Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 17

Ass~ssaiEST Copyright 0 199G by Psycliological Assessment

1996, Volume 3, Nunibcr 2, 115-133 Resources, Inc. All rights reserved.

EGO-STRENGTH, HARDINESS, SELF-ESTEEM,


ACY, 0PTIMISM, AND MALADJUSTMENT:
SELF-EFFIC
HEALTH-RELATED PERSONALITY CONSTRUCTS
AND THE “BIGFIVE” MODELOF PERSONALITY
Larry C. Bernard
Steven Hutchison
Alexander Lavin
Pamela Pennington
Loyola Marymount University

Six personality measures used in health psychology; the NEO Five-Factor Inventory
(NEO-FFI) criterion measures of stress, self-reported health status, and coping; and a
measure of social desirability were administered to samples of college students and adult
community volunteers (AT = 589) in a series of four confirmatory and exploratory factor
analytic studies. The hypothesis that the six independently developed personality mea-
sures of ego-strength, hardiness, self-esteem, self-efficacy, optimism, and maladjustment
would share common variance and that a hierarchical factor model with a single, higher-
order Health Proneness factor loading two lower-order factors-Self-Confidence and
Adjustment-would account for the covariance in these measures was tested against sin-
gle and three-factor models and confirmed. The factor model was examined with respect
to general personality as represented in the “Big Five” hlodel. Adjustment was related
negatively to NEO-FFI Neuroticism and positively to NEO-FFI Conscientiousness and
Agreeableness, whereas Self-Confidence was related to NEO-FFI Extraversion. None of
these relationships is extensive, nor does any one account for more than 40% of the vari-
ance. Evidence of the validity of Self-Confidenceand Adjustment was found in their mod-
erate relationships to measures of stress, health status, and coping, and in their weak rela-
tionships to social desirability and negative affectivity.

Psychologists have been interested in the i-elation- \\‘elsh, & D a h l s t r o m , 1975). More recently,
ship between personality and physical health ‘for Kobasa’s (1979) proposed construct of psychologi-
more than 50 years (e.g., Alexander, 1950; French cal hardiness renewed interest in the relationship
& Alexander, 1941). Thirty years ago, the relation- between personality, stress, a n d health (Suls &
ship b e t w e e n ego-strength and physiological Rittenhouse, 1990) and Bandura’s (1977) concept
responses received much study (e.g., see Dahlstrom, of self-efficacy prompted research into health
bchavior (O’Leary, 1985). At the same time, the
Correspondence concerning this article should b e Big Five Model-a general taxonomy of “normal”
addressed to Larry C. Bernard, Psychology Department,
Loyola Xlarymount University, Loyola Boulevard at \Vest p e r s o n a l i t y - a l s o received m u c h a t t e n t i o n
80th Street, Los Angeles, CA 90045. (Goldberg, 1990, 1992).

115

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Freie Universitaet Berlin on May 7, 2015


Bernard, Hutchison, Lavin, and Pcnnington

Health psychology is the most popular area of Hard iness


clinical research in doctoral programs (Sayette & Psychological hardiness (H) (Gentry & Kobasa,
hlayne, 1990), and when research accelerates in 1984; Kobasa, 1979) is also theorized to reflect
any field, it usually results in the proliferation of adaptational capacity and general coping ability,
new constructs and measures. At times like these, which may buffer the effects of stress on health
it behooves us to take a close look at this prolifera- (Iiobasa, hladdi, S: Courington, 198 1; Kobasa,
tion of new measures and consider issues of con- hiaddi, Puccetti, & Zola, 1985). Like Es, hardiness
vergent and discriminant validity-to evaluate the is negatively related to maladjustment (Bernard 8r
new constructs, their related measures, their inter- Belinsky, 1992; Funk & Houston, 1987) and neu-
relationships, and their associations with general roticism (Funk, 1992) and positively related to self-
dimensions of personality. If not, we risk con- esteem (Bernard & Belinsky, 1992).
stantly “reinventing the wheel.”
Self-Efficacj
First, we reviewed the literature and found several Self-efficacy (S-Ef) is a cognitive mechanism that
personality constructs that have been used in is also presumed to be related to a sense of control
health psychology research and have a putative and may reduce stress-induced autonomic arousal
relationship to health status. Ego-strength is the (Bandura, 1977, 1982, 1986). Self-efficacy is
older of these constructs; two other constructs- related to some of the same stress-induced physio-
hardiness and self-efficacy-have been proposed logical responses as Es: catecholamine secretion,
more recently. The. literature review also sug- autonomic arousal, and pain tolerance (Bandura
gested three additional personality constructs- Cioffi, Taylor, & Brouillard, 1988; Bandura, Taylor,
self-esteem, optimism, and maladjustment-that Williams, Meford, & Barchas, 1985; Loring,
may be related to the first three. Schoor, 8r Holman, 1985). In people 65 to 75
years of age, self-efficacy is positively related to
Health-Related Constructs
psychological and physical health for men and to
Ego-Streiigtli psychological health for women and, like hardi-
Barron’s (1953) Ego-Strength (Es)scale on the ness and Es, it is negatively related to maladjust-
Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory ment (Holahan, Holahan, & Belk, 1984).
(MMPI; Hathaway 8r hlcKinley, 1967) was one of
t h e first personality measures used to study Additional Constructs ’

health-related phenomena. Es has been described Self Esteem


as “adaptational capacity” and “situational con- Self-esteem would seem to reflect ego-strength,
trol” (Dahlstrom et al., 1975, p. 94), both compo- hardiness, and self-efficacy. As measured by the
nents of “coping,” which is believed to mediate Global Self-Esteem (GSE) scale of the Multidi-
stress, and psychological and physical health (e.g., mensional Self-Esteem Inventory (O’Brien &
Cohen & Lazarus, 1973; Folkman B Lazarus, Epstein, 1989), self-esteem is positively related to
1988; Lazarus & Folkman 1989). Es is negatively hardiness (Bernard & Belinsky, 1992) a n d ES
related to maladjustment (Dahlstrom et al., 1975) (O’Brien B Epstein, 1989). High self-esteem is
and positively related to intravenous glucose toler- associated iv i t h chi 1d re n’ s h e a1t h b e h av i o r s
ance (Pfaehler B Roessler, 1965), recovery from (Dielman, Campanelli, Shope, 22 Butchart, 1987)
mononucleosis (Greenfield, Roessler, & Crosley, and low self-esteem is associated with percep-
1959), and the ability to cope with a variety of tion of ill health in adults, especially females
stressors (Alexander, Roessler, & Greenfield, (Antonucci & Jackson, 1983).
1963; Greenfield, Roessler, B Crosley, 1959;
Roessler, Alexander, B Greenfield, 1963; Roessler, Optimistn
Burch, & Childers, 1966; Roessler, Burch, & The Generalized Expectancy for Success scale
Mefferd, 1967; Roessler & Collins, 1970; Roessler, (GES; Fibel 8r Hale, 1978) is a measure of opti-
Greenfield, B Alexander, 1964). mistic disposition, which is positively correlated

116

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Freie Universitaet Berlin on May 7, 2015


