Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 13

Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy Copyright 2008 by the American Psychological Association

2008, Vol. S, No. 1, 101–113 1942-9681/08/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/1942-9681.S.1.101

Loss, Trauma, and Human Resilience:


Have We Underestimated the Human Capacity to Thrive After
Extremely Aversive Events?
George A. Bonanno
Teachers College, Columbia University

Many people are exposed to loss or potentially traumatic events at some point in their
lives, and yet they continue to have positive emotional experiences and show only
minor and transient disruptions in their ability to function. Unfortunately, because
much of psychology’s knowledge about how adults cope with loss or trauma has come
from individuals who sought treatment or exhibited great distress, loss and trauma
theorists have often viewed this type of resilience as either rare or pathological. The
author challenges these assumptions by reviewing evidence that resilience represents a
distinct trajectory from the process of recovery, that resilience in the face of loss or
potential trauma is more common than is often believed, and that there are multiple and
sometimes unexpected pathways to resilience.

Most people are exposed to at least one vio- The importance of protective psychological
lent or life-threatening situation during the factors in the prevention of illness is now well
course of their lives (Ozer, Best, Lipsey, & established (Taylor, Kemeny, Reed, Bower, &
Weiss, 2003). As people progress through the Gruenewald, 2000). Moreover, developmental
life cycle, they are also increasingly confronted psychologists have shown that resilience is
with the deaths of close friends and relatives. common among children growing up in disad-
Not everyone copes with these potentially dis- vantaged conditions (e.g., Masten, 2001). Un-
turbing events in the same way. Some people fortunately, because most of the psychological
experience acute distress from which they are knowledge base regarding the ways adults cope
unable to recover. Others suffer less intensely with loss or potential trauma has been derived
and for a much shorter period of time. Some from individuals who have experienced signif-
people seem to recover quickly but then begin icant psychological problems or sought treat-
to experience unexpected health problems or ment, theorists working in this area have often
difficulties concentrating or enjoying life the underestimated and misunderstood resilience,
way they used to. However, large numbers of viewing it either as a pathological state or as
people manage to endure the temporary up- something seen only in rare and exceptionally
heaval of loss or potentially traumatic events healthy individuals. In this article, I challenge
remarkably well, with no apparent disruption in this view by reviewing evidence that resilience
their ability to function at work or in close in the face of loss or potential trauma represents
relationships, and seem to move on to new a distinct trajectory from that of recovery, that
challenges with apparent ease. This article is resilience is more common than often believed,
devoted to the latter group and to the question of and that there are multiple and sometimes un-
resilience in the face of loss or potentially trau- expected pathways to resilience.
matic events.
Point 1: Resilience Is Different From
Recovery
Correspondence concerning this article should be ad- A key feature of the concept of adult resil-
dressed to George A. Bonanno, Department of Counseling ience to loss and trauma, to be discussed in the
and Clinical Psychology, Teachers College, Columbia Uni- next two sections, is its distinction from the
versity, 525 West 120th Street, Box 218, New York, NY
10027. E-mail: gab38@columbia.edu process of recovery. The term recovery con-
This article is reprinted from American Psychologist, notes a trajectory in which normal functioning
2004, Vol. 59, No. 1, 20 –28. temporarily gives way to threshold or sub-
101
102 BONANNO

threshold psychopathology (e.g., symptoms of emotions (Bonanno, Papa, & O’Neill, 2001).
depression or posttraumatic stress disorder The prototypical resilience and recovery trajec-
[PTSD]), usually for a period of at least several tories, as well as chronic and delayed disrup-
months, and then gradually returns to pre-event tions in functioning, are illustrated in Figure 1.
levels. Full recovery may be relatively rapid or In the loss and trauma literatures, researchers
may take as long as one or two years. By have tended to assume a unidimensional re-
contrast, resilience reflects the ability to main- sponse with little variability in possible out-
tain a stable equilibrium. In the developmental come trajectory among adults exposed to poten-
literature, resilience is typically discussed in tially traumatic events. Bereavement theorists
terms of protective factors that foster the devel- have tended to assume that coping with the
opment of positive outcomes and healthy per- death of a close friend or relative is necessarily
sonality characteristics among children exposed an active process that can and in most cases
to unfavorable or aversive life circumstances should be facilitated by clinical intervention.
(e.g., Garmezy, 1991; Luthar, Cicchetti, & Trauma theorists have focused their attentions
Becker, 2000; Masten, 2001; Rutter, 1999; primarily on interventions for PTSD. Nonethe-
Werner, 1995). Resilience to loss and trauma, as less, trauma theorists and practitioners have at
conceived in this article, pertains to the ability times assumed that virtually all individuals ex-
of adults in otherwise normal circumstances posed to violent or life-threatening events could
who are exposed to an isolated and potentially benefit from active coping and professional in-
highly disruptive event, such as the death of a tervention. In this section, I discuss how the
close relation or a violent or life-threatening failure of the loss and trauma literatures to ad-
situation, to maintain relatively stable, healthy equately distinguish resilience from recovery
levels of psychological and physical function- relates to current controversies about when and
ing. A further distinction is that resilience is for whom clinical intervention might be most
more than the simple absence of psychopathol- appropriate. This failure also helps explain why
ogy. Recovering individuals often experience in some cases clinical interventions with ex-
subthreshold symptom levels. Resilient individ- posed individuals are sometimes ineffective or
uals, by contrast, may experience transient per- even harmful.
turbations in normal functioning (e.g., several
weeks of sporadic preoccupation or restless The Grief Work Assumption
sleep) but generally exhibit a stable trajectory of
healthy functioning across time, as well as the Traditionally, mental health professionals in
capacity for generative experiences and positive the industrialized West have understood grief

