Ethical Reflections Examining Reflexivity Through The Narrative Paradigm

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

405800

hop and ShepherdQualitative Health Research


QHRXXX10.1177/1049732311405800Bis

Ethics
Qualitative Health Research

Ethical Reflections: Examining


XX(X) 1­–12
© The Author(s) 2011
Reprints and permission:

Reflexivity Through the sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav


DOI: 10.1177/1049732311405800
http://qhr.sagepub.com
Narrative Paradigm

Emily C. Bishop1 and Marie L. Shepherd1

Abstract
Being reflexive and providing these reflections for public scrutiny is often considered a key element of ethical, rigorous
qualitative research. Prevalent conceptualizations of reflexivity, however, need interrogating and sharpening. We aim
to contribute to this by examining reflexive practice, and in particular researchers’ reflexive accounts, through the
lens of the narrative paradigm. Our aim is to demonstrate that acknowledging the role of narrative reconstruction in
reflexivity creates more ethical research, and that it is therefore crucial for researchers to more explicitly recognize
this. Both authors present an analysis of one particular exchange between interviewer and participant. This analysis
highlights that despite our best efforts at “doing reflexivity,” both immediately following and when reflecting back on an
interview, there are influential factors that escape our gaze. Reflections of the past are particularly imperfect. Without
fully recognizing this, we are not utilizing all the tools available for ensuring honest, ethical research.

Keywords
biographical analysis; narrative inquiry; reflexivity; research, qualitative; self

Emerging from key feminist analyses, it is now well Doucet, 2003; St. Louis & Barton, 2002), the extent to
established in the research methods literature that qualita- which research reports, including published articles, should
tive researchers’ subjectivity/standpoint/autobiography/ entail reflections on the impact of personal biography
self-biography can “filter, skew, [and] shape” their research remains contentious (see Kobayashi, 2003; Riach,
(Armstead, 1995, p. 628; Cotterill & Letherby, 1993; 2009). Unfortunately, researchers must tread carefully
DeVault, 1996; Harding, 1987, 1991; Letherby, 2003, 2005; to avoid their work being criticized as self-indulgent
Peshkin, 1988; Reinharz, 1992; Smith, 1987, 1990; Stanley (see DeVault, 1997; Letherby, 2000, 2003; Mykhalovskiy,
& Wise, 1983, 1993). Researchers are encouraged to iden- 1996; Sparkes, 2002).
tify, be sensitive to, and document how their “social back- Certainly, researchers should prioritize participants’
ground, assumptions, positioning and behavior” affect all voices and avoid placing themselves at the epicenter of
stages of the research process (Finlay, 2006, p. 21; Green, their research interpretations and output (Alvesson, 2003;
Franquiz, & Dixon, 1997; Letherby, 2005; Poland, 2002; Riach, 2009). It is only through systematic, ongoing reflex-
Silverman, 2004; Willis, 2010). It is also widely accepted ivity, however—including a continuing examination of
that the temporal nature of self-biography precludes our personal subjectivity—that we can avoid self-indulgence,
ability to replicate past research findings (see Arvay, 2003). that we can “avoid the trap of perceiving just that which
In this article, we aim to go beyond these conventional my own untamed sentiments have sought out and served
ideas by further highlighting the utility of viewing reflex- up as data” (Peshkin, 1988, p. 20). Through self-reflexivity
ivity through the narrative paradigm. Specifically, we we gain awareness of the “political/ideological agendas
emphasize that acknowledging the role of narrative recon- hidden in our writing” and insight (albeit limited) into
struction in the development of reflexive accounts cre-
ates more ethical research, and therefore it is essential ¹University of Tasmania, Launceston, Tasmania, Australia
for researchers to more explicitly recognize this.
Corresponding Author:
Although it is generally agreed that reflexivity, in some Emily C. Bishop, University of Tasmania, School of Sociology and
form and to some degree, is necessary (see, for example, Social Work, Locked Bag 1340, Launceston, Tasmania, 7250, Australia
Breuer, Mruck, & Roth, 2002; Knuuttila, 2002; Mauthner & Email: Emily.Bishop@utas.edu.au

Downloaded from qhr.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 20, 2016


2 Qualitative Health Research XX(X)

how factors such as our social location and personal assump- interview and with the presumed benefit of hindsight—
tions shape research encounters and interpretations (Hertz, when doing reflexivity, we can never be certain of our
1996, p. 7). Providing these insights alongside brief conclusions.
expositions of personal biography for public scrutiny dem-
onstrates moral integrity (Kvale, 1996, pp. 241-242),
a commitment to honest, transparent, ethical research Narrativity and Ethical Reflexivity
practice (Finlay, 2002; Hertz; Pini, 2004). This should be Whereas interviews often remain seen as portals to per-
encouraged rather than disparaged. sonal thoughts and subjectivities (Sarantakos, 2005), nar-
With our position on the necessity of reflexivity articu- rative theorists have emphasized that participants’ accounts
lated, we introduce our key argument: that a more explicit are narrative constructions (see Atkinson & Delamont,
focus on the reconstructed nature of reflexive accounts is 2006; Chase, 2005; Riessman, 1997, 2002, 2008). We
required. This argument emerged through observations of cannot know whether we access true subjectivity because
our own research experiences and observations of the we cannot read people’s thoughts, and because interviews
reflexive accounts of other researchers. Despite a synthe- can be opportunities for identity construction and perfor-
sis of systematic self-analysis and fieldwork analysis, we mance (see Langellier, 2001; Langellier & Peterson, 2004;
sensed that there had still been various influential factors Riessman, 1991, 1993). The narrative approach has been
at play that had escaped our awareness. When interview- valuable for understanding the nature of our data and for
ing, for example, it was likely that we held certain assump- engaging reflexively with our role in creating this.
tions that even deep sociological introspection (Sparkes, Postmodern perspectives view the self1 as fragmented,
2002) did not reveal, and that affected our interviewees in incoherent, and multiple (see Castells, 2002; Featherstone,
ways that we did not (and could not) see. Furthermore, 1995; Turkle, 1984, 1995). Narrative theorists however,
we were conscious that we had affected different partici- recognize that we experience a coherent and unified self
pants in different ways, and that even if we were to ques- (see Ezzy, 1998a; Lucius-Hoene & Deppermann, 2000;
tion them about their thoughts on the interview experience, Polkinghorne, 1991, 1996; Somers, 1994). The latter is far
such information is only imperfectly knowable. more useful to the reflexive researcher, because it is only
We found that rather than often or overtly acknowledg- through conceiving of the self as in some way locatable
ing this, researchers tended to present their reflexive analy- that the role of the self in the research can be examined.2
ses as objective reports, or truth claims about the ways they For narrative theorists, identity is constructed through
had shaped their research process and findings. This is experiencing life events, composing and telling a narra-
problematic for several reasons. It conflicts with qualita- tive of these. Furthermore, although we tend to view our-
tive epistemologies; specifically, it conflicts with the idea selves in terms of unified, fixed attributes, over time, and
that truth can never be confirmed (Kvale, 1996), it fails to in light of new experiences, we reconstruct our identities
fully appreciate the existence of forces that lie beyond our (see Ezzy, 1998a; Ricoeur, 1984; also see Polkinghorne,
grasp, and finally, it suggests a tendency for researchers 1991) and, related to this, our ideological frameworks
to position their reflexive accounts as more sociologically and subjective perceptions.
informed and thus more accurate than participants’ Social scientists are increasingly acknowledging the
accounts are typically considered. narrative construction of identity3; that life, and the self, is
As an entry point into our discussions, we briefly storied (see Ezzy, 1998a, 1998b, 2000a, 2000b; Hurwitz,
overview key elements of narrative identity theory and Skultans, & Greenhalgh, 2004; Mischler, 1992, 1999;
highlight the implications of this for reflexivity. Next, we Plummer, 1995; Riessman, 1991, 2002, 2003, 2008). Also,
demonstrate why the idea of the benefit of hindsight is as noted earlier, the temporal nature of the self and the
problematic for reflexivity, and following this, both authors consequences of this for replicating our research findings
provide a brief positioning account (see Arvay, 2003, are well recognized. The implications of narrativity for the
p. 163). These accounts outline the personal experiences ontological status of researchers’ reflexive accounts are
that motivated our respective research projects and provide yet to be fully examined, and require more focused atten-
insight into how our current self-stories differ from how tion, far beyond the account provided here.
we viewed ourselves during the data collection phases of Pertinent to our argument is the way that participants’
the research projects informing this article. We then each self-narratives have been increasingly referred to as per-
analyze one interview excerpt, discussing some of the sonal myths4 to highlight their constructed and temporally
possible ways in which elements of our social and per- located nature (see May, 1975, 1991; McAdams, 1997; see
sonal identity might have shaped this exchange. Our chief also Passerini, 1990). We suggest conceptualizing and dis-
aim is to demonstrate some of the shortcomings of cur- cussing researchers’ reflexive accounts in similar terms.
rent conceptualizations of reflexivity by highlighting that Rather than stable, objective analyses, they too are tempo-
despite our best efforts—both immediately following an rally situated, often written some time after the completion