Health-RelatedConstructs

with self-esteem in children (Fischer & Leitenberg, with individual criteria, but there have been few
1986) and adults (hlearns, 1989). There is a mod- attempts to look at shared variance among several
est relationship between optimism and problem- measures. Such attempts could have implications
focused coping (Scheier, Ileintraub, & Carver, for the validity of the original measures and could
1986). Optimism is also negatively related to col- possibly reveal underlying health-related con-
lege students’ reports of prospective health prob- structs. These underlying constructs could be
lems (Scheier 8r Carver, 1985) and to self-reported helpful for understanding t h e relationship
physical symptoms and positively related to speed between personality and health.
of recovery from coronary artery bypass surgery
In addition, Funk (1992) reviewed psychological
(Scheier & Carver, 1987).
hardiness and concluded that it is correlated with
Mnladjustment neurotic ism. The term “neu ro t icism” was used
The College hfaladjustment Scale (Aft; Iueinmuntz, because of its relationship with somatic com-
1961) has frequently been used as a covariate in plaints (Allred & Smith, 1989). Neuroticism is a
health psychology research (see Funk, 1992). Aft is dimension of normal personality (Eysenck &
positively related to neuroticism (Funk &: Houston, Eysenck, 1968; Costa & McCrae 1985), one of the
1987) and negatively related to hardiness (Bernard Big Five personality factors, and has been identi-
&: Belinsky, 1992; Funk & Houston, 1987), ego- fied with negative affectivity in general and low
strength (Dahlstrom et al., 1975), and self-efficacy self-esteem, depression, and anxiety, in particular
(Holahan et al., 1984). (Costa 8: McCrae, 1985; hlcCrae, 1990). Similarly,
\Vatson and Pennebaker (1989) contend that the
Research Hypothesis and Rationale
observed relationship between personality mea-
Our literature review led us to suspect that these
sures-in particular, hardiness-and health is due
six constructs were interrelated and that measures
to neuroticism, or “negative affectivity.” If correct,
of them might share common variance. We car-
this would seriously challenge the construct valid-
ried out a series of studies to investigate two
ity of ego-strength, hardiness, and self-efficacyand
hypotheses: First, that measures of ego-strength,
necessitate a rethinking of t h e relationship
hardiness, self-efficacy, self-esteem, optimism, and
between personality and health.
maladjustment all would be intercorrelated, and
second, that a single latent factor could be This factor model would have iniplications for
extracted from their covariance. These hypotheses understanding the relationship between personal-
need further explanation. Careful evaluation of ity and health. Ego-strength, hardiness, self-
the theoretical constructs of each of these mea- esteem, optimism, and maladjustment were con-
sures and their interrelationships found in the lit- ceived as separate domains of personality and self-
erature review suggested that there might be two efficacy as a behavioral self-evaluation,hence the
covariance groupings: (a) a Self-confidence factor necessity of evaluating the model in relation to
that would load nieasures of self-esteem, opti- general personality such as that represented in the
mism, and self-efficacy;and (b) an Adjustment fac- Big Five Model. The Big Five hlodel also incorpo-
tor that would load measures of maladjustment, rates neuroticism, which allows for an assessment
ego-strength, and hardiness. We set out to confirm of the role of negative affectivity. Neuroticism has
this factor model in several samples, to relate it to often been treated as a nuisance variable in health
general dimensions of personality in the Big Five studies (Funk, 1992), but it may be important in
hlodel, and to address its convergent and discrimi- its own right. For example, individuals who are
nant validity. high in neuroticism might be “disease prone”
Our review had revealed a lack of such integrative (Friedman & Booth-Kewley, 1987) and vulnerable
studies. Most measures used in health psychology to stress (hfcCrae, 1990). However, if neuroticism
have undergone some degree of construct valida- were highly correlated with Health Proneness, the
tion, primarily consisting of bivariate correlations factor model would be redundant.

117

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Freie Universitaet Berlin on May 7, 2015


Bernard, Hutchison, Lavin, and Pennington

From the outset, we were mindful of Carlson’s 2. Hardiness was measured by the revised 50-
(1984) suggestion that subjects in personality item Hardiness scale (S.C.O. Kobasa, per-
research be diverse, particularly that they include sonal communication, February 14, 1989).
nonstudents, so we conducted this series of stud- This “t h i rd-gen er a t i o n ” hardiness s cale i s
ies using both college and noncollege samples. considered to be an improvement over earlier
The first in this series of studies was conducted to versions (Funk, 1992). Internal consistency
test: (a) whether, as we had come to suspect, the has been reported as .88 (S.C.O. Kobasa, per-
Es, Hardiness, Self-Efficacy, Global Self-Esteem, sonal communication, February 14, 1989)
Generalized Expectancy of Success, and scales and .83 (Bernard & Belinsky, 1992). This ver-
are intercorrelated; and (b) whether tlie hierarchi- sion permits calculation of separate scores
cal model would fit the data. on three subscales, which are combined to
yield a single score o n Hardiness (H):
Study 1 Commitment (approaching life with a sense
hle t hod of meaning), Control (perceiving one can
influence events), and Challenge (viewing
Part icipa 11ts change as an opportunity for growth instead
The first sample ( N =134) consisted of 92 under- of a threat). Although, in the first- and second-
graduate students at a private parochial university generation versions of this scale, combining
located in a large urban area who received partial subscale scores was questionable on psycho-
course credit for their participation (approxi- metric grounds, combining them in this
mately 65% of whom were Catholic) and 42 third-generation version is acceptable (Funk,
recruited noncollege volunteers ages 30 to 50 1992).
years. Mean ages of the college, noncollege, and
combined samples were 19 years (SD= 4 years), 41 3. General Self-Efficacy \vas measured by a spe-
years (SD= 8 years), and 26 years (SD= 12 years), cially constructed instrument. Self-efficacy
respectively. Approximately one third of t h e (S-Ef) is a cognitive mechanism that is also
sample irere male, and approximately one third presumed to be related to a sense of control
were minorities. (Bandura, 1977, 1982, 1986). Perceived self-
efficacy is a judgment about one’s capability
Procedure to perform tasks in a limited domain. Self-
Subjects received survey packets containing all of efficacy scales are usually constructed to
the measures in random order. A cover letter measure selfjudgments of efficaciousness
explaining t h e purpose of t h e study and an within a specific domain of interest following
informed consent form were attached to the out- “microanalytic methodology” (A. Bandura,
side. Subjects were assured that their responses personal communication, November 1990;
would be kept confidential. Bandura & Adams, 1977; for examples, see
Measures Bandura et al., 1988; Major et al., 1990). Our
The following measures were administered: objective was to measure a general tendency
to perceive oneself as efficacious, so the
1. Ego-Strength was measured by the 52-item usual measurement procedure was modified.
revised version of the Es scale from t h e
bliiinesota Alultipliasic Personality Inventory-2 Separate scales that tap self-efficacy in three
(MMPI-2; Hathaway & hlcI<inley, 1989). Es domains were constructed. The doni?’ins were
measures overall psychological adjustment intended to be viewed by subjects as having mod-
and the ability to cope with problems and erately high value (i.e,, importance) in their lives.
stresses (Graham, 1990). Internal consistency Therefore, two versions were designed, one for
was reported as .60 for males and .65 for the college and one for the noncollege samples.
females in the RlAlPI-2 normative samples The three college domains, representative items,
(Graham, 1990). and total number of items [in brackets] were the

118

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Freie Universitaet Berlin on May 7, 2015


