Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 4

15th ICPhS Barcelona

Phonological typology in geographical perspective


Ian Maddieson
University of California, Berkeley CA, USA
E-mail: ianm@socrates.berkeley.edu

size of consonant and vowel quality inventories, the


ABSTRACT complexity of syllable structure, the occurrence of tone
systems, the distributions of front rounded vowels and
Typological patterns of phonological properties have been distinctive vowel nasalization, as well as of the occurrence
studied extensively for the insights they provide on the of laterals, uvulars, glottalized consonants, and notable
structure and evolution of human language. However presences or absences of several other kinds of segments
such patterns have not been much examined from the geo- in the phonological inventories of the languages surveyed.
graphical perspective applied to typological properties in
morphology and syntax. This paper provides brief discus- Some of the mapped properties show very strong patterns
sion and exemplification of the results of a longer-term of local geographical concentration while others are more
project to map phonological features of a large sample of dispersed. Shared characteristics often extend over large
languages, and points to some of the insights gained. areas. Marked phonological properties frequently co-occur
in the languages of particular regions of the world. The
remainder of this paper will discuss a few salient results.
1. INTRODUCTION
Typological patterns of segment inventories, syllable 2. MISSING /p/ AND MISSING /g/
structures, suprasegmental features and other phonological
properties have been studied extensively for the insights An example of contrasting localized versus dispersed
they provide on the structure and evolution of human distribution is provided by the patterns denoted here as
language. However such patterns have not been much ex- ‘missing /p/’ and ‘missing /g/’. It has long been noted
amined using the kind of large-scale geographical perspec- that if one of the major places of articulation is missing in
tive which has been applied to typological properties in a stop series it is likely to be bilabial if voiceless and
morphology and syntax [1]. This paper reports on the velar if voiced. In this study, a /p/ or /g / is only
mapping of typological properties in a sample of geo- considered missing if there is strong reason for it to have
graphically and genetically dispersed languages now ap- been expected to occur. Specifically, this means that the
proaching 600 in number, combining those of the UPSID language must have a simple voicing contrast in plosives
database [2] and the World Atlas of Linguistic Typology and the missing place must be represented in the opposite
[3], plus judicious additions in areas otherwise geographi- voicing series. Thus if the consonant inventory includes
cally underrepresented as well as languages covered by an the set of plosives in (i) below, it has a ‘missing /p/’; if it
ongoing project for phonetic research on endangered includes the set in (ii) it has a ‘missing /g/’; if it includes
languages. As is common in such studies the mapping the set in (iii) then both are missing.
reflects an idealized ethnolinguistic situation which
ignores the spread of major world languages and places i) t k ii) p t k iii) t k
indigenous languages in their recent historic locations. b d g b d b d
Each language is represented by one particular speech
variety, for example, English by a standard Southern In the sample of 565 languages examined ‘missing /p/’
British variety. Naturally the reliability of the and ‘missing /g/’ patterns are very close in frequency of
information available on such a large number of languages occurrence, there being 33 of ‘missing /p/’ and 34 cases of
varies considerably. In addition, to ensure uniformity ‘missing /g/’ and 3 languages with both segments
many choices of interpretation must be made based on the missing. Yet ‘missing /p/’ and ‘missing /g/’ have
descriptions at hand. It is believed that the large number noticeably different geographical distributions. ‘Missing
of data points should guarantee that any genuinely robust /p/’ occurs with a primarily localized distribution
distributional patterns will emerge. predominantly in the northern half of Africa and is almost
entirely absent from the Americas and the Eurasian
Viewing typological data in spatial terms can provide a landmass, as shown in Figure 1. ‘Missing /g/’, as shown
way of distinguishing between patterns which result from in Figure 2, occurs considerably more widely dispersed in
genuinely universal causes and those which may be better Africa, Asia and the Pacific islands and most strikingly
explained by local factors. Co-occurrence patterns of on both the eastern and western sides of the Americas,
different properties can also be examined geographically to areas that are often quite distinct in their phonological
visualize the strength or weakness of their association, as typology. Therefore ‘missing /g/’ more convincingly
well as to check whether an association might arise from calls for an explanation based on universal phonetic
bias in analytical choice. Among properties that have principles than does ‘missing /p/’. (For reasons of space
been examined in the light of these perspectives are the Figures 1 and 2 are the only maps included here.)