Downloaded from qhr.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 20, 2016


Bishop and Shepherd 3

of the research, subjective and constructed in part through we cannot. Presenting reflections as objective accounts, or
guesswork—albeit theoretically informed guesswork. There even failing to fully acknowledge their temporality and the
is much we cannot know about our role in data produc- myriad factors that we cannot discern, is a form of fabrica-
tion, and our reflections can thus be thought of as mythical tion—a key facet of unethical research. To adhere more
constructions. closely to the ideal of producing ethical research then, it is
To further illustrate this, consider the interview process. important that as researchers we unambiguously engage
There is much to think about: careful observing and with and demonstrate a commitment to these ideas within
active listening without leaving lengthy pauses; providing our reflections. Enduring beliefs in the benefit of hind-
encouraging responses but not interrupting; maintaining sight, however, might be precluding this conceptual shift.
positive, open body language; creating engaging probes;
and balancing the tension between staying on topic and
allowing the participant to speak what they consider rele- Hindsight and Insight
vant (Minichiello, Aroni, Timewell, & Alexander, 1995; Mauthner and Doucet (2003) agreed that determining the
Walter, 2006). We are also managing our own identity per- impact of the researcher on the research is difficult and
formance. With so much to contemplate, we cannot simul- limited. We disagree however, with their explanation:
taneously undertake a comprehensive deconstruction of that many of these influential forces “only become
our impact on the interviewee and of how they are affect- apparent once we have left the research behind” (p. 425).
ing us, particularly because these socially shaped, inter- They argued that hindsight brings greater insight; “more”
personal factors and their outcomes are so dynamic. There reflexivity:
are also forces at play on an unconscious or embodied
level that, despite rigorous, ongoing analysis, we cannot Time, distance and detachment from our doctoral
become fully cognizant of. Even reinterviewing partici- work have allowed us to be more reflexive about
pants to investigate their perceptions of us, seeking to our processes. The security of a job, and a position
discover exactly how they see us as having affected them, within academia make it easier to admit and articu-
cannot address this gap in knowledge. This is because late the confusions, the tensions we felt and how
their accounts would be both subjective and narratively these manifested themselves in our research . . .
reconstructed. We cannot provide complete and accurate hindsight has enabled us to understand and articu-
reflections. late . . . what is influencing our knowledge produc-
Social researchers seek to create valid, meaningful, tion and how this is occurring. (Mauthner &
quality, and perhaps of most importance, ethical research. Doucet, 2003, pp. 418-420, emphasis in original)
Discussions of research ethics have often centered on
confidentiality, informed consent, opportunity to with- Their use of the term detachment is peculiar. It conflicts
draw, and not doing harm (see Habibis, 2010; Punch, with qualitative epistemologies that contest objectivity and
1994), and it has become taken for granted that honesty detachment (see Finlay, 2002; Finlay & Gough, 2003; Pini,
and integrity (see Kvale, 1996) are fundamental to ethical 2004), and with qualitative methodologies that encourage
research. Reflexivity is particularly valuable to qualita- intimacy and closeness to the data (see Birch & Miller,
tive research because it brings honesty to the fore, asking 2000; Denscombe, 1998; Lucius-Hoene & Deppermann,
us not to feign objectivity or reach post hoc conclusions, 2000; Oakley, 1981). Their account also conflicts with
but to acknowledge that multiple factors, including our relativist ontologies, which often underpin qualitative
personal narratives, shape the data we produce and our research, because they suggest that with distance and
interpretations of this data (Kemmis & McTaggart, 2003; detachment comes an ability to capture the past—as
Mays & Pope, 2000; see in particular McCorkel & Myers, though the past exists “out there,” independent of our
2003). Being completely honest about the situated, cocon- subjective and dynamic view of this (Neuman, 1991).
structed nature of our research findings does not “dissolve Other authors have taken a similar stance when discuss-
ethical tensions” (Cloke, Cooke, Cursons, Milbourne, & ing their reflexive journey (see Carolin, 2003; Doane,
Widdowfield, 2000, p. 133), but it does help to address 2003; McKay, Ryan, & Sumsian, 2003). Pini, for example,
some of the “moral tensions in interpretation and repre- argued: “Clearly, my reflexive journey on this issue is not
sentation” (Riach, 2009, p. 367). complete” (2004, p. 175). This suggests that she foresaw
Although this aspect of the relationship between reflex- an end to this journey; that at some point she would man-
ivity and honesty is established in the literature, a further age to accurately elucidate the myriad factors that shaped
step forward is required. Within our reflexive accounts we her research. Certainly time produces greater detachment
must be honest about the fact that even though we might from the data. Hindsight, however, does not make the
reflect deeply, analyze from multiple perspectives, and seek effects of the researcher–researched relationship more
industriously to unravel and explain the factors at play, apparent (Mauthner & Doucet, 2003). Rather, our

Downloaded from qhr.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 20, 2016


