Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

This article was downloaded by: [137.189.74.

205] On: 02 February 2018, At: 01:24


Publisher: Institute for Operations Research and the Management Sciences (INFORMS)
INFORMS is located in Maryland, USA

Management Science
Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://pubsonline.informs.org

Everything We Do, You Do: The Licensing Effect of


Prosocial Marketing Messages on Consumer Behavior
Maryam Kouchaki, Ata Jami

To cite this article:


Maryam Kouchaki, Ata Jami (2018) Everything We Do, You Do: The Licensing Effect of Prosocial Marketing Messages on
Consumer Behavior. Management Science 64(1):102-111. https://doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.2016.2571

Full terms and conditions of use: http://pubsonline.informs.org/page/terms-and-conditions

This article may be used only for the purposes of research, teaching, and/or private study. Commercial use
or systematic downloading (by robots or other automatic processes) is prohibited without explicit Publisher
approval, unless otherwise noted. For more information, contact permissions@informs.org.

The Publisher does not warrant or guarantee the article’s accuracy, completeness, merchantability, fitness
for a particular purpose, or non-infringement. Descriptions of, or references to, products or publications, or
inclusion of an advertisement in this article, neither constitutes nor implies a guarantee, endorsement, or
support of claims made of that product, publication, or service.

Copyright © 2016, INFORMS

Please scroll down for article—it is on subsequent pages

INFORMS is the largest professional society in the world for professionals in the fields of operations research, management
science, and analytics.
For more information on INFORMS, its publications, membership, or meetings visit http://www.informs.org
MANAGEMENT SCIENCE
Vol. 64, No. 1, January 2018, pp. 102–111
http://pubsonline.informs.org/journal/mnsc/ ISSN 0025-1909 (print), ISSN 1526-5501 (online)

Everything We Do, You Do: The Licensing Effect of Prosocial


Marketing Messages on Consumer Behavior
Downloaded from informs.org by [137.189.74.205] on 02 February 2018, at 01:24 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.

Maryam Kouchaki,a Ata Jamib


a Kellogg School of Management, Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois 60208; b College of Business Administration, University of
Central Florida, Orlando, Florida 32816
Contact: m-kouchaki@kellogg.northwestern.edu (MK); ata.jami@ucf.edu (AJ)

Received: January 15, 2015 Abstract. Do prosocial corporate marketing messages promote consumers’ altruistic
Revised: September 3, 2015; February 3, 2016 behaviors, or do they advance self-interested and self-indulgent actions? To answer this
Accepted: June 10, 2016 question, the current research investigates the impact of different framings of prosocial
Published Online in Articles in Advance: marketing messages on consumers’ behaviors and choices more generally. Results from
October 13, 2016 six laboratory studies and a field experiment demonstrate that exposure to messages
https://doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.2016.2571 that praise customers for good deeds can increase subsequent self-interested and self-
indulgent behaviors more than messages that publicize a company’s good deeds or thank
Copyright: © 2016 INFORMS consumers for their patronage. Our findings demonstrate the possibility that a temporary
boost in one’s self-concept drives this observed effect. In addition, the recipient’s level
of support for the issue praised for moderates the effect of customer-praise messages on
the recipient’s less altruistic behaviors. This paper concludes with a discussion of the
theoretical and managerial implications.

History: Accepted by Pradeep Chintagunta, marketing.


Supplemental Material: The supplemental material is available at https://doi.org/10.1287/
mnsc.2016.2571.

Keywords: licensing • praise • message framing • corporate societal marketing • morality

Introduction can have negative consequences for the firm (Torelli


YOU. Are a pioneer in using recycled cups. Everything et al. 2012, Wagner et al. 2009). For example, research
we do, you do. Your business lets Starbucks do business reveals that a luxury brand communicating its CSR
in a way that’s better for the planet. Like leading the actions causes disfluency and a decline in brand eval-
way in cup technology with the first U.S. hot cups made uations (Torelli et al. 2012). We depart from the focus
with 10% post-consumer recycled fiber, and cup sleeves of prior research on either positive or negative conse-
that use 60%. Starbucks is working on a cup that’s 100% quences of CSR for a firm and focus on consequences
recyclable too. It’s what we can do, because of what you of CSR messages on consumers’ unrelated actions. We
do. Good for you, you. posit that corporate societal marketing (CSM) not only
—Message from a Starbucks coffee cup (Figure 1) invokes company-related behavior and responses, but
The above message printed on a Starbucks cup is one also influences consumers’ behaviors more generally.
example of many socially and environmentally respon- Prior research shows that CSM is persuasive and
sible corporate messages consumers encounter. These can significantly affect consumers’ behaviors and atti-
tudes toward the company and its products (Brown
messages are framed to praise customers for their indi-
and Dacin 1997, Drumwright 1996, Du et al. 2007,
rect good deeds resulting from their decision to pur-
Hoeffler and Keller 2002, Olsen et al. 2014, Sen and
chase the company’s product or service.
Bhattacharya 2001). Because CSR programs seek to cre-
Environmentally and socially responsible concerns
ate value for the firm’s shareholders, most research
have reshaped the organizational landscape, push- investigates the effects of socially responsible initiatives
ing companies toward socially responsible practices and/or CSM on consumers’ attitudes, behaviors, and
(Menon and Menon 1997). Each year, companies spend decisions related to the company (Lichtenstein et al.
billions of dollars on corporate philanthropy, sponsor- 2004, Olsen et al. 2014, Sen and Bhattacharya 2001).
ships, and cause-related marketing (Bonini et al. 2006, Currently, only a dearth of research addresses the psy-
Brown and Dacin 1997, Luo and Bhattacharya 2009). chological mechanisms that drive consumer behavior
Corporations’ participation in corporate social respon- after their exposure to CSR messages. Thus, compa-
sibility (CSR) activities and programs tends to elicit nies and consumers are not necessarily considering the
favorable responses from stakeholders (e.g., Sen et al. possible consequences of CSM broadly.
2006). However, there is a small body of literature It may seem intuitive that socially responsible mes-
that investigates situations in which engaging in CSR sages would help make consumers aware of many
102
Kouchaki and Jami: The Licensing Effect of Prosocial Marketing Messages
Management Science, 2018, vol. 64, no. 1, pp. 102–111, © 2016 INFORMS 103

Figure 1. (Color online) Starbucks Cup (Customer-Praise backfire effects of CSR messages (Torelli et al. 2012,
Message) Wagner et al. 2009). Although there are obvious ben-
efits to CSM, our research indicates that there may be
a negative by-product of such messages. Support for
our predictions would suggest that organizations must
weigh the benefits of CSM with the potential negative
effects of their messages on consumers and society.
Downloaded from informs.org by [137.189.74.205] on 02 February 2018, at 01:24 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.