Health-Related Constructs

following: Acadeniic/scAool performance, “Attending expectancy held by an individual that in most


class regularly,” “Keeping up with readings” [141; situations he/she will be able to attain
Piijs ica 1 f i t n ess/a t ii 1et ic p e r - or iii a rice , “Agi1ity ,” desired goals” (Fibel lk Hale, 1978, p. 924).
“Physical speed/quickness” [141; and Dating per- This echoes self-efficacy, suggesting a rela-
formance: “Introducing myself to someone,” tionship with it, although apparently this has
“Expressing affection” [20]. The three noncollege not been investigated. Each scale item com-
domains, representative items, and total number pletes the stem “In the future I expect that I
of items [in brackets] were the following: Il’ork per- will...”with a belief in some future success or
forinance, “Start work on time,” “Finish assign- failure that is endorsed in a Likert-type for-
ments ahead of time” [ 161; Physical fitness/athletic mat ranging from 1 (higlilj itnprobable) to 5
performance: same items as for students [14]; and (11 igl2 8 probable). Six-week test-retest reliability
Relationship performance, “Having pleasing sexual was reported as .89 for males and .SO for
relations,” “Able to compromise,” “Agreeing on females (Fibel 8c Hale, 1978).
future plans” [181.’ Subjects self-rated their confi-
6. i\faladjtistinent was measured by the College
dence in performing each task in a domain by
hialadjustment scale ( M t ) from the hihIPI-2.
recording a number on a scale ranging from 0
Alt consists of 41 items that distinguish
(Highl-y tiiicertain [cannot do at all]) to 100 (Co~n-
between college students who receive psycho-
pletely certain [can do it]). Perceived general self-
logical treatment and those who do not, and
efficacy was determined by summing across items
it is considered a valid method of identifying
for all three domains and dividing by the total
existing emotional problems (Kleinmuntz,
number of items.
1961). Graham (1990) does not recommend
In order to determine to what extent subjects val- its use with noncollege samples, but it has
ued the three domains, they were asked to indicate been used in studies of hardiness in adults
the value they placed on each using a scale ranging (e.g., Funk 8c Houston, 1987; Hull, Van
from 0 (Low value in m y life) to 100 (High value in Treuren, & Virnelli, 1987). Therefore, it Ivas
~njlye). The average of the students’ value ratings used here. However, at a minimum, internal
across the three domains was ’77.1 (SD = 12.3), and consistency would have to be demonstrated
the nonstudents’ average was 82.3 (SD = 12.7). for the noncollege sample as well as a pattern
These ratings indicate that the domains selected of intercorrelations with the other measures
were valued moderately highly by both students similar to that of the college sample. To
and nonstudents. maintain consistency with the other measures
and aid in interpretation, illt was reverse-
4. Self-Esteem was measured by the 10-item
scored in some of the analyses.
Global Self-Esteem (GSE) scale of t h e
hl u 1ti d imen s io nal Sel f-Est e e m Inventory Results
(O’Brien 8c Epstein, 1989). GSE is conceptu-
Internal Consistency (Reliability) and Zero-Order
alized as the highest level of self-evaluation,
Ziitercorrelations
involving feelings of significance as a person,
Table 1 presents t h e internal consistency
self-confidence, pleasure from past experi-
(Cronbach’s coefficient a) of each scale for the
ences, and expectancy of future successes.
college, noncollege, a n d combined samples
The scale has an internal consistency of .90,
(except for Self-Efficacy, which could not be com-
and it correlates 3 1 and 3’7, respectively,
bined because the scales had different items) and
with the Rosenberg (1965) and Eagly (1967)
the zero-order intercorrelations for the combined
self-esteem scales.
samples (there were no significant differences,
5. Opimism was measured by Fibel and Hale’s
(1978) 30-item Generalized Expectancy of
‘About 30% of the noncollege participants were not in a
Success (GES) scale. The GES scale is a mea- relationship and, therefore, used the same Dating domain
sure of optimism which is defined as “the items as the college sample.

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Freie Universitaet Berlin on May 7, 2015


Bernard, Hutchison, Lavin, and Pennington

Table 1
Internal Coiuistenc). (Coeflcieizt a)and Zero-Order Intercorrelations

Sample a Scale r
Students Nonstudents Combined
Scale (92 = 92) ( n = 42) (Ar= 134) S-Ef I.3 GSE GES Aft
H 83 91 87 2ja* 45*** 37**" 24% -58"**
S-Ef 90 94 - 26** 468g:F 25** -30""
ES 69 66 70 41*w 4 0 -59w*
~ ~
GSE 89 90 89 4 9 ~ " -52""~
GES 88 94 92 -31""
Aft 83 84 85
Note. Decimals are omitted. Combined a \\-as not computed for Self-Efficacy, because the lengths of the college and noncollege versions
were not identical. H = Hardiness scale; S-Ef = Self-Efficacy scale; Ex = Ego-Strength scale; GSE = Global Self-Esteem scale; GES =
Generalized Fkpectancy of Success scale; Aft = College hlaladjustmentscale.
*I < .01. **/I
< .001. ***I< .0001.

/I > .05, between the college and noncollege sam- Hardiness (H), and College Maladjustment (nlt)
ples). All of the scales had sufficiently good inter- scales predicted to load on an Adjustment factor.
nal consistency to proceed with further analyses. There were no restrictions placed on the correla-
Each of the intercorrelations was a one-tailed test tion between factors so that, if the model fit, Self-
of a separate hypothesis in which the direction of Confidence and Adjustment could load on the
the correlation was predicted. All 15 intercorrela- predicted higher-order factor.
tion coefficients are significant (at least p < .01).
Eight are .40 or larger, and the remaining 7 range The results of this analysis are shbnn in Table 2.
from .25 to .37. The proportion of variance This model adequately captured the covariance in
accounted for between any two scales ranges from the data. The goodness-of-fit index was equal to
5% to 34%. This is not exceptionally large, but it .98 (adjusted goodness-of-fit index = .92) and the
does support the hypothesis that all of the scales associated Chi-square test was not significant, x2
are interrelated; of course, the size of the correla- (8, N = 134) = 7.64, /I = .47. Self-confidence and
tion coefficients is restricted by the lowest value of Adjustment were both highly correlated with the
the scales' pairwise reliabilities. The use of Aft in a higher-order factor (r = .86 and .80, respectively),
noncollege population was questionable, but its suggesting that each represents a different aspect
internal consistency and its intercorrelations with of the same general construct we have labeled
the other scales in the noncollege subsample were Health Proneness.
similar to those in the college subsample, support- A second confirmatory analysis was performed in
ing the use of illt in the subsequent analyses.
order to compare this two-factor, hierarchical
Factor AnaZysis model to a single-factor model. The goodness-of-
A confirqatory factor analysis was conducted fit index for the single-factor model was equal to
using LISREL VII (Joreskog & Sorbom, 1981). -93 (adjusted goodness-of-fit index = .83), and the
The hypothesized two-factor hierarchical model associated Chi-square test was significant, (9, x2
was tested with the Global Self-Esteem (GSE), Self- N = 134) = 31.29, /I < .0001, indicating that a large
Efficacy (S-Ef), and Generalized Expectancy of proportion of covariance remained unexplained by
Success (GES) scales predicted to load on a Self- the single-factor model, and suggesting that the
confidence factor and the Ego-Strength (Es), two-factor hierarchical model fit the data better.

120

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Freie Universitaet Berlin on May 7, 2015


Health-RelatedConstructs

Table 2 Study 2
Hierarchical Factor iZlodel Loadings
hlethod
Higher-order factor
Factor Health Proneness Participants
A new sample (AT= 264) was obtained. It consisted
Adjustment 89 of noncollege volunteers ages 30 to 50 years (n =
Self-Confidence 87 92) and students ( n = 172) at a large urban state
Lower-order factors university who participated in return for partial
course credit. Approximately 66% of the sample
Scale Adjustment Self-Confidence
were female; 47% were European American, 22%
nrt -83 were Asian American, and 20% were Latin
ES 66 American; 29% were high school graduates, 59%
H 64 had attended some college, and 13% were college
GSE 80 graduates. Mean age was 42 years (SD = 8) in the
GES 51 noncollege sample and 20 years (SD = 3) in the
S-Ef 47 college sample.