4 Qualitative Health Research XX(X)

memories and recollections become differently colored and continued to face in their sex lives. I aimed to challenge
restoried by our new experiences in the social world, by our the stereotypical view of young, rural youth (in particular)
newly acquired knowledge and attitudes, and by our shift- as “at risk,” and to highlight their risk-minimization strat-
ing personal and professional identities. Narrative episte- egies. I interviewed 31 young men and women who spent
mology thus helps illuminate that hindsight is not an open their childhood and adolescence in a rural area, and con-
window through which we can clearly review the past. ducted a narrative analysis of the data. Key findings
included the way participants avoided presenting them-
selves as “real” risk takers and sought to portray them-
Reflections as Self-Stories selves as moral and cautious and that their interpretations
Even when seeking to access and scrutinize our own sub- of risk and sex were highly moralized (Bishop, 2008).
jectivity, our personal values and persuasions (Peshkin, My interest lies chiefly in the implications of this for
1988), and to make these accounts of the self public in understandings of and approaches to preventing sexual
some form, elements of subjectivity remain beyond our risk behavior among young people (Bishop, in press).
consciousness, “like a garment that cannot be removed”
(Peshkin, p. 17). This has implications for the status of
both self-biographies and our reflexive practices. Because A Biographical Précis
it is integral to identity development (Plummer, 1995, see My research was closely connected to, but not consciously
also Rankin, 2002), people find many contexts and oppor- motivated by, my personal experiences. At age 18 I uninten-
tunities for telling self-stories. Storytelling is “doing iden- tionally became pregnant and chose to become a young
tity,” confirming an existing self or renarrating the self in mother. I remember that I felt negatively judged as lacking
a different way (Lee & Roth, 2004; Warren, 2006; see self-restraint and ambition. Then, when my son’s father
also Snow & Anderson, 1987; Snow & McAdam, 2000). left me, I constructed a particularly negative self-narrative
Researchers’ written reflexive accounts provide oppor- around tropes of betrayal, lost youth, and optimism.
tunities to position the self in a particular way. They are However, in light of a newfound engagement with feminist
as much stories of the present self as they are stories of theory during my undergraduate studies, I crafted a more
the past. Although true for all researchers, because iden- positive story. During data collection for my PhD degree
tity is renarrated over time, early career academics such as I worked hard, however, to be cognizant of and to mini-
ourselves are perhaps more invested (either consciously mize the impact of my feminist beliefs on the research
or unconsciously) in telling stories to construct a particu- process. Through ongoing self-scrutiny, particularly dur-
lar identity: that of the legitimate, competent, reflexive ing data analysis, I was able to recognize that despite my
researcher. beliefs about broader gender relations, gender was not the
In light of the above, our brief self-biographical accounts, most salient variable within my participants’ stories.
presented below, can be considered reconstructions of the I feel satisfied that I have arrived at my research knowl-
past. Phrases such as “I remember” and “I recall” should edge through a high degree of self-scrutiny. Nevertheless,
be interpreted from the narrative perspective. These I believe that both my past experiences and my feminist
accounts nonetheless provide an indication of our personal sympathies still permeated through to my research in
myth during data collection. We cannot be sure of our role some way, particularly my interview encounters, which
in shaping the excerpts analyzed. The factors that escaped I discuss further below. I have significantly redrafted
our perception then—in part because we could not access my self-narrative since achieving my doctorate and secur-
our participant’s thoughts—we can no better explicate ing an academic position. No longer do I hear that “You’re-
now. Furthermore, our identities have changed, shading a-disappointment” story playing in the background. This
our memories of these events. It is useful to know that we identity shift, as well as time away from data collection,
are both at different stages in our personal and professional has not, however, brought me closer to objective insights
lives. Emily Bishop is in her early 30s, completed her doc- into how I influenced my research. Rather, time and
toral research in November 2008, and currently holds an distance have made for even murkier recollections of the
academic position. Marie Shepherd is in her 50s, has a numerous hours spent talking to participants about their
30-year background in nursing, and plans to complete her pleasurable and problematic sex lives.
postgraduate studies in February 2013.
A Reflexive Account
Bishop’s Research Below are comments from 26-year-old Charlotte,5 who
Research Outline discussed some of her sexual risk experiences. Prior to this
point, we had been discussing her enjoyment of sex gener-
My research examined the stories told by young, rural ally, and teenage sex in particular. My question here was,
Australians to make sense of the risks they had faced and “So were you, like, thinking much about the risks when

Downloaded from qhr.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 20, 2016


Bishop and Shepherd 5

you were having sex, then?” [as a 16-17 year old]. Her university” (Armstead, 1995, p. 631). We suggest—but
response: emphasize that we cannot confirm—that for this inter-
view, my age, and specifically my youthful appearance,
I mean, I didn’t really think about the risks though, manner, and lexis, facilitated a relatively balanced dis-
like, intentionally “take” risks. It was just like, yeah tribution of power, at least more so than might have
you’re hot! I’m gonna [have sex with] you! You otherwise been achieved. Like many of my participants,
know what I mean. . . . Yeah it was only like, when Charlotte often punctuated her sentences with “you know
I got pregnant the first time, and I had to catch the what I mean” and “you know what it’s like.” Based on my
train all the way down to Bigtown that like, I really journals and an intimate (hopefully attached, rather than
sort of. I mean, that was awful . . . sitting in the detached) knowledge of the data, Shepherd and I argue that
waiting room and going through it all [termination these were statements rather than questions. Had I appeared
of the pregnancy]. I mean, young guys, god even older, part of a past when social norms and practices were
older guys [ha-ha], they just don’t get that stuff. different, Charlotte might have been less likely to assume
They don’t have to deal with that stuff, you know that I knew what she meant. Had she viewed me as
what I mean. more of an adult—a responsible authority figure—then it
is less likely that she would have considered me someone
When interviewing Charlotte I could sense that somehow, with whom to laugh about her sexual exploits and prow-
both my physical presence and my beliefs had some bear- ess. A different interaction, and a different account, might
ing on our interactions. I reflected afterward on whether it have ensued.
was my feminist standpoint—more specifically my belief I was (perhaps partly consciously) self-narrating and
that men’s sexual pleasure remains privileged and that performing a young, casual, nonacademic-type self-identity,
feminine sexuality remains constructed as passive and which was expressed through factors such as the way
receptive—was breaking through, and encouraging her to I dressed, sat, spoke, laughed, drank, and smoked. It is
tell her story in this particular way. I reflected on the inter- possible that Charlotte (even prior to any of my self-
view many times. I had not said anything explicitly refer- disclosures, discussed below) viewed me as the kind of
encing such ideas. Had I asked leading questions? I did not person who had shared similar experiences to her own,
think so. I actually asked very few questions, because the was comfortable speaking candidly about them, and was
interview flowed in a conversational manner (see Rubin & young enough to recall these. We do not argue, however,
Rubin 1995), and regardless, each line of questioning was that my youth led directly to rapport with Charlotte, or
integral to pursuing the interview topics. I did manage to with any of my participants, because it did not. More than
identify some instances where I had assumed a shared just age was at play, and like other subgroups, young peo-
meaning, rather than following up on a potentially ambig- ple are diverse (see Cohen, 1997; White & Wyn, 2004;
uous answer. Even immediately following the encounter, Wyn & White, 1997, 2004). It is possible (but not verifi-
however, I could not isolate my ideological approach as a able) that some of the young rural participants (particu-
key factor in shaping the data created. larly those who had never lived outside of their regional
Rather, there were multiple social, personal, and envi- areas) saw me as an urban, intellectual type, who could
ronmental factors at play. Reanalyzing this encounter not empathize with their experiences of growing up in an
from the present, I still cannot ascertain just how these isolated region. Indeed, this was evident in that, at times,
operated. In part, this is because many of these elements some of the participants were openly anxious, uncomfort-
are omnipresent, embodied (see, Ezzy, 2010; Sharma, able, and withdrawn, whereas others spoke comfortably
Reimer-Kirkham, & Cochrane, 2009), and to an extent for hours, appearing to enjoy the experience. The impact
unknowable. We cannot step outside of the self, to view of my personal/social identity on my research was never
the impact of the self. Also, because I cannot discover pre- patent, uniform, or straightforward.
cisely how Charlotte perceived me, any argument I make Age, of course, intersects with gender. If I was a man,
here regarding how I affected her account cannot be con- Charlotte might have been less likely to assume that I knew
firmed. Furthermore, our impact on each other was fluid; what she meant by men “don’t get that stuff.” Her assump-
it shifted throughout the interview as we both narrated fur- tion was also relatively accurate (as I clarified this with her
ther chapters of our stories. My reflexive account of this later in the interview). She was referring to the way that
interview encounter is therefore closer to a mythical con- young men often consider contraception to be the wom-
struction than to an objective truth. an’s responsibility, and that they lack intimate knowledge
Interviewing is often described as being characterized of the stress and trauma women endure, more than even
by a power imbalance (Karnieli-Miller, Strier, & Pessach, the fear that an unprotected sexual encounter has led to
2009). Some interviewees however, see academics as lack- a pregnancy, and of the experience of termination. Even
ing useful, real-world knowledge, and in these instances though gender is therefore likely to have been an influen-
researchers hold “little status and power outside the tial factor, we suggest that this worked in conjunction with,