Figure 1. Prototypical Patterns of Disruption in Normal Functioning Across Time Follow-


ing Interpersonal Loss or Potentially Traumatic Events.
LOSS, TRAUMA, AND HUMAN RESILIENCE AFTER ADVERSE EVENTS 103

and bereavement from a single dominant per- ments actually got worse relative to no-
spective characterized by the need for grief treatment controls, whereas the most clear ben-
work (Stroebe & Stroebe, 1991). The concep- efits were evidenced primarily with bereaved
tion of grieving as work originated in Freud’s individuals experiencing chronic grief (Neim-
(1917/1957) metaphoric use of the term to de- eyer, 2000). In summarizing these findings,
scribe the idea that virtually every bereaved Neimeyer (2000) concluded that “such inter-
individual needs to review “each single one of ventions are typically ineffective, and perhaps
the memories and hopes which bound the libido even deleterious, at least for persons experienc-
. . . to the non-existent object” (p. 154). Theo- ing a normal bereavement” (p. 541).
rists following Freud emphasized even more
strongly the critical importance to all bereaved Trauma Interventions and Critical
individuals of working through the negative Incident Debriefing
thoughts, memories, and emotions about a loss
(see Bonanno & Field, 2001). Although for centuries practitioners have
As researchers began to devote more atten- linked violent or life-threatening events with
tion to the bereavement process, however, it psychological and physiological dysfunction,
became apparent that, despite the near unanim- historically there also has been confusion and
ity with which mental health professionals en- controversy over the nature of traumatic events
dorsed the grief work perspective, there was a and over whether to consider psychological re-
surprising lack of empirical support for such a actions as malingering, weakness, or genuine
view (Wortman & Silver, 1989). What’s more, dysfunction (Lamprecht & Sack, 2002). The
recent studies that have directly examined the inclusion of the PTSD category in the Diagnos-
legitimacy of the grief work approach have not tic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders
only failed to support this approach but actually (3rd ed. [DSM–III]; American Psychiatric As-
suggest that it may be harmful for many be- sociation, 1980) resulted in a surge of research
reaved individuals to engage in such practices and theory about clinically significant trauma
(see Bonanno & Kaltman, 1999). A more plau- reactions. There is now considerable support for
sible alternative would be that grief work pro- the usefulness of interventions with individuals
cesses are appropriate for only a subset of be- meeting PTSD criteria. Cognitive– behavioral
reaved individuals (Stroebe & Stroebe, 1991), treatments that aim to help traumatized individ-
most likely those actively struggling with the uals understand and manage the anxiety and
most severe levels of grief and distress (Bon- fear associated with trauma-related stimuli have
anno et al., 2001). proved the most effective (Resick, 2001). Al-
The idea that grief work may characterize though outcome studies generally show few dif-
only the more highly distressed bereaved indi- ferences between treatments, there is some ev-
viduals (i.e., those exhibiting either the recovery idence for superior results with prolonged ex-
or chronic symptom trajectories) is further sup- posure therapy (e.g., Foa et al., 1999). The
ported by data indicating that the practice of essential components of exposure treatment
engaging a wide array of bereaved individuals usually involve repeated confrontations with
in grief counseling has proved remarkably inef- memories of the traumatic stressor (imaginal
fective. Grief-focused interventions typically exposure) and with situations that evoke unre-
target both acute or prolonged grief reactions as alistic fears (in vivo exposure; Zoellner,
well as the absence of a grief reaction (e.g., Fitzgibbons, & Foa, 2001).
Rando, 1992). Two recent meta-analyses inde- Ironically, the effectiveness of reliving trau-
pendently reached the conclusion that grief- matic experiences for individuals with PTSD
specific therapies tend to be relatively ineffica- may have helped blur the distinction between
cious (Kato & Mann, 1999; Neimeyer, 2000). A recovery and resilience. Researchers have made
third meta-analytic study reported that grief remarkably few attempts to distinguish sub-
therapies can be effective but generally to a groups within the broad category of individuals
lesser degree than usually observed for other not showing PTSD. Resilient and recovering
forms of psychotherapy (Allumbaugh & Hoyt, individuals are often lumped into a single cate-
1999). In one of these analyses, an alarming gory (e.g., King, King, Foy, Keane, & Fairbank,
38% of the individuals receiving grief treat- 1999; McFarlane & Yehuda, 1996). As with
104 BONANNO

bereavement, however, when researchers do not life-threatening events will show a genuine re-
address this distinction, they risk making the silience that should not be interfered with or
faulty assumption that resilient people must en- undermined by clinical intervention.
gage in the same coping processes as do ex-
posed individuals who struggle with but even- Point 2: Resilience Is Common
tually recover from more intense trauma
symptoms. Because research on acute and chronic grief
The possible untoward nature of this assump- and PTSD historically has dominated the liter-
tion is evidenced keenly in the often contentious ature on how adults cope with aversive life
debate about the appropriateness of psycholog- events, such reactions have generally come to
ical debriefing. Whereas genuinely traumatized be viewed as the norm. As I discuss below,
individuals were once doubted as malingerers, bereavement theorists have been highly skepti-
the pendulum has recently swung so far in the cal about individuals who do not show pro-
opposite direction that many practitioners be- nounced distress reactions or who display pos-
lieve that virtually all individuals exposed to itive emotions following loss, assuming that
violent or life-threatening events should be of- such individuals are rare and suffer from patho-
fered and would benefit from at least some form logical or dysfunctional forms of absent grief.
of brief intervention. Critical incident stress de- Trauma theorists have been less suspicious
briefing was originally developed for relatively about the absence of PTSD but have often ig-
limited use as a brief group intervention to help nored and underestimated resilience. A review
mitigate psychological distress among emer- of the available research on loss and violent or
gency response personnel (Mitchell, 1983). life-threatening events clearly indicates that the
Over time, however, debriefing has been ap- vast majority of individuals exposed to such
plied individually and broadly (Mitchell & Ev- events do not exhibit chronic symptom profiles
erly, 2000) and sometimes, as after the recent and that many and, in some cases, the majority
September 11th terrorist attacks on the World show the type of healthy functioning suggestive
Trade Center (Miller, 2002), as a blanket inter- of the resilience trajectory.
vention for virtually all exposed individuals.
Critics of psychological debriefing argue, how- Resilience to Loss
ever, that such a broad application may patholo-
gize normal reactions to adversity and thus may Bereavement theorists have typically viewed
undermine natural resilience processes. Indeed, the absence of prolonged distress or depression
growing evidence shows that global applica- following the death of an important friend or
tions of psychological debriefing are ineffective relative, often termed absent grief, as a rare and
(Rose, Brewin, Andrews, & Kirk, 1999) and can pathological response that results from denial or
impede natural recovery processes (Bisson, Jen- avoidance of the emotional realities of the loss.
kins, Alexander, & Bannister, 1997; Mayou, Bowlby (1980), for example, described the
Ehlers, & Hobbs, 2000). “prolonged absence of conscious grieving” (p.
An alternative form of early trauma interven- 138) as a type of disordered mourning and
tion, recently proposed by Litz, Gray, Bryant, viewed the experience or expression of positive
and Adler (2002), resonates with the distinction emotions during the early stages of bereave-
proposed here between resilience and recovery. ment as a form of defensive denial. Summariz-
Litz et al. argued that, while offering debriefing ing the first wave of bereavement research, Os-
to all individuals exposed to a potentially trau- terweis, Solomon, and Green (1984) concluded
matic event is misguided, some individuals “that the absence of grieving phenomena fol-
would indeed benefit from early intervention. lowing bereavement represents some form of
They proposed the development of initial personality pathology” (p. 18). More recently,
screening practices for intervention with indi- in a survey of self-identified bereavement ex-
viduals who show possible risk factors (e.g., perts, the majority (65%) endorsed beliefs that
prior trauma, low social support, hyperarousal) absent grief exists, that it usually stems from
for developing chronic PTSD. Implicit in this denial or inhibition, and that it is generally
approach is the idea, central to the current arti- maladaptive in the long run (Middleton, Moy-
cle, that many individuals exposed to violent or lan, Raphael, Burnett, & Martinek, 1993).
LOSS, TRAUMA, AND HUMAN RESILIENCE AFTER ADVERSE EVENTS 105