More than 90% of U.S. organizations participate in


some sort of social and environmental activism, and
this number has been increasing every year (Sheehan
2004). Accordingly, our study will provide organiza-
tions, marketers, and policy makers with information
to better design CSM messages that promote altruistic
behaviors. Our research evaluates exposure to a rel-
atively impersonal company message, which mirrors
real-life commercials or mass mailings that consumers
often receive. As such, we emphasize that companies
must carefully consider the wider consequences of the
framing of their CSR communications to their con-
sumers. Doing so will more effectively link their CSR
efforts to improve the greater good for society as a
social/environmental issues and therefore promote whole.
subsequent altruistic behaviors and choices. How-
ever, we posit that in some circumstances, the oppo- Theoretical Background
site occurs. Environmental and social messages could Despite an ongoing debate about the positive effects
license consumers to act less altruistically and more of CSR activities in augmenting profitability and firm
self-interestedly. Specifically, messages that credit indi- value (e.g., Margolis and Walsh 2003), participating
vidual customers for their indirect or direct contribu- in CSR and CSM activities may be a necessity for
tion to the good acts (Figure 1) enhance consumers’ firms today. Highlighting the consumer as a key stake-
self-concept, unlike messages focused exclusively on holder, prior research suggests that consumer aware-
the group’s behavior. This enhanced self-concept ness of a firm’s CSR activities can modify consump-
can decrease the consumer’s propensity for altruistic tion behavior and/or investment decisions to influence
behaviors and choices. Despite the potential benefits of the firm’s value (Bhattacharya and Sen 2004, Sen
CSM messages for a company, such frames could there- and Bhattacharya 2001, Sen et al. 2006). Regardless
fore harm society and consumers as a whole. There- of whether the CSR activities derive from moral or
fore, we revisit the link between CSM and consumer economic concerns, companies need to communicate
behavior, taking into account research on the psy- these actions to their stakeholders to collect the asso-
chological mechanisms that drive consumer responses ciated benefits. We seek to examine the effects of dif-
to CSM to examine whether or not CSM messages ferent framings of CSM messages on ensuing con-
increase consumers’ self-indulgent and self-interested sumer behaviors. Cognitive models of information
behaviors. processing and social-psychological approaches to self-
Our research contributes to literature in a number perception provide insights into how people selectively
of important ways. First, this study complements prior perceive, evaluate, and interpret messages according to
research that has examined the impact of CSR mes- the implications for their self-concept (Bandura 2001).
sages and initiatives on consumers’ specific behaviors One potential difference in the framing of CSR mes-
and attitudes toward the company and/or its products sages that may have relevance to the recipient is the
(e.g., Hoeffler and Keller 2002, Sen and Bhattacharya consumer versus company referencing (you versus
2001, Torelli et al. 2012). However, our approach dif- us/other) invoked in the message. In the consumer-
fers from existing research by focusing on the underly- referencing frame, the focus is on the individual read-
ing psychological processes that drive individual con- ing the message—the person is directly being credited
sumers’ broader responses to CSR messages and the for their involvement in the CSR actions (referred to
effects on their unrelated behavior more generally. Sec- as “you” in the message)—whereas in the company-
ond, by predicting consumer behavior after their expo- referencing frame, the CSR message focuses on the
sure to different socially responsible message framings actions of the company/group and its members.
and identifying circumstances that might result in neg- We suggest that receiving a customer-praise (“you”)
ative consequences, we contribute to the domain of message compared to a company-praise message
Kouchaki and Jami: The Licensing Effect of Prosocial Marketing Messages
104 Management Science, 2018, vol. 64, no. 1, pp. 102–111, © 2016 INFORMS

increases consumers’ self-indulgent and self-interested company-praise can thus increase retrieval of self-
behaviors. Generally, people wish to make choices that relevant thoughts related to behaviors in the message.
reflect positively on themselves both privately and This increase leads to a temporary boost in the indi-
publicly (Rosenberg 1979). In matters of self-interest vidual’s self-concept related to the dimension in the
and self-indulgence, people are torn between their self- message, leading people to avoid the feelings of guilt
ish desires and maintaining a positive self-concept. We that might result from engaging in self-interested, self-
argue that in an attempt to find a balance between the indulgent behavior.
Downloaded from informs.org by [137.189.74.205] on 02 February 2018, at 01:24 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.