Downloaded from qhr.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 20, 2016


6 Qualitative Health Research XX(X)

and cannot be disentangled from, the impact of some brief reinforce dominant, middle-class ideals. Within these par-
self-disclosures I made during the interview, namely that enting discourses, working-class (low socioeconomic)
my (amazing) child was the outcome of an unintended values and practices are positioned as inferior. The aims
teenage pregnancy. of my research are two-fold: first, to identify how par-
Researcher self-disclosure is a contentious issue ticipants attribute meaning to parenthood, and second, to
(see Cotterill, 1992; Dickson-Swift, James, Kippen, & better understand the ways in which these meanings are
Liamputtong, 2007; Miller, 1998). Cotterill, for example, shaped and negotiated through the interactions between
has argued that participants do not necessarily want to child health nurses and parents. I employed both partici-
hear about interviewers’ private lives. Resistance to self- pant observation and in-depth interview methods. My
disclosure, however, limits the relationship between two ongoing analyses indicate first, that when nurses find
people and increases the imbalance and distance between that their ideas about good parenting differ from those
researcher and interviewee (Wenger, 2002). I adopted expressed by the parents they encounter through their work,
Oakley’s (1981, p. 49) idea that intimacy is important for to avoid appearing critical and judgmental, they rarely con-
successful interviews and that there can be “no intimacy front them outright with this. Second, that by utilizing par-
without reciprocity” (see also Lucius-Hoene & Deppermann, ticular strategies, nurses nevertheless impart their opinions
2000). We suggest that, given the private nature of the on the matter in question, and thus shape parents’ values
topic, revealing to Charlotte (and to a number of other and attitudes.
participants) that I had engaged in the type of practices
that I was probing into, assisted her to voice her “risky”
(condomless/unprotected) behaviors, because it demon- A Biographical Précis
strated that I was unlikely to be shocked by or to judge My research relates directly to my employment experi-
her for these. It is impossible, however, to determine the ences: 30 years of working as a nurse, the last 15 as a child
extent to which I have reconstructed this rationale, how it health nurse. Prior to taking on further study, I remember
might reflect my current self-story. Perhaps I am uncon- holding on to a distinctly positive self-narrative, of a skilled
sciously seeking to justify my self-disclosures to under- and experienced nurse. Returning to university, however,
stand and present myself as an informed, thoughtful, and I realized that my nursing practices lacked a critical, social
capable researcher. framework, and I came to see myself as a novice, an
Although I was armed with self-knowledge and worked interloper in the world of social research. I worked hard
hard to avoid expressing my various beliefs (feminist or to examine and challenge my biomedical assumptions,
otherwise), it is inevitable that I also held certain assump- and have also endeavored to minimize the possible impact
tions that even this self-interrogation did not reveal, and of my past on the research process. In my field journal
that these assumptions fashioned the questions I asked and however, I documented a tendency during my early inter-
the responses that I gave to her comments. That I even asked views to sometimes feel inadequate, and therefore return
certain questions and therefore unconsciously excluded to the more familiar nurse identity, which I discuss below.
others illustrates the inescapable and imperfectly know- A few years in, I have again recrafted my narrative. My
able role of the researchers’ personal myth in shaping nursing practices are more critically informed, and I am a
research data. capable, reflexive social researcher. Nevertheless, reflect-
Charlotte and I have shared some similar experiences ing our analyses of Bishop’s interview, we argue that two
(both having taken sexual risks and both becoming teen- key things confound my reflections, or more specifically
age mothers) and a similar social position (in terms of age, my ability to accurately portray these past events here.
gender, ethnicity, and income). Our shared knowledge First, that I am viewing these encounters through the prism
of this “invited intimacy” (Birch & Miller, 2000) was of my current self-narrative, and second, that I cannot
therefore conducive to particular interactions and disclo- know how I was perceived by my participants. Researchers
sures. My reflections on this interview encounter are might choose, within their reflexive accounts, to view the
clouded, however, through the very process of looking past through the restricted lens of one or more particular
back. Distance and time have not brought me closer to the social or personal variables (such as age and gender), but
truth of exactly what happened. for the most part, there is a complex interplay between
multiple factors.
Shepherd’s Research
Research Outline A Reflexive Account
My account further illustrates the difficulties researchers
In our society, good parenting is chiefly measured through face when seeking to meaningfully disentangle the factors
the application of expert knowledges that reflect and that might have influenced their interview encounters.

Downloaded from qhr.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 20, 2016


Bishop and Shepherd 7

One of my first interviewees was 26-year-old Emma. The thus ethical choice (see, Eder & Fingerson, 2002; Holmes,
interview took place in her lounge room, as she breastfed 2010). My embodied nurse/mother identity was likely to
her baby, with the television on quietly in the background. become evident and to be an invisible force anyway—
After settling in, one of my first questions to her was, whether I revealed it to participants or not. Perhaps, know-
“What was life like for you before you had kids, family, ing I was also a nurse, Emma perceived me as a caring
that sort of thing? What did you do?” Her response was professional, trustworthy enough to be told her story of past
immediate and (I believe) frank: bad behaviour, but also as someone who would maintain
her clinical distance.
Before I had kids? I was—Well, I moved out of It is also possible that my gender, or more specifically
home when I was 15. Lived on the streets till I was the fact that we were both women, increased the likeli-
16, and then got heavily involved in drugs and alco- hood that Emma would tell her story in the way that she
hol and that. And like, it was only when I found out did. Had I been a man, for example, Emma might have
I was pregnant with twins that I, well, at that felt less comfortable openly breastfeeding in front of me
stage I was only [only knew I was] pregnant with as we spoke. Because I am a woman it is possible that she
one of them, but I was like, “Wow. Okay. This is viewed me through the prism of dominant constructions
not going to be good for my baby.” So um, just of femininity; as more likely than a man to be caring and
gave it ALL up. a good listener, and that this contributed to her many dis-
closures (similar to those provided above). Emma might
As noted, this excerpt is from one of my early interviews, have unconsciously assumed that, as a mother, I was more
when my sense of being a novice researcher was a key likely than a man (or a childless woman) to have the capac-
theme in my self-story. It is possible that my lack of con- ity to fully empathize with her sudden sense of connec-
fidence in this new researcher identity was noticeable to tion to and desire to protect her unborn child.
Emma in some way, and this actually helped to redress Perhaps, however, Emma did not hold such assump-
the interview power imbalance. When reflecting on the tions about femininity (it was not my aim to investigate
interview, I realized that I had also, however, at times per- these during the interview process). It might be that Emma
formed my child health nurse identity. Perhaps it was this actually perceived me as too old to remember the pain and
which chiefly shaped the confessional nature of her story, beauty of childbirth and breastfeeding, or as too different
as evidenced in the exchange above? As a nurse, I was (in terms of education and social position) to empathize
used to listening to people. This was usually undertaken, with her. The forces that were at play are more likely to
however, to treat, to provide support, information, and have been a synthesis of multiple factors, including both
advice, and to develop a plan for the delivery of care, gender and age. I was not just any woman. I revealed
which is obviously not the role of the social researcher. (both explicitly and through appearance and speech)
I found that on occasions during this interview, when that I was an older woman, with great experience in a car-
asked specifically by Emma, I provided my professional ing profession. Maybe on some level she perceived me as
opinion and advice. a kindly mother figure; someone with experiential knowl-
By disclosing my clinical background from the out- edge of life’s trials and tribulations; someone willing to
set, I might have encouraged Emma’s questions to some listen and likely to offer supportive congratulations for her
extent. Like Bishop, I had a sound understanding of exist- transformation from risk-taking teen to protective mother.
ing debates over self-disclosure (see above), and had Ribbens (1998) however, has argued that it is unlikely for
sided with Eder and Fingerson’s (2002) position that first an interviewee who is also a mother to believe that
and foremost, researchers have an ethical responsibility researchers can avoid making judgments about them, so
toward reciprocity with their participants, particularly the maybe despite my assurances that I was not there to evalu-
more vulnerable, because of the inherent power imbalance. ate her mothering practices in any way, Emma felt that a
I nevertheless acknowledged Miller’s (1998, p. 62; see also level of judgment or appraisal was inevitable, and thus she
Liamputtong & Ezzy, 2005) point that we must consider sought to make this a positive one.
the possible implications of self-disclosures carefully; for It is also possible, of course, that the effects on Emma
example, how might they affect what participants “feel of my novice researcher and older child health nurse iden-
able to voice during an interview?” I considered the pos- tities were negligible. She might have seen me mainly as a
sibility that participants might have experienced a nega- highly educated, professional researcher; someone with a
tive encounter with a child health nurse or related service social status quite distant from hers. Later in the interview
provider, and what this could mean for the encounter. I heard more of Emma’s story. She described feeling nega-
Ultimately, I determined that because my background was tively labeled as a single teenage mother, particularly
such a key part of my self-identity, revealing that I was a by hospital staff when her twins were born. It is pos-
child health nurse and mother of three was the honest and sible that Emma was not at all concerned with who I was