These same bereavement experts (76%) also dence is unequivocal on this point: No empirical
endorsed the compatible assumption that absent study has ever clearly demonstrated the exis-
grief eventually surfaces in the form of delayed tence of delayed grief. For example, Middleton,
grief reactions. Burnett, Raphael, and Martinek (1996) used
The available empirical literature, however, cluster analyses to examine longitudinal out-
suggests a very different story: Resilience to the come patterns among groups of bereaved
unsettling effects of interpersonal loss is not spouses, adult children, and parents. Despite
rare but relatively common, does not appear to their conviction that delayed grief would
indicate pathology but rather healthy adjust- emerge, Middleton et al. concluded that “no
ment, and does not lead to delayed grief reac- evidence was found for the pattern of response
tions. Over a decade ago, Wortman and Silver which might be expected for delayed grief”
(1989) first drew attention to the somewhat star- (Middleton et al., 1996, p. 169). Data from a
tling fact that there was no empirical basis for recent five-year longitudinal study indicated a
either the assumption that the absence of dis- similar conclusion (Bonanno & Field, 2001).
tress during bereavement is pathological or that This study contrasted the common assumption
it is always followed by delayed manifestations that delayed grief is a robust phenomenon with
of grief. Unfortunately, at the time their article an alternative assumption that a few participants
was published, there were relatively few longi- might show delayed elevations but only on iso-
tudinal bereavement studies from which to fully lated measures because of random measurement
evaluate their claim. error. The results were consistent with the mea-
More recent prospective studies have now surement-error explanation. In fact, when a psy-
begun to shed greater light on individual differ- chometrically more reliable, weighted compos-
ences in grief reactions (for a review, see Bon- ite measure was used, not a single participant
anno & Kaltman, 2001). Although the DSM has evidenced delayed grief.
not specified a unique category for acute or The idea that the absence of grief is patho-
complicated grief reactions, the available re- logical is rooted in the assumptions that be-
search generally shows that chronic depression reaved individuals showing this pattern must
and distress tend to occur in 10% to 15% of have had a superficial attachment to the de-
bereaved individuals. Considerable numbers of ceased or that they are cold and emotionally
bereaved individuals also tend to show more distant people (Bowlby, 1980). Such explana-
time-limited disruptions in functioning (e.g., tions are notoriously difficult to rule out be-
cognitive disorganization, dysphoria, health cause, for obvious reasons, most bereavement
deficits, disrupted social and occupational func- studies take place after the death already has
tioning) lasting at least several months to one or occurred. When measured during bereavement,
two years. Most important, in studies that report factors such as the quality of the lost relation-
aggregate data, bereaved individuals who ex- ship or the situational context of the loss are
hibited relatively low levels of depression or confounded with current functioning and the
distress have consistently approached or ex- possible influence of memory biases (e.g.,
ceeded 50% of the sample. For example, in a Safer, Bonanno, & Field, 2001).
recent study that examined various levels of However, a recent prospective study pro-
depression among conjugally bereaved adults, vided a rare opportunity to address this issue
approximately half of a sample did not show using data gathered on average three years prior
even mild depression (these individuals en- to the death of a spouse (Bonanno, Wortman, et
dorsed fewer than two items from the DSM–IV al., 2002). This study provided strong evidence
symptom list) following the loss (Zisook, Pau- in support of the idea that many bereaved indi-
lus, Shuchter, & Judd, 1997). In addition, there viduals will exhibit little or no grief and that
is now solid prospective evidence that associ- these individuals are not cold and unfeeling or
ates resilience to loss with the experience and lacking in attachment but, rather, are capable of
expression of positive emotion (e.g., Bonanno genuine resilience in the face of loss. Almost
& Keltner, 1997). half of the participants in this study (46% of the
How many of the bereaved individuals who sample) had low levels of depression, both prior
do not exhibit overt grief reactions will eventu- to the loss and through 18 months of bereave-
ally develop delayed grief reactions? The evi- ment, and had relatively few grief symptoms
106 BONANNO