A’ok Decimals are omitted. The LISREL model constrains rari- Meastires and Procediire
ables not predicted to load o n a factor to zero, so there are n o The same procedure and the same six personality
cross-factor loadings. S-Ef = General Self-Efficacyscale; 23 = Ego-
Strength scale; GSE = Global Self-Esteem scale; GES = measures used in Study 1 were used in this study;
Generalized Expectancy o f Success scale; 1\11 = College there were two additional measures:
Maladjustment scale; H = Hardiness scale.
1. The NEO Five-Factor Iizventory (NEO-FFI),
Form S, (Costa 8c hlcCrae, 1989) was used to
Discussion represent the Big Five factor structure of
The results of Study 1 provided initial support for personality (Goldberg, 1990, 1992), which
both hypotheses. The six personality measures appears to correspond closely to earlier mod-
were intercorrelated, and the hierarchical model els of personality (Digman, 1990). The NEO-
seemed to explain their covariance . However FFI contains scales that assess Neuroticism
promising, several issues remained. First, the fac- (N), Extraversion (E), Openness (0),Agree-
tor model would need to be cross-validated in a ableness (A), and Conscientiousness (C).
broader sample. Second, if it survived cross- 2. Social Desirability was measured by t h e
validation, it would be necessary to explore the Crowne-Marlowe Social Desirability (SD)
relationship between Self-confidence, Adjust- Scale (Crowne 8c hlarlowe, 1964), which was
ment, and general personality factors-something designed to identify individuals who describe
that has not been done with respect to the six themselves in favorable, socially acceptable
lower-order measures. If Self-confidence and terms, with approximately half of the items
Adjustment were closely related to Neuroticism, designed to be culturally acceptable, but
they would be redundant domains of personality probably untrue, and the other half designed
and unlikely to add any discriminant power to be undesirable, but true. Internal consis-
beyond it. Third, given the socially desirable value tency has been estimated at .88 (KR-20) for
placed on ego-strength, hardiness, self-efficacy, this scale (Crowne 8c hlarlowe, 1964).
self-esteem, and optimism and the undesirable
value placed on maladjustment, it would’also be Results
necessary to determine the relationship of Self- Descriptive Statistics and Reliability
Confidence and Adjustment to social desirability. Scale means, standard deviations, reliability coeffi-
We addressed these issues in Study 2. cients (Cronbach’s coefficient a), and zero-order

121

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Freie Universitaet Berlin on May 7, 2015


Bernard, Hutchison, Lavin, and Pennington

intercorrelations are shown in Table 3. There were models would have to be tested, and because the
no significant differences ( p > .05) between the number of subjects would have to be increased for
college and noncollege samples. The pattern of this additional testing. Under these conditions,
intercorrelations among the Es,H, Art, GSE, S-Ef, because a maximum likelihood approach is known
and GES scales was the same as that found in to be sensitive to sample size, it might produce too
Study 1. Two additional results are worth noting. many common factors (Kim 8c hlueller, 1978;
First, there were significant moderate zero-order Tabachnick 8c Fidell, 1989), which would increase
correlations between Neuroticism (N) and all six the probability of identifying an inappropriate
measures from Study 1. Among the other NEO- model by chance alone. Therefore, instead of
FFI scales, although the relationships were rather another confirmatory analysis, we performed
small, Extraversion (E) and Openness (0)had few three oblique exploratory factor analyses involving
significant correlations with the six measures, the NEO-FFI factors and social desirability with
whereas Agreeableness (A) and Conscientiousness the following three variable subsets: (a) the six
(C) were significantly correlated with many of individual measures (Es,H, S-Ef, GSE, GES, and
them. Aft); (b) the Self-confidence and Adjustment fac-
tors; and (c) the higher-order Health Proneness
The second result worth noting was the significant
factor.
moderate correlations between social desirability
and all of the six measures from Study 1. Social Principal components analysis indicated that the
desirability was also correlated with Neuroticism, data could be described with two factors for each
Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness on the of the analyses. All three of the subsequent analy-
NEO-FFI. This cautions that social desirability ses produced consistent findings and accounted
responding may inflate scores on all of t h e for similar proportions of the total variance in the
affected measures. data (given in parentheses): (a) the six individual
measures from Study 1 (56.3%), which provided a
Confirntatory Factor Aiiabsis baseline of total variance accounted for; (b) the
The factor model specified in Study 1 was again Health Proneness factor (55.9%);and (c) the Self-
tested using LISREL VII. The Es, Hardiness, and Confidence and Adjustment factors (55.3%).
Mt scales loaded on the Adjustment factor (at .66,
.64, and -.83, respectively), whereas the Self- The results of the factor analyses with Health
Esteem, Self-Efficacy,and Generalized Expectancy Proneness and the Self-confidence and Adjust-
of Success scales loaded on the Self-Confidence ment factors are shown in Table 4. Two clear,
factor (at .SO, .47,and 3 1 , respectively). Self- moderately correlated factors emerged in each
Confidence and Adjustment loaded .87 and .89, analysis with the NEO-FFI scales. On Factor 2,
respectively, on the single higher-order factor, Health Proneness had a moderate loading with
Health Proneness. All indexes suggested a n only NEO-FFI Extraversion, perhaps reflecting the
adequate fit of the model to the data, x2 (5, N = general social involvement of people prone to
264) = 7.64, p = .18; goodness-of-fit index = -98; healthy b ehav io r s . IVhen Self-Co n f id e nce and
adjusted goodness-of-fit index = .92; root mean Adjustment were entered with the NEO-FFI scales,
square residual = .03). the former also moderately loaded on Factor 2
with Extraversion, whereas the latter moderately
Exploratory Factor Atialjxis loaded on Factor 1 with a negative loading of
A second objective for Study 2 was to investigate Neuroticism and positive loadings of Conscientious-
the relationship of these factors to the NEO-FFI ness and Agreeableness. This suggests that Self-
personality factors and to social desirability. If a Confidence reflects general social involvement,
confirmatory approach were used, the additional whereas Adjustment reflects relaxed emotional
number of variables in Study 2 could be a source stability, organized goal-directedness, and a trust-
of problems, because many more competing ing and warm interpersonal orientation.

122

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Freie Universitaet Berlin on May 7, 2015


Table 3
Means, Slandard Deviations, Internal Consistency (CronDach's a) and Zero-Order Interconelalions

Scales NEO-FFI scales


Scale M SD a H S-Ef GSE GES MI SD N E 0 A C
Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Freie Universitaet Berlin on May 7, 2015
Bernard, Hutchison, Lavin, and Pennington

Table 4
Exploratov Factor Analjses of the Health Proneness, SelJ-Confidence,and Adjiutinent
Factors of the Hierarchical Model With tile Big Five Personalitj Factors

Oblimin rotated
factor loadings
Factor Scales 1 2
Single higher- NEO-FFI Conscientiousness 84 -20
order factor: NEO-FFI Neuroticism -57 -18
Health Proneness Health Proneness 21 60
NEO-FFI Extraversion 27 52
NEO-FFI Openness -07 30
NEO-FFI Acreeableness 29 12
Eigenvalue 2.26 1.10
% of variance 37.7 18.3
r = .34

Lower-order factors: NEO-FFI Neuroticisin -8 1 03


Adjustment and Adjustment 75 23
Self-Confidence NEO-FFI Conscientiousness 58 -07
NEO-FFI Agreeableness 37 04
Self-confidence -08 64
NEO-FFI Extraversion 22 53
NEO-FFI Openness 01 .23
Eigenvalue 2.73 1.14
% of variance 39.1 16.3
r = .48
Note. Decimals are omitted; loadings > .35 are in bold face. The College hlaladjustment (Ail) scale
was reverse-scored before Adjustment Factor scores were calculated.

