy eating behaviors such as fruit and on sex differences in communication of emotion (Flynn et al.,
vegetable consumption (Anderson, Winett, & Wojcik, 2007; Perry 2010; Sonnby-Borgstrom et al., 2008), as well as sex differences in
et al., 2004; Steptoe, Perkins-Porras, Rink, Hilton, & Cappuccio, emotion suppression (Benjamin & Kamin-Shaaltiel, 2004; Sobal,
2004). Social support for eating behaviors may be particularly Rauschenbach, & Frongillo, 1995).
important for children and adolescents (Neumark-Sztainer, Wall, However, our study involves examining parent–adolescent dy-
Perry, & Story, 2003). Thus, to the extent that suppression-induced ads, where parents may have greater “power” or authority, in
disruptions in social support extend to social support specifically contrast to prior research in romantic dyads. Given that suppres-
for eating behaviors, suppression could affect unhealthy eating sion is most often engaged when individuals feel they are low in
(among both the suppressor and their interaction partners) by the social hierarchy (Catterson et al., 2016), a more complex
disrupting social support. analysis of these factors would necessitate examination of whether
Partner pathways were also largely supported, with direct and dyadic associations between suppression and eating behaviors
indirect effects of adolescent suppression on parent outcomes depended on whether each particular dyad member was male/
emerging. However, and as predicted in moderation hypotheses, female and adolescent/parent (see also Lloyd et al., 2014). This
partner pathways from parent suppression to adolescent outcomes study was not powered for such analyses, and such an examination
were less robust. Although the indirect association of parent sup- is beyond the scope of our inquiry. It is also possible that exam-
pression and adolescent hedonic food consumption was signifi- ining a more complex interplay that accounts for examination of
cant, there were no main associations between parent emotion the makeup of the dyad would yield different findings; for exam-
suppression and any adolescent outcome and no indirect associa- ple, research has demonstrated that suppression may have different
tion between parent suppression and adolescent fruit and vegetable cardiovascular effects among same- versus opposite sex dyads
consumption. These findings are consistent with another study (Mendes, Reis, Seery, & Blascovich, 2003).
examining dyadic influences on eating behavior among family The mechanisms underlying the general findings— both models
dyads (Baiocchi-Wagner & Talley, 2013). One possible reason for of the individual outcomes and structural modeling of the media-
this finding is that parents are more invested in the outcomes of tion model—remain undiscovered. Although results are consistent
emotional communication with their children than the reverse, with the psychological mechanism proposed in Figure 1 (i.e.,
such that disruptions in this communication are more consequen- negative affect), this putative mediator was not assessed in these
tial for parents. Future research should address this possibility. data. However, the emergence of both direct and indirect paths
While much prior literature has focused on parental influence on from parent and adolescent suppression to unhealthy eating out-
child behaviors, this finding highlights the bidirectional nature of comes supports the notion that there are multiple mechanisms and
parent– child relationships and the role that adolescents may have exhibits the general pattern one would expect if negative affect
on parent eating behaviors. (and other potential mechanisms) undergirded the association be-
Of note, we found significant support for partner pathways from tween emotion suppression and unhealthy eating behaviors at a
parent emotional eating to adolescent hedonic food consumption, dyadic level. However, it is also possible that other psychological
as well as indirect associations between parent suppression and processes play a role. For example, cognitive load is associated
adolescent hedonic food consumption, via parent emotional eating. with disinhibited eating (Boon, Stroebe, Schut, & Ijntema, 2002;
10 FERRER, GREEN, OH, HENNESSY, AND DWYER

Cools et al., 1992; Macht, 2008; Ward & Mann, 2000), and the importance of the bidirectional interpersonal relationships
emotion suppression produces cognitive load (Gross, 2002; Rich- within families and home settings to influence diet behaviors.
ards & Gross, 1999, 2000); as such, cognitive load may be a These findings have implications for the ways in which eating
psychological mechanism underlying the association between behavior is intervened upon, particularly with respect to emotion
emotion suppression and unhealthy eating. Competing explana- (e.g., Telch, Agras, & Linehan, 2001; Wiser & Telch, 1999).