719 ISBN 1-876346-48-5 © 2003 UAB


15th ICPhS Barcelona

Figure 1. Distribution of languages with ‘missing /p/’ in a 565-language sample

Figure 2. Distribution of languages with ‘missing /g/’ in a 565-language sample

As often remarked, in /g/ the space between glottis and voicing contrast in plosives does not occur).
oral closure is much smaller than in /b/ or /d/.
Consequently, voicing is more likely to be extinguished The African concentration of ‘missing /p/’ undermines any
during the closure in /g/ due to loss of sufficient trans- assumption that there are equally general principles work-
glottal pressure difference [4]. This could lead to confu- ing against the combination of bilabial place and voice-
sion of /g/ with /k/, and over time to loss of the distinc- lessness. Instead, it suggests that one or more areal fac-
tion between the two sounds. Alternatively if the plosive tors may have operated to favor the initiation and/or
is pronounced with less than a complete closure (as often spread of this pattern within the circum-Saharan zone.
occurs in more relaxed speech) and consequently voicing These areal factors may have been as subtle as the conver-
is able to continue through its duration, the pronunciation gence on a regional norm for pronunciation of /p/-sounds.
norm may shift away from the plosive realization. Either For example, voiceless plosives might have come to be
path may provide a route by which /g/ is eliminated from typically pronounced with aspiration, that is, with the
the consonant set. Further, in a language undergoing a voiceless air flow continuing for some time after the clo-
process creating a series of voiced plosives, the difficulty sure has been released (distinctive use of aspiration is
of combining voicing with velar articulation may block largely absent from the area). Because the lips separate
the process from operating to change /k/ into /g/ under the more quickly at release than the other articulators [5], the
same conditions which change /p/ into /b/ and /t/ into /d/. noise generated by the release itself is less easy to distin-
Because of the aerodynamic facts, /g/ can be seen to be a guish from the noise of the following aspiration in /ph/
less favored plosive than /b/ or /d/, and this factor can be than in /th/ or /kh/. Hence only /h/, or a fricative such as
expected to operate in much the same way wherever /f/ might be identified by a listener (cf. Ancient Greek
voiced plosives occur (note that ‘missing /g/’ cannot occur /ph/ as the source of /f/ in modern words such as
in areas like Australia and much of East Asia where a ‘phonetics’), and the pronunciation norm would change.