Discussion of negative affectivity, or (c) the hypothesized


Study 2 cross-validated the factor model reported Health Proneness factor.
in Study 1 in a broader sample, suggesting that the The fact that the hierarchical model fits these
model is reliable. Although the six original mea- data, with nonsignificant residual variance, argues
sures were independently developed and, theoreti- against the existence of a general method factor.
cally, tap different phenomena, they covary. This However, in order to test for a method factor, we
model suggests that this covariance is captured in conducted another LISREL VII confirmatory fac-
the closely related factors of Self-Confidence and tor analysis specifying a nonhierarchical, three-
Adjustment. The factors' discriminant validity was factor model. If the covariance in the six original
further supported by their rather modest degree measures merely reflected method variance, there
of variance shared with some of the general per- should be a general method factor and then, per-
sonality factors represented in the NEO-FFI scales. haps, additional factors such as Self-confidence
T h e r e are at least three explanations for the and Adjustment. This three-factor model did not
covariance in the six original measures as it is fit the data as well as the hierarchical model, x2
represented in these two apparently reliable fac- (5, N = 264) = 42.755, p < .001; goodness-of-fit
tors: (a) method variance, (b) a mood disposition index was .92; adjusted goodness-of-fit index was

124

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Freie Universitaet Berlin on May 7, 2015


Health-Related Constructs

.79, indicating that the covariance in the original Study 3


sis measures reproduced by the hierarchical model hiet ho d
does not appear to reflect method variance.
Pa rf icipaii fs
The second possible explanation for covariance in Another sample (N = 191) was obtained. It con-
the six original measures has important theoreti- sisted of noncollege volunteers ( n = 131) ages 30 to
cal implications for studying the relationship 50 years, recruited in the same manner as before
between personality and health and the putative and students (n = 60) at a large state university
role of negative affectivity. A moderate-to-large who participated in return for partial course
loading of NEO-FFI Neuroticism and Health credit. Approximately 56% of the sample were
Proneness should be found if negative affectivity female; 49.2% were European American, 11.5%
were related to the common variance in these were Asian American, and 18.3% were Latin
health measures. This was not the case. However, American; 13%were high school graduates, 37.7%
Neuroticism did load negatively on Factor 1 with had attended some college, and 38.7% were col-
Adjustment, so negative affectivity may be related lege graduates. The mean age of the total sample
only to Adjustment and not Self-Confidence. An was 34.6 years (SD = 11.7).
indirect way of exploring the relationship of nega- iVenszires and Procedures. The same procedures as
tive affectivity and Adjustment is by evaluating the in Study 1 and Study 2 were followed, with the
role of social desirability. addition of the following three measures:
Social desirability was, in fact, significantly corre- 1. Stress was measured with the Life Experi-
lated with Neuroticism in Study 2, as it was with ences survey (Samson, Johnson, & Siege],
each of the six original measures. Interestingly, in 1978), which has been used in prior studies
Study 2, social desirability did not correlate signif- of health and stress (e.g., Roth, W e b e ,
icantly with Self-confidence (r = .11; N = 246; p > Fillingim, 8c Shay, 1989; Wiebe & McCallum,
.05).It did correlate significantly with Adjustment 1986). It is a self-report checklist that assesses
( r = -18;N = 246; fi < .Ol), but they share very little the occurrence and impact of various life
variance. Although this is admittedly an indirect experiences. The scale uses a 7-point rating
inference, given the social undesirability of nega- scale with scores ranging from -3 to -1 (nega-
tive affectivity, this argues against a major influ- tive impact), 0 (no impact), and 1 to 3 (positive
ence of social desirability or negative affectivity impact). Respondents indicate which of the
on Self-confidence or Adjustment and supports events have occurred in the past 12 months,
the factor's discriminant validity compared to the then rate the impact of these events. Two
original six measures. The possible relationship scores were calculated from this scale: one
between negative affectivity and Adjustment will derived from the sum of the negative impact
be discussed later. scores and the other derived from the sum of
the positive impact scores.
This leaves the third explanation for the covari- The second measure of stress was the Daily
ance in the original six measures captured in Self- Hassles scale (Lazarus 8: Folkman, 1989),
Confidence and Adjustment. The initial literature which also has been used i n studies of
review revealed several correlations between the somatic health. This is a 117-item self-report
six measures and some criteria representing the inventory with responses measured on a 4-
health-related variables of coping, personal con- point response scale: 0 (Did not o c c w ) , 1
trol, and physiological responsivity. One way to (Somewhat severe), 2 (Afoderutely severe), and 3
evaluate the presumed health-relatedness of Self- (Extremely severe). It was used to assess respon-
Confidence and Adjustment would be to deter- dents' perception of the severity of daily
mine whether they were related to variables repre- events (irritants) over the past month. A com-
senting stress, health status, and coping. This was posite scale score was obtained by summing
the objective of Study 3. the responses across all 117 items.

125

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Freie Universitaet Berlin on May 7, 2015


Bernard, Hutchison, Lash, and Pennington

2. Illness was measured by the Seriousness of Adjustment loading .91 on the single, higher-
Illness scale (IVyler, Alasuda, & Holmes, order factor. This time, however, Chi-square was
1968),a 127-item self-report checklist of com- significant, x2 (8, N = 191) = 31.59, /I < .001, sug-
monly recognized physical symptoms and dis- gesting some degree of misfit to the model.
eases. It, too, has been used in prior studies However, the significance of Chi-square is only
of health and stress (e.g., Roth et al., 1989). one among several ways to judge the degree of
Subjects were asked to indicate which symp- model fit. Because Chi-square is known to be sen-
toms or illnesses had occurred during the sitive to large sample sizes (Boomsma, 1982), and
past month or the past 12 months. Severity because the goodness-of-fitindexes and root mean
weights (determined in previous studies) for square residual were quite similar to those in
all endorsed items were summed to yield Studies 1 and 2, we believe that the hierarchical
total illness scores. factor model represents a good fit to the data. In
3. Coking was measured by the Ways of Coping addition, Byrne (1989) has suggested that, when
Questionnaire (Folkman & Lazarus, 1988), a comparing two competing models, the difference
66-item self-report measure, that assesses in Chi-square can be used as an indicator of
situation-specific coping strategies. It uses a improvement in model fit. IVhen the two-factor,
4-point scale, ranging from 0 (Does not a))b hierarchical model was compared to a competing
or not used) to 4 (used a great deal) to indicate single-factor model, x2
(9, N = 191) = 63.78, f~ <
the frequency of use of various coping tech- .001, the difference in Chi-square was significant
niques. items are grouped into eight coping (Ax2 = 32.19, p < ,001). The significant change in
strategies: Confrontive Coping, Distancing, Chi-square suggests that the two-factor, hierarchi-
Self-Controlling, Seeking Social Support, cal model provides a substantially better fit to
Accepting Responsibility, Escape-Avoidance, the data.
Planful Problem Solving, a n d Positive Zero-Order Correlations
Reappraisal. Separate scores were calculated
Correlations of the Es,Hardiness, Aft, Self-Esteem,
by summing those items related to a particu-
Generalized Expectancy of Success, and Self-
lar strategy. Additionally, a composite coping
Efficacy scales and the Self-confidence and
score was calculated as the sum of the eight
Adjustment factors with the measures of stress,
separate coping strategy scores. Although
health status, and coping are shown in Table 5.
there a r e concerns about this measure’s
validity (Stone, Greenberg, Kennedy-Moore, The Se1f-Co n f i d e n ce and Adjustment factors
& Newman, 1991), Folkman and Lazarus reflect the same pattern and extent of relation-
(1988) maintain that the assessment of cop- ships with the stress, health status, and coping
ing is embryonic and that this is one of the scales as the six original measures, which suggests
most widely used instruments. that the factors are valid replacements for them.
The Self-confidence factor is negatively correlated
Results and Discussion with stress (the Daily Hassles and negative Life
Confirmatory Factor Atialjsis Events scales), health status (the Seriousness of
LISREL VII was used to perform another confir- illness scale, past 12 months), and coping (the
matory factor analysis on the six health-related \Vays of Coping Escape-Avoidance and Distance
scales. Results were consistent with those reported subscales). The Adjustment factor is also nega-
in Studies 1 and 2 for the two-factor, hierarchical tively correlated with stress (the Daily Hassles and
model (goodness-of-fitindex = -95, adjusted good- negative Life Events scales), health status (the
ness-of-fit index = .87, root mean square residual = Seriousness of Illness scale, past month and past
.05). There was a correlation of .79 between the 12 months), and coping (the \trays of Coping Total
factors, with Self-Confidence loading .87 and and Escape-Avoidance, Distance, Accepting