tions underscore the need for focused, laboratory experiments to Although many interventions target more social– cognitive deter-
elucidate psychological processes linking emotion suppression to minants of eating behavior (Shaikh et al., 2008), these findings
unhealthy eating behaviors. suggest that emotional factors ostensibly unrelated to decisions
Regardless of mechanism, these findings have important impli- about eating (i.e., emotion suppression) can influence eating be-
cations, given that emotion suppression is relatively common in havior and highlight the inherently social or interpersonal influ-
social exchanges (Gross & John, 2003). Prior research on deter- ences on eating behaviors. Moreover, these findings suggest that
minants or predictors of eating behaviors have focused on envi- even interventions that target emotion regulation may be more
ronmental (i.e., access to healthful foods) and social– cognitive effective if extended to focus not only on one’s own emotion
(i.e., self-efficacy for healthy eating, attitudes) determinants (Ras- regulatory processes, but also those of close others (specifically
mussen et al., 2006). This study extends current literature to parents or adolescent children). Finally, these findings have im-
consider the nature of dyadic relationships and the role of emotion plications for the study of and intervention upon other unhealthy
suppression on individuals and their partners to influence eating behaviors that may be engaged as compensatory emotion regula-
behaviors. Note that these findings emerged even in the context of tory strategies, including smoking (Fucito, Juliano, & Toll, 2010;
statistical modeling that accounts for interdependence among the Szasz, Szentagotai, & Hofmann, 2012).
dyadic data (important given that research suggests the use of
suppression is correlated in mother-adolescent dyads; Bariola,
Hughes, & Gullone, 2011). References
The present study has several limitations. Most notably, data are Adam, T. C., & Epel, E. S. (2007). Stress, eating and the reward system.
cross-sectional and preclude causal examination, including a full Physiology & Behavior, 91, 449 – 458. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j
examination of the conceptual framework presented in Figure 1. .physbeh.2007.04.011
However, note that analyses are consistent with causal hypotheses Agras, W. S., & Telch, C. F. (1998). The effects of caloric deprivation and
generated by laboratory research (Butler & Gross, 2004; Evers et negative affect on binge eating in obese binge-eating disordered women.
al., 2010; Taut et al., 2012). Moreover, although analyses rely on Behavior Therapy, 29, 491–503. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-
standard dietary screeners (National Cancer Institute, 2015; Subar 7894(98)80045-2
et al., 2015) that have been validated against food frequency Andersen, L. F., Johansson, L., & Solvoll, K. (2002). Usefulness of a short
Food Frequency Questionnaire for screening of low intake of fruit and
questionnaires (Block, Gillespie, Rosenbaum, & Jenson, 2000),
vegetable and for intake of fat. European Journal of Public Health, 12,
diet records (Andersen et al., 2002), and 24-hr recalls (Nelson &
208 –213. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/eurpub/12.3.208
Lytle, 2009), results are limited in that outcomes are self-reported. Anderson, E. S., Winett, R. A., & Wojcik, J. R. (2007). Self-regulation,
Thus, future research could examine suppression in the context of self-efficacy, outcome expectations, and social support: Social cognitive
more intensively and/or objectively assessed eating behaviors, theory and nutrition behavior. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 34, 304 –
especially if there is an interest in examination of dyadic relation- 312. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02874555
ships and energy intake. Moreover, there was no measure of Armitage, C. J. (2015). Randomized test of a brief psychological interven-
closeness among parent–adolescent dyads, which could potentially tion to reduce and prevent emotional eating in a community sample.
modify results of actor-partner interdependence models; future Journal of Public Health, 37, 438 – 444. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/
research should examine whether results differ with the closeness pubmed/fdv054
Arnow, B., Kenardy, J., & Agras, W. S. (1995). The Emotional Eating
of a dyadic relationship. Finally, analyses did not take into account
Scale: The development of a measure to assess coping with negative
possible differences in access to and availability of fruits and
affect by eating. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 18,
vegetables (based on neighborhood environment or season of the 79 –90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/1098-108X(199507)18:1⬍79::
year). These limitations are offset by a number of strengths, AID-EAT2260180109⬎3.0.CO;2-V
including the use of a national sample of parent–adolescent dyads Baiocchi-Wagner, E. A., & Talley, A. E. (2013). The role of family
to extend knowledge of how emotion suppression influences dy- communication in individual health attitudes and behaviors concerning
adic regulatory processes (Butler et al., 2009) and implications for diet and physical activity. Health Communication, 28, 193–205. http://
maladaptive eating behavior as a secondary regulatory strategy dx.doi.org/10.1080/10410236.2012.674911
(Taut et al., 2012). Bariola, E., Gullone, E., & Hughes, E. K. (2011). Child and adolescent
These findings present important hypotheses that can inform emotion regulation: The role of parental emotion regulation and expres-
future research. For example, although these analyses suggest that sion. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 14, 198 –212.