ISBN 1-876346-48-5 © 2003 UAB 720


15th ICPhS Barcelona

It is also possible to speculate on rôles for more straight- sequences. For example, Fjian /mbamba/ ‘slope’ is
forwardly social factors, such as the prestige of Arabic, a usually considered to contain two prenasalized stops and
‘missing /p/’ language, over much of this area for centur- thus has the syllable structure CV.CV, but might have
ies, or the impact of cosmetic modification of the lips, been analyzed as CCVC.CV. To a degree, therefore,
once practiced among a number of the peoples of the Sa- evaluations of size of consonant inventory might in an
hel, the northern rain forest, and the Ethiopian highlands. arbitrary way be inversely correlated with evaluations of
syllabic structure. Comparing the distribution of
3. CONSONANTS AND SYLLABLES consonant inventory size and syllable structure complexity
provides a test of whether traditions among linguists
The number of distinctive consonants was surveyed in working in different regions might have biased analyses
565 languages. The range extends from 6 to 122, but the towards either greater syllabic complexity or larger
mode is 22, and the median 21. The languages were consonant inventories in different areas.
classed into five bins (small, moderately small, average,
moderately large and large consonant inventories). A total of 445 languages were included in a survey of
Languages with 19-25 consonants are classed as ‘average’. syllable structure. The most frequent type (58%) are those
These are found in most areas of the world, suggesting with moderately complex syllable structure, that is onsets
that this size truly is a representative of something typical of a single C or obtruent-glide or obstruent-liquid
for spoken human languages. The languages with larger sequence, and codas of only one C. This type is
or smaller inventories on the other hand display quite widespread, but is particularly frequent in Africa, the more
marked regional disparities in their distribution. easterly part of Asia and in much of Australia. Only 51
languages in the sample, about 11%, limit themselves to
Those with smaller than average consonant inventories ‘simple’ syllables (the universal syllable type (C)V).
predominate in Oceania including New Guinea, in South These tend to be distributed somewhere near the equator,
America and in the eastern part of North America, with in Africa, New Guinea and South America. Recall that
particularly strong concentrations of ‘small’ inventories this is a similar distribution to that found for languages
(less than 15) in New Guinea and the Amazon basin. A with small consonant inventories. There are 131
typological similarity with respect to consonant inventory languages (about 29%) which permit complex syllables of
size between the languages of New Guinea and Australia one or more types. Languages with complex syllable
is intriguing. The received idea is that the population structures are predominantly found in the northern two-
ancestral to speakers of today’s Australian languages thirds of the northern hemisphere, that is, in northern
reached the continent when New Guinea and Australia North America and northern Eurasia, where this type
were joined in the now partly-submerged landmass known actually dominates over the others. Here there is a degree
to geologists as the Sahul shelf [6]. After the land-bridge of overlap with the areas in which large consonant
linking New Guinea and Australia was severed around inventories tend to be more frequent, most especially in
7000 years ago, contact is believed to have been minimal the more northerly parts of the North American continent.
between Australian and New Guinea peoples except in the A smaller cluster of languages with complex syllable
immediate region of the Torres Straits. Might this structure is found in northern Australia.
similarity represent the conservation of a trait common to
languages spoken long ago when the lands were joined? The areal overlaps between small consonant inventories
and simple syllable structure and large consonant invento-
Languages with larger than average consonant inventories ries and complex syllable structures provide an interesting
are strongly represented in Africa, especially south of the example of how complexity in different areas of phono-
equator, as well as in an area in the heart of Eurasia, and logical structure may work together as mutual reinforce-
most spectactularly in the northwest of North America. ment rather than being mutually offsetting, or artifacts of
The languages in this latter area belong to several different analysis. Across the set of 440 languages for which both
language families with no demonstrable genetic consonant inventory size and syllable structure data are in-
relationship, including Eskimo-Aleut, Na-Dene, Salishan, cluded, there is a significant though not strong correlation
Tsimshianic and Wakashan. There is no evidence that the between these two measures of complexity (r 2 = .202,
predominance of large consonant inventories in this area is p < .0001). The languages with simple canonical syllable
the result of direct borrowing between these languages, structure have a mean of 19.1 consonants in their inven-
although cultural contacts between the peoples concerned tory, those with moderately complex syllable structure a
are in many cases intense and long-rooted. The situation mean of 21.7 consonants, and those with complex sylla-
is clearly different in part of the African region where large ble structures a mean of 25.8 consonants.
consonant inventories occur. Several Bantu languages
(part of the larger Niger-Congo family), such as Zulu and 4. VOWEL SYSTEMS
Yeyi, enlarged their consonant inventory by borrowing
clicks and other sounds which they did not previously use Three properties of vowel systems have been mapped, the
from neighboring languages of the Khoisan group, which number of distinct contrasting vowel qualities (abstracting
already had many consonants. away from features such as quantity, nasalization and
laryngealization), and the occurrence of front rounded and
The estimate of consonant inventory size is dependent, nasalized vowels. The size of vowel quality inventories
among other factors, on whether certain phonetic elements ranges between 2 and 14, with the mean being 6 and the
are analyzed as single complex segments or as consonant mode 5. They were binned into three categories, those