two forces, while pursuing multiple, conflicting goals In sum, in the context of CSR consumer-framing
(Mullen and Monin 2016), people use contextual cues messages, we argue that a message can boost individu-
that allow them to engage in limited self-interest and als’ self-concept related to the dimension stated in the
self-indulgence (Mazar et al. 2008). In fact, a temporary message, which in turn can increase consumers’ like-
boost to one’s self-concept helps one avoid guilty feel- lihood to engage in self-indulgent and self-interested
ings that may result from self-indulgent choices and behaviors.
self-interested behaviors. In the first three studies, we test our main predic-
We rely on multiple accounts to argue that receiv- tion using different variations of praise messages with
ing a customer-praise (“you”) message compared to different participant groups across both field and lab
a company-praise message can lead to a temporary settings. In Studies 1a and 1b, we examine the effects
boost in individuals’ self-concept. First, research on of two real-world customer-praise messages compared
moral licensing shows that prior prosocial actions can with company-praise messages (i.e., messages that
help people boost their self-concept, liberating them publicize the company’s good deeds) on subsequent
to engage in compensatory behaviors (Khan and Dhar altruistic and self-indulgent decisions. In Study 1c, we
2006, Kouchaki 2011). For example, Khan and Dhar test whether performing a good deed influences par-
(2006) show that even imagining engagement in an ticipants’ reaction to customer- and company-praise
altruistic act (e.g., volunteering for charity) can lead messages. Then, in Study 2, we conduct a field exper-
participants to establish an altruistic self-concept (e.g., iment to test our prediction in a real-world setting.
“I am a helpful person”). This can motivate them to In Study 3, we add an additional control condition
prefer a luxury good (e.g., designer jeans) over a neces- with no praise. In Study 4, we conduct a mediation
sity (e.g., vacuum cleaner). Fishbach and Dhar (2005) analysis to examine the possibility that a temporary
find that when people were led to believe they made boost in one’s self-concept explains the proposed effect
progress toward weight loss goals, they subsequently of customer-praise messages. Finally, in Study 5, we
chose a chocolate bar over an apple as a snack. Thus, examine the impact of variations in the recipient’s level
focus on goal progress can reduce motivation to act of support for the issue praised for in customer-praise
consistently with the goal. Additionally, work on vicar- messages.
ious licensing demonstrates that observing other peo-
ple’s good deeds can enhance one’s self-concept, lead-
Study 1a
ing to less altruistic behaviors.
Method
Additionally, a customer-praise message provides in-
Participants. Fifty individuals were recruited to com-
formation for self-evaluation. Social comparison the-
plete a short, paid online study on Amazon’s Mechan-
ory suggests that individuals are motivated to de-
ical Turk website. Three individuals who did not
fine and evaluate themselves relative to others in
respond to questions regarding the study variables of
regards to the dimension under evaluation (Festinger
interest were excluded from analyses. The final sam-
1954). Given individuals’ desire for self-enhancing
ple consisted of 47 participants (36% male, mean age 
evaluations (Leary 2007), a customer-praise message
33.2 years, SD  11.8).
(compared to a company-praise message) can make
individuals feel better about themselves and better Design and Procedure. The study employed a be-
compared to others in the dimension under evalua- tween-participants design with two conditions (cus-
tion by influencing their perceptions of the described tomer praise and company praise). To minimize suspi-
behaviors’ prevalence. The perception of prevalence has cion, the instructions indicated that participants would
been shown to govern behavior (Cialdini 2003, Cialdini participate in several unrelated tasks, and the first task
et al. 1990). required them to evaluate a company’s commercial.
Finally, a customer-praise message employs a self- Participants were randomly assigned to watch a short
referencing strategy (Burnkrant and Unnava 1995). video clip and then choose between green (environ-
Consumer research demonstrates that self-referencing mentally friendly) and nongreen products in several
prompts higher involvement with messages and facil- categories. They watched a short, 40-second commer-
itates retrieval of self-relevant information (Sood cial and then answered filler questions about the com-
and Forehand 2005, Symons and Johnson 1997). mercial. The commercials were actual Starbucks text-
A customer-praise (“you”) message compared to a heavy commercials, in which Starbucks praised either
Kouchaki and Jami: The Licensing Effect of Prosocial Marketing Messages
Management Science, 2018, vol. 64, no. 1, pp. 102–111, © 2016 INFORMS 105

its customers or itself for choosing fair trade certified did not respond to questions regarding the study vari-
coffee (for the transcript, see the online supplemental ables of interest were excluded from analyses. The final
material). In the customer-praise condition, Starbucks sample consisted of 65 participants (66% male, mean
acknowledged its customers for buying fair trade cer- age  25.1 years, SD  4.8).
tified coffee and mentioned, “Everything we do, you
Design and Procedure. The study had two between-
do.” For the company-praise condition, we chose a participant conditions: customer praise and company
commercial similar in length and content, describing
Downloaded from informs.org by [137.189.74.205] on 02 February 2018, at 01:24 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.

praise. Participants were randomly assigned to view


the benefits of buying fair trade coffee and crediting one of the messages printed on a company cup (see the
Starbucks for doing so. online supplemental material). To minimize suspicion,
Participants then chose two products in ran- the instructions indicated that the participants would
dom order: a backpack and batteries (adapted from participate in multiple unrelated tasks, and the first
Griskevicius et al. 2010). For each product, they con- study involved evaluations of a company’s coffee cup.
sidered both green and traditional options, equal in The message for the customer-praise condition came
price and made by the same company. The two versions from actual Starbucks cups. In the company-praise con-
appeared at the same time on the screen, and partic- dition, the message had similar information but did
ipants were asked, “If you were shopping for a back- not praise customers. Next, participants responded to
pack/batteries, which of these two products would filler items about the cup. For the dependent variable
you buy?” We considered choices of green products (adapted from Khan and Dhar 2006), we asked par-
altruistic (Griskevicius et al. 2010). ticipants to engage in an ostensibly unrelated product
preference task by indicating their relative preference
Results and Discussion
for a pair of designer jeans (relative luxury) or a vac-
We predicted that watching the customer-praise clip, uum cleaner (relative necessity), on a scale ranging
rather than the company-praise clip, would increase from (1) “most likely to buy the vacuum cleaner” to
the likelihood of choosing the traditional, nongreen (7) “most likely to buy the designer jeans.” They were
product. Indeed, participants in the customer-praise to imagine that they were at a mall that was having
condition were less likely to choose the green bat- a sale and that they had to choose between the two
teries (33.3% chose green, 66.7% chose traditional) products, both priced at $50. Participants were told to
and the green backpack (25.0% green, 75% tradi- assume that they had been planning to buy both items
tional). In contrast, in the company-praise condition, but could afford only one at the moment.
participants tended to choose the green batteries
(69.6% green, 30.4% traditional) and had no preference Results and Discussion
between the backpack options (52.2% green, 47.8% tra- As predicted, relative preference for the designer
ditional). Consistent with our prediction, participants jeans in the customer-praise condition was signifi-
who watched the customer-praise commercial selected cantly higher (mean  3.50, SD  2.20) than in the
the green products significantly less frequently than company-praise condition (mean  2.36, SD  1.61,
participants who watched the company-praise com- t(63)  2.38, p  0.02). Accordingly, Study 1b replicated
mercial (batteries, χ2 (1)  6.17, p  0.01; backpack, and extended the results of Study 1a, providing addi-
χ2 (1)  3.67, p  0.05). This study offers an initial tional evidence that customer-praise messages affect
demonstration of the impact of exposure to praise mes- subsequent choices and behavior.
sages on consumers’ purchase intentions. A customer- In Studies 1a and 1b, participants are exposed to a
praise message lowers intentions to buy green prod- message that thanks them for something they may not
ucts, illustrating the effect of consumer-praising CSR have done. To further demonstrate the influence of cus-
messages on altruistic behaviors. tomer praise, in Study 1c, participants actually perform
In the next study, we consider a different depen- a good deed and are then praised for the good deed
dent variable, namely, self-indulgent choices. Prior re- they have done.
search has generally assumed that the purchase of lux-
ury goods is associated with guilt and thus negative
self-attributions (Dahl et al. 2003, Kivetz and Simonson Study 1c
2002), because of an inability to justify the purchase Method
(Okada 2005). Participants. One hundred sixty individuals were re-
cruited to complete a short, paid online study on Ama-
zon’s Mechanical Turk website. Nine individuals who
Study 1b did not respond to questions regarding the study vari-
Method ables of interest were excluded from analyses. The final
Participants. Sixty-seven students participated in the sample consisted of 151 participants (38% male, mean
study for partial course credit. Two individuals who age  33.8 years, SD  11.7).
Kouchaki and Jami: The Licensing Effect of Prosocial Marketing Messages
106 Management Science, 2018, vol. 64, no. 1, pp. 102–111, © 2016 INFORMS