Benjamin, O., & Kamin-Shaaltiel, S. (2004). It’s not because I’m fat:
emotion regulatory processes have dyadic implications among
Perceived overweight and anger avoidance in marriage. Health Care for
parents and adolescents, they do not speak to whether such dyadic
Women International, 25, 853– 871. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
processes are present among other types of dyads. For example, it 07399330490493403
is possible that these findings would extend to marital relation- Block, G., Gillespie, C., Rosenbaum, E. H., & Jenson, C. (2000). A rapid
ships, sibling relationships, and may even vary by the age of the food screener to assess fat and fruit and vegetable intake. American
child (e.g., younger children; Hughes et al., 2011)—a possibility Journal of Preventive Medicine, 18, 284 –288. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
that should be addressed in future research. Findings also support S0749-3797(00)00119-7
EMOTION SUPPRESSION IN PARENT–ADOLESCENT DYADS 11

Boon, B., Stroebe, W., Schut, H., & Ijntema, R. (2002). Ironic processes in dations. In D. Sharron (Ed.), Overweight and weight management: The
the eating behaviour of restrained eaters. British Journal of Health health professional’s guide to understanding and practice (pp. 439 –
Psychology, 7, 1–10. http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/135910702169303 465). New York, NY: An Aspen Publication.
Brener, N. D., Merlo, C., Eaton, D., Kann, L., Park, S., & Blanck, H. M. Ferrer, R. A., Green, P. A., & Barrett, L. F. (2015). Affective science
(2011). Beverage consumption among high school students—United perspectives on cancer control: Strategically crafting a mutually bene-
States, 2010. MMWR, 60, 778 –780. ficial research agenda. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10, 328 –
Butler, E. A., Egloff, B., Wlhelm, F. H., Smith, N. C., Erickson, E. A., & 345.
Gross, J. J. (2003). The social consequences of expressive suppression. Fisher, E. B., Walker, E. A., Bostrom, A., Fischhoff, B., Haire-Joshu, D.,
Emotion, 3, 48 – 67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.3.1.48 & Johnson, S. B. (2002). Behavioral science research in the prevention
Butler, E. A., & Gross, J. J. (2004). Hiding feelings in social contexts: Out of diabetes: Status and opportunities. Diabetes Care, 25, 599 – 606.
of sight is not out of mind. In P. Philippot & R. S. Feldman (Eds.), The http://dx.doi.org/10.2337/diacare.25.3.599
regulation of emotion (pp. 101–126). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Flynn, J. J., Hollenstein, T., & Mackey, A. (2010). The effect of suppress-
Associates. http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9781410610898 ing and not accepting emotions on depressive symptoms: Is suppression
Butler, E. A., & Gross, J. J. (2009). Emotion and emotion regulation: different for men and women? Personality and Individual Differences,
Integrating individual and social levels of analysis. Emotion Review, 1, 49, 582–586. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.05.022
86 – 87. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1754073908099131 Ford, E. S., Zhao, G., Tsai, J., & Li, C. (2011). Low-risk lifestyle behaviors
Butler, E. A., Lee, T. L., & Gross, J. J. (2007). Emotion regulation and and all-cause mortality: Findings from the National Health and Nutrition
culture: Are the social consequences of emotion suppression culture- Examination Survey III Mortality Study. American Journal of Public
specific? Emotion, 7, 30 – 48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7 Health, 101, 1922–1929.
.1.30 Fucito, L. M., Juliano, L. M., & Toll, B. A. (2010). Cognitive reappraisal
Butler, E. A., Young, V. J., & Randall, A. K. (2010). Suppressing to please, and expressive suppression emotion regulation strategies in cigarette
eating to cope: The effect of overweight women’s emotion suppression smokers. Nicotine & Tobacco Research, 12, 1156 –1161. http://dx.doi
on romantic relationships and eating. Journal of Social and Clinical .org/10.1093/ntr/ntq146
Psychology, 29, 599 – 623. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2010.29.6.599 Gagnon-Girouard, M. P., Bégin, C., Provencher, V., Tremblay, A., Boivin,
Campbell, L., & Kashy, D. A. (2002). Estimating actor, partner, and S., & Lemieux, S. (2009). Can we apply the dual-pathway model of
interaction effects for dyadic data using PROC MIXED and HLM: A overeating to a population of weight-preoccupied overweight women?
