Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Adolescent Mental Health

Sally I. Powers University of Massachusetts at Amherst


and Harvard Medical School
Stuart T. Hauser Harvard Medical School and
Massachusetts Mental Health Center
Linda A. Kilner Harvard University and
McLean Hospital

I I
ABSTRACT." Definitions and descriptions of adolescent is that a healthy adolescent should appear to have a mild
mental health are beginning to be grounded in psychol- case of mental illness. In other words, many of the symp-
ogists" empirical studies of a wide variety of patterns of toms or serious diff culfies that would signal mental illness
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

adaptation to adolescence. Three major themes guiding in adulthood or childhood are understood to be a normal
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

current research relevant to mental health in adolescence part of adolescence. Adolescents may be expected to be
are discussed in this article." (a) a focus on analysis of extremely moody and depressed one day and excitedly
interindividual differences to describe variety in adapta- "high" the next. Explosive conflict with family, friends,
tion; (b) the integration of biological, psychological, social, and authorities is thought of as commonplace. This pop-
and cultural variables in models of adolescence; and (c) ular view is, to a great extent, based on the theoretical
an emphasis on the developmental aspects of adolescent work of the psychiatric clinical community in the 1950s
mental health. Empirical investigations of adolescent and early 1960s (Blos, 1962; Erikson, 1959, 1966, 1968;
mental health and development within the social context Freud, 1946, 1958). Largely on the basis of their expe-
of the family are reviewed. rience with adolescent psychiatric patients, these clini-
cians and theorists described adolescence as necessarily
a time of extreme psychic and interpersonal stress. Emo-
Adolescent psychology is in the midst of writing a new tional crisis and upheaval were viewed as appropriate re-
definition of adolescent mental health--a definition based sponses by the adolescent to major psychological and so-
on the description of a wide variety of patterns of adap- cietal tasks required during this phase of life: dramatically
tation to the biological, psychological, and social chal- reducing psychological dependency on parents, separating
lenges of adolescence. Psychologists' current understand- from the family, and forming an adult identity. Adolescent
ing of the range and variety of these developmental paths, turmoil was not only inevitable but necessary for sub-
strengths, and profiles of adaptation is central to this ar- sequent normal personality integration.
ticle. Toward a New Understanding of Adolescent
An earlier, and still popular, understanding of ado- Mental Health
lescent mental health viewed adolescence as a time of
profound inner turmoil and outward conflictua time of The storm and stress theory of adolescent mental health
"storm and stress." We briefly discuss this previous un- brought attention to the considerable number of physical,
derstanding of adolescent mental health in an effort to social, and psychological changes experienced by adoles-
place psychologists' present understanding in context. We cents in this culture. The theory emphasized the stress-
then review three major themes guiding current research fulness of managing these concurrent changes, and the
relevant to mental health in adolescence: (a) a focus on adolescent's attempts to cope with these changes were
analysis of interindividual differences to describe variety accorded dignity. In addition, the storm and stress per-
in adaptation; (b) the integration of biological, psycho- spective of adolescent mental health invigorated the
logical, social, and cultural variables in models of ado- scholarly community in the mid-1960s and prompted
lescence; and (c) an emphasis on the developmental as- important new research. This research was stimulated by
pects of adolescent mental health. Research examining the idea that adolescence is necessarily and typically a
reciprocal effects between adolescent development and time of emotional disequilibrium and unpredictable be-
family relationships is then reviewed. havior (Bandura, 1964; Douvan & Adelson, 1966; Offer,
1969; Offer & Offer, 1975). Data from study after study
Prior Definition: Adolescent Mental Health were analyzed to determine whether the majority of ad-
as Turmoil olescents exhibited serious psychological turmoil. These
empirical analyses led to a radical revision in the psy-
To be normal during the adolescentperiod is by itselfabnormal.
--A. Freud, 1958, p. 275 chological community's understanding of adolescent de-
velopment and mental health.
One understanding of adolescent mental health, of- The majority of this research of the past 20 years
ten perpetuated by the media and assumed by the public, clearly refutes the notion that most adolescents undergo