126

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Freie Universitaet Berlin on May 7, 2015


Health-Related Constructs

Table 5
Comelatioils of Self-ConJdence and Adjustment and Their Coinponent Scales with Stress, Health Status, and Coping
Criteria

Adjustment Aft ES H Self-confidence GSE GES S-Ef


Stress
LE negative -16"
LE positive 05
Hassles -43***

Health status
SI past month -16
SI past 12 months -23""

Ways of coping
Confrontive -06 -02 -08 -01
Distance -19" -23""" -14 -14
Self-control -08 -12 -04 -03
Seeking support 05 -02 04 07
Accepting responsibility -15 -20"" -12 -07
Escape -32**:2: -35""" -2gew - 14
Planful 14 11 15 08
Reappraisal 09 06 11 01
Total -09 -14 -07 05
A'ofe. Decirnals are omitted. The Aft scale was reverse-scoredbefore Adjustment Factor scores were calculated to aid in interpretationof the
correlation coefficients. II = Hardiness scale; S-Ef = General Self-Efficacyscale; Er = EgoStrength scale; GSE = Global Self-Esteemscale; GES
= Generalized Expectancy of Success scale; Aft = College Maladjustment scale; LE = Life F-xpericnces scale; SI = Seriousness of Illness scale;
Hassles = Daily Hassles scale.
* p < .OJ. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

Responsibility, and Confrontive subscaIes). This One way to expIore this relationship is to deter-
suggests the factors may be correlated with some mine the lower-order factorial structure of Adjust-
health-related phenomena, and provides evidence ment. By evaluating the relationships among the
of their construct validity. individual illt, Es, and Hardiness scale items, the
However, in order to evaluate the divergent valid- role of negative affectivity in Adjustment could be
ity of Adjustment, we return to the potential role further delineated. This was the goal of Study 4.If
of negative affectivity (Watson 8: Pennebaker, many Hardiness and Es items were confounded
1989). The i W scale, which is construed as a indi- ivith negative affectivity, then there should be one
cator of negative affectivity and is moderately cor- o r more lower-order factors with item loadings
related with NEO-FFI Neuroticism, is a prominent from all three of the scales comprising Adjust-
component of the Adjustment factor. Funk (1992) ment. However, if most Hardiness and Es items
proposed that, rather than eliminating the infIu- were not confounded with negative affectivity, then
ence of neuroticism, it should be explored in items from the ilft scale should load on a single fac-
order to provide a better understanding of the tor, and items from the Hardiness and Es scales
relationship between stress and illness. should load on one or more additional factors.

127

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Freie Universitaet Berlin on May 7, 2015


Bernard, Hutchison, Lavin, and Pennington

Study 4 Discussion
These results suggest that Adjustment consists of
Method
three lower-order factors. The first is a direct rep-
Pa rticipa11 ts resentation of the Mt scale, whereas the second
The respondents from Studies 2 and 3 were com- consists of Es and Hardiness items and the third
bined, yielding a total sample of AT = 455. only of Hardiness items. Interestingly, the second
and third factors, without the Aft scale items, have
Procedtire
the same degree of association with the criterion
Respondents’ scores on the 143 individual items
measures of stress, health status, and coping as
of the Es, Hardiness, and illt scales were factor
Adjustment itself. This suggests that the relation-
analyzed. Although it would be ideal to have at
ship of Adjustment with the health-related crite-
least five cases per item, samples of about 300 sub- rion variables is ?tot dependent on the Mt items
jects are considered adequate, depending on such
and probably does not reflect the influence of
considerations as the number of predicted factors
negative affectivity.
and the magnitude of population correlations
(Tabachnick & Fidell, 1989). Because of the small
number of factors predicted in this analysis and General Discussion
the relatively reliable scale intercorrelations If‘e began this series of studies by examining six
demonstrated in the prior studies, the subjects-to- independently developed health-related constructs
item ratio was judged to be adequate. that appeared to be related. We conclude with a
better understanding of the relatiomhip between
Results personality and physical health. hIeasures of a
A preliminary principal components analysis was variety of constructs related to coping, control,
conducted and indicated that three factors should and health status-ego-strength, hardiness, self-
be retained. A subsequent orthogonal analysis was efficacy, self-esteem, optimism, and maladjust-
restricted to three factors and rotated to varimax ment-were all intercorrelated, and their covari-
criteria. The eigenvalues and percentage of total ance could be accounted for better by a latent hier-
variance (in parentheses) of the three extracted archical factor model than by the alternative mod-
factors were: 25.03 (17.5%) for Factor 1; 8.94 els tested. This hierarchical model suggests that
(6.3%)for Factor 2; and 5.18 (3.6%)for Factor 3. the original measures may not be independent. As
Factor 1 consisted of loadings ranging from .62 to predicted, the model consists of a single, higher-
.84 for all 41 of the Aft items, with no Hardiness order factor tentatively labeled Health Proneness
or ES items loading at > .20. Factor 2 contained because of the original six covarying measures’
loadings ranging from .32 to -65 for 24 of the 50 relationships with aspects of health. Health
Hardiness itenis (representing all three of its sub- Proneness loads two lower-order factors: Self-
scales), loadings ranging from .32 to .55 for 22 of Confidence and Adjustment. Both of these factors
the 52 Es items, and loadings ranging from -.21 to are more parsimonious and may account for more
-.35 for 9 of the 41 Aft items. Factor 3 consisted of the variance in health-related criteria than the
primarily of loadings ranging from .46 to .84 for 9 original scales.
items from the Hardiness scale (representing all We also investigated the convergent and discrimi-
three of its subscales)? nant validity of Self-confidence and Adjustment.
We then examined the correlations of Factors 2 Self-confidence and Adjustment are related to the
and 3 with the stress, health status, and coping broad personality factors of the Big Five hlodel.
variables from Study 3. Both factors had the same
*Six of the Hardiness items with larger loadings on the sec-
pattern and extent of relationship with these crite- ond factor also had loadings ranging from .20 to .24 on the
rion variables as the Adjustment factor itself. third factor.