user–friendly guide. Personal Relationships, 9, 327–342. http://dx.doi International Journal of Eating Disorders, 42, 244 –252. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1111/1475-6811.00023 .org/10.1002/eat.20614
Carlson, S. M., & Wang, T. S. (2007). Inhibitory control and emotion Ganley, R. M. (1989). Emotion and eating in obesity: A review of the
regulation in preschool children. Cognitive Development, 22, 489 –510. literature. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 8, 343–361. http://dx
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2007.08.002 .doi.org/10.1002/1098-108X(198905)8:3⬍343::AID-EAT2260080310⬎3
Catterson, A. D., Eldesouky, L., & John, O. P. (2016). An experience .0.CO;2-C
sampling approach to emotion regulation: Situational suppression use Garg, N., Wansink, B., & Inman, J. J. (2007). The influence of incidental
and social hierarchy. Journal of Research in Personality. Advance affect on consumers’ food intake. Journal of Marketing, 71, 194 –206.
online publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2016.04.004 http://dx.doi.org/10.1509/jmkg.71.1.194
Cook, W. L., & Kenny, D. A. (2005). The actor–partner interdependence Gilboa-Schechtman, E., Avnon, L., Zubery, E., & Jeczmien, P. (2006).
model: A model of bidirectional effects in developmental studies. Inter- Emotional processing in eating disorders: Specific impairment or gen-
national Journal of Behavioral Development, 29, 101–109. http://dx.doi eral distress related deficiency? Depression and Anxiety, 23, 331–339.
.org/10.1080/01650250444000405 http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/da.20163
Cools, J., Schotte, D. E., & McNally, R. J. (1992). Emotional arousal and Greeno, C. G., & Wing, R. R. (1994). Stress-induced eating. Psychological
overeating in restrained eaters. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, Bulletin, 115, 444 – 464. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.115.3.444
348 –351. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.101.2.348 Gross, J. J. (1998). Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation:
Crockett, A. C., Myhre, S. K., & Rokke, P. D. (2015). Boredom proneness and Divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology.
emotion regulation predict emotional eating. Journal of Health Psychology, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 224 –237. http://dx
20, 670 – 680. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1359105315573439 .doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.224
DeSteno, D., Gross, J. J., & Kubzansky, L. (2013). Affective science and Gross, J. J. (1999). Emotion regulation: Past, present, future. Cognition and
health: The importance of emotion and emotion regulation. Health Emotion, 13, 551–573. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/026999399379186
Psychology, 32, 474 – 486. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0030259 Gross, J. J. (2002). Emotion regulation: Affective, cognitive, and social
English, T., & John, O. P. (2013). Understanding the social effects of consequences. Psychophysiology, 39, 281–291. http://dx.doi.org/10
emotion regulation: The mediating role of authenticity for individual .1017/S0048577201393198
differences in suppression. Emotion, 13, 314. Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion
English, T., John, O. P., Srivastava, S., & Gross, J. J. (2012). Emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-
regulation and peer-rated social functioning: A 4-year longitudinal being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 348 –362.
study. Journal of Research in Personality, 46, 780 –784. http://dx.doi http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348
.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2012.09.006 Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1993). Emotional suppression: Physiol-
Fairburn, C. G., & Cooper, P. J. (1982). Self-induced vomiting and bulimia ogy, self-report, and expressive behavior. Journal of Personality and
nervosa: An undetected problem. British Medical Journal, 284, 1153– Social Psychology, 64, 970 –986. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514
1155. .64.6.970
Evers, C., Marijn Stok, F., & de Ridder, D. T. D. (2010). Feeding your Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1997). Hiding feelings: The acute effects
feelings: Emotion regulation strategies and emotional eating. Personal- of inhibiting negative and positive emotion. Journal of Abnormal Psy-
ity and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 792– 804. http://dx.doi.org/10 chology, 106, 95–103. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.106.1.95
.1177/0146167210371383 Haedt-Matt, A. A., Keel, P. K., Racine, S. E., Burt, S. A., Hu, J. Y., Boker,
Faith, M. S., Allison, D. B., Geliebter, A., & Dalton, S. (1997). Emotional S., . . . Klump, K. L. (2014). Do emotional eating urges regulate affect?