(e.g., intense yearning for the spouse) during ans (Sutker, Davis, Uddo, & Ditta, 1995), to
bereavement. An examination of the pre- 16.5% for hospitalized survivors of motor vehi-
bereavement functioning of this group revealed cle accidents (Ehlers, Mayou, & Bryant, 1998),
no signs of maladjustment; these participants to 17.8% for victims of physical assault
were not rated as emotionally cold or distant by (Resnick, Kilpatrick, Dansky, Saunders, &
the interviewers, did not report difficulties in Best, 1993).
their marriages, and did not show dismissive Although chronic PTSD certainly warrants
attachment. They did, however, have relatively great concern, the fact that the vast majority of
high scores on several prebereavement mea- individuals exposed to violent or life-threaten-
sures suggestive of the ability to adapt well to ing events do not go on to develop the disorder
loss (e.g., acceptance of death, belief in a just has not received adequate attention. It is well
world, instrumental support). As in previous established that many exposed individuals will
studies, no unequivocal evidence for delayed evidence short-lived PTSD or subclinical stress
grief was found. Finally, it is important to note reactions that abate over the course of several
that even among these resilient individuals, the months or longer (i.e., the recovery pattern). For
majority reported experiencing at least some example, a population-based survey conducted
yearning and emotional pangs, and virtually all one month after the September 11th terrorist
participants reported intrusive cognition and ru- attacks in New York City estimated that 7.5%
mination at some point early after the loss (Bo- of Manhattan residents would meet criteria for
nanno, Wortman, & Nesse, in press). The dif- PTSD and that another 17.4% would meet the
ference between the resilient individuals and the criteria for subsyndromal PTSD (high symptom
other participants, however, was that these ex- levels that do not meet full diagnostic criteria;
periences were transient rather than enduring Galea, Ahern, et al., 2002). As in other studies,
and did not interfere with their ability to con- a subset eventually developed chronic PTSD,
tinue to function in other areas of their lives, and this was more likely if exposure was high.
including the capacity for positive affect. However, most respondents evidenced a rapid
decline in symptoms over time: PTSD preva-
Resilience to Violent and Life-Threatening lence related to 9/11 dropped to only 1.7% at
Events four months and 0.6% at six months, whereas
subsyndromal PTSD dropped to 4.0% and
Epidemiological studies estimate that the ma- 4.7%, respectively, at these times (Galea et al.,
jority of the U.S. population has been exposed 2003).
to at least one traumatic event, defined using the What about exposed individuals who exhibit
DSM–III criteria of an event outside the range relatively little distress? Trauma theorists are
of normal human experience, during the course sometimes surprised when exposed individuals
of their lives. Although grief and trauma symp- do not show more than a few PTSD symptoms.
toms are qualitatively different, the basic out- For example, body handlers in the aftermath of
come trajectories following trauma tend to form the Oklahoma City bombing have been de-
patterns similar to those observed following be- scribed as showing “unexpected resilience”
reavement (see Figure 1). Summarizing this re- (Tucker et al., 2002). Indeed, whereas those
search, Ozer et al. (2003) recently noted that who cope well with bereavement are sometimes
“roughly 50%– 60% of the U.S. population is viewed as cold and unfeeling, those who cope
exposed to traumatic stress but only 5%–10% well with violent or life-threatening events are
develop PTSD” (p. 54). However, because there often viewed in terms of extreme heroism.
is greater variability in the types and levels of However justified, this practice tends to rein-
exposure to stressor events, there also tends to force the misperception that only rare individu-
be greater variability in PTSD rates over time. als with “exceptional emotional strength” (e.g.,
Estimates of chronic PTSD have ranged, for Casella & Motta, 1990) are capable of
example, from 6.6% and 9.9% for individuals resilience.
experiencing personally threatening and violent The available evidence suggests that resil-
events, respectively, during the 1992 Los An- ience to violent and life-threatening events is far
geles riots (Hanson, Kilpatrick, Freedy, & more common. The vast majority of individuals
Saunders, 1995), to 12.5% for Gulf War veter- (78.2%) exposed to the 1992 Los Angeles riots
LOSS, TRAUMA, AND HUMAN RESILIENCE AFTER ADVERSE EVENTS 107

reported three or fewer PTSD symptoms (Han- who eventually manifest delayed PTSD tend to
son et al., 1995). Similarly, among hospitalized have had relatively high levels of symptoms in
survivors of motor vehicle accidents (Bryant, the immediate aftermath of the stressor event
Harvey, Guthrie, & Moulds, 2000), the majority (e.g., Buckley et al., 1996). Thus, these individ-
(79%) did not meet criteria for PTSD and av- uals appear to be immediately distinguishable
eraged only 3.3 PTSD symptoms, indicating from more truly resilient individuals (see Fig-
that many participants must have shown little or ure 1).
no PTSD. In a study of PTSD among Gulf War Perhaps trauma reactions might manifest in-
veterans (Sutker et al., 1995), the majority directly through behavioral or health problems?
(62.5%) had no psychological distress when Although PTSD is frequently comorbid with
examined within one year of their return to the health and behavior problems, individuals ex-
United States. In their post-9/11 survey, Galea, posed to putative traumatic events sometimes
Resnick, et al. (2002) reported that over 40% of do evidence these problems in the absence of
Manhattan residents did not report a single PTSD. As was the case with delayed PTSD,
PTSD symptom. Cardeña et al. (1994) exam- however, even when health and behavior prob-
ined data on a wide range of cognitive, affec- lems are accounted for, many survivors do not
tive, and somatic symptoms (e.g., exaggerated show such problems. This was evidenced, for
startle, recurrent distressing dreams, fatigue) example, in a longitudinal study of survivors of
measured among survivors of five different di- the North Sea oil rig disaster— by all accounts a
saster events within one to four weeks of each horrific and disturbing event (Holen, 1990). In
event. Although they did not assess the type of the first year following the disaster, 13.7% of
specific symptom trajectories that would allow the survivors were assigned psychiatric diag-
direct inferences about resilient individuals, noses (at the time of the study, PTSD was not a
Cardeña et al. did report that “even with such a well-established diagnosis), compared with
diverse series of events and forms of data col- only 1.1% of a matched comparison sample. In
lection . . . the percentages we obtained for im- contrast, medical diagnoses were assigned to
mediate reactions to disaster were very similar” 31% of the survivors. Although these rates were
(Cardeña et al., 1994, p. 387). And their data markedly higher than those found in the com-
were consistent with the idea that resilience is parison sample (4.5%), they nonetheless under-
common: The vast majority of symptoms they score the fact that most if not the majority of
measured were apparent in only a minority of survivors exhibited neither extreme distress nor
respondents. Finally, although relatively little unusual health problems.
research has been done on the experience or
expression of positive emotion following poten- Point 3: There Are Multiple and
tially traumatic events, two recent studies have Sometimes Unexpected Pathways to
provided important preliminary data linking Resilience
positive emotions in the context of trauma with
resilient functioning (Colak et al., 2003; If resilience and recovery represent distinct
Fredrickson, Tugade, Waugh, & Larkin, 2003). trajectories that are informed by different cop-
Positive emotion is revisited in the final section ing habits, then what factors promote resil-
of this article. ience? Meta-analytic studies have consistently
How many exposed individuals eventually revealed several clear predictors of PTSD reac-
show delayed trauma reactions? In contrast to tions, including lack of social support, low in-
the absence of evidence for delayed grief during telligence and lack of education, family back-
bereavement, delayed PTSD does appear to be a ground, prior psychiatric history, and aspects of
genuine, empirically verifiable phenomenon. the trauma response itself, such as dissociative
Nonetheless, delayed PTSD is still relatively reactions (Brewin, Andrews, & Valentine,
infrequent, occurring in approximately 5% to 2000; Ozer et al., 2003). It seems likely that at
10% of exposed individuals (Buckley, Blan- least some of these factors, if inverted, would
chard, & Hickling, 1996), and thus applies at predict resilient functioning. However, rela-
best only to a subset of the many individuals tively little research has attempted to address
who do not show initial PTSD reactions. It is this question. What’s more, because so little
noteworthy, however, that exposed individuals attention has been devoted to resilience, when
108 BONANNO

