2012 Neuropatia e IC

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology 76 (2012) 1332–1338

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ijporl

Speech perception and cortical auditory evoked potentials in cochlear implant


users with auditory neuropathy spectrum disorders
Kátia F. Alvarenga a,b,*, Raquel Beltrão Amorim a, Raquel Sampaio Agostinho-Pesse a,
Orozimbo Alves Costa a,b, Leandra Tabanez Nascimento b, Maria Cecilia Bevilacqua a,b
a
Department of Audiology and Speech Pathology at the School of Dentistry, University of São Paulo, Bauru Campus, Brazil
b
Center for Audiological Research at the University of São Paulo, Bauru Campus, Brazil

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Article history: Objective: To characterize the P1 component of long latency auditory evoked potentials (LLAEPs) in
Received 20 December 2011 cochlear implant users with auditory neuropathy spectrum disorder (ANSD) and determine firstly
Received in revised form 29 May 2012 whether they correlate with speech perception performance and secondly whether they correlate with
Accepted 3 June 2012
other variables related to cochlear implant use.
Available online 15 July 2012
Methods: This study was conducted at the Center for Audiological Research at the University of São Paulo.
The sample included 14 pediatric (4–11 years of age) cochlear implant users with ANSD, of both sexes,
Keywords:
with profound prelingual hearing loss. Patients with hypoplasia or agenesis of the auditory nerve were
Long latency auditory evoked potentials
Cochlear implant
excluded from the study. LLAEPs produced in response to speech stimuli were recorded using a Smart EP
Hearing impaired USB Jr. system. The subjects’ speech perception was evaluated using tests 5 and 6 of the Glendonald
Auditory neuropathy spectrum disorder Auditory Screening Procedure (GASP).
Results: The P1 component was detected in 12/14 (85.7%) children with ANSD. Latency of the P1
component correlated with duration of sensorial hearing deprivation (*p = 0.007, r = 0.7278), but not
with duration of cochlear implant use. An analysis of groups assigned according to GASP performance (k-
means clustering) revealed that aspects of prior central auditory system development reflected in the P1
component are related to behavioral auditory skills.
Conclusions: In children with ANSD using cochlear implants, the P1 component can serve as a marker of
central auditory cortical development and a predictor of the implanted child’s speech perception
performance.
ß 2012 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction Brainstem and cortical mechanisms involved in processing


auditory stimulation can be investigated non-invasively via
The use of cochlear implants in the treatment of individuals recording of auditory evoked potentials (AEPs). Physiological
with severe or profound sensory hearing loss, such as that due to changes in the auditory system during the development of
auditory neuropathy spectrum disorder (ANSD), has been studied peripheral and central systems are reflected in AEP latency and
extensively and, therefore, become widely accepted by the amplitude, allowing the relationship between these changes and
scientific community. When the cochlear implant is switched behavioral development of listening skills to be studied. Many
on, electrical stimulation from the implant commences, triggering researchers have chosen to examine long latency auditory evoked
cortical reorganization in the auditory system following a period of potentials (LLAEPs), which reflect the general electrical activity
sensory deprivation. The development of auditory structures in generated in the cortico–thalamo–cortical pathway, primary
these patients has often been similar to that which occurs in auditory cortex, and associative cortical areas [1]. More specifical-
hearing people. ly, the P1 component of the LLAEP has been used as a biomarker to
infer the state of maturation of auditory structures in infants and
children [2]. Numerous studies have described the latency and
amplitude of this component (see Tables 1 and 2) in normally
* Corresponding author at: Departamento de Fonoaudiologia da Faculdade de hearing children under 12 years of age [3–11]. A recent study at our
Odontologia da Universidade de São Paulo, Alameda Dr. Octávio Pinheiro Brisola, 9-
75 Bauru, São Paulo, CEP 17.012-901, Brazil. Tel.: +55 14 32358332;
center that examined children in the age range of 3–12 years
fax: +55 14 32234679. demonstrated that the latency of the P1 component decreases by
E-mail address: katialv@fob.usp.br (K.F. Alvarenga). 1.6 ms per year following installment of a cochlear implant [12].

0165-5876/$ – see front matter ß 2012 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijporl.2012.06.001
K.F. Alvarenga et al. / International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology 76 (2012) 1332–1338 1333

Table 1
Summary of previously published mean P1 component latencies (SD) in normally hearing children 0–12 years.