200 February 1989 • American Psychologist


Copyright 1989 by the American PsychologicalAssociation, Inc. 00034)66X/89/$00.75
VOl. 44, No. 2, 200-208
severe emotional stress during this period of life. Although average for adolescents or true for the majority of ado-
experts concur that adolescence is a period of development lescents has been a useful one for studying the storm and
that requires multiple changes (in fact, signifiCant pro- stress perspective. An empirical examination of that per-
grams of research have been focused on each major area spective required determination of whether turmoil was
of changembiological, social, and psychological) the cur- a necessary phenomenon for most adolescents. The av-
rent consensus is that adolescence is not ordinarily a time erage/majority definition is less useful, however, for iden-
of great psychological turmoil. Epidemiological studies tifying and describing the range of behavioral and devel-
reveal that only about 10-20% of adolescents exhibit some opmental patterns that indicate adaptive functioning in
type of severe emotional disturbance--approximately the adolescence.
same percentage as in the adult population (Offer, Ostrov, The definition of mental health as an ideal of optimal
& Howard, 1981; Petersen, 1988)) development and adaptive functioning is clearly an im-
Much effort was devoted to clarifying the extent to portant and needed aspect of the study of adolescent
which adolescence was a severely stressful time for all mental health. Some investigators (e.g., Irwin, 1987) rec-
youth. When this global, and perhaps romantic, concept ommend that health should be defined as optimal func-
was finally set aside, researchers and clinicians were left tioning because, in the past, our attention has been con-
with little coherent conception of positive mental health fined almost exclusively to studying indicators of abnor-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

mal functioning. Irwin pointed out how little is known


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

in adolescence. The psychological community was ill in-


formed about the different ways in which well-functioning about factors that contribute to positive growth and
adolescents cope with the unusually high number oftran, health-enhancing behaviors. A better understanding of
sitions required of them. A new understanding of mental "optimal functioning" in adolescence would balance our
health in adolescence was needed. knowledge of adolescent maladaptive functioning. How-
Current perspectives on adolescence are no longer ever, exclusive reliance on this "ideal" or optimal defi-
grounded exclusively in theoretical formulations of what nition of mental health could lead to an ignorance of
should occur in adolescence, but rather are based on em- interindividual differences in adaptation and a limited
pirically derived profiles of adolescent adaptation and (though positive) picture of what adolescent mental health
growth. Most important, the present understanding of should be. The ideal/optimal definition implies a single,
adolescence no longer derives from a single picture of best profile of mental health. If designation of such a
mental health. Instead, a wide variation in adolescent profile is possible, it can be achieved only after a thorough
behaviors, attitudes, social relations, and developmental understanding of the varieties and individual differences
paths is considered relevant to conceptualizing adolescent of mental health profiles.
mental health. Three themes, discussed below, are sig- The third definition of mental health--health as the
nificant when one adopts this multidimensional and plu- absence of clinically diagnosed psychopathologymmay
ralistic view of adolescent mental health. be, at present, the definition most conducive to the study
ofinterindividual differences in mental health. In the past,
Interlndividual Differences in Adolescence this definition has essentially been a negative one: Health
A focus on interindividual differences is particularly was viewed merely as the absence of pathology. Offer and
helpful in describing the entire range of adolescent be- Sabshin (1984) correctly pointed out that a primary re-
haviors and developmental patterns that may be char- liance on this definition has caused us to focus on the
acterized as healthy. Psychologists' attempts to more ac- study of pathology, rather than on health. This need not
curately describe these patterns of mental health in ad- be the inevitable result of this definition. A conception
olescence are problematic, however. In large part, this is of mental health as the absence of psychopathology can
because the concept of health can be defined in several be used simply to define the limits of the range of func-
ways. Offer and Sabshin (1984) discussed four primary
definitions of mental health: 2 (a) mental health as the National Institutesof Health BiomedicalResearchSupportGrant 9878
empirical profile of the statistically average adolescent or to SallyI. Powerssupportedpreparationof this articleby SallyI. Powers
the majority of adolescents, (b) mental health as optimal and Linda A. Kiiner.National Institute of Mental Health Grant K-05-
MH70178-5 ResearchScientistAwardto StuartT. Hauseralsosupported
functioning of the "ideal" adolescent, (c) mental health preparation of this article.
as the absence of clinically diagnosed disorders or symp- Correspondenceconcerningthis articleshouldbe addressedto Sally
toms, and (d) mental health as a result of a changing I. Powers, Department of Psychology,Tobin Hall, Universityof Mas-
system of time and culture. We need not adopt only one sachusetts, Amherst, MA 01003.
definition; each represents meaningful facets of mental
' An important di~culty with the storm and stress viewof adoles-
health. It is important, however, to clarify the definition cence is that it blurs the boundaries between normality and pathology
of mental health that one uses because data will be an- during adolescence.Reviewingthe research consensusthat only a mi-
alyzed and interpreted differently depending on the def- nority of adolescents experiencesevere emotional disturbance, Offer
inition. Some definitions more easily accommodate the (1987)warnedthat a viewof adolescenceas inherentlytumultuous mis-
study of interindividual differences than others and thus takenly causes us to normalize serious emotional and behavioral dis-
turbance in adolescence,running the danger of neglectingto identify
encourage the discovery of the range and variety o f healthy and help those adolescentsin true difficulty.
adaptation in adolescence. 2 Offer and Sabshin (1984) used the terms "mental health" and
The definition of mental health as what is statistically "normality" interchangeably(p. ix).