loss and trauma theorists have looked for resil- & Kahn, 1982) helps to buffer exposure to
ience, they have tended to look in the wrong extreme stress. Hardiness consists of three di-
places. Indeed, the assumption that all adults mensions: being committed to finding meaning-
exposed to loss or to potentially traumatic ful purpose in life, the belief that one can influ-
events experience prolonged distress and dis- ence one’s surroundings and the outcome of
ruptions in functioning goes hand in hand with events, and the belief that one can learn and
the belief that resilience must be rare and found grow from both positive and negative life expe-
only in exceptionally healthy people (e.g., Ca- riences. Armed with this set of beliefs, hardy
sella & Motta, 1990). individuals have been found to appraise poten-
Recent studies suggest a far more complex tially stressful situations as less threatening,
picture; as developmental psychologists have thus minimizing the experience of distress.
long asserted, there is no single means of main- Hardy individuals are also more confident and
taining equilibrium following highly aversive better able to use active coping and social sup-
events, but rather there are multiple pathways to port, thus helping them deal with the distress
resilience (e.g., Luthar, Doernberger, & Zigler, they do experience (e.g., Florian, Mikulincer, &
1993; Rutter, 1987). This evidence further sug- Taubman, 1995).
gests that, contrary to myths about unusually
healthy beings, adults resilient to loss or trauma Self-Enhancement
often appear to cope effectively in ways that,
under normal circumstances, may not always be Another dimension linked to resilience is self-
advantageous. For example, recall the bereave- enhancement. Somewhat ironically, around the
ment study by Bonanno, Wortman, et al. time PTSD was formalized as a diagnostic cat-
(2002), discussed earlier, that identified a large egory, social psychologists had begun to chal-
resilient group with a relatively healthy profile lenge the traditional assumption that mental
prior to the loss. This study also revealed a health requires realistic acceptance of personal
second, smaller group of resilient individuals limitations and negative characteristics (Green-
who had improved following the death of their wald, 1980; Taylor & Brown, 1988). These
spouse. At prebereavement, members of the im- scholars argued instead that unrealistic or overly
proved group had spouses who were ill; were positive biases in favor of the self, such as
highly depressed, neurotic, and introspective; self-enhancement, can be adaptive and promote
had more conflicted, ambivalent marriages; and well-being. Although most people engage in
believed that they were treated less fairly in life self-enhancing biases at least some of the time,
than other people. A recent follow-up study of measurable individual differences are also
these individuals (Bonanno et al., in press) in- found. Trait self-enhancement has been associ-
dicated that they showed no adverse reactions ated with benefits, such as high self-esteem, but
through 18 months of bereavement, gave little also with costs: Self-enhancers score high on
indication of denial or avoidance, perceived measures of narcissism and tend to evoke neg-
greater benefits to widowhood, gained increas- ative impressions in others (Paulhus, 1998).
ing comfort from positive memories of their This trade-off may be less problematic, how-
spouses over time, and reported that they too ever, in the context of highly aversive events,
were somewhat surprised by their own coping when threats to the self are most salient (Taylor
efficacy. Thus, although dramatically different & Brown, 1988).
from the larger resilient group at prebereave- Support for this idea comes from a recent
ment, the improved respondents also appeared to study of individual differences in self-enhanc-
exhibit genuine resilience during bereavement. ing biases among bereaved individuals in the
In this section, a number of distinct dimen- United States and among Bosnian civilians liv-
sions suggestive of different types or pathways ing in Sarajevo in the immediate aftermath of
of resilience to loss and trauma are considered. the Balkan civil war (Bonanno, Field, Ko-
vacevic, & Kaltman, 2002). In both samples,
Hardiness self-enhancers were rated by mental health pro-
fessionals as better adjusted. What’s more, self-
A growing body of evidence suggests that the enhancement proved to be particularly adaptive
personality trait of hardiness (Kobasa, Maddi, for bereaved individuals suffering from more
LOSS, TRAUMA, AND HUMAN RESILIENCE AFTER ADVERSE EVENTS 109

severe losses. In a similar study of individuals Positive Emotion and Laughter


who were in or near the World Trade Center
towers at the time of the September 11th attacks One of the ways repressors and others show-
(Bonanno, Rennicke, Dekel, & Rosen, 2003), ing resilience appear to cope well with adversity
self-enhancers reported better adjustment and is through the use of positive emotion and
more active social networks and were rated laughter (Bonanno, Noll, et al., 2003; Keltner &
more positively and as better adjusted by their Bonanno, 1997). Historically, the possible use-
close friends. Further, self-enhancers’ salivary fulness of positive emotion in the context of
cortisol levels exhibited a profile suggestive of extremely aversive events was either ignored or
minimal stress responding. dismissed as a form of unhealthy denial (e.g.,
Bowlby, 1980). Recently, however, research
has shown that positive emotions can help re-
Repressive Coping duce levels of distress following aversive events
both by quieting or undoing negative emotion
Resilience to loss and trauma has also been (Fredrickson & Levenson, 1998; Keltner & Bo-
found among another perhaps less likely nanno, 1997) and by “increasing continued con-
group: repressive copers (Weinberger, tact with and support from important people in
Schwartz, & Davidson, 1979). A considerable the . . . person’s social environment” (Bonanno
body of literature documents that individuals & Keltner, 1997, p. 134).
identified by either questionnaire or behav- Several recent studies have supported these
ioral measures as repressors tend to avoid ideas in the specific contexts of loss or trauma.
unpleasant thoughts, emotions, and memories Bereaved individuals who exhibited genuine
(Weinberger, 1990). In contrast to hardiness laughs and smiles when speaking about a recent
and self-enhancement, which appear to oper- loss had better adjustment over several years of
ate primarily on the level of cognitive pro- bereavement (Bonanno & Keltner, 1997) and
cesses, repressive coping appears to operate also evoked more favorable responses in ob-
primarily through emotion-focused mecha- servers (Keltner & Bonanno, 1997). Recently,
nisms, such as emotional dissociation. For Fredrickson et al. (2003) demonstrated that the
instance, repressors typically report relatively links between personality measures of resil-
little distress in stressful situations but exhibit ience and adjustment following the September
elevated distress on indirect measures, such 11th attacks were mediated by the experience of
as autonomic arousal (Weinberger et al., positive emotions (e.g., gratitude, interest,
1979). Emotional dissociation is generally love). Finally, the expression of positive emo-
viewed as maladaptive and may be associated tion among young adult survivors of childhood
with long-term health costs (Bonanno & sexual abuse predicted better adjustment and
Singer, 1990). However, these same tenden- better social relations over time (Colak et al.,
cies also appear to foster adaptation to ex- 2003). The latter study also suggested, however,
treme adversity. For example, repressors have that although laughter in the context of a so-
been found to show relatively little grief or cially stigmatized event like childhood sexual
distress at any point across five years of be- abuse predicts better adjustment, it may also
reavement (Bonanno & Field, 2001; Bon- carry social costs (e.g., decreased social com-
anno, Keltner, Holen, & Horowitz, 1995). petence). Clearly, this is an important area for
Further, although they initially reported in- further research.
creased somatic complaints, over time repres-
sors did not show greater somatic or health Toward a Broader Conceptualization of
problems than other participants. Recently, Stress Responding
among a sample of young women with docu-
mented histories of childhood sexual abuse, The evidence reviewed above presents an
repressors were less likely to voluntarily dis- important challenge to the view that adults who
close their abuse when provided the opportu- do not show distress following a loss or violent
nity to do so, but they also showed better or life-threatening event are either pathological
adjustment than other survivors (Bonanno, or rare and exceptionally healthy. Rather, this
Noll, Putnam, O’Neill, & Trickett, 2003). evidence suggests that resilience is common, is
110 BONANNO