eating and obesity: theoretical considerations and practical recommen- Concurrent and prospective associations and implications for risk mod-
12 FERRER, GREEN, OH, HENNESSY, AND DWYER

els of binge eating. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 47, Lehman, A. K., & Rodin, J. (1989). Styles of self-nurturance and disor-
874 – 877. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/eat.22247 dered eating. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 117–
Haga, S. M., Kraft, P., & Corby, E. K. (2009). Emotion regulation: 122. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.57.1.117
Antecedents and well-being outcomes of cognitive reappraisal and ex- Leith, K. P., & Baumeister, R. F. (1996). Why do bad moods increase
pressive suppression in cross-cultural samples. Journal of Happiness self-defeating behavior? Emotion, risk taking, and self-regulation. Jour-
Studies, 10, 271–291. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10902-007-9080-3 nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 1250 –1267. http://dx.doi
Harris, C. R. (2001). Cardiovascular responses of embarrassment and .org/10.1037/0022-3514.71.6.1250
effects of emotional suppression in a social setting. Journal of Person- Lloyd, A. B., Lubans, D. R., Plotnikoff, R. C., Collins, C. E., & Morgan,
ality and Social Psychology, 81, 886 – 897. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ P. J. (2014). Maternal and paternal parenting practices and their influ-
0022-3514.81.5.886 ence on children’s adiposity, screen-time, diet and physical activity.
Heatherton, T. F., & Baumeister, R. F. (1991). Binge eating as escape from Appetite, 79, 149 –157. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2014.04.010
self-awareness. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 86 –108. http://dx.doi.org/ Lopez, N. V., Ayala, G. X., Corder, K., Eisenberg, C. M., Zive, M. M.,
10.1037/0033-2909.110.1.86 Wood, C., & Elder, J. P. (2012). Parent support and parent-mediated
Heatherton, T. F., Herman, C. P., & Polivy, J. (1991). Effects of physical behaviors are associated with children’s sugary beverage consumption.
threat and ego threat on eating behavior. Journal of Personality and Journal of the Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics, 112, 541–547.
Social Psychology, 60, 138 –143. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jand.2011.11.013
.60.1.138 Macht, M. (2008). How emotions affect eating: A five-way model. Appe-
Hofmann, W., Rauch, W., & Gawronski, B. (2007). And deplete us not into tite, 50, 1–11. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2007.07.002
temptation: Automatic attitudes, dietary restraint, and self-regulatory Macht, M., Haupt, C., & Ellgring, H. (2005). The perceived function of
resources as determinants of eating behavior. Journal of Experimental eating is changed during examination stress: A field study. Eating
Social Psychology, 43, 497–504. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2006 Behaviors, 6, 109 –112. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2004.09.001
.05.004 Macht, M., & Mueller, J. (2007). Immediate effects of chocolate on
Hughes, S. O., Power, T. G., Papaioannou, M. A., Cross, M. B., Nicklas, experimentally induced mood states. Appetite, 49, 667– 674. http://dx
T. A., Hall, S. K., & Shewchuk, R. M. (2011). Emotional climate, .doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2007.05.004
Macht, M., & Simons, G. (2000). Emotions and eating in everyday life.
feeding practices, and feeding styles: An observational analysis of the
Appetite, 35, 65–71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/appe.2000.0325
dinner meal in Head Start families. The International Journal of Behav-
Mendes, W. B., Reis, H. T., Seery, M. D., & Blascovich, J. (2003).
ioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 8, 60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/
Cardiovascular correlates of emotional expression and suppression: Do
1479-5868-8-60
content and gender context matter? Journal of Personality and Social
John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. (2004). Healthy and unhealthy emotion regu-
Psychology, 84, 771–792. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.771
lation: Personality processes, individual differences, and life span de-
Moore, S. A., Zoellner, L. A., & Mollenholt, N. (2008). Are expressive
velopment. Journal of Personality, 72, 1301–1334. http://dx.doi.org/10
suppression and cognitive reappraisal associated with stress-related
.1111/j.1467-6494.2004.00298.x
symptoms? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 993–1000. http://dx
Katterman, S. N., Kleinman, B. M., Hood, M. M., Nackers, L. M., &
.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2008.05.001
Corsica, J. A. (2014). Mindfulness meditation as an intervention for
Morrison, H., Power, T. G., Nicklas, T., & Hughes, S. O. (2013). Exploring
binge eating, emotional eating, and weight loss: A systematic review.
the effects of maternal eating patterns on maternal feeding and child
Eating Behaviors, 15, 197–204. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2014
eating. Appetite, 63, 77– 83.