Age (y) P1 (ms)

Ohlrich Barnet [4] Satterfield Satterfield Sharma Albrecht et al. [3] Ponton Ceponiene Gilley
and and et al. [10] et al. [11] et al. [8] et al. [5] et al. [6]
Barnet [7] Braley [9] LH RH

1 (month) 63.0 (10) 65.0 (24) – – – – – – – –


6 (months) 89.0 (39) 99.0 (27) – – – – – – – –
1 66.0 (20) 65.0 (11) – – – – – – – –
2 – 76.0 (19) – – – – – – – –
3 – 67.0 (27) – – – – – – – 107.0 (10)
4 – – – – – – – – 110.0 (14) 107.0 (10)
5 – – – – – 106.0 (9.0) 92.0 (25) 85.0 (16) – 99.0 (21)
6 – – 85.6 (19) 81.33 87.0 (14) 106.0 (9.0) 92.0 (25) 85.0 (16) – 99.0 (21)
7 – – 85.6 (19) 81.33 81.0 (10) 105.0 (10) 94.0 (11) 79.0 (22) – 100.0 (16)
8 – – 89.4 (16) 86.44 79.0 (10) 105.0 (10) 94.0 (11) 66.0 (13) – 100.0 (16)
9 – – 89.4 (16) 86.44 81.0 (5) 99.0 (13) 88.0 (12) 68.0 (16) 111.0 (21) 89 (17)
10 – – 97.8 (6) 95.31 74.0 (18) 99.0 (13) 88.0 (12) 64.0 (6) – 89 (17)
11 – – 97.8 (6) 95.31 78.0 (11) 84.0 (18) 80.0 (26) 61.0 (10) – 82.0 (8)
12 – – 97.8 (6) 95.31 74.0 (15) 84.0 (18) 80.0 (26) 54.0 (16) – 82.0 (8)

LH, left hemisphere; RH, right hemisphere.

In this context, several studies have evaluated auditory function correlation was observed between AEP parameters and speech
in cochlear implant users to elucidate how the acoustic signal perception scores.
processing occurs. Moreover, LLAEPs in implanted individuals with A study examining juvenile hearing aid users (6 months to 7.6
sensory hearing loss (as opposed to ANSD) have been studied years of age) showed a correlation between cortical potentials (P1,
extensively [13–21], and LLAEP findings have been correlated with N1, and P2) and performance on the phonetically balanced
their speech perception performance [22–35]. Thus, the applica- kindergarten speech perception test [39]. The cortical potentials,
bility of LLAEP recording to assessment of the auditory system in which were evoked by tone burst and speech stimuli, were present
cochlear implant users has been well demonstrated. in 17/18 children with sensory hearing loss, but only 11/18
Previous studies have demonstrated a correlation between children with auditory neuropathy. The latencies and amplitudes
duration of cochlear implant use and the latency of LLAEP of the cortical potentials did not correlate with subject age or
components, which gradually decreases [14,16,18,36,37]. Howev- hearing loss type. There was, however, a trend towards an increase
er, when the relationship between P1 component latency and in latency for the P1 and N1 components with increased degree of
duration of sensory deprivation was examined, it was observed hearing impairment. Furthermore, there was a strong correlation
that later cochlear implant implantation was associated with between the occurrence of cortical potentials and speech percep-
reduced variability of the P1 component latency over repeated tion score in subjects using hearing aids, indicating that cortical
recordings, independent of duration of cochlear implant use [37]. potentials can be used as an index of perceptual skills and as a
Assessment LLAEP recordings, which are thought to be means of assessing the benefits of amplification.
reflective of acoustic signal processing, in cochlear implant users The relationship between speech identification scores and the
with ANSD may provide important information for understanding latencies and amplitudes of AEP components (P1, N1 and P2) was
the variability of speech perception performance results reported also previously investigated in a quiet setting in 10 individuals
in this population. Prior studies analyzing the LLAEP in individuals with auditory neuropathy and 10 subjects with normal hearing,
with ANSD, however, have not included cochlear implant users in ranging in age from 12 to 39 years [40]. The auditory neuropathy
their cohorts. For example, the AEPs and psychophysical abilities group had significantly worse speech identification scores than the
were studied in 14 adults with ANSD with the aim of characterizing normal hearing group. Moreover, the mean N1/P2 range in a
their perceptual capacities [38]. The P2/N2 complex was recorded subgroup of the auditory neuropathy subjects who showed poor
in all subjects and the P1/N1 complex was obtained in 10/14 speech identification was significantly lower than that of normal
subjects; however, when present, the P1/N1 presented with hearing subjects as well as that of the auditory neuropathy subjects
latencies and amplitudes within the normal range, and no in the good speech identification subgroup. Performance in the

Table 2
Summary of previously published mean P1 component amplitudes (SD) in normally hearing children 0–12 years.

Age (y) P1 (mV)

Sharma et al. [11] Satterfield et al. [10] Albrecht et al. [3] Ponton et al. [8] Ceponiene et al. [5] Gilley et al. [6]

LH RH

3 – – – – – – 2.3 (0.9)
4 – – – – – 3.64 (1.16) 2.3 (0.9)
5 – – 2.3 (0.9) 1.3 (0.8) 1.86 (0.56) – 3.2 (1.6)
6 2.6 (0.9) 5.14 2.3 (0.9) 1.3 (0.8) 1.86 (0.56) – 3.2 (1.6)
7 2.4 (0.7) 5.14 2.8 (1.5) 1.4 (0.7) 1.51 (0.60) – 3.6 (1.4)
8 1.8 (1.1) 3.88 2.8 (1.5) 1.4 (0.7) 1.36 (0.61) – 3.6 (1.4)
9 2.6 (1.6) 3.88 2.6 (1.1) 1.8 (0.9) 1.80 (0.82) 4.58 (2.3) 1.6 (0.7)
10 2.0 (1.1) 4.22 2.6 (1.1) 1.8 (0.9) 1.45 (0.57) – 1.6 (0.7)
11 2.0 (0.9) 4.22 0.9 (0.8) 0.8 (0.7) 0.86 (0.56) – 1.9 (1.0)
12 1.6 (1.2) 4.22 0.9 (0.8) 0.8 (0.7) 0.66 (0.56) – 1.9 (1.0)

LH, left hemisphere; RH, right hemisphere.