Downloaded from qhr.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 20, 2016


8 Qualitative Health Research XX(X)

(or appeared to be). Maybe she was utilizing the interview understand the cocreated, situated nature of research
as an opportunity to challenge the negative labels she felt findings. Although we do suggest that more advice on
imposed on her, and to document her story of redemption. exactly how researchers can come to identify their self-
Rather than a coparticipant in the discourse, I was the audi- story, their assumptions, and their epistemological stand-
ence, enabling the transformative power of telling self- point would be useful to early career researchers. Our aim
stories to flourish (Warren, 2006). has not been to redress this gap or to provide “how-to”
Presumably, we would have created different data had guidelines, but to contribute to a more nuanced under-
I not disclosed that I was a mother and child nurse. My standing of some of the issues reflexive researchers face,
age, gender, background, and self-story, however, were all and in doing so, to sharpen current perspectives on pro-
variables at play—all dynamically influencing our inter- ducing ethical qualitative research.
actions and the data produced. Bishop and I suggest that it It is one thing to know on an intellectual or intuitive
was the combination of “older/female/child health nurse/ level that we have an effect on other people—that we
interloper” that assisted me to form trusting relationships shape our research encounters and cannot be completely
with many participants. It is not possible to say in any objective in our interpretations of these; it is another thing,
definitive way, however, what exactly it was about me (if however, to document when and in exactly what way. We
anything), or the interview scenario, that Emma perceived, cannot know or articulate the effect we have on others
responded to, or was affected by—in part, because I do (which changes depending on the person). We cannot
not and cannot fully know what she thought about me. know how we are being perceived by our participants, or,
This further illustrates the need to construct a more of particular importance, exactly what self-stories and
nuanced conceptualization of reflexivity. assumptions they bring to the research. The researcher can
endeavor to make his or her comprehensive standpoint
and/or self-biography explicit for private and/or public
Ethical Reflexivity examination, but ultimately we cannot achieve this, and
Analyzing even the short interview excerpts above dem- we cannot evade its influence. Angen (2000, p. 383) artic-
onstrates our key point: that we cannot say with certainty ulated this:
which aspects of our past experiences, social background
and position, personal assumptions, self-narrative, appear- We cannot step outside of our intersubjective
ance, and behavior during the interview shaped our data involvement with the lifeworld and into some
collection and research as a whole. We cannot unravel or mythical, all-knowing, and neutral standpoint. . . .
quantify which of the factors outlined above was more By our very being in the world, we are already mor-
influential than another. Nor can we speak objectively ally implicated. Our values and beliefs will show
about how exactly these forces functioned, or the precise themselves in our actions whether we stop and think
impact of these. This is because they are often omnipres- about them or not.
ent and imperceptible, or embodied and difficult to articu-
late, and because of the way that we reconstruct narratives Hindsight and distance do not allow us to see the past.
of the past in light of new experiences and new identities. They provide a different view of this. Our memories are
Our analyses therefore illustrate our key argument: that a obscured and reimagined over time. Narrative epistemol-
more explicit focus on the reconstructed nature of reflex- ogy helps us to open up a space where we can recognize
ive accounts is required. They support our claim that we this; where we can be explicit about what we can and can-
are ethically obliged to ensure that reflexive accounts not achieve through our reflexive accounts. To improve
explicitly acknowledge that we cannot fully capture our our research skills and to enhance researcher transparency,
role in data production. we should continue to be reflexive; to throw as much light
What we have provided above are particular, tempo- as possible onto our research practices and processes. We
rally bound accounts that are as much stories of the past should do so, however, within a framework that more
as they are stories of our contemporary selves. They are overtly recognizes the reconstructed nature of our reflec-
inevitably (and to some extent strategically) attempts to tions. This will contribute to creating more nuanced and
portray ourselves as insightful, qualified researchers and ethical accounts of our qualitative research endeavors.
academics. We hope to have made a modest contribution
to current understandings of the partial and mythical Authors’ Note
nature of not only qualitative research findings, but of our This article is based on our doctoral research studies, undertaken
reflections on these. We have not, however, questioned in the School of Sociology and Social Work at the University of
the importance of reflexivity for enhancing qualitative Tasmania. Ethics approval was granted by the University Ethics
research. Reflexivity is a necessity, helping us to better Committee.

Downloaded from qhr.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 20, 2016