128

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Freie Universitaet Berlin on May 7, 2015


Health-Related Constructs

Adjustment is related negatively to Neuroticism incidence of illness. The increase in variance


and positively to Conscientiousness and Agree- might reveal even stronger associations between
ableness, whereas Self-confidence is related to the hierarchical model and health.
Extraversion. However, these relationships are not
extensive-accounting for not more than 40% of References
the variance in Adjustment and Self-Confidence-
which suggests that these factors have specificity Alexander, A. A., Roessler, R., k Greenfield, N. S.
(1963). Ego strength and physiological responsivity 111: The
in their own right. This is consistent with the relationship of the Barron Ego-Strength scale to sponta-
results of hlarshall, Wortman, Vickers, Kusulas, neous periodic actil-ity in skin resistance, finger blood vol-
and Hervig (1994), who found that the NEO-FFI ume, heart rate, and muscle potential. Archives of Geiteral
Ps)ciiiat?y,9, 58-61.
factors accounted for less than 30% of the vari-
Alexander, F. (1950). Psyhoso~naticmedicine. New York:
ance in the health-related personality measures Norton.
they investigated. Other evidence for the conver- Allred, K. D., 8.Smith, T. 11’. (1989). The hardy person-
gent validity of Self-Confidence and Adjustment ality: Cognitive and physiological responses to evaluative
threat. Journal of Personalitj and Social PsjcholoD, 56, 257-
was provided by their correlations with some mea- 266.
sures of stress, health status, and coping. Antonucci, T. C., 8; Jackson, J. S. (1983). Physical health
\Vith regard to divergent validity, although and self-esteem. Fainil~and Cointnunit):Health, G, 1-9.
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy:Toward a unifjing the-
Adjustment would appear to be vulnerable to a ory of behavioral change. Psjchological Rmiew. 84, 191-215.
mood disposition of negative affectivity, this does Bandura, A. (1982). Self-efficacy mechanism in human
not seem to substantially affect its relationship to agency. American Psjchologist,37, 122-147.
measures of stress, health status, and coping. Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and
adion. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Finally, in contrast to the six original measures,
Bandura, A., Cioffi, D., Taylor, C. B., k Brouillard, ME.
neither Self-confidence nor Adjustment were (1988). Perceived self-efficacy in coping with cognitive stres-
much affected by socially desirable responding. sors and opioid activation. Journal of Personalitj and Social
Psjclzology, 55,479488.
There are some limitations of this study. First, we Bandun, A., Taylor, C. B., ii’illiams, S. L., hleford, I. N.,
employed no objective criterion measure of actual 8.Barchas,J. D. (1985). Catecholamine secretion as a fiinc-
health status or illness. Second, interpreting and tion of perceived self-efficacy. Journal of Conslilting and
Clinical Psjcholoa, 53,406-414.
identifying the sources of common variance in fac-
Barron, F. (1953). An ego strenfi-thscale which predicts
tor analytic data is fraught with difficulty. We response to psychotherapy-Jorcrnalvof Coifiulting P.&holos>.,
believe that some progress has been made in this 17,327-333.
area with respect to these six measures and in Bernard, L. C., k Belinsky, D. (1990, April). Hardiness
naming their common factors, but further refine- and self-Psteem: Independent constructs? Paper presented at the
meeting of the Western Psychological Association, Los
ment is necessary. In particular, we are somewhat Angeles, CA.
concerned about the factors with items that Bernard, L. C., & Belinsky, D. (1992). Hardiness, stress,
require a consistently negative response. These and maladjustment: Effects on self-reported retrospective
health problems and prospective health center visits.
items could be rewritten so that they do not risk Journal ofsocial Behavior and PersonaliQ, 8,97-110.
tapping an artifactual response set. Third, presum- Boomsma, A. (1982). The robustness of LISREL against
ably our samples have been healthy individuals, small sample sizes in factor analysis models. In H. Wold
which might serve to restrict the variance and, & K. Jiireskog (Eds.), Systetns under indirect observation
(pp. 149-173). New York: Elsevier North-Holland.
thus, the degree of relationship that could be Byrne, B. hi. (1989). A primer of LISREL. New York:
demonstrated between many of the measures that Springer-Verlag.
we investigated. Although this might have pro- Carlson, R. (1984). IVhat’s social about social psycliol-
vided a more conservative context in which to test ow?Where’s the person in personality research?Journal of
Personalitj and Social PsjcIioloB, 47, 1304-1309.
the hierarchical factor model, a necessary next Cohen, F., k Latarus, R. S. (1973). Active coping
step would be to see whether the model fits data processes, coping dispositions, and recovery from surgery.
from a sample of people experiencing a greater Psjchosomatic Aledicine, 3.5, 375-389.

129

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Freie Universitaet Berlin on May 7, 2015