1334 K.F. Alvarenga et al. / International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology 76 (2012) 1332–1338

Table 3
Demographic and clinical characteristics of the present study sample.

Subject Sex Age at testing Age at surgery Cochlear implant device

Internal component Processor Signal processing


model strategy

1 F 8y3m 2y9m Nucleus 24 K Sprint ACE


2 F 6y1m 4y HiRes 90 K Platinum HR-P
Sound Fidelity 120
3 F 6y5m 1y10m Nucleus 24 K Sprint ACE
4 F 8y3m 3y Nucleus 24 K Sprint ACE
5 F 10y3m 3y10m Nucleus 24 K Sprint ACE
6 M 11y 6y2m Nucleus 24 K Sprint ACE
7 M 6y6m 1y10m Nucleus 24 K Sprint ACE
8 F 6y 1y9m Nucleus 24 K Sprint ACE
9 M 5y8m 2y2m Pulsar CI 100 Opus 1 FSP
10 M 4y10m 3y HiRes 90 K Harmony HR-P
Fidelity 120
11 F 8y11m 3y5m Nucleus 24 K Sprint ACE
12 F 11y1m 4y3m Nucleus 24 K Sprint ACE
13 M 9y4m 4y9m Nucleus 24 K Sprint ACE
14 M 9y2m 4y4m Pulsar CI 100 Opus 1 FSP

F, female; M, male; ages are given in condensed combination of years and months (y, m).

speech identification test correlated with the amplitude of cortical model, processor model, and signal processing strategy are
potentials (N1/P2 complex) but not with the latency of cortical summarized in Table 3. In all cases, the diagnosis of ANSD was
potentials. The authors concluded that measuring cortical poten- made by an interdisciplinary team based on the results of pre-
tial can provide a means of predicting perceptual skills in surgical tests. The individuals enrolled in this study did not have
individuals with auditory neuropathy. any other neurological impairments beyond ANSD, nor did they
The morphology, latency, and amplitude of the P1 component exhibit other changes that could compromise auditory and
were used to evaluate physiological maturity in 21 juvenile language development. Patients with hypoplasia or agenesis of
hearing aid users with ANSD, aged 9 months to 11.5 years. And the auditory nerve detected by magnetic resonance imaging in pre-
those data were analyzed relative to the children’s auditory operative testing were excluded from the study.
behavioral development as reflected by their infant-toddler Each subject’s skin was cleaned with NuPrep abrasive gel before
meaningful auditory integration scale (IT-MAIS) scores [41]. The the experiment. Disposable MeditraceTM 200 electrodes were
authors used the speech stimulus /ba/, presented in at a free-field used with TEM 20TM conductive gel for the recording of LLAEPs
intensity of 75 dB hearing level (HL). The P1 records were classified and eye-evoked potentials. The impedance of the electrodes was
into three types: normal latency and morphology; normal maintained between 1 and 3 kV. LLAEPs were recording using the
morphology with increased latency; and absent or abnormal two-channel Smart EP USB Jr. system produced by Intelligent
response. The group with normal P1 morphology and latency Hearing Systems. The P1 component of LLAEP research was
showed the expected decrease in P1 latency with age, and also conducted following previous study [12]. The electrodes were
showed greater development of listening skills as reflected by their placed so that the recording of evoked potentials occurred through
IT-MAIS scores. Thus, the authors concluded that P1 appears to be a channel A and the recording of eye movements and blinking was
good predictor of behavioral outcomes in juveniles with ANSD and made through channel B.
suggested that this component could be used as a clinical tool to For channel A, the active electrode was positioned at Cz and
guide decisions related to intervention and to evaluate the connected to the (+) input of the pre-amplifier and the reference
effectiveness of this response in this population. electrode was placed on the earlobe on the side with the cochlear
In light of the aforementioned findings, the primary aim of this implant (A1/A2) and connected to the ( ) input. The ground
study was to characterize the P1 component of the LLAEP in electrode was placed at Fpz, connected to the ground position. For
implanted individuals with ANSD and correlate parameters of the channel B, the active electrode was placed in the supra-orbital
P1 component with speech perception performance. Our secondary position contralateral to the implant and plugged into the (+) input
aim was to correlate parameters of the P1 component with other of the pre-amplifier and the reference electrode was placed in the
variables related to the use of cochlear implants. infra-orbital position on the same side and connected to the ( )
input. This arrangement of electrodes was used to allow us to
2. Materials and methods establish the extent of eye movement and blinking before
recording AEPS so that we could define the appropriate level of
2.1. Setting and subjects rejection within each test, and thereby minimize artifacts related
to eye movement.
This study was conducted at the Center for Audiological The speech sample was constructed in an acoustically treated
Research (Centro de Pesquisas Audiológicas, CPA) at the University of room of the Laboratory of Phonetics and Psycholinguistics
São Paulo, and approved by the Ethics Committee of the National (Laboratorio de Fonetica e Psicolinguistica, LAFAPE) of the Institute
Survey on Case No. 181/2004. The parents of the enrolled subjects of Language Studies (Instituto de Estudos da Linguagem, IEL), at the
were informed of the purpose of the study and provided informed Universidade Estadual de Campinas (UNICAMP) [42]. The emis-
consent at the time of testing. sions were recorded via a unidirectional microphone, placed
A total of 14 boys and girls, 4–11 years of age were enrolled in directly into the PC card, through the open source program Praat
the study. All 14 subjects had profound prelingual hearing loss (www.praat.org), in a 22-kHz sample. The voice of a young adult
attributable to ANSD, received treatment at the CPA and were male, 22 years of age, with fluid voice quality was used for the
cochlear implant users. The characteristics of the sample, in terms standard speech signal. The syllable /ba/ was extracted from the
of gender ratio, age at testing, age at surgery, internal component second syllable of the utterance of the word /ba’ba/. F1, F2, and F3
K.F. Alvarenga et al. / International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology 76 (2012) 1332–1338 1335