distinct from the process of recovery, and can Werner, 1995). Although in some ways adult
potentially be reached by a variety of different resilience to loss and trauma presents a simpler
pathways. What lessons might these points offer problem (e.g., the aversive context is centered
for future understanding of human stress re- on a single event, and the developmental issues
sponding? Within a broader context, psycholo- unfold at a more gradual pace), it is nonetheless
gists might try to understand why resilience in crucial to determine how resilience to loss or
the face of loss or trauma has so often been trauma may vary across the life span, how adult
misunderstood by considering the myriad errors resilience relates to developmental experiences,
and biases in judgment that occur under condi- and whether the various factors that inform
tions of uncertainty (e.g., the availability heu- adult resilience might also function in a cumu-
ristic; Tversky & Kahneman, 1974). Others al- lative and interactive manner (McFarlane & Ye-
ready have probed the limitations of clinical huda, 1996). Researchers might also ask
inference from this perspective (e.g., Dawes, whether adults can learn to be more resilient to
1994). However, what might be particularly in- aversive events by, for example, extending
teresting to explore is the frequent failure not some of the wellness-promotion factors devel-
only to grasp the prevalence of resilience to loss oped for children (e.g., Cowen, 1991) or
and trauma but also to comprehend its many whether different protective factors foster resil-
forms. Clearly, researchers and theorists need to ience for different types of events, as has been
move beyond overly simplistic conceptions of suggested by studies of risk factors for PTSD
health and pathology to embrace the broader (Brewin et al., 2000). As we move into the next
costs and benefits of various dispositions and millennium, it will be imperative to address
adaptive mechanisms. Trade-offs of this sort these questions and to take a fresh look at the
can be found everywhere in nature. Cheetahs, various ways people adapt and even flourish in
for example, possess breath-taking speed but the face of what otherwise would seem to be
have poor stamina and must catch their prey potentially debilitating events.
quickly or starve. In a similar vein, people prone
to the use of self-enhancing biases enjoy high
self-esteem but tend to annoy those who do not References
know them well (Paulhus, 1998). Overly sim- Allumbaugh, D. L., & Hoyt, W. T. (1999). Effective-
plistic conceptions of self-enhancers as dys- ness of grief therapy: A meta-analysis. Journal of
functional obfuscate the coping advantage these Counseling Psychology, 46, 370 –380.
individuals show when confronted with truly American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnos-
aversive situations (Bonanno, Field, et al., tic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd
2002). ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
It is imperative that future investigations of Bisson, J., Jenkins, P. L., Alexander, J., & Bannister,
loss and trauma include more detailed study of C. (1997). Randomised controlled trial of psycho-
the full range of possible outcomes; simply put, logical debriefing for victims of burn trauma. Brit-
ish Journal of Psychiatry, 171, 78 – 81.
dysfunction cannot be fully understood without Bonanno, G. A., & Field, N. P. (2001). Examining
a deeper understanding of health and resilience. the delayed grief hypothesis across five years of
By viewing resilient functioning through the bereavement. American Behavioral Scientist, 44,
same empirical lens as chronic forms of dys- 798 – 806.
function and more time-limited recovery pat- Bonanno, G. A., Field, N. P., Kovacevic, A., &
terns, researchers will be able to examine and Kaltman, S. (2002). Self-enhancement as a buffer
contrast each of these patterns. Many questions against extreme adversity: Civil war in Bosnia and
await investigation. A crucial issue pertains to traumatic loss in the United States. Personality
the commonalities and differences in resilient and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 184 –196.
functioning across the life span. Developmental Bonanno, G. A., & Kaltman, S. (1999). Toward an
integrative perspective on bereavement. Psycho-
theorists have argued that resilience to aversive logical Bulletin, 125, 760 –776.
childhood contexts results from a cumulative Bonanno, G. A., & Kaltman, S. (2001). The varieties
and interactive mix of genetic (e.g., disposi- of grief experience. Clinical Psychology Review,
tion), personal (e.g., family interaction), and 21, 705–734.
environmental (e.g., community support sys- Bonanno, G. A., & Keltner, D. (1997). Facial expres-
tems) risk and protective factors (Rutter, 1999; sions of emotion and the course of conjugal be-
LOSS, TRAUMA, AND HUMAN RESILIENCE AFTER ADVERSE EVENTS 111