.01.005
National Cancer Institute. (2015). Screeners at a glance. Dietary Assess-
Kennedy-Moore, E., & Watson, J. C. (2001). How and when does emo-
ment Primer. Retrieved from http://dietassessmentprimer.cancer.gov/
tional expression help? Review of General Psychology, 5, 187–212. profiles/screeners/
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.5.3.187 Nebeling, L. C., Hennessey, E. M., Oh, A. Y., Dwyer, L. A., Patrick, H.,
Khaw, K.-T., Wareham, N., Bingham, S., Welch, A., Luben, R., & Day, N. Blanck, H. M., . . . Yaroch, A. L. (in press). The FLASHE Study: Survey
(2008). Combined impact of health behaviours and mortality in men and development, dyadic perspectives, and participant characteristics. Amer-
women: The EPIC-Norfolk Prospective Population Study. PLoS Medi- ican Journal of Preventive Medicine.
cine, 5, e12. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.0050012 Nelson, M. C., & Lytle, L. A. (2009). Development and evaluation of a
Klein, W. M., Bloch, M., Hesse, B. W., McDonald, P. G., Nebeling, L., brief screener to estimate fast-food and beverage consumption among
O’Connell, M. E., . . . Tesauro, G. (2014). Behavioral research in cancer adolescents. Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 109, 730 –
prevention and control: A look to the future. American Journal of 734. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jada.2008.12.027
Preventive Medicine, 46, 303–311. Neumark-Sztainer, D., Wall, M., Perry, C., & Story, M. (2003). Correlates
Labott, S. M., Martin, R. B., Eason, P. S., & Berkey, E. Y. (1991). Social of fruit and vegetable intake among adolescents. Findings from Project
reactions to the expression of emotion. Cognition and Emotion, 5, EAT. Preventive Medicine, 37, 198 –208. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
397– 417. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699939108411050 S0091-7435(03)00114-2
Laitinen, J., Ek, E., & Sovio, U. (2002). Stress-related eating and drinking Nguyen-Michel, S. T., Unger, J. B., & Spruijt-Metz, D. (2007). Dietary
behavior and body mass index and predictors of this behavior. Preven- correlates of emotional eating in adolescence. Appetite, 49, 494 – 499.
tive Medicine, 34, 29 –39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/pmed.2001.0948 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2007.03.005
Larson, N. I., Neumark-Sztainer, D. R., Harnack, L. J., Wall, M. M., Story, Oh, A. Y., Davis, T., Dwyer, L. A., Hennessy, E., Li, T., Yaroch, A. L., &
M. T., & Eisenberg, M. E. (2008). Fruit and vegetable intake correlates Nebeling, L. C. (in press). Recruitment, enrollment, and response of
during the transition to young adulthood. American Journal of Preven- parent-adolescent dyads in the FLASHE study. American Journal of
tive Medicine, 35, 33–37. e3. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2008.03 Preventive Medicine.
.019 Oliver, G., Wardle, J., & Gibson, E. L. (2000). Stress and food choice: A
Le, B. M., & Impett, E. A. (2016). The costs of suppressing negative laboratory study. Psychosomatic Medicine, 62, 853– 865. http://dx.doi
emotions and amplifying positive emotions during parental caregiving. .org/10.1097/00006842-200011000-00016
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 42, 323–336. http://dx.doi Pearson, T. A., Blair, S. N., Daniels, S. R., Eckel, R. H., Fair, J. M.,
.org/10.1177/0146167216629122 Fortmann, S. P., . . . Taubert, K. A. (2002). AHA guidelines for primary
EMOTION SUPPRESSION IN PARENT–ADOLESCENT DYADS 13

prevention of cardiovascular disease and stroke: 2002 update. Circula- Cancer Research, 69, 7151–7156. http://dx.doi.org/10.1158/0008-5472
tion, 106, 388 –391. http://dx.doi.org/10.1161/01.cir.0000020190 .can-08-4005
.45892.75 Steptoe, A., Perkins-Porras, L., Rink, E., Hilton, S., & Cappuccio, F. P.