Table 4
Inter-observer comparison of parameters of the P1 component by paired t-test and Dahlberg’s method (1940) of evaluating systematic error and random error.

P1 1st 2nd Difference t p Random


parameter observer observer error

Mean SD Mean SD

Latency (ms) 110.00 29.506 107.45 28.133 2.545 0.955 0.362 6.22
Amplitude (mV) 2.80 1.358 2.83 1.372 0.029 0.335 0.744 0.20

p  0.05, statistically significant.

values for the initial and stable portions of the isolated syllable The systematic error is significant and its interpretation indicates
were verified. Bandwidths were taken from the stable frequency that an observer tends to identify larger values of p  0.05. The
forming region. With these values, we compiled a script in Praat random error is an ‘‘average’’ value of the error on the marking of
(version 2.4.31) and a re-synthesized syllable. The duration of the components presented in the same unit that was measured, in this
synthesized syllable /ba/ was 180 ms. The linguistic stimuli study the latency in ms and amplitude in mV.
produced, manipulated, and saved on CD at LAFAPE/UNICAMP, The Kolmogorov–Smirnov normality test was used to confirm
were scanned and saved on the hard drive of a computer running normal distributions of the datasets, and then parametric tests
USB Smart EP Jr. software. The potentials evoked by acoustic were applied. Pearson’s test was used to test for P1 component
stimuli presented to the individuals were recorded by the USB correlations with duration of cochlear implant use and duration of
Smart EP Jr. system. sensorial hearing deprivation.
The speech stimulus was presented with a 526-ms inter- The k-means clustering procedure was used to divide the
stimulus interval, at an intensity of 70 dB HL and a presentation subjects into two groups based on their GASP test results. Student’s
rate of 1.9 stimuli/s. The stimuli were applied with a band-pass t-tests were used to compare the LLAEP parameters between the
filter of 10–30 Hz, and a gain of 100 dB in both channels. On groups. In all analyses p  0.05 was adopted as the significance
average, 512 stimuli were applied within each test session. The level.
response analysis window extended from 100 ms prestimulus to
500 ms poststimulus. The procedure was performed in an open
field, with the speaker positioned at a 908 azimuth, 40 cm away 3. Results
from the implanted ear.
The system was calibrated in free field hearing level (dB HL) The two audiologists were found to have good inter-observer
before the start of the study, with the following characteristics: concordance (92.86%, Kappa 0.76). LLAEPs were not recorded in 2/
power amplifier with 30 W of RMS output, together with a 50-W 14 children (14.3%). Systematic error and random error analyses
RMS speaker with tripod. In the (passive) signal input of the were carried out on data from subjects in whom the component
amplifier, we installed an isolating transformer with an input had been identified by both observers. The mean latency (SD) and
impedance of 440 V (impedance equal to the insertion headphone amplitude (SD) for the P1 components recorded in our subjects are
used in audiometry brainstem response (ABR)) and a 5-kV output reported in Table 4 together with the systematic error and random
amplifier. error data. Notably, as shown in Fig. 1, our Pearson’s correlational
The tests were conducted in a quiet environment. Each subject analysis indicated that duration of sensorial hearing deprivation
was seated comfortably in a reclining chair and instructed to watch correlated with the LLAEP P1 latency (p = 0.007, r = 0.7278), but not
a silent video. Once the presence of AEPs was identified in the the LLAEP P1 amplitude (Pearson’s test: p = 0.171, r = 0.4224).
registry through visual observation, the P1 component was Duration of cochlear implant use did not correlate with P1 latency
identified and its latency and amplitude were analyzed. The (p = 0.181, r = 0.4142) or P1 amplitude (p = 0.921, r = 0.0323).
component’s amplitude was determined as the difference between A descriptive statistical analysis of sensory deprivation dura-
the point corresponding to 0.0 mV (baseline record) and the tion, cochlear implant use duration, P1 latency, and P1 amplitude
maximum positive value. The P1 component’s latency was for subjects whose performance placed them in the lower
considered to end at the point of maximum amplitude. The performing cluster (Group 1) and the higher performing cluster
records were analyzed separately by two audiologists experienced (Group 2) on the GASP test are reported in Table 5. After the
in auditory electrophysiology to verify the concordance of the subjects were divided into the two GASP performance groups
analysis. (Group 1: subjects 5, 6, 9, 10, and 11; Group 2: subjects 1, 2, 3, 4, 7,
Tests 5 and 6 of the GASP [43,44] were applied and analyzed, 8, 12, 13, and 14) by the k-means clustering procedure (Fig. 2),
enabling us to classify speech perception ability into two levels:
word recognition and comprehension of sentences. The tests were
conducted in a soundproof booth and applied through the MD622
Madsen audiometer at the intensity of 60 dB HL with presentation
in free field at a 08 azimuth. To remove variability in how the tests
were applied, stimuli recorded on CD [45] were utilized, and at the
beginning of each test a calibration of the audiometer volume unit
meter was performed. The test 5 results (range, 0–100%) were
analyzed and test 6 was applied when the subject had a score 50%
on test 5.
The analysis of inter-observer variations was analyzed by Kappa
statistic, which assesses the concordance between the observers
through the paired analysis and shows the percentage of
concordance, which is interpreted by the value of Kappa. The
paired t-error calculation and the error calculation were used to Fig. 1. Statistically significant (*p  0.05) relationship between P1 latency and
check the systematic and random error in inter-observer analysis. duration of sensory deprivation. *p  0.05, statistically significant.
1336 K.F. Alvarenga et al. / International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology 76 (2012) 1332–1338