Bernard, Hutchison, Lavin, and Pennington

Costa, P. T., Jr., 8: McCrae, R. R. (1985). The NEO Greenfield, N. S., Roessler, R., 8: Crosley, A. P., Jr.
Personalitj Inventog tnanual. Odessa, FL: Psychological (1959). Ego strength and length of recovery from infectious
Assessment Resources. mononucleosis. Joicnial of Neruoza and Afental Disease, 128,
Costa, P. T., Jr., 8 hIcCrae, R. R. (1989). A7E0 PvFFi 125128.
manual stip,bleinent. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Hathaway, S. R., 8: XlcKinley, J. C. (1967). The i\liiznesofa
Resources. Af tilt iph asic Perso nu 1i f y i n v e n t o r j ma n tia I. New York:
Crorvne, D. P., k hlarlowe, D. (1964). The approval Psychological‘Corporation.
motive: Studies in evahtatiue dependence. New York: IViley. Hathairay, S. R., 8: hlcKinley, J. C. (1989). The Afinnesota
Dahlstrom, I\’. G., IVelsh, G. S., 8: Dahlstrom, L. E. Multiphasic Personalitj inventoq. tnanual-2. hlinneapolis,
(1975). A n i\fiIlPI handbook: 1’01. II. Research a,bplications. XIN: National Computer Systems.
hlinneapolis, h l X University of hlinnesota Press. Holahan, C. K.. Holahan, C. J., 8: Belk, S. (1984).
Dielman, T. E., Campanelli, P. C., Shope, J. T., 8: Adjustment in aging: The role of life stress, hassles, and
Butchart, A. T. (1987). Susceptibility to peer pressure, self- self-efficacy.Healfh Psjcholog>.,3, 315-328.
esteem, and health locus of control as correlates of adoles- Hull, J. G., Van Treuren, R. R., 8: Virnelli, S. (1987).
cent substance abuse. Health Education Quarterb, 14, 207- Hardiness and health: A critique and alternative approach.
221. Journal of Personalitj and Social Psjchology, 53, 518-530.
Digman, J. hl. (1990). Personality Structure: Emergence Jiireskog, I(. G., S: Sorbom, D. (1981). LISREL VII.
of the Five-Factor hlodel. Annual Xeuim of Psyhology, 41, User’s Guide. Chicago: National Educational Resources.
441477. Kim, J-0..8: hlueller, C. I\‘. (1978). Factor analjsis:
Eagly, A. H. (1967). Involvement as a determinant of Statistical methods and practical issues. Newbury Park, C A
response to favorable and unfavorable information. Journal Sage.
of Personalitj and Social Psjchology, 7, 1-15. Kleinmuntz, B. (1961). The college maladjustment scale
Epstein, S. (1983). Aggregation and beyond Some basic ( A f t ) : Norms and predictive validity. Edricational a n d
issues in the prediction of behavior. Journal of Personalitj, Psjchological i\leasurement, 21, 1029-1033.
51.360-392. Kobasa, S. C. (1979). Stressful life. events, personality,
Eysenck, H. J., k Eysenck, S. (1968). i\fantcal for the and health: An inquiry into hardiness. Journal of Personalit).
Ejsenck Personalitj Inventory. San Diego, CA. Educational and Social Psjcfiolog, 37, 1-11.
and Industrial Testing Senice. Robasa, S. C., hladdi, S. R., 8: Courington, S. (1981).
Fibel, B., 8: Hale, It’. D. (1978). The Generalized Personality and constitution as mediators in the stress-ill-
Expectancy for Success S c a l e 4 new measure. Journal of ness relationship. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 22,
Corntilting and Clinical Psjchology, 46, 924-931. 368-378.
Fischer, hl., 8: Leitenberg. H. (1986). Optimism and pes- Kobasa, S. C., hladdi, S. R., Puccetti, hl. C., 8: Zola, XI. A.
simism in elementary school-aged children. Child (1985). Effectiveness of hardiness, exercise, and social sup-
Development, 5 7,24 1-248. port as resources against illness. Journal of Psjchosomatic
Folkman, S., 8: Lazarus, R. S. (1988). Alanital for the Research, 29, 525-533.
Ways of Coping Questionnaire. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Lazarus, R. S., 8: Folkman, S. (1989). Afaniral for the
Psychologists Press. Hassles and Uplijis Scales. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting
French, T. hi., k Alexander, F. (1941). Psychogenic fac- Psychologists Press.
tors in bronchial asthma. Ps~chmomaticMedicine Alonographs, Loring, R., Schoor, S., 8; Holman, H. R. (1985).
ATO. 4. Experimental evidence that changes in’ self-efficacy are
Friedman, H. S., 8c Booth-Kewley, S. (1987). The “dis- associated with changes in arthritis pain. Arthritis and
ease-prone personality”: A meta-analytic view of the con- Xheunatisin, 28 (Suppl.), S29.
struct. American Psychologist, 42,539-555. hlajor, B., Cozzarelli, C., Sciacchitano, A. hl., Cooper,
Funk, S . C. (1992). Hardiness: A review of theory and hl. Lynne, Testa, hi., 8 hlueller, P. hi. (1990). Perceived
research. Health Psyholog, 11, 335-345. social support, self-efficacy, and adjustment to abortion.
Funk, S. C., 8: Homton, B. K. (1987). A critical analysis Journal of Personalitj and Social Psychology, 59,452463.
of the hardiness scale’s validity and utility. Journal of hlarshall, G . N., IVortman, C. B., Vickers, R. R., Jr.,
Personality and Social Psyhology, 53,572-578. Kusulas, J. Itr., 8- Hcrvig, L. I<. (1994). The Five-Factor
Gentry, I\‘. D., 8- Kobasa, S. C. (1984). Social and psy- Xlodel of personality as a framework for personality-health
chological resources mediating stress-illness relationships in research.Journal of Personality and Social Psycholos>’,67, 278-
humans. In IV. D. Gentry (Ed.), Handbook of behavioral niedi- 286.
cine (pp. 87-116). New York Guilford. XlcCrae, R. R. (1990). Controlling neuroticism in the
Goldberg, L. R. (1990). An alternative “Description of measurement of stress [Special Issue: 11-IV. Advances in
Personality”: The Big-Five factor structure. Jotrrnal of measuring life stress]. Sfress hledicine, 6, 237-241.
Personalitj and Social PsycholoB, 59. 1216-1229. hlearns, J. (1989). Aleasuring self-acceptance:
Goldberg, L. R. (1992). The development of markers Expectancy for success vs. self-esteem.Journal of Clinical
of the Big-Five factor structure. Psjchological Assessmenf. 4, ~ , 90-397.
P s j c f ~ o l o45,3
2642. O’Brien, E. J., 8: Epstein, S. (1989). llfaniial for the
Graham, J. R. (1990). i\h\fPi-2: Assessing personalify and ilitrlfidiinensional Self-Esteem Inventory. Odessa, FL:
psjchopathology. New York Oxford University Press. Psychological Assessment Resources.

130

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Freie Universitaet Berlin on May 7, 2015


Health-Related Constructs

O’Leary, A. (1985). Self-efficacg and health. Behauior Scheier, hi. F., h Carver, C. S. (1985). Optimism, cop-
Research and Therakj, 23,437451. ing. and health: Assessment and implications of genenl-
Pfaehler, G. T., 8: Roessler, R. (1965). Ego strength and ized outcome expectancies. Health Psjchology,4, 219-247.
intravenous glucose tolerance. Journal of Psyhosotnatic Scheier, hl. F., 8; Carver, C. S. (1987). Dispositional opti-
Research, 8, 431439. mism and physical well-being: The influence of generalized
Roessler, R., Alexander, A. A., 8; Greenfield, N. S. outcome expectancies on health. Journal of Personalig, 55,
(1963). Ego strength and physiological responsivity I: The 169-210.
relationship of the Barron Es scale to skin resistance, finger Scheier, X i . F., Weintraub, J. K.,8: Caner, C. S. (1986).
blood volume, heart rate, and muscle potential responses Coping with stress: Divergent strategies of optimists and
to sound. Archives of General Psjcliiatq, 8, 142-154. pessimists. Journal of Persoiialitj and Social Psjchology, 51,
Roessler, R., Burch, N. R., 8; Childers, H. E. (1966). 1257-1264.
Personality and arousal correlates of specific galvanic skin Stone, A. A., Greenberg, XI. A., Kennedy-Moore, E., h
responses. Psyho)hjsiology, 3, 115-130. Newman, bl. G. (1991). Self-report, situation-specific cop-
Roessler, R., Burch, N. R., h hlefferd, R. B., Jr. (1967). ing questionnaires: I\’hat are they measuring? Journal o j
Personality correlates of catecholamine excretion under Personality and Social Psjcliology, GI,648-658.
stress. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 11, 181-185. Suls, J., & Rittenhousc, J. D. (1990). Models of linkages
Roessler, R., 8. Collins, F. (1970). Personality correlates between personality and disease. In H. S. Friedman (Ed.),
of physiological responses to motion pictures. Psjcho- Penotialitj and disease (pp. 155-167). New York IViley.
phjsiology, G, 732-739. Tabachnick, B. G., 8: Fidell, L. S. (1989). Using multivari-
Roessler, R., Greenfield, K . S., 8; Alexander, A. A. a t e s f a f i s f h(2nd ed.). New York: Harper 8; Row.
(1964). Ego strength and response stereotypy. Psycho- IVatson, D., 8: Pennebaker, J. I\‘. (1989). Health corn-
phjsiology, I, 142-150. plaints, stress, and distress: Exploring the central role of
Rosenberg, hi. (1965). Society and the adolacent self-image. negative affectivity. Psjchological Review, 96, 234-234.
Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Wiebe, D. J., 8: hlcCallum, D. hi. (1986). Health prac-
Roth, D. L., Wiebe, D. J., Fillingim, R. B., & Sliay, K. A. tices and hardiness as mediators in the stress-illness rela-
(1989). Life events, fitness, hardiness, and health: A simul- tionship. Healfh Psjchology, 5,425-438.
taneous analysis of proposed stress-resistance effects. Wyler, A. R., hfasuda, Xi., 8; Holmes, T. H. (1968).
Journal of Personality and Social Psyholog>, 57, 136-142. Seriousness of Illness Rating Scale. Journal of
Sarason, I. G., Johnson, J. H., 8; Siegcl, J. XI. (1978). Psychosomatic Research, 11,363-374.
Assessing the impact of life changes: Development of the
Life Experiences Survey.Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 46,932-946.
Sayette, hI.A., 8; hlayne, T. J. (1990). Survey of current
clinical and research trends in clinical psychology. Ainerican
P~~chologist, 45, 1263-1266.

131

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at Freie Universitaet Berlin on May 7, 2015

You might also like