February 1989 ° American Psychologist 201


tioning that is adaptive in adolescence. At that point, in- teracting influences of social institutions, social roles, and
terindividual differences in patterns of development and pubertal growth trajectories in an adolescent's life. Their
behavior within that ranse may be clarified by research integrative analyses show that if adolescents are com-
and clinical observations.~ For example, different patterns fortable in some of their environments, life arenas, and
of adaptive functioning may be described by determining role relationships, then discomfort in other arenas can be
consistent relations among mental health variables found tolerated and mastered.
for subsets of subjects (Block, 1971). Anthropological studies investigating the interaction
Thus far, however, studies that describe multiple of biological, psychological, and cultural features of ad-
pathways of development and adaptive functioning in ad- olescence are also beginning to inform our characteriza-
olescence are rare (notable exceptions are Block, 1971, tions of adolescent mental health. For example, the cross-
and Offer & Offer, 1975). One focus of our own work cultural and interdisciplinary work of John and Beatrice
(Hauser, Houlihan, et al., 1987; Hauser, Powers, Noam, Whiting and their colleagues (Burbank, 1988; Whiting,
& Bowlds, 1987; Hauser, Jacobson, et. al., in press; Pow- Burbank, &Ratner, 1986; Worthman, 1986) examines
ers, Hauser, & Jacobson, 1985; Powers, 1988, in press) is biological variables (e.g., rate of pubertal development),
the description of multiple types of longitudinal trajec- psychological variables (e.g., cognitive and moral devel-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tories or profiles of adolescent ego and moral development opment), and sociocultural variables (e.g., the transmis-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

throughout the high school years. Within a sample of sion of skills, proper social behavior and values necessary
adolescents with no known psychiatric diagnoses, a va- for adulthood, and initiation rites) in seven cultures. These
riety of types of developmental trajectories are found. studies emphasize that the social structuring of the ad-
Each trajectory type identifies the base levels of ego and olescent experience partly determines the consequences
moral development for subjects in early adolescence and and meaning of changes in adolescence.
characterizes their rate of progression, regression, or sta- Worthman (1986) examines the psychological costs
bility in development throughout high school. We are to adolescents of particular cultural environments and
currently investigating how these varieties of develop- social expectations regarding puberty. Whether or not
mental patterns are related to adolescent adaptive func- physiological changes in the pubertal process are cultur-
tioning and family interactions. ally recognized depends on the society. She notes that
unlike Western societies, the Kikuyu in Kenya mark onset
Interaction of Biological, Psychological, Social, and of puberty with circumcision rites for females. According
Cultural Aspects of Adolescence to Worthman, the Kikuyu structuring of meaningful
Investigators of adolescence are increasingly turning to a maturational experiences, such as an initiation rite, helps
multivariate and systemic approach to understand ado- to deflect anxiety in adolescence. Pubertal changes are
lescent development, adaptation, and mental health thereby normalized and integrated with broader social
(Brooks-Gunn & Petersen, 1983; Hill, 1987; Petersen, processes.
1988). This approach integrates biological, psychological, More research with an explicit focus on the social