Design and Procedure. Similar to previous studies, “group members” for hours volunteered. The contents
the instructions indicated that they would participate of the messages were similar aside from the self- versus
in several unrelated tasks. All participants first com- member-praise manipulation (see the online supple-
pleted a filler task. Then they were introduced to a mental material). At the bottom of the email, recipients
nonprofit organization (Sharetix) and were informed read that they would receive a gift as a thank you.
that Sharetix is planning to expand and is targeting Then, they were asked to click on a link within seven
more communities. Thus, the organization is looking days to choose their gift and receive instructions for
Downloaded from informs.org by [137.189.74.205] on 02 February 2018, at 01:24 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.

for help from community members to answer a few gift pickup.


survey questions. After introducing the nonprofit in To test the effect of the framing of the message on
detail, participates answered a few filler questions, recipients’ subsequent self-indulgent behavior, we pro-
ostensibly helping Sharetix. Afterward, participants vided the recipients with a choice of two bags for which
were randomly assigned to receive a thank you note their perception of indulgence might differ. The bags
from Sharetix that differed with respect to whether the were equal in price and were made by the same com-
message was framed as “you,” thus praising the recip- pany. Given that this was a naturalistic field experi-
ient uniquely for the help, or was more inclusive and ment, we did not provide any information about the
praised the “donors and volunteers” for their help (for bags to the participants and displayed the images of
the notes, see the online supplemental material). In the each, labeled “Backpack” and “Travel Utility Bag,”
subsequent task, participants indicated their relative respectively, and asked them to select one.
preference for a luxury product, similar to Study 1b. To find potential gift options, we consulted the vol-
unteering group’s management. They noted that they
Results and Discussion have used promotional items such as T-shirts and
Similar to previous studies, the relative preference for pens before. Given that we wanted to provide partici-
the designer jeans in the self-praise condition was pants with a relatively self-indulgent option, we chose
significantly higher (mean  2.58, SD  1.95) than a premier backpack versus a travel utility bag. We
in the volunteer-praise condition (mean  1.96, SD  conducted a pilot study with a group of participants
1.55, t(149)  2.16, p  0.03). The results confirm that (n  56) from the study population. Participants in the
the customer-praise message leads to self-indulgent pilot study were presented with the same images and
behavior even when participants receive well-deserved labels of the two options that participants in the real
praise. field experiment would receive.
So far we have provided support for the effect of In the pilot, we asked participants to imagine that
consumer framing of CSR messages on behaviors in they were to choose one of the following two options
controlled settings. In our next study, we attempt to as a gift to receive. Participants were then asked two
replicate our findings in a real setting. We believe there questions in random order: “Which one would you
is a need for research to move beyond the controlled choose?” and “If you were to indulge yourself, which
empirical contexts to paint a more externally valid pic- one would you choose?” Participants recorded their
ture of the forces determining consumers’ reactions to choices on a seven-point scale (1  “definitely the Back-
CSR initiatives and their compensatory behaviors. We pack,” 7  “definitely the Travel Utility Bag”). A paired-
ran a field experiment in which participants randomly sample t-test was conducted to compare participants’
received a version of a thank you email from their choice of product when indulging themselves with
volunteering group and subsequently chose a product their general choice. Participants were more likely to
offered to them as a gift. In this study, rather than mea-
prefer the premier backpack when indulging them-
suring what participants would do, we measured their
selves (mean  3.14, SD  2.09) than in general (mean 
real choices, as whether they choose the more indul-
3.63, SD  2.15, t(55)  2.26, p  0.02; the order of the
gent choice or not.
questions was counterbalanced). The findings show
that the backpack is perceived as a relatively self-
Study 2 indulgent choice compared to the travel utility bag.
Method
We ran a field experiment at an American volunteer- Results and Discussion
ing group. A random sample of active members of a We predicted that those participants who received
volunteer group (n  200) received a thank you email the self-praise email, rather than the member-praise
from the director’s office. To test the effect of framing email, would choose the self-indulgent product (pre-
of the thank you message, participants were randomly mier backpack) more. Examining the number of mem-
assigned to one of two messages that differed with bers who followed the link and chose a product
respect to whether the message was framed as “you,” (92 members in total), there was no significant differ-
thus praising the recipient uniquely for hours volun- ence between the two message conditions (p  0.4). In
teered (similar to the Starbucks cup) or praised the other words, those in the self-praise condition were
Kouchaki and Jami: The Licensing Effect of Prosocial Marketing Messages
Management Science, 2018, vol. 64, no. 1, pp. 102–111, © 2016 INFORMS 107

no more likely to choose a gift than were those in the as allocators. All participants were told that some par-
member-praise condition (50% versus 42%). Consistent ticipants would be randomly selected to receive money
with our prediction, participants receiving the self- earned in the task. After reading the instructions, par-
praise message selected the self-indulgent backpack ticipants indicated how much they would offer the
(42 of 50, 84%) significantly more frequently than those recipient.
participants who received the member-praise message
(27 of 42, 64%; χ2 (1)  4.73, p  0.03). In sum, the Results and Discussion
Downloaded from informs.org by [137.189.74.205] on 02 February 2018, at 01:24 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.