Perry, C. L., Bishop, D. B., Taylor, G. L., Davis, M., Story, M., Gray, (2004). Psychological and social predictors of changes in fruit and
C., . . . Harnack, L. (2004). A randomized school trial of environmental vegetable consumption over 12 months following behavioral and nutri-
strategies to encourage fruit and vegetable consumption among children. tion education counseling. Health Psychology, 23, 574 –581. http://dx
Health Education & Behavior, 31, 65–76. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ .doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.23.6.574
1090198103255530 Subar, A. F., Freedman, L. S., Tooze, J. A., Kirkpatrick, S. I., Boushey, C.,
Rasmussen, M., Krølner, R., Klepp, K. I., Lytle, L., Brug, J., Bere, E., & Neuhouser, M. L., . . . Krebs-Smith, S. M. (2015). Addressing current
Due, P. (2006). Determinants of fruit and vegetable consumption among criticism regarding the value of self-report dietary data. The Journal of
children and adolescents: A review of the literature. Part I: Quantitative Nutrition, 145, 2639 –2645. http://dx.doi.org/10.3945/jn.115.219634
studies. The International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Subar, A. F., Heimendinger, J., Patterson, B. H., Krebs-Smith, S. M.,
Activity, 3, 22. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1479-5868-3-22 Pivonka, E., & Kessler, R. (1995). Fruit and vegetable intake in the
Richards, J. M., Butler, E. A., & Gross, J. J. (2003). Emotion regulation in United States: The baseline survey of the Five A Day for Better Health
romantic relationships: The cognitive consequences of concealing feel- Program. American Journal of Health Promotion, 9, 352–360. http://dx
ings. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 20, 599 – 620. http:// .doi.org/10.4278/0890-1171-9.5.352
dx.doi.org/10.1177/02654075030205002 Szasz, P. L., Szentagotai, A., & Hofmann, S. G. (2012). Effects of emotion
Richards, J. M., & Gross, J. J. (1999). Composure at any cost? The regulation strategies on smoking craving, attentional bias, and task
cognitive consequences of emotion suppression. Personality and Social persistence. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 50, 333–340. http://dx
Psychology Bulletin, 25, 1033–1044. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ .doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2012.02.010
01461672992511010 Tackman, A. M., & Srivastava, S. (2016). Social responses to expressive
Richards, J. M., & Gross, J. J. (2000). Emotion regulation and memory: suppression: The role of personality judgments. Journal of Personality
The cognitive costs of keeping one’s cool. Journal of Personality and and Social Psychology, 110, 574 –591. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
Social Psychology, 79, 410 – 424. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514 pspp0000053
.79.3.410 Tanofsky, M. B., Wilfley, D. E., Spurrell, E. B., Welch, R., & Brownell,
Rhee, K. (2008). Childhood overweight and the relationship between
K. D. (1997). Comparison of men and women with binge eating disor-
parent behaviors, parenting style, and family functioning. The ANNALS
der. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 21, 49 –54. http://dx.doi
of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 615, 11–37.
.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-108X(199701)21:1⬍49::AID-EAT6⬎3.0.CO;
Rhoades, B. L., Greenberg, M. T., & Domitrovich, C. E. (2009). The
2-3
contribution of inhibitory control to preschoolers’ social– emotional
Tanofsky-Kraff, M., Ranzenhofer, L. M., Yanovski, S. Z., Schvey, N. A.,
competence. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 30, 310 –
Faith, M., Gustafson, J., & Yanovski, J. A. (2008). Psychometric prop-
320.
erties of a new questionnaire to assess eating in the absence of hunger in
Seghers, J., & Rutten, C. (2010). Clustering of multiple lifestyle behaviours
children and adolescents. Appetite, 51, 148 –155. http://dx.doi.org/10
and its relationship with weight status and cardiorespiratory fitness in a
.1016/j.appet.2008.01.001
sample of Flemish 11- to 12-year-olds. Public Health Nutrition, 13,
Taut, D., Renner, B., & Baban, A. (2012). Reappraise the situation but
1838 –1846. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1368980010000418
express your emotions: Impact of emotion regulation strategies on ad
Shaikh, A. R., Yaroch, A. L., Nebeling, L., Yeh, M.-C., & Resnicow, K.