Table 5
Descriptive statistical analysis of the P1 component parameters and clinical characteristics of subjects separated by speech perception performance.

Feature Group 1 (lower performers) Group 2 (higher performers)

Mean SD Min Max Mean SD Min Max

Duration of sensorial deprivation 47.80 15.658 37 75 40.55 15.597 23 63


Duration of cochlear implant use 49.00 23.569 20 75 52.88 13.977 24 70
Latency 134.25 26.132 102 166 97.00 20.709 67 132
Amplitude 2.12 0.471 1.5 2.49 3.00 1.551 0.8 5.45

Min, minimum; Max, maximum.

Student’s t-tests revealed that Group 2 had significantly shorter P1 potential of the auditory nerve that relays encoding signals from
component latencies than Group 1 (p = 0.022). the cochlea through the midbrain up to the auditory cortex. In
children with congenital ANSD, it has been speculated that sensory
deprivation may alter the maturation process of central auditory
4. Discussion system structures. Some hearing aid users with ANSD have been
reported to show an increase in the latency of the P1 component,
The development and neural organization of cortical struc- suggesting that the central auditory system is hyporesponsive and
tures are influenced by sensory experiences, that is, by that this hyporesponsivity correlates with compromised speech
stimulation from the environment [12]. Hearing loss during perception [39,41].
the period of greatest plasticity (i.e., the first years of life) results Since this is the first study, to the best of our knowledge, to
in sensory deprivation, which prevents normal growth and examine the P1 component of the LLAEP in cochlear implant users
proper formation of the connections necessary to form a with ANSD, it is difficult to compare our findings directly to those
functional sensory system. Thus, children with a history of of previous studies, remembering that stimulation of the auditory
early sensorial deprivation present anatomical and functional system in individuals with ANSD will differ from that in individuals
changes in their auditory system, which in turn affects the with sensory hearing loss. The strong inter-observer concordance
results they can achieve using hearing devices, such as hearing observed in our study (Kappa = 0.76, substantial), with no
aids or cochlear implants, since the nervous system is rendered significant systematic errors for latency and amplitude
unable to adequately process afferent activity [17,28,36]. (p = 0.362, p = 0.744) and a random error of 6.22 ms for latency
Importantly, if the development of central auditory structures and 0.2 mV for amplitude, provide confidence in our inter-observer
does not occur normally, it can be assumed that the perceptual reliability, which is important given the subjectivity that can be
skills that are the basis for speech perception and production involved in AEP analysis.
will not develop normally either. On the other hand, with early Our finding that it was possible to register the P1 component in
diagnosis and intervention, while the central auditory system is 85.7% of our subjects shows that electrical stimulation from the
highly plastic and degeneration has not been extensive, cochlear implant supplied the initial neural dyssynchrony and
restoration of function may be possible with a prosthesis such generated a P1 component with typical morphology, similar to that
as the cochlear implant [46]. observed in implanted individuals who suffer from sensory
The present findings complement prior research indicating that hearing loss [26,31,34], in hearing children [40,48], and in children
the P1 component correlates with speech perception ability and with ANSD who are using hearing aids [39,41].
that a shorter time of deprivation is associated with a P1 In children diagnosed with ANSD, indicating the use of a
component that more closely resembles that found in hearing cochlear implant requires the assurance that the hearing aid does
children [22–39,47]. ANSD is characterized by compromised not bring benefits to speech perception, a result observed in a small
neural synchrony, which is the basis for the compound action percentage of children, but likely to occur. This reflects directly on
the age to indicate the cochlear implant, usually around two years
old according to international recommendations of the NHS
Confederation (2008), and consequently the time of sensory
deprivation.
In this study, we observed a significant, positive correlation
between duration of sensorial hearing deprivation and P1
component latency. A longer duration of sensory deprivation
was associated with a greater P1 component latency. This result
confirms that dyssynchrony observed in these children can also
lead to an abnormal pattern of maturation of the auditory system
[16,34,37]. Such findings have important ramifications for clinical
decisions for children with ANSD and the optimal time for
provision of cochlear implants. One must question whether
carrying out testing with hearing aids in this population for an
extended period of time may be preventing stimulation of the
auditory system that could be achieved with a cochlear implant in
a period crucial to the development of cortical structures and
acquisition of listening skills and language.
Our negative finding of no correlation between P1 component
parameters and duration of cochlear implant use differs from
Fig. 2. Speech perception performance of Group 1 (lower performers) and Group 2 previously reported findings in cochlear implant users with
(higher performers), divided by application of k-means clustering technique to sensory hearing loss [14,16–18,37]. This unexpected finding
GASP tests 5 and 6. should be researched further with controls for more variables,
K.F. Alvarenga et al. / International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology 76 (2012) 1332–1338 1337