results in a naturalistic field experiment demonstrate An analysis of variance of the amount of money given
that a message praising individuals for their good to the recipient (in dollars) revealed significant vari-
deeds increases the likelihood of their subsequent self- ation among the three conditions (F(2,86)  10.60,
indulgent behaviors. p < 0.001). Participants in the customer-praise con-
The findings from the previous two studies pro- dition shared less money (mean  $4.87, SD  4.54)
vide support for our hypothesis; however, these stud- than those in the no-praise condition (mean  $7.85,
ies use simple, two-cell designs, such that they lack SD  4.44, t(57)  2.55, p  0.01) or company-praise
a “no praise” control group. Therefore, we introduce condition (mean  $10.50, SD  5.07, t(57)  4.49, p <
a control condition, which also enables us to test for 0.001). Participants in the company-praise condition
the effects of company praise and explore whether it (mean  $10.50, SD  5.07) shared significantly more
prompts more consistent behavior than no praise as a money than those in the no-praise condition (mean 
result of social influence. Additionally, in Study 3, we $7.85, SD  4.44, t(58)  2.15, p  0.04). Thus, Study 3
implement a behavioral measure to engage people’s demonstrates that exposure to a customer-praise mes-
self-interest (Mazar and Zhong 2010). sage led to more self-interested behavior, compared
with exposure to a no-praise message. Consistent with
Study 3 social influence literature, exposure to a company-
Method praise message produced less self-interested behavior
Participants. Ninety-six students participated in the overall.
study for partial course credit. Seven individuals who
did not respond to questions regarding the study vari- Temporary Boost in One’s Self Concept as
ables of interest were excluded from analyses. The final
sample consisted of 89 participants (63% male, mean
a Potential Explanation
age  24.2 years, SD  3.9). Self-concept refers to individuals’ beliefs about who
they are (Baumeister 1999). Self-concept is generally
Design and Procedure. The study featured three be- viewed as a collection of numerous self-aspects, only
tween-participant conditions: customer praise, com- a few of which are active in focal awareness at any
pany praise, and no praise. Participants were ran- given time (Baumeister 2010). Prior studies on compen-
domly assigned to view one of the messages, all satory responses have reported support for a tempo-
written from the perspective of the chief executive offi- rary boost in one’s self-concept as the underlying psy-
cer (CEO) of an ostensibly real company. They evalu- chological explanation (Khan and Dhar 2006, Kouchaki
ated a praise message, whether it praised customers 2011). None of the prior examinations explicitly dif-
or the company, for using recyclable cans and bot-
ferentiated between one’s general positive self-concept
tles (the content was adapted from Coca-Cola’s recy-
and the specific self-concept more relevant to the ini-
cling program, but we did not use the brand name),
tial act. We argue that customer-praise messages boost
or the control message, which simply thanked cus-
individuals’ specific self-concept relevant to the CSR
tomers for their loyalty (see the online supplemen-
action in the message (compared to a general positive
tal material). After reading the message, participants
self-concept), which in turn increases self-indulgent
responded to filler items. We again minimized suspi-
behaviors. As such, we are able to provide a more
cion by telling participants that they would participate
nuanced understanding of how consumers regulate
in several unrelated tasks, including an evaluation of
their socially desirable behaviors. In the next study,
a company’s message.
we test our proposition by measuring boosts both in
The measurement of self-interested behavior came
from participants’ actions while playing a dictator one’s general positive self-concept and the specific self-
game (Mazar and Zhong 2010). Specifically, they were concept relevant to the CSR action to examine the
led to believe they were allocating $20 between them- contribution of each in explaining the observed phe-
selves and another participant in the room. They were nomenon.
assured their identity would be kept confidential. The
task thus included an allocator, who kept whatever was Study 4
not offered, and one recipient, who chose to accept or Method
reject the offer. The recipient’s choice did not affect the Participants. One hundred individuals were recruited
allocator’s outcome. In reality, all participants served to complete a short, paid online study on Amazon’s
Kouchaki and Jami: The Licensing Effect of Prosocial Marketing Messages
108 Management Science, 2018, vol. 64, no. 1, pp. 102–111, © 2016 INFORMS

Mechanical Turk website. Eight individuals who did Consumer Support of the Issue
not respond to questions regarding the study variables Thus far, we have examined the aggregated effect
of interest were excluded from analyses. The final sam- of praise messages across respondents. However, we
ple consisted of 92 participants (40% male, mean age  expect consumers’ support of an issue and their per-
37.5 years, SD  13.3). sonal values toward the cause highlighted in the CSR
Design and Procedure. The study had two between- activity to influence their responses to the company’s
messages (Du et al. 2010). First, for licensing effect to
Downloaded from informs.org by [137.189.74.205] on 02 February 2018, at 01:24 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.

participant conditions: customer praise and no praise.