reavement. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, sourcebook (pp. 377–391). Washington DC:
126 –137. American Psychiatric Association.
Bonanno, G. A., Keltner, D., Holen, A., & Horowitz, Casella, L., & Motta, R. W. (1990). Comparison of
M. J. (1995). When avoiding unpleasant emotions characteristics of Vietnam veterans with and with-
might not be such a bad thing: Verbal–autonomic out posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychological
response dissociation and midlife conjugal be- Reports, 67, 595– 605.
reavement. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- Colak, D., Bonanno, G. A., Keltner, D., Noll, J. G.,
chology, 69, 975–989. Putnam, F. W., & Trickett, P. (2003). Positive
Bonanno, G. A., Noll, J. G., Putnam, F. W., O’Neill, emotion and long-term adjustment among young
M., & Trickett, P. (2003). Predicting the willing- adult survivors of childhood sexual abuse. Manu-
ness to disclose childhood sexual abuse from mea- script in preparation.
sures of repressive coping and dissociative expe- Cowen, E. L. (1991). The pursuit of wellness. Amer-
riences. Child Maltreatment, 8, 1–17. ican Psychologist, 46, 404 – 408.
Bonanno, G. A., Papa, A., & O’Neill, K. (2001). Loss Dawes, R. M. (1994). House of cards. New York:
and human resilience. Applied and Preventive Psy- Free Press.
chology, 10, 193–206. Ehlers, A., Mayou, R. A., & Bryant, B. (1998).
Bonanno, G. A., Rennicke, C., Dekel, S., & Rosen, J. Psychological predictors of chronic posttraumatic
(2003). Self-enhancement and resilience among stress disorder after motor vehicle accidents. Jour-
survivors of the September 11th terrorist attack on nal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 508 –519.
the World Trade Center. Manuscript in prepara- Florian, V., Mikulincer, M., & Taubman, O. (1995).
tion. Does hardiness contribute to mental health during
Bonanno, G. A., & Singer, J. L. (1990). Repressor a stressful real-life situation? The roles of appraisal
personality style: Theoretical and methodological and coping. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
implications for health and pathology. In J. L. chology, 68, 687– 695.
Singer (Ed.), Repression and dissociation (pp. Foa, E. B., Dancu, C. V., Hembree, E. A., Jaycox,
435– 470). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. L. H., Meadows, E. A., & Street, G. P. (1999). A
Bonanno, G. A., Wortman, C. B., Lehman, D. R., comparison of exposure therapy, stress inoculation
Tweed, R. G., Haring, M., Sonnega, J., et al. training, and their combination for reducing post-
(2002). Resilience to loss and chronic grief: A traumatic stress disorder in female assault victims.
prospective study from pre-loss to 18 months post- Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
loss. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- 67, 194 –200.
ogy, 83, 1150 –1164. Fredrickson, B. L., & Levenson, R. W. (1998). Pos-
Bonanno, G. A., Wortman, C. B., & Nesse, R. M. (in itive emotions speed recovery from the cardiovas-
press). Patterns of resilience and maladjustment cular sequelae of negative emotions. Cognition
before and after the death of a spouse. Psychology and Emotion, 12, 191–220.
and Aging. Fredrickson, B. L., Tugade, M. M., Waugh, C. E., &
Bowlby, J. (1980). Loss: Sadness and depression: Larkin, G. R. (2003). What good are positive emo-
Vol. 3. Attachment and loss. New York: Basic tions in crisis? A prospective study of resilience
Books. and emotion following the terrorist attacks on the
Brewin, C. R., Andrews, B., & Valentine, J. D. United States on September 11th, 2001. Journal of
(2000). Meta-analysis of risk factors for posttrau- Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 365–376.
matic stress disorder in trauma-exposed adults. Freud, S. (1957). Mourning and melancholia. In J.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, Strachey (Ed.), The standard edition of the com-
68, 748 –766. plete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (Vol.
Bryant, R. A., Harvey, A. G., Guthrie, R. M., & 14, pp. 152–170). London: Hogarth Press. (Origi-
Moulds, M. L. (2000). A prospective study of nal work published 1917)
psychophysiological arousal, acute stress disorder, Galea, S., Ahern, J., Resnick, H., Kilpatrick, D.,
and posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Ab- Bucuvalas, M., Gold, J., & Vlahov, D. (2002).
normal Psychology, 109, 341–344. Psychological sequelae of the September 11 ter-
Buckley, T. C., Blanchard, E. B., & Hickling, E. J. rorist attacks in New York City. New England
(1996). A prospective examination of delayed on- Journal of Medicine, 346, 982–987.
set PTSD secondary to motor vehicle accidents. Galea, S., Resnick, H., Ahern, J., Gold, J., Bucuvalas,
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 617– 625. M., Kilpatrick, D., et al. (2002). Posttraumatic
Cardeña, E., Holen, A., McFarlane, A., Solomon, Z., stress disorder in Manhattan, New York City, after
Wilkinson, C., & Spiegel, D. (1994). A multisite the September 11th terrorist attacks. Journal of
study of acute stress reactions to a disaster. In T. A. Urban Health Studies, 79, 340 –353.
Widiger, A. J. Frances, H. A. Pincus, R. Ross, Galea, S., Vlahov, D., Resnick, H., Ahern, J., Ezra,
M. B. First, W. Davis, & M. Kline (Eds.), DSM–IV S., Gold, J., et al. (2003). Trends of probably
112 BONANNO