such as the stimulation the child receives, family collaboration and [11] A. Sharma, N. Kraus, T.J. McGee, T.G. Nicol, Developmental changes in P1 and N1
central auditory responses elicited by consonant–vowel syllables, Electroence-
involvement, and participation in speech therapy, among other phalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 104 (November (6)) (1997) 540–545.
variables, which can affect the results achieved with a cochlear [12] L.M. Ventura, O.A. Costa, K.F. Alvarenga, Central hearing system maturation in
implant. normally hearing children, Pró-Fono Revista de Atualização Cientı́fica 21 (abr–jun
(2)) (2009).
The dissociation of P1 component latency that we observed [13] M. Pelizzone, A. Kasper, R. Hari, J. Karhu, P. Montandon, Bilateral electrical
between subjects with better versus worse performance in the stimulation of a congenitally-deaf ear and of an acquired-deaf ear, Acta Otolar-
GASP test suggests that prior development of central auditory yngol. 111 (2) (1991) 263–268.
[14] C.W. Ponton, M. Don, J.J. Eggermont, M.D. Waring, B. Kwong, A. Masuda, Auditory
structures, reflected in the P1 component, is related to real system plasticity in children after long periods of complete deafness, Neuroreport
differences in behavioral auditory skills [30,38–40]. That is, our 8 (1) (1996) 61–65.
findings indicate that a lesser P1 latency is predictive of better [15] C.W. Ponton, M. Don, J.J. Eggermont, M.D. Waring, A. Masuda, Maturation of
human cortical auditory function: differences between normal-hearing children
GASP test performance.
and children with cochlear implants, Ear Hear. 17 (5) (1996) 430–437.
The two children (14.3%) for which LLAEPs could not be [16] J.J. Eggermont, C.W. Ponton, M. Don, M.D. Waring, B. Kwong, Maturational delays
detected had vastly different speech perception performance on in cortical evoked potentials in cochlear implant users, Acta Otolaryngol. 117 (2)
the GASP test. One scored 0% on the sentence comprehension (1997) 161–397.
[17] A. Sharma, M.F. Dorman, A.J. Spahr, Rapid development of cortical auditory
part of the GASP test, and the other scored 100%. The unexpected evoked potentials after early cochlear implantation, Neuroreport 13 (10)
finding of an absence of the P1 component in the child with good (2002) 1365–1368.
speech proficiency, in principle, could be explained by the way [18] P.W. Bauer, A. Sharma, K. Martin, M. Dorman, Central auditory development in
children with bilateral cochlear implants, Arch. Otolaryngol. Head Neck Surg. 132
the LLAEPs were evoked: through a passive paradigm, in which (10) (2006) 1133–1136.
the child was not asked to pay attention to the stimuli [19] S. Burdo, S. Razza, F. Di Berardino, G. Tognola, Auditory cortical responses in
presented. Attending to the stimulus can have a major effect patients with cochlear implants, Acta Otorhinolaryngol. Ital. 26 (2) (2006) 69–77.
[20] L. Friesen, K. Tremblay, Acoustic change complexes recorded in adult cochlear
on the recording of cortical potentials in individuals with implant listeners, Ear Hear. 27 (6) (2006) 678–685.
auditory neuropathy; attending has been reported to shift [21] J. Guiraud, S. Gallego, L. Arnold, P. Boyle, E. Truy, L. Collet, Effects of auditory
latency or even produce a shift from the absence to the presence pathway anatomy and deafness characteristics? Part 2: On electrically evoked
late auditory responses, Hear. Res. 228 (1–2) (2007) 44–57.
of a response [48]. [22] P.A. Groenen, M. Makhdoum, J.L. van den Brink, M.H. Stollman, A.F. Snik, P. van
Correlation between a P1 component parameter and speech den Broek, The relation between electric auditory brain stem and cognitive
perception is extremely important since in children with ANSD, responses and speech perception in cochlear implant users, Acta Otolaryngol.
116 (6) (1996) 785–790.
determining the functional gain with a hearing aid (i.e., the
[23] P.R. Kileny, A. Boerst, T. Zwolan, Cognitive evoked potentials to speech and tonal
threshold for behavioral audiometric frequencies of speech) has stimuli in children with implants, Otolaryngol. Head Neck Surg. 117 (3) (1997)
no predictive value for speech perception performance. Thus, as 161–169.
found in children with sensory hearing loss, in children with [24] M. Okusa, T. Shiraishi, T. Kubo, Y. Nageishi, Effects of discrimination difficulty on
cognitive event-related brain potentials in patients with cochlear implants,
ANSD, the P1 component has the potential to serve as a predictor of Otolaryngol. Head Neck Surg. 121 (5) (1999) 610–615.
performance in those who use cochlear implants for speech [25] A.J. Beynon, A.F. Snik, P. Van den Broek, Evaluation of cochlear implant
perception [39,48–50]. benefit with auditory cortical evoked potentials, Int. J. Audiol. 41 (7)
(2002) 429–435.
In conclusion, it was possible to register the P1 component in [26] J. Maurer, L. Collet, H. Pelster, E. Truy, S. Gallégo, Auditory late cortical response
implanted children diagnosed with ANSD, and P1 component and speech recognition in Digisonic cochlear implant users, Laryngoscope 112