The manipulation of praise was similar to that in occur, individuals must pursue multiple, conflicting
Study 3, where the participants viewed a message goals; that is, there should be some tension between
written from the perspective of the CEO of a com- doing the right thing and self-interest (Mullen and
pany. After the filler questions, participants were asked Monin 2016). As such, in the case of proenvironmental
to rate themselves on a number of traits. The items praise messages, we do not expect to observe the licens-
were presented in a random order on a seven-point ing effects of praise for consumers with high envi-
scale (1  “strongly disagree,” 7  “strongly disagree”). ronmental consciousness because they do not experi-
Four items (helpful, compassionate, generous, and car- ence any temptation of conflict. Moreover, relying on a
ing; α  0.95) measured general positive self-concept psychological approach to self-perceptions, we should
(adapted from Khan and Dhar 2006). Since the mes- take into account the level of the relevant self-concept
sages praised customers for their help in reducing envi- for each individual. For example, proenvironmental
ronmental footprint, one item (environment friendly) praise allows people with low environmental values to
measured this specific self-concept relevant to the boost this aspect of their self-concept, allowing for sub-
praise. Next, in an unrelated task similar to Studies 1b sequent self-interested or self-indulgent behaviors. In
and 1c, participants indicated preferences between a contrast, for consumers that value their environmen-
pair of designer jeans (relative luxury) or a vacuum tal activism highly, praise does not boost this aspect of
cleaner (relative necessity) both priced at $50. their self-concept significantly. Thus, they should not
exhibit the increase in self-interested or self-indulgent
Results and Discussion behaviors that usually results from customer-praise
Relative preference for the designer jeans in the messages. This moderating factor allows us to further
customer-praise condition was significantly higher examine the possibility that the relationship between
(mean  2.60, SD  2.04) than in the no-praise con- customer-praise messages and self-interested or self-
dition (mean  1.68, SD  1.18, t(90)  2.63, p  indulgent behaviors is explained through licensing and
0.01). Moreover, participants rated themselves as more a boost in specific self-concept relevant to the CSR
environment-friendly in the customer-praise condition action in the message.
(mean  4.23, SD  1.36) than in the no-praise condi-
tion (mean  3.61, SD  1.45, t(90)  2.10, p  0.04).
Study 5
However, there were no significant differences on the
Method
general positive self-concept items (customer praise,
Participants. Fifty-four students participated in the
mean  4.45, SD  1.16 versus no praise, mean  4.15,
study for partial course credit. Four individuals who
SD  1.23; t(90)  1.21, p  0.23). Looking at each of the
did not respond to questions regarding the study vari-
other four positive attributes individually, none were
ables of interest were excluded from analyses. The final
significantly different between conditions.
sample consisted of 50 participants (60% male, mean
Next, we ran a multiple mediation analysis follow-
age  23.8 years, SD  4.9).
ing procedures recommended by Preacher and Hayes
(2008). We included both the general positive self- Design and Procedure. The study featured two be-
concept and the domain-specific self-concept simulta- tween-participant conditions: customer praise and
neously in the model to examine whether they sig- no praise. To minimize suspicion, participants were
nificantly mediate the effect of message condition on informed that they would participate in several unre-
indulgent choice. The results of the bootstrapping lated tasks. They received a comprehensive survey
analysis (with 5,000 iterations) indicated that specific containing a variety of questionnaires from different
self-concept (being environment friendly; confidence research projects, beginning with a seemingly unre-
interval [0.003, 0.513]), rather than the general posi- lated questionnaire that measured their environmental
tive self-concept (confidence interval [−0.384, 0.029]), values.
mediated the effect of message condition on indulgent For this study, we used a 10-item version of the
choice. Schwartz Value Survey (Schwartz 1992). Participants
These results provide initial evidence for the pos- considered descriptions of various people and their
sibility that a temporary boost in specific self-concept goals and values, and then indicated the extent to
relevant to the CSR action in the message drives the which the descriptions reflected their personal char-
increases in self-indulgent behaviors. acteristics, using a six-point scale from (1) “does not
Kouchaki and Jami: The Licensing Effect of Prosocial Marketing Messages
Management Science, 2018, vol. 64, no. 1, pp. 102–111, © 2016 INFORMS 109

describe me at all” to (6) “very much describes me.” shows no difference in the behavior of people with rela-
One item measured environmental values—“Looking tively higher environmental value scores across condi-
after the environment is important to this person; to tions but an increase in self-interested behavior among
care for nature.” Furthermore, we introduced an addi- those with relatively lower scores on environmental
tional direct measure of environmental values in which values in the customer-praise versus no-praise condi-
participants reviewed various proenvironmental activ- tions. We used the second measure of environmental
ities (Wade-Benzoni et al. 2007) and rated the impor- values, that is, the proenvironmental value importance
Downloaded from informs.org by [137.189.74.205] on 02 February 2018, at 01:24 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.

tance of each activity (e.g., “Supportive of addressing score, to test our prediction. Consistent with the first
environmental issues/problems”) to them on a seven- measure, the regression analyses revealed a significant
point scale ranging from (1) “not at all important” to (7) interaction between the praise message condition and
“extremely important.” We then averaged these items participants’ proenvironmental values score (b  1.17,
to form a proenvironmental value importance score t(46)  2.15, p  0.04) and a significant main effect for
(α  0.92). the praise message condition (b  −1.48, t(46)  −2.69,
We isolated the environmental values measures p  0.01), but no significant main effect of environmen-
from the main study by asking participants to complete tal values (b  0.38, t(46)  0.69, p  0.50).
several tasks from different research projects for about The results of this study indicate that participants
30 minutes. Then participants were randomly pre- with relatively lower scores on both measures of envi-
sented with one of the two messages used in Study 3 ronmental values allocated less money (i.e., more self-
and responded to some filler items. Finally, they played interested behavior) after reading the customer-praise
the dictator game (see Study 3). message than the participants that received the no-
praise message. For participants with stronger proenvi-
Results and Discussion ronmental values, the praise message did not influence
Consistent with the previous studies, the amount their behavior.
of money allocated to the recipient (in dollars) was
significantly lower in the customer-praise condition General Discussion
(mean  $5.04, SD  4.21) than in the no-praise condi- We examine the proposition that customer-praise mes-
tion (mean  $8.04, SD  3.77, t(48)  2.66, p  0.01). In sages used in CSM can lead to consumers’ self-
a regression analysis, we also examined the hypoth- interested and self-indulgent behaviors more gener-
esized moderating effect of environmental values on ally. Studies 1a–1c provide empirical support for our
the relationship between the praise message (−1  main hypothesis by showing that participants behave
no praise, 1  customer praise) manipulation and the less altruistically and more self-indulgently after their
amount of money allocated to the recipient. The anal- exposure to customer-praise messages rather than
yses revealed a significant main effect of the praise company-praise messages. In Study 2, in a natural-
message (b  −1.50, t(46)  −2.75, p  0.01) and a sig- istic field experiment, we demonstrate that praising
nificant interaction between the praise message condi- individuals for their good deeds increases the like-
tion and participants’ environmental values (b  0.84, lihood of subsequent self-indulgent behaviors. With
t(46)  1.99, p  0.05). However, the main effect of envi- Study 3, we generalize the effect of praise on subse-
ronmental values was not significant (b  0.46, t(46)  quent behavior using the realistic dictator game. We
1.09, p  0.28). In support of our prediction, Figure 2 add a control condition of no praise and reveal that
exposure to customer-praise messages prompts more
Figure 2. Mean Amount of Money Allocated in Study 5 self-interested behavior than exposure to company-
praise or no-praise messages. Study 4 demonstrates