721 ISBN 1-876346-48-5 © 2003 UAB


15th ICPhS Barcelona

with 5 or 6 vowel qualities labeled ‘average’, those with fewer nasalized than oral vowel qualities and those with
fewer ‘small’ and those with more ‘large’. Of 557 equal numbers are differently distributed. In the Americas
languages surveyed for this purpose, just over 51% have equal-number systems predominate (33 of 56). In the rest
average vowel inventory size. Languages with smaller of the world there are usally fewer nasalized than oral
than average vowel inventories are especially concentrated vowels. Nasalization of vowels tends to function
in two areas, Australia and the Americas (particularly on differently in the languages of these areas. American
the west side of South America and the northern half of languages more often have a pattern where nasalization is
North America). Only a few languages in Africa, Eurasia shared by all vowels in a word or serves as a marker of
and the Pacific outside Australia have small vowel particular grammatical categories. In such cases each oral
inventories. Larger than average vowel quality inventories vowel very naturally remains paired with a nasalized
are especially concentrated in Africa, particularly in the counterpart. The difference may also be linked to the
linguistically diverse area roughly south of the Sahara and overall number of vowels. For example 23 out of the 28
north of the Equator, but they are also common over African languages have 7 or more vowel qualities, whereas
much of the Eurasian mainland, being most concentrated larger than average vowel inventories are rarer in the
in a zone covering South-East Asia and much of China. Americas. Overall the mean number of vowel qualities in
Larger vowel inventories are geographically associated the 51 languages with equal numbers of oral and nasalized
with the occurrence of vowel harmony in Africa, Europe vowel qualities is 5.45 and the mean number of vowel
and West Asia, as well as with short canonical word qualities among the 65 languages with fewer nasalized
length in the East Asian zone. The geographical than oral vowel qualities is 7.12. It is more difficult for a
perspective confirms a finding reported earlier, that vowel listener to distinguish between different nasalized vowels
inventory size is not inversely related to the number of than to distinguish between their oral counterparts [8].
consonants in a language [2]. For example, in the This factor can be expected to operate with particular force
languages of the African zone in which larger vowel when the number of vowels is larger. In most languages
inventories occur almost exclusively, the size of conso- with nasalized vowels their historical origin can be traced
nant inventories mirrors quite closely the overall propor- to reduction of an earlier sequence of a nasal consonant
tions of the five bins found worldwide. Overall, there is adjacent to a vowel. Such processes are likely to produce
no correlation whatsoever (r 2 = -.004) between vowel nasalized counterparts to each oral vowel. However, if the
quality inventory and consonant inventory sizes in the number of vowels is large, the distinction between all the
sample of 561 languages examined for this calculation. nasalized counterparts is unlikely to be maintained.

Front rounded vowels occur in just 37 out of 558 lan- REFERENCES


guages surveyed. These are typically languages with
‘large’ vowel quality inventories, and in many cases the [1] Dryer, M. Large linguistic areas and language samp-
history of these languages is well enough known that we ling. Studies in Linguistics 13: 257-292. 1989.
know the front rounded vowels were added to an inven-
[2] Maddieson, I. Patterns of Sounds. Cambridge
tory which previously did not include them and this de-
University Press, Cambridge. 1984.
velopment contributed to the enlargement of the vowel
inventory size. Almost 80% of these languages occur in [3] Comrie, B., M. Haspelmath, M. Dryer, & D. Gil,
the more northerly regions of Eurasia. There are a number eds. World Atlas of Linguistic Typology. In progress
of quite natural paths by which front rounded vowels can at Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary
evolve, including assimilation to adjoining front vowels Anthropology, Leipzig.
(as in German), assimilatory effects of front consonants
(as in some varieties of Chinese), ‘spontaneous’ fronting [4] Ohala, J. J. & C. Riordan. Passive vocal tract en-
of back vowels (as in French) perhaps by a ‘random walk’ largement during voiced stops. In J. Wolf & D. Klatt,
process [7], and probably by an extension of vowel har- eds, Speech Communication Papers presented at the
mony to additional vowel features (likely in Turkish, for 97th Meeting of the Acoustical Society of America:
example). In view of the different historical scenarios 98-92. ASA, New York. 1979.
which produced the front rounded vowels in various [5] Fujimura, O. Bilabial stop and nasal consonants: A
languages, it is quite striking that their occurrence is so motion picture study and its acoustical implications.
relatively concentrated in a particular geographic area. It J. of Speech and Hearing Research 4: 233-247. 1961.
seems possible that hearing sounds of this sort in some
languages of the area may have given critical support to [6] Allen, J., J. Golson & R. Jones. Sunda and Sahul:
phonetically natural processes in other languages whose Prehistoric Studies in South East Asia, Melanesia
end result is to add and maintain front rounded vowels in and Australia. Academic Press, London. 1977.
their inventories. Without support of this kind these pro- [7] Goldstein, L. Vowel shifts and articulatory-acoustic
cesses apparently much more rarely reach their conclusion. relations. In A. Cohen & M. P. R. van den Broeke,
eds, 10th International Congress of Phonetic
Distinctive nasalized vowels occur in about one fifth of Sciences, Vol IIA: 267-273. Foris, Dordrecht.
the languages (116 of 556 surveyed). Though absent from
some areas, e.g. the American North-West and Australia, [8] Wright, J. T. The behavior of nasalized vowels in the
they are relatively widespread around the world with perceptual vowel space. In J. J. Ohala & J. J. Jaeger,
particular concentrations in western and southern Africa eds, Experimental Phonology: 45-67. Academic
and the remainder of the Americas. Vowel systems with Press, Orlando. 1986.

ISBN 1-876346-48-5 © 2003 UAB 722

You might also like