latency correlated with speech perception performance and (12) (2002) 2220–2224.
[27] C. Pantev, B. Ross, A. Wollbrink, M. Riebandt, K.W. Delank, E. Seifert, et al.,
with duration of sensory deprivation. These findings indicate Acoustically and electrically evoked responses of the human cortex before and
that, in children with ANSD, the P1 component has the potential after cochlear implantation, Hear. Res. 171 (1–2) (2002) 191–195.
to serve as a marker of the development of auditory cortical [28] A. Sharma, E. Tobey, M. Dorman, S. Bharadwaj, K. Martin, P. Gilley, et al., Central
auditory maturation and babbling development in infants with cochlear
structures and a predictor of the child’s potential for speech
implants, Arch. Otolaryngol. Head Neck Surg. 130 (5) (2004) 511–516.
perception. [29] S. Singh, A. Liasis, K. Rajput, A. Towell, L. Luxon, Event-related potentials in
pediatric cochlear implant patients, Ear Hear. 25 (6) (2004) 598–610.
[30] K.A. Gordon, S. Tanaka, B.C. Papsin, Atypical cortical responses underlie poor
References speech perception in children using cochlear implants, Neuroreport 16 (18)
(2005) 2041–2045.
[31] A.S. Kelly, S.C. Purdy, P.R. Thorne, Electrophysiological and speech perception
[1] C. Ponton, J.J. Eggermont, D. Khosla, B. Kwong, M. Don, Maturation of human measures of auditory processing in experienced adult cochlear implant users,
central auditory system activity: separating auditory evoked potentials by dipole Clin. Neurophysiol. 116 (6) (2005) 1235–1246.
source modeling, Clin. Neurophysiol. 113 (3) (2002) 407–420. [32] S. Roman, G. Canévet, P. Marquis, J.M. Triglia, C. Liégeois-Chauvel, Relationship
[2] M.F. Dorman, A. Sharma, P. Gilley, K. Martin, P. Roland, Central auditory develop- between auditory perception skills and mismatch negativity recorded in free field
ment: evidence from CAEP measurements in children fit with cochlear implants, J. in cochlear-implant users, Hear. Res. 201 (1–2) (2005) 10–20.
Commun. Disord. 40 (4) (2007) 284–294. [33] C. McNeill, M. Sharma, S.C. Purdy, Are cortical auditory evoked potentials useful in
[3] R. Albrecht, W.V. Suchodoletz, R. Uwer, The development of auditory evoked the clinical assessment of adults with cochlear implants? Cochlear Implants Int.
dipole source activity from childhood to adulthood, Clin. Neurophysiol. 111 (12) 10 (Suppl. 1) (2009) 78–84.
(2000) 2268–2276. [34] K.A. Gordon, S. Tanaka, D.D. Wong, B.C. Papsin, Characterizing responses from
[4] A.B. Barnet, Auditory evoked potentials during sleep in normal children from ten auditory cortex in young people with several years of cochlear implant experi-
days to three years of age, Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 39 (1) (1975) ence, Clin. Neurophysiol. 119 (10) (2008) 2347–2362.
29–41. [35] C. McNeill, M. Sharma, S.C. Purdy, K. Agung, Cortical auditory evoked responses
[5] R. Ceponiene, T. Rinne, R. Näätänen, Maturation of cortical sound processing as from an implanted ear after 50 years of profound unilateral deafness, Cochlear
indexed by event-related potentials, Clin. Neurophysiol. 113 (6) (2002) 870–882. Implants Int. 8 (4) (2007) 189–997.
[6] P.M. Gilley, A. Sharma, M. Dorman, K. Martin, Developmental changes in refrac- [36] A. Sharma, M.F. Dorman, A.J. Spahr, A sensitive period for the development of the
toriness of the cortical auditory evoked potential, Clin. Neurophysiol. 116 (2005) central auditory system in children with cochlear implants: implications for age
648–657. of implantation, Ear Hear. 23 (6) (2002) 532–539.
[7] E.S. Ohlrich, A.B. Barnet, Auditory evoked responses during the first year of life, [37] A. Sharma, M.F. Dorman, A. Kral, The influence of a sensitive period on central
Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 32 (2) (1972) 161–169. auditory development in children with unilateral and bilateral cochlear implants,
[8] C.W. Ponton, J.J. Eggermont, B. Kwong, M. Don, Maturation of human central Hear. Res. 203 (1–2) (2005) 134–143.
auditory system activity: evidence from multi-channel evoked potentials, Clin. [38] A.U. Kumar, M. Jayaram, Auditory processing in individuals with auditory neu-
Neurophysiol. 111 (2) (2000) 220–236. ropathy, Behav. Brain Funct. 1 (2005) 21.
[9] J.H. Satterfield, B.W. Braley, Evoked potentials and brain maturation in hyperac- [39] G. Rance, B. Cone-Wesson, J. Wunderlich, R. Dowell, Speech perception and
tive and normal children, Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 43 (1) (1977) cortical event related potentials in children with auditory neuropathy, Ear Hear.
43–51. 23 (3) (2002) 239–253.
[10] J.H. Satterfield, A.M. Schell, R.W. Backs, K.C. Hidaka, A cross-sectional and longi- [40] V.K. Narne, C. Vanaja, Speech identification and cortical potentials in individuals
tudinal study of age effects of electrophysiological measures in hyperactive and with auditory neuropathy, Behav. Brain Funct. 4 (2008) 15.
normal children, Biol. Psychiatry 19 (7) (1984) 973–990.
1338 K.F. Alvarenga et al. / International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology 76 (2012) 1332–1338