the possibility that a temporary boost in the domain-
-ONEYGIVENTORECIPIENT


specific self-concept drives the observed phenomenon.

In Study 5, we provide evidence for the moderating
role of consumers’ internal support of the company’s
DOLLARS


 cause on the relationship between praise messages and
 self-interested behavior. Participants with a lower envi-
 %NVVALUE 3$ ronmental value score exhibited the proposed effect of
 %NVVALUEn3$ praise messages, whereas no such effect arose for par-
 ticipants with stronger proenvironmental values.
.OPRAISE #USTOMERPRAISE
Theoretical and Practical Contributions
Notes. The figure shows a two-way interaction of condition and
environmental value importance. The plus and minus signs rep- This research contributes to the existing literature on
resent values one standard deviation above and below the mean, corporate social marketing. Most research has focused
respectively. on the positive and negative consequences of CSM for
Kouchaki and Jami: The Licensing Effect of Prosocial Marketing Messages
110 Management Science, 2018, vol. 64, no. 1, pp. 102–111, © 2016 INFORMS

the company. The more recent literature on backfire inherent limitations of our studies into account. For
effects of CSR messages focuses on the negative effects instance, we used a single-item measure for the specific
for the evaluations of the company or its products self-concept. Additionally, even though we adapted
(Torelli et al. 2012, Wagner et al. 2009). Although prior messages from the brands (Starbucks, Coca-Cola) that
work addresses consumers’ behaviors, it has focused are ubiquitous enough that all participants are poten-
relatively narrowly on attitudes, behaviors, and deci- tial users, we did not use the brand names and did not
sions pertaining to the company, rather than on evalu- measure participants’ frequency of their use. Another
Downloaded from informs.org by [137.189.74.205] on 02 February 2018, at 01:24 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.

ating positive or negative consequences for individual issue that warrants consideration is the generalizability
consumers. Departing from studying corporate out- of our findings in terms of the messages and prod-
comes, we focus on the consequences of social con- ucts used. At a theoretical level, we believe that the
cerns for individual behaviors and thus reveal that in types of messages and products we used reflect con-
societal marketing settings, customer-praise messages sumers’ common marketplace decisions. Further stud-
can lead to less socially desirable behavior and more ies employing different methods and populations are
self-interested behavior, whereas company-praise mes- needed to confirm this claim.
sages produce more socially desirable behavior and Although our findings and implications provide an
less self-interested behavior. Moreover, we discuss the important extension of previous examinations of CSM,
underlying psychological processes that drive con- they also spark new questions. First, individual dif-
sumers’ responses to CSR messages and provide ini- ferences in self-esteem could be potential moderators.
tial evidence for the possibility that a temporary boost Second, our study evaluated the effects of CSM mes-
in the self-concept relevant to the CSR action explains sages on one subsequent decision; we hope further
the preference for self-indulgent and self-interested research expands this finding by focusing on multi-
behaviors. ple decisions. Also, it would be interesting to observe
This paper also contributes to literature on the rel- the effects of these messages over time, exploring their
ative effectiveness of message framing on persuasion magnitude and gradual depreciation.
and intentions to perform prosocial (e.g., environmen-
tal) or health-related behaviors (Block and Keller 1995, Acknowledgments
White et al. 2011). Our research suggests that the ref- Both authors contributed equally.
erence point (you or we) invoked in a persuasive com-
munication could impact recipients’ subsequent choice References
of action. Bandura A (2001) Social cognitive theory: An agentic perspective.
This research in turn has important practical impli- Annual Rev. Psych. 52(1):1–26.
Baumeister RF (1999) The Self in Social Psychology (Psychology Press,
cations for marketing practitioners, proenvironmental Philadelphia).
groups, and corporate social responsibility programs. Baumeister RF (2010) The self. Baumeister RF, Finkel EJ, eds.
Although CSR programs can enhance consumers’ Advanced Social Psychology: The State of the Science (Oxford Uni-
attitudes toward the firm (Brown and Dacin 1997), versity Press, New York), 139–176.
Bhattacharya CB, Sen S (2004) Doing better at doing good: When,
and nonprofit organizations in particular can use
why and how consumers respond to corporate social initiatives.
CSR to instill positive attitudes toward the organiza- Calif. Management Rev. 47(1):9–24.
tion and increase consumers’ donations (Lichtenstein Block LG, Keller PA (1995) When to accentuate the negative: The
et al. 2004), our findings specify that CSR pro- effects of perceived efficacy and message framing on intentions
to perform a health-related behavior. J. Marketing Res. 32(2):
grams should avoid customer-praise messages, which
192–203.
dampen prosocial and proenvironmental behaviors Bonini SMJ, Mendonca LT, Oppenheim JM (2006) When social issues
more generally. In particular, customer-praise mes- become strategic. McKinsey Quart. 2(2):20–31.
sages may reduce customers’ donations to corporate Brown TJ, Dacin PA (1997) The company and the product: Corpo-
or nonprofit programs, diminishing their effective- rate associations and consumer product responses. J. Marketing
61(1):68–84.
ness. Instead, company-referencing messages augment Burnkrant RE, Unnava HR (1995) Effects of self-referencing on per-
prosocial behaviors and are likely to increase dona- suasion. J. Consumer Res. 22(1):17–26.
tions. In brief, companies should think about the “net Cialdini RB (2003) Crafting normative messages to protect the envi-
effect” of businesses to make sure that they benefit soci- ronment. Current Directions Psych. Sci. 12(4):105–109.
Cialdini RB, Reno RR, Kallgren CA (1990) A focus theory of nor-
ety significantly more than they harm society. mative conduct: Recycling the concept of norms to reduce
littering in public places. J. Personality Soc. Psych. 58(6):
Limitations and Future Directions 1015–1026.
Despite the good empirical rigor and generalizabil- Dahl DW, Honea H, Manchanda RV (2003) The nature of self-
ity we achieved by using commercials and messages reported guilt in consumption contexts. Marketing Lett. 14(3):
159–171.
employed by a real corporation and different types of Drumwright ME (1996) Company advertising with a social dimen-
participants, both online and student participants, the sion: The role of noneconomic criteria. J. Marketing 60(4):
conclusions drawn from this research must take the 71–87.
Kouchaki and Jami: The Licensing Effect of Prosocial Marketing Messages
Management Science, 2018, vol. 64, no. 1, pp. 102–111, © 2016 INFORMS 111