Bishop and Shepherd 9

Acknowledgments Angen, M. (2000). Evaluating interpretive inquiry: Reviewing


We thank all of the participants who graciously shared their the validity debate and opening the dialogue. Qualitative
private stories with us. Health Research, 10, 378-395. doi:10.1177/10497323000
1000308
Declaration of Conflicting Interests Armstead, C. (1995). Writing contradictions: Feminist research
The authors declared no conflicts of interest with respect to the and feminist writing. Women’s Studies International Forum,
authorship and/or publication of this article. 18, 627-636. doi:10.1016/0277-5395(95)80099-B
Arvay, M. (2003). Doing reflexivity: A collaborative, narrative
Funding approach. In L. Finlay & B. Gough (Eds.), Reflexivity: A
The authors received no financial support for the research and/ practical guide for researchers in health and social sciences
or authorship of this article. (pp. 163-175). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
Ashmore, R., & Jussim, L. (Eds.). (1997). Self and identity:
Notes Fundamental issues. New York: Oxford University Press.
1. Postmodern approaches to identity are of course widely Atkinson, P., & Delamont, S. (2006). Rescuing narrative
varied (see Butler, 1993a, 1993b; Danet, 1998; Halbers- from qualitative research. Narrative Inquiry, 16, 173-181.
tam, 1998; Harraway, 1991; Heelas, Lash, & Morris, 1996; doi:10.1075/ni.16.1.21atk
Touraine, 1997). Furthermore, it is not our aim to examine Birch, M., & Miller, T. (2000). Inviting intimacy: The inter-
the varied attempts to distinguish “self” from “identity” view as therapeutic opportunity. International Journal of
(see Ashmore & Jussim, 1997; Gergen, 1968; Jenkins, Social Research Methodology, 3, 189-202. doi:10.1080/
2004; Swann & Read, 1981). In line with most narrative 13645570050083689
approaches to identity, we use the terms self and identity Bishop, E. (2008). ‘I’m not a ‘real’ risk taker’: Constructing moral
interchangeably. identities through sexual storytelling. (Unpublished doctoral
2. Nevertheless, some researchers have tried to capture the dissertation). University of Tasmania, Launceston, Australia.
ways that their multiple or fragmented selves shaped their Bishop, E. (in press). ‘I’m not a ‘real’ risk taker’: Moral identity
research, depending on which self was given prominence in construction and sexual risk perceptions among a group of
a particular context (see, for example, McDowell, 1998; Pini, young rural Tasmanians. Sex Education: Sexuality, Society
2004; see also, Etherington, 2004). and Learning.
3. Although in the literature different authors have used slightly Breuer, F., Mruck, K., & Roth, W. (2002). Subjectivity and
different terms, they are referring to the same idea: the self- reflexivity: An introduction. Forum: Qualitative Social
narrative (Gergen & Gergen, 1984, 1988; Polkinghorne, Research 1, 3(3). Retrieved from http://www.qualitative-
1991); narrative identity (Ezzy, 1998a; 2000a; Lucius- research.net/index.php/fqs/article/view/822/1785
Hoene & Deppermann, 2000; Somers, 1994); and narrative Bruner, J. (1987). Life as narrative. Social Research, 54, 11-32.
self-understanding (Rosenwald & Ochberg, 1992). Butler, J. (1993a). Bodies that matter. New York: Routledge.
4. Accounts of the utility of thinking of identity as a “personal Butler, J. (1993b). Critically queer. Gay & Lesbian Quarterly, 1,
myth” initially arose, and continue to be discussed, within 17-32. doi:10.1215/10642684-1-1-17
psychoanalytic and social-psychological frameworks (see Carolin, M. (2003). Reflexivity: A personal journey during data
May, 1975, 1991; McAdams, 1997). These terms however, collection. Nurse Researcher, 10(3), 7-14.
have been adopted and extended by narrative theorists Castells, M. (2002). The Internet and the network society: Series
working within the social sciences. We argue that narrative editors preface. In B. Wellman & C. Haythornthwaite (Eds.),
understanding and terms such as mythbiography (Passerini, The Internet in everyday life (pp. xxix–xxxi). Oxford, UK:
1990) and personal myth capture the same meaning. This Blackwell.
view is in part shaped by the subtlety of accounts of narra- Chase, S. (2005). Narrative inquiry: Multiple lenses, approaches,
tive truth (see Bruner, 1987). voices. In N. Denzin & Y. Lincoln (Eds.), The Sage handbook
5. Pseudonyms are used throughout. Ethics approval was of qualitative research (3rd ed., pp. 651-680). London: Sage.
received for both research projects from the combined univer- Cloke, P., Cooke, P., Cursons, J., Milbourne, P., & Widdowfield, R.
sity and health department Social Sciences Human Research (2000). Ethics, reflexivity and research: Encounters with
Ethics Committee. homeless people. Ethics, Place & Environment, 3, 133-154.
doi:10.1080/713665889
References Cohen, P. (1997). Rethinking the youth question. London:
Alvesson, M. (2003). Beyond neopositivists, romantics and Macmillan.
localists: A reflexive approach to interviews in organisa- Cotterill, P. (1992). Interviewing women: Issues of friendship,
tional research. Academy of Management Review, 28,13-33. vulnerability and power. Women’s International Forum, 15,
Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/30040687 593-606. doi:10.1016/0277-5395(92)90061-Y

Downloaded from qhr.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 20, 2016


10 Qualitative Health Research XX(X)

Cotterill, P., & Letherby, G. (1993). Weaving stories: Personal Gergen, K. (1968). Personal consistency and the presentation
auto/biographies in feminist research. Sociology, 27, 67-79. of self. In C. Gordon & K. Gergen (Eds.), The self in social
doi:101177/003803859302700107 interaction (pp. 299-308). New York: Wiley.
Danet, B. (1998). Text as mask: Gender, play and performance Gergen, K., & Gergen, M. (1984). The social construction of
on the Internet. In S. Jones (Ed.), Cybersociety 2.0: Revis- narrative accounts. In M. Gergen & K. Gergen (Eds.), His-
iting computer-mediated communication and community torical social psychology (pp. 173-190). Englewood Cliffs,
(pp. 129-158). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Denscombe, M. (1998). The good research guide: For small scale Gergen, K., & Gergen, M. (1988). Narrative and the self as rela-
research projects. Buckingham, UK: Open University Press. tionship. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 21,
DeVault, M. (1996). Talking back to sociology: Distinctive con- 17-55. doi:10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60223-3
tributions of feminist methodology. Annual Review of Soci- Green, J., Franquiz, M., & Dixon, C. (1997). The myth of the
ology, 22, 29-50. doi:10.1146/annurev.soc.22.1.29 objective transcript: Transcribing as a situated act. Teachers
DeVault, M. (1997). Personal writing in social research: Issues of English to Speakers of Other Languages Quarterly, 31,
of production and interpretation. In R. Hertz (Ed.), Reflexivity 172-176. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/pss/3587984
and voice (pp. 216-228). London: Sage. Habibis, D. (2010). Ethics and social research. In M. Walter
Dickson-Swift, V., James, E., Kippen, S., & Liamputtong, P. (Ed.), Social research methods: An Australian perspective
(2007). Doing sensitive research: What challenges do quali- (2nd ed., pp. 89-121). Melbourne, Australia: Oxford Uni-
tative researchers face? Qualitative Research, 7, 327-353. versity Press.
doi:10.1177/1468794107078515 Halberstam, J. (1998). Female masculinity. Durham, NC: Duke
Doane, G. (2003). Reflexivity as presence: A journey of self- University Press.
inquiry. In L. Finlay & B. Gough (Eds.), Reflexivity: A prac- Harding, S. (1987). Introduction. Is there a feminist method? In
tical guide for researchers in health and social sciences S. Harding (Ed.), Feminism and methodology: Social sci-
(pp. 93-102). Oxford, UK: Blackwell. ence issues (pp. 1-14). Bloomington, IN: Indiana University
Eder, D., & Fingerson, L. (2002). Interviewing children and ado- Press.
lescents. In J. F. Gubrium & J. A. Holstein (Eds.), Handbook Harding, S. (1991). Who knows? Identities and feminist epis-
of interview research: Context and method (pp. 181-201). temology. In J. Hartman & E. Masser-Davidson (Eds.),
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. (En)gendering knowledge: Feminists in academe (pp. 100-
Etherington, K. (2004). Becoming a reflexive researcher: Using 120). Knoxville, TN: University of Tennessee Press.
ourselves in research. London: Jessica Kingsley. Harraway, D. (1991). Simians, cyborgs, and women: The rein-
Ezzy, D. (1998a). Theorising narrative identity: Symbolic inter- vention of nature. London: Free Association.
actionism and hermeneutics. Sociological Quarterly, 39, Heelas, P., Lash, S., & Morris, P. (1996). Detraditionalisation:
239-252. doi:10.1111/j.1533-8525.1998.tb00502.x Critical reflections on authority and identity. Cambridge,
Ezzy, D. (1998b). Lived experience and interpretation in narra- UK: Blackwell.
tive theory: Experiences of living with HIV/AIDS. Qualita- Hertz, R. (1996). Introduction: Ethics, reflexivity and voice.
tive Sociology, 21, 169-180. doi:10.1023/A:1023486611891 Qualitative Sociology, 19, 3-9. doi:10.1007/BF02392245
Ezzy, D. (2000a). Fate and agency in job loss narratives. Qualita- Holmes, M. (2010). The loves of others: Autoethnography and
tive Sociology, 23, 121-134. doi:10.1023/A:1005459701480 reflexivity in researching distance relationships. Qualita-
Ezzy, D. (2000b). Illness narrative: Time, hope and HIV. Social tive Sociology Review 6(2), 89-104. Retrieved from http://
Science & Medicine, 50, 605-617. doi:10.1016/S0277- www.qualitativesociologyreview.org/ENG/Volume16/
9536(99)00306-8 QSR_6_2_Holmes.pdf
Ezzy, D. (2010). Qualitative interviewing as an embodied Hurwitz, B., Skultans, V., & Greenhalgh, T. (Eds.). (2004).
emotional performance. Qualitative Inquiry, 16, 163-170. Narrative research in health and illness. Oxford, UK: BMJ
doi:10.1177/1077800409351970 Books.
Featherstone, M. (1995). Undoing culture: Globalization, post- Jenkins, R. (2004). Social identity (2nd ed.). London: Routledge.
modernism and identity. London: Sage. Karnieli-Miller, O., Strier, R., & Pessach, L. (2009). Power rela-
Finlay, L. (2002). ‘Outing’ the researcher: The provenance, pro- tions in qualitative research. Qualitative Health Research,
cess and practice of reflexivity. Qualitative Health Research, 19, 279-289. doi:10.1177/1049732308329306
12, 531-545. doi:10.1177/104973202129120052 Kemmis, S., & McTaggart, R. (2003). Participatory action
Finlay, L. (2006). Mapping methodology. In L. Finlay & research. In N. K. Denzin & Y. S. Lincoln (Eds.), Strategies
C. Ballinger (Eds.), Qualitative research for health profes- of qualitative inquiry (pp. 336-396). London: Sage.
sionals: Challenging choices (pp. 9-29). Chichester, UK: Knuuttila, T. (2002). Signing for reflexivity: Construction-
Wiley & Sons. ist rhetoric and its reflexive critique in science and tech-
Finlay, L., & Gough, B. (Eds.). (2003). Reflexivity: A practical nology studies. Forum: Qualitative Social Research, 3(3).
guide for researchers in health and social sciences. Oxford, Retrieved from www.qualitative-research.net/fqs-texte/3-02/
United Kingdom: Blackwell. 3-02knuuttila-e.htm