post-traumatic stress disorder in New York City victims. British Journal of Psychiatry, 176, 589 –
after the September 11th terrorist attacks. Ameri- 593.
can Journal of Epidemiology, 158, 514 –524. McFarlane, A. C., & Yehuda, R. (1996). Resilience,
Garmezy, N. (1991). Resilience and vulnerability to vulnerability, and the course of posttraumatic re-
adverse developmental outcomes associated with actions. In B. A. van der Kolk, A. C. McFarlane, &
poverty. American Behavioral Scientist, 34, 416 – L. Weisaeth (Eds.), Traumatic stress (pp. 155–
430. 181). New York: Guilford Press.
Greenwald, A. G. (1980). The totalitarian ego: Fab- Middleton, W., Burnett, P., Raphael, B., & Martinek,
rication and revision of personal history. American N. (1996). The bereavement response: A cluster
Psychologist, 35, 603– 618. analysis. British Journal of Psychiatry, 169, 167–
Hanson, R. F., Kilpatrick, D. G., Freedy, J. R., & 171.
Saunders, B. E. (1995). Los Angeles County after Middleton, W., Moylan, A., Raphael, B., Burnett, P.,
the 1992 civil disturbance: Degree of exposure and & Martinek, N. (1993). An international perspec-
impact on mental health. Journal of Consulting tive on bereavement related concepts. Australian
and Clinical Psychology, 63, 987–996. and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 27, 457–
Holen, A. (1990). A long-term outcome study of 463.
survivors from a disaster: The Alexander L. Kiel- Miller, J. (2002). Affirming flames: Debriefing sur-
land disaster in perspective. Oslo, Norway: Uni- vivors of the World Trade Center attack. Brief
versity of Oslo Press. Treatment and Crisis Intervention, 21, 85–94.
Kato, P. M., & Mann, T. (1999). A synthesis of Mitchell, J. T. (1983). When disaster strikes. . . : The
psychological interventions for the bereaved. Clin- critical incident stress debriefing process. Journal
ical Psychology Review, 19, 275–296. of Emergency Medical Services, 8, 36 –39.
Keltner, D., & Bonanno, G. A. (1997). A study of Mitchell, J. T., & Everly, G. S., Jr. (2000). Critical
laughter and dissociation: Distinct correlates of incident stress management and critical incident
laughter and smiling during bereavement. Journal stress debriefing: Evolutions, effects, and out-
of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 687– comes. In B. Raphael & J. P. Wilson (Eds.), Psy-
702. chological debriefing: Theory, practice, and evi-
King, D. W., King, L. A., Foy, D. W., Keane, T. M., dence (pp. 71–90). Cambridge, England: Cam-
& Fairbank, J. A. (1999). Posttraumatic stress dis- bridge University Press.
order in a national sample of female and male Neimeyer, R. A. (2000). Searching for the meaning
Vietnam veterans: Risk factors, war-zone stres- of meaning: Grief therapy and the process of re-
sors, and resilience–recovery variables. Journal of construction. Death Studies, 24, 541–558.
Abnormal Psychology, 108, 164 –170. Osterweis, M., Solomon, F., & Green, F. (1984).
Kobasa, S. C., Maddi, S. R., & Kahn, S. (1982). Bereavement: Reactions, consequences, and care.
Hardiness and health: A prospective study. Jour- Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, Ozer, E. J., Best, S. R., Lipsey, T. L., & Weiss, D. S.
168 –177. (2003). Predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder
Lamprecht, F., & Sack, M. (2002). Posttraumatic and symptoms in adults: A meta-analysis. Psycho-
stress disorder revisited. Psychosomatic Medicine, logical Bulletin, 129, 52–71.
64, 222–237. Paulhus, D. L. (1998). Interpersonal and intrapsychic
Litz, B. T., Gray, M. J., Bryant, R. A., & Adler, A. B. adaptiveness of trait self-enhancement: A mixed
(2002). Early intervention for trauma: Current sta- blessing? Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
tus and future directions. Clinical Psychology: Sci- chology, 74, 1197–1208.
ence and Practice, 9, 112–134. Rando, T. A. (1992). The increasing prevalence of
Luthar, S. S., Cicchetti, D., & Becker, B. (2000). The complicated mourning: The onslaught is just be-
construct of resilience: A critical evaluation and ginning. Omega, 26, 43–59.
guidelines for future work. Child Development, 71, Resick, P. A. (2001). Stress and trauma. Philadel-
543–562. phia: Taylor and Francis.
Luthar, S. S., Doernberger, C. H., & Zigler, E. Resnick, H. S., Kilpatrick, D. G., Dansky, B. S.,
(1993). Resilience is not a unidimensional con- Saunders, B. E., & Best, C. L. (1993). Prevalence
struct: Insights from a prospective study of inner- of civilian trauma and posttraumatic stress disorder
city adolescents. Development and Psychopathol- in a representative national sample of women.
ogy, 5, 703–717. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
Masten, A. S. (2001). Ordinary magic: Resilience 61, 984 –991.
processes in development. American Psychologist, Rose, S., Brewin, C. R., Andrews, B., & Kirk, M.
56, 227–238. (1999). A randomized controlled trial of individual
Mayou, R. A., Ehlers, A., & Hobbs, M. (2000). psychological debriefing for victims of violent
Psychological debriefing for road traffic accident crime. Psychological Medicine, 29, 793–799.
LOSS, TRAUMA, AND HUMAN RESILIENCE AFTER ADVERSE EVENTS 113

Rutter, M. (1987). Psychosocial resilience and pro- American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 72, 469 –
tective mechanisms. American Journal of Ortho- 475.
psychiatry, 57, 316 –331. Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974, September 27).
Rutter, M. (1999). Resilience concepts and findings: Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases.
Implications for family therapy. Journal of Family Science, 185, 1124 –1131.
Therapy, 21, 119 –144. Weinberger, D. A. (1990). The construct validity of
Safer, M. A., Bonanno, G. A., & Field, N. P. (2001). the repressive coping style. In J. L. Singer (Ed.),
It was never that bad: Biased recall of grief and Repression and dissociation: Implications for per-
long-term adjustment to the death of a spouse. sonality theory, psychopathology and health (pp.
Memory, 9, 195–204. 337–386). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Stroebe, M. S., & Stroebe, W. (1991). Does “grief Weinberger, D. A., Schwartz, G. E., & Davidson,
work” work? Journal of Consulting and Clinical R. J. (1979). Low-anxious and repressive coping
Psychology, 59, 479 – 482. styles: Psychometric patterns of behavioral and
physiological responses to stress. Journal of Ab-
Sutker, P. B., Davis, J. M., Uddo, M., & Ditta, S. R.
normal Psychology, 88, 369 –380.
(1995). War zone stress, personal resources, and
Werner, E. E. (1995). Resilience in development.
PTSD in Perian Gulf War returnees. Journal of
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 4,
Abnormal Psychology, 104, 444 – 452. 81– 85.
Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and Wortman, C. B., & Silver, R. C. (1989). The myths of
well-being: A social psychological perspective on coping with loss. Journal of Consulting and Clin-
mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 193– ical Psychology, 57, 349 –357.
210. Zisook, S., Paulus, M., Shuchter, S. R., & Judd, L. L.
Taylor, S. E., Kemeny, M. E., Reed, G. M., Bower, (1997). The many faces of depression following
J. E., & Gruenewald, T. L. (2000). Psychological spousal bereavement. Journal of Affective Disor-
resources, positive illusions, and health. American ders, 45, 85–94.
Psychologist, 55, 99 –109. Zoellner, L. A., Fitzgibbons, L. A., & Foa, E. B.
Tucker, P., Pfefferbaum, B., Doughty, D. B., Jones, (2001). Cognitive– behavioral approaches to
D. E., Jordan, F. B., & Nixon, S. J. (2002). Body PTSD. In J. P. Wilson, M. J. Friedman, & J. D.
handlers after terrorism in Oklahoma City: Predic- Lindy (Eds.), Treating psychological trauma and
tors of posttraumatic stress and other symptoms. PTSD (pp. 159 –182). New York: Guilford Press.

You might also like