Du S, Bhattacharya CB, Sen S (2007) Reaping relational rewards from Mullen E, Monin B (2016) Consistency versus licensing effects of past
corporate social responsibility: The role of competitive position- moral behavior. Annual Rev. Psych. 67:363–385.
ing. Internat. J. Res. Marketing 24(3):224–241. Okada EM (2005) Justification effects on consumer choice of hedonic
Du S, Bhattacharya CB, Sen S (2010) Maximizing business returns to and utilitarian goods. J. Marketing Res. 42(1):43–53.
corporate social responsibility (CSR): The role of CSR commu- Olsen MC, Slotegraaf RJ, Chandukala SR (2014) Green claims and
nication. Internat. J. Management Rev. 12(1):8–19. message frames: How green new products change brand atti-
Festinger L (1954) A theory of social comparison processes. Human tude. J. Marketing 78(5):119–137.
Relations 7(2):117–140. Preacher KJ, Hayes AF (2008) Asymptotic and resampling strategies
Downloaded from informs.org by [137.189.74.205] on 02 February 2018, at 01:24 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.

Fishbach A, Dhar R (2005) Goals as excuses or guides: The liberat- for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple media-
ing effect of perceived goal progress on choice. J. Consumer Res. tor models. Behav. Res. Methods 4(3):879–891.
32(3):370–377. Rosenberg M (1979) Conceiving the Self (Robert E. Krieger, Mal-
Griskevicius V, Tybur JM, Bergh BV (2010) Going green to be seen: abar, FL).
Status, reputation, and conspicuous conservation. J. Personality Schwartz SH (1992) Universals in the content and structure of val-
Soc. Psych. 98(3):392–404. ues: Theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries.
Hoeffler S, Keller KL (2002) Building brand equity through cor- Zanna MP, ed. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 25
porate societal marketing. J. Public Policy Marketing 21(1): (Academic Press, New York), 1–65.
78–89. Sen S, Bhattacharya CB (2001) Does doing good always lead to doing
Khan U, Dhar R (2006) Licensing effect in consumer choice. J. Mar- better? Consumer reactions to corporate social responsibility.
keting Res. 43(2):259–66. J. Marketing Res. 38(2):225–243.
Kivetz R, Simonson I (2002) Earning the right to indulge: Effort as Sen S, Bhattacharya CB, Korschun D (2006) The role of corpo-
a determinant of customer preferences toward frequency pro- rate social responsibility in strengthening multiple stakeholder
gram rewards. J. Marketing Res. 39(2):155–170. relationships: A field experiment. J. Acad. Marketing Sci. 34(2):
Kouchaki M (2011) Vicarious moral licensing: The influence of oth- 158–166.
ers’ past moral actions on moral behavior. J. Personality Soc. Sheehan KB (2004) Controversies in Contemporary Advertising (Sage,
Psych. 101(4):702–715. Thousand Oaks, CA).
Leary MR (2007) Motivational and emotional aspects of the self. Sood S, Forehand M (2005) On self-referencing differences in
Annual Rev. Psych. 58:317–344. judgment and choice. Organ. Behav. Human Decision Processes
Lichtenstein DR, Drumwright ME, Braig BM (2004) The effect of cor- 98(2):144–154.
porate social responsibility on customer donations to corporate- Symons CS, Johnson BT (1997) The self-reference effect in memory:
supported nonprofits. J. Marketing 68(4):16–32. A meta-analysis. Psych. Bull. 121(3):371–394.
Luo X, Bhattacharya CB (2009) The debate over doing good: Cor- Torelli CJ, Monga AB, Kaikati A (2012) Doing poorly by doing good:
porate social performance, strategic marketing levers, and firm- Corporate social responsibility and brand concepts. J. Consumer
idiosyncratic risk. J. Marketing 73(6):198–213. Res. 38(5):948–963.
Margolis JD, Walsh JP (2003) Misery loves companies: Rethinking Wade-Benzoni KA, Li M, Thompson LL, Bazerman MH (2007) The
social initiatives by business. Admin. Sci. Quart. 48(2):268–305. malleability of environmentalism. Analyses Soc. Issues Public Pol-
Mazar N, Zhong C (2010) Do green products make us better people? icy 7(1):163–89.
Psych. Sci. 21(4):494–98. Wagner T, Lutz RJ, Weitz BA (2009) Corporate hypocrisy: Overcom-
Mazar N, Amir O, Ariely D (2008) The dishonesty of honest people: ing the threat of inconsistent corporate social responsibility per-
A theory of self-concept maintenance. J. Marketing Res. 45(6): ceptions. J. Marketing 73(6):77–91.
633–644. White K, MacDonnell R, Dahl DW (2011) It’s the mind-set that mat-
Menon A, Menon A (1997) Enviropreneurial marketing strategy: The ters: The role of construal level and message framing in influenc-
emergence of corporate environmentalism as market strategy. ing consumer efficacy and conservation behaviors. J. Marketing
J. Marketing 61(1):51–67. Res. 48(3):472–485.

You might also like