Downloaded from qhr.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 20, 2016


Bishop and Shepherd 11

Kobayashi, A. (2003). GPC ten years on, is self-reflexivity Miller, T. (1998). Shifting layers of professional, lay and per-
enough? Gender, Place and Culture, 10, 345-349. doi:10.1080/ sonal narratives. In J. Ribbens & R. Edwards (Eds.), Feminist
0966369032000153313 dilemmas in qualitative research: Public knowledge and pri-
Kvale, S. (1996). InterViews: An introduction to qualitative vate lives (pp. 58-71). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
research interviewing. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Minichiello, V., Aroni, R., Timewell, E., & Alexander, L.
Langellier, K. (2001). ‘You’re marked’: Breast cancer, tattoo (1995). In-depth interviewing: Principals, techniques and
and the narrative performance of identity. In J. Brockmeier analysis (2nd ed.). Melbourne, Australia: Longman.
& D. Carbaugh (Eds.), Narrative and identity: Studies in Mischler, E. (1992). Work, identity and narrative: An artist–
autobiography, self and culture (pp. 145-184). Amsterdam, craftsman’s story. In G. Rosenwald & R. Ochberg (Eds.),
The Netherlands: John Benjamins. Storied lives: The cultural politics of self-understanding
Langellier, K., & Peterson, E. (2004). Storytelling in daily life: (pp. 21-40). New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Performing narrative. Philadelphia: Temple University Press. Mischler, E. (1999). Storylines: Craft artists’ narratives of tran-
Lee, Y., & Roth, W. (2004). Making a scientist: Discursive ‘doing’ scription. Journal of Narrative & Life History, 1, 255-280.
of identity and self-presentation during research interviews. Mykhalovskiy, E. (1996). Reconsidering table talk: Critical
Forum: Qualitative Social Research, 5(1). Retrieved from thoughts on the relationship between sociology, autobiography
http://www.qualitative-research.net/index.php/fqs/article/ and self-indulgence. Qualitative Sociology, 19(1), 131-151.
viewArticle/655/1418 doi:10.1007/BF02393251
Letherby, G. (2000). Dangerous liaisons: Auto/biography in research Neuman, W. (1991). Social research methods: Qualitative and
and research writing. In G. Lee-Treweek & S. Linkogle quantitative approaches. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
(Eds.), Danger in the field: Risk and ethics in social research Oakley, A. (1981). Interviewing women: A contradiction in terms.
(pp. 91-131). London: Routledge. In H. Roberts (Ed.), Doing feminist research (pp. 30-61).
Letherby, G. (2003). Feminist research in theory and practice. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Buckinhgam, UK: Open University Press. Passerini, L. (1990). Mythbiography in oral history: The myths
Letherby, G. (2005). Personal narratives research group: we live by. London: Routledge.
Auto/biographical reflections or ‘how who we are affects Peshkin, A. (1988). In search of subjectivity–One’s own.
what we know’ [Occasional paper]. Leeds, UK: University Educational Researcher, 17(7), 17-21. doi:10.3102/
of Leeds. 0013189X017007017
Liamputtong, P., & Ezzy, D. (2005). Qualitative research meth- Pini, B. (2004). On being a nice country girl and an academic
ods (2nd ed.). Melbourne, Australia: Oxford University Press. feminist: Using reflexivity in rural social research. Journal of
Lucius-Hoene, G., & Deppermann, A. (2000). Narrative iden- Rural Studies, 20, 169-179. doi:10.1016/j.jrurstud.2003.08
tity empiricized: A dialogical and positioning approach to Plummer, K. (1995). Telling sexual stories: Power, change and
autobiographical research interviews. Narrative Inquiry, 10, social worlds. London: Routledge.
199-222. doi:10.1075/ni.10.1.15luc Poland, B. (2002). Transcription quality. In J. Gubrium &
Mauthner, N., & Doucet, A. (2003). Reflexive accounts and J. Holstein (Eds.), Handbook of interview research: Con-
accounts of reflexivity in qualitative data analysis. Sociol- text and method (pp. 629-649). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
ogy, 37, 413-431. doi:10.1177/00380385030373002 Polkinghorne, D. (1991). Narrative and self-concept. Journal of
May, R. (1975). Values, myths and symbols. American Jour- Narrative & Life History, 1, 135-153.
nal of Psychiatry, 132, 703-706. Retrieved from http://ajp Polkinghorne, D. (1996). Explorations of narrative identity.
.psychiatryonline.org/cgi/content/abstract/132/7/703 Psychological Inquiry, 7, 363-367. Retrieved from http://
May, R. (1991). The cry for myth. New York: Norton. www.jstor.org/stable/1448825
Mays, N., & Pope, C. (2000). Qualitative research in health care: Punch, M. (1994). Politics and ethics. In N. K. Denzin & Y. Lincoln
Assessing quality in qualitative research. British Medical (Eds.), Handbook of qualitative research (pp. 83-104).
Journal, 320, 50-52. doi:10.1136/bmj.320.7226.50 Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
McAdams, D. (1997). The stories we live by: Personal myths Rankin, J. (2002). What is narrative? Ricoeur, Bakhtin, and pro-
and the making of the self. New York: Guilford Press. cess approaches. Concrescence, 3, 1-12. Retrieved from http://
McCorkel, J., & Myers, K. (2003). What difference does differ- www.concrescence.org/index.php/ajpt/article/view/120/82
ence make? Position and privilege in the field. Qualitative Reinharz, S. (1992). Feminist methods in social research.
Sociology, 26, 199-231. doi:10.1023/A:1022967012774 New York: Oxford University Press.
McDowell, P. (1998). The women of Grub Street: Press, politics, Riach, K. (2009). Exploring participant-centred reflexivity in
and gender in the London literary marketplace. London: the research interview. Sociology, 43, 356-370. doi:10.1177/
Oxford University Press. 0038038508101170
McKay, E., Ryan, S., & Sumsian, T. (2003). Three journeys Ribbens, J. (1998). Hearing my feeling voice? An autobiograph-
towards reflexivity. In L. Finlay & B. Gough (Eds.), Reflex- ical discussion of motherhood. In J. Ribbens & R. Edwards
ivity: A practical guide for researchers in health and social (Eds.), Feminist dilemmas in qualitative research: Public
sciences (pp. 52-66). Oxford, UK: Blackwell. knowledge and private lives (pp. 24-38). London: Sage.