libitum food intake. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 359. http://dx.doi.org/
(2008). Psychosocial predictors of fruit and vegetable consumption in
10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00359
adults a review of the literature. American Journal of Preventive Med-
icine, 34, 535–543. e11. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2007.12.028 Tavris, C. (1984). On the wisdom of counting to ten: Personal and social
Smith, T. M., Calloway, E. E., Pinard, C. A., Hennessy, E., Oh, A. Y., dangers of anger expression. Review of Personality and Social Psychol-
Nebeling, L. C., & Yaroch, A. L. (in press). Using secondary 24-hour ogy, 5, 170 –191.
dietary recall data to estimate daily dietary factor intake from the Family Telch, C. F., Agras, W. S., & Linehan, M. M. (2001). Dialectical behavior
Life, Activity, Sun, Health, and Eating (FLASHE) Study dietary therapy for binge eating disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
screener. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. Psychology, 69, 1061–1065. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.69.6
Sobal, J., Rauschenbach, B. S., & Frongillo, E. A. (1995). Obesity and .1061
marital quality. Journal of Family Issues, 16, 746 –764. http://dx.doi.org/ Tomiyama, A. J., Dallman, M. F., & Epel, E. S. (2011). Comfort food is
10.1177/019251395016006004 comforting to those most stressed: Evidence of the chronic stress re-
Sonnby-Borgström, M., Jönsson, P., & Svensson, O. (2008). Gender dif- sponse network in high stress women. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 36,
ferences in facial imitation and verbally reported emotional contagion 1513–1519. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.04.005
from spontaneous to emotionally regulated processing levels. Scandina- Trudeau, E., Kristal, A. R., Li, S., & Patterson, R. E. (1998). Demographic
vian Journal of Psychology, 49, 111–122. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j and psychosocial predictors of fruit and vegetable intakes differ: Impli-
.1467-9450.2008.00626.x cations for dietary interventions. Journal of the American Dietetic As-
Spoor, S. T. P., Bekker, M. H. J., Van Strien, T., & van Heck, G. L. (2007). sociation, 98, 1412–1417. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0002-8223
Relations between negative affect, coping, and emotional eating. Appe- (98)00319-8
tite, 48, 368 –376. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2006.10.005 Van Strien, T., Engels, R. C., Van Leeuwe, J., & Snoek, H. M. (2005). The
Srivastava, S., Tamir, M., McGonigal, K. M., John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. Stice model of overeating: Tests in clinical and non-clinical samples.
(2009). The social costs of emotional suppression: A prospective study Appetite, 45, 205–213. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2005.08.004
of the transition to college. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- Van Strien, T., Frijters, J. E. R., Bergers, G. P. A., & Defares, P. B. (1986).
ogy, 96, 883– 897. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0014755 The Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire (DEBQ) for assessment of
Stefanek, M. E., Andrykowski, M. A., Lerman, C., Manne, S., & Glanz, K., restrained, emotional, and external eating behavior. International Jour-
on behalf of the AACR Behavioral Sciences Task Force. (2009). Be- nal of Eating Disorders, 5, 295–315. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/1098-
havioral oncology and the war on Cancer: Partnering with Biomedicine. 108X(198602)5:2⬍295::AID-EAT2260050209⬎3.0.CO;2-T
14 FERRER, GREEN, OH, HENNESSY, AND DWYER

Vohs, K. D., & Heatherton, T. F. (2000). Self-regulatory failure: A Wiser, S., & Telch, C. F. (1999). Dialectical behavior therapy for Binge-
resource-depletion approach. Psychological Science, 11, 249 –254. Eating Disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 55, 755–768. http://dx
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00250 .doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1097-4679(199906)55:6⬍755::AID-JCLP8⬎3
Ward, A., & Mann, T. (2000). Don’t mind if I do: Disinhibited eating under .0.CO;2-R
cognitive load. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 753– Young, E. M., Fors, S. W., & Hayes, D. M. (2004). Associations between
763. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.78.4.753 perceived parent behaviors and middle school student fruit and vegetable
Wegner, D. M., Schneider, D. J., Carter, S. R., III, & White, T. L. (1987). consumption. Journal of Nutrition Education and Behavior, 36, 2–12.
Paradoxical effects of thought suppression. Journal of Personality and http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1499-4046(06)60122-X
Social Psychology, 53, 5–13. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.53
.1.5 Received April 21, 2016
Westat, Inc. (2015). Family Life, Activity, Sun, Health, and Eating (FLASHE) Revision received January 13, 2017
Study Methodology Report. Rockville, MD: National Cancer Institute. Accepted January 16, 2017 䡲

You might also like