[41] A. Sharma, G. Cardon, K. Henion, P. Roland, Cortical maturation and behavioral [46] P.M. Gilley, A. Sharma, M.F. Dorman, Cortical reorganization in children with
outcomes in children with auditory neuropathy spectrum disorder, Int. J. Audiol. cochlear implants, Brain Res. 1239 (2008) 56–65.
50 (2) (2011) 98–106. [47] K. Kurnaz, B. Satar, S. Yetiser, Evaluation of cochlear implant users’ performance
[42] M. Banhara, Potenciais auditivos de longa latência: N1, P2, N2 e P300 evocados using middle and late latency responses, Eur. Arch. Otorhinolaryngol. 266 (3)
por estı́mulo de fala em usuários de implante coclear, Dissertação (Mestrado), (2009) 343–350.
Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, 2007. [48] H.J. Michalewski, A. Starr, T.T. Nguyen, Y.Y. Kong, F.G. Zeng, Auditory temporal
[43] M.C. Bevilacqua, E.A. Tech, Elaboração de um procedimento de avaliação de processes in normal-hearing individuals and in patients with auditory neuropa-
percepção de fala em crianças deficientes auditivas profundas a partir de cinco thy, Clin. Neurophysiol. 116 (3) (2005) 669–680.
anos de idade, in: I.Q. Marchesan, J.M. Zorzi, I.C.D. Gomes (Org.), Tópicos em [49] W. Pearce, M. Golding, H. Dillon, Cortical auditory evoked potentials in the
Fonoaudiologia, Lovise, São Paulo, 1996, pp. 411–433. assessment of auditory neuropathy: two case studies, J. Am. Acad. Audiol. 18
[44] N.P. Erber, Auditory Training, Alexander Graham Bell Association for the Deaf, (5) (2007) 380–390.
Washington, 1982. [50] B. Cone, The electrophysiology of auditory neuropathy spectrum disorder, in:
[45] C.A. Quintino, Desenvolvimento de um software para avaliação da percepção de Guidelines for Identification and Management of Infants and Young Children with
fala em crianças deficientes auditivas, Dissertação (Mestrado), Universidade de Auditory Neuropathy Spectrum Disorder, NHS Confederation, Aurora, 2008, pp.
São Paulo, São Paulo, 2007. 20–27.

You might also like