Downloaded from qhr.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 20, 2016


12 Qualitative Health Research XX(X)

Ricoeur, P. (1984). Time and narrative (Vol. 1, K. McLaughlin & cally speaking: Autoethnography, literature and aesthetics
D. Pellauer, Trans.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. (pp. 209-231). Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira Press.
Riessman, C. K. (1991). Beyond reductionism: Narrative genres Stanley, L., & Wise, S. (1983). Breaking out: Feminist con-
in divorce accounts. Journal of Narrative & Life History, 1, sciousness and feminist research. London: Routledge &
41-68. Kegan Paul.
Riessman, C. K. (1993). Narrative analysis. Qualitative Research Stanley, L., & Wise, S. (1993). Breaking out again: Feminist
Methods Series: Vol. 30. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. ontology and epistemology. London: Routledge.
Riessman, C. K. (1997). A short story about long stories. Jour- St. Louis, K., & Barton, A. C. (2002). Tales from the science
nal of Narrative & Life History, 7(1-4), 155-158. Retreived education crypt: A critical reflection of positionality, subjec-
from https://www2.bc.edu/~riessman/pdf/A%20short%20 tivity, and reflexivity in research. Forum: Qualitative Social
story%20about%20l-42CE2.pdf Research, 3(3). Retrieved from www.qualitative-research
Riessman, C. K. (2002). Doing justice: Positioning the interpreter .net/fqs-texte/3-02/3-02stlouisbarton-e.htm
in narrative work. In W. Patterson (Ed.), Strategic narrative: Swann, W., Jr., & Read, S. (1981). Self-verification processes:
New perspectives on the power of personal and cultural sto- How we sustain our self-conceptions. Journal of Experi-
rytelling (pp. 193-214). Lanham, MD: Lexington Books. mental Social Psychology, 17, 351-372. doi:10.1016/0022-
Riessman, C. K. (2003). Performing identities in illness nar- 1031(81)90043-3
rative: Masculinity and multiple sclerosis. Qualitative Touraine, A. (1997). What is democracy? Boulder, CO:
Research, 3, 5-33. doi:10.1177/146879410300300101 Westview Press.
Riessman, C. K. (2008). Narrative methods for the human Turkle. S. (1984). The second self: Computers and the human
sciences. London: Sage. spirit. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Rosenwald, G., & Ochberg, R. (1992). Storied lives: The cul- Turkle, S. (1995). Identity in the age of the Internet. In S. Turkle
tural politics of self-understanding. New Haven, CT. Yale (Ed.), Life on the screen: Identity in the age of the Internet
University Press. (pp. 9-21). New York: Touchstone.
Rubin, H., & Rubin, I. (1995). Qualitative interviewing: The art Walter, M. (Ed.). (2006). Social research methods: An Australian
of hearing data. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. perspective. Melbourne, Australia: Oxford University Press.
Sarantakos, S. (2005). Social research (3rd ed.). Basingstoke, Warren, J. (2006). Introduction: Performance ethnography:
UK: Palgrave Macmillan. A TPQ symposium. Text & Performance Quarterly, 26,
Sharma, S., Reimer-Kirkham, S., & Cochrane, M. (2009). Prac- 317-319. doi:10.1080/10462930600828667
ticing the awareness of embodiment in qualitative health Wenger, G. C. (2002). Interviewing older people. In F. Gubrium &
research: Methodological reflections. Qualitative Health J. A. Holstein (Eds.), Handbook of interview research: Context
Research, 19, 1642-1650. doi:10.1177/1049732309350684 and method (pp. 259-278). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Silverman, D. (Ed.). (2004). Qualitative research theory, White, R., & Wyn, J. (2004). Youth and society: Exploring the
method and practice (2nd ed.). London: Sage. social dynamics of youth experience. Melbourne, Australia:
Smith, D. (1987). The everyday world as problematic: A femi- Oxford University Press.
nist sociology. Boston: Northeastern University Press. Willis, K. (2010). Analysing qualitative data. In M. Walter (Ed.),
Smith, D. (1990). The conceptual practices of power: A feminist Social research methods: An Australian perspective (2nd ed.,
sociology of knowledge. Boston: Northeastern University pp. 407-432). Melbourne, Australia: Oxford University Press.
Press. Wyn, J., & White, R. (1997). Rethinking youth. London: Sage.
Snow, D., & Anderson, L. (1987). Identity work among the Wyn, J., & White, R. (2004). Negotiating social change: The
homeless: The verbal construction and avowal of personal paradox of youth. Youth & Society, 32, 165-183. doi:10.117
identities. American Journal of Sociology, 92, 1336-1371. 7/0044118X00032002002
Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/2779840
Snow, D., & McAdam, D. (2000). Identity work processes in the Bios
context of social movements: Clarifying the identity/movement Emily C. Bishop, PhD, BA, is a lecturer in the school of
nexus. In S. Stryker, T. Owens, & R. White (Eds.), Self, Sociology and Social Work at the University of Tasmania,
identity, and social movements (pp. 41-68). Minneapolis, MN: Launceston, Tasmania, Australia.
University of Minnesota Press.
Somers, M. (1994). The narrative constitution of identity: A rela- Marie L. Shepherd, BHSc, BN, RN, is a PhD student in soci-
tional and network approach. Theory & Society, 23, 605-649. ology at the University of Tasmania, School of Sociology and
doi:10.1007/BF00992905 Social Work, and a child and family health nurse for the
Sparkes, (2002). Autoethnography: Self-indulgence, or some- Department of Health and Human Services in Launceston,
thing more? In A. Bochner & C. Ellis (Eds.), Ethnographi- Tasmania, Australia.

Downloaded from qhr.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 20, 2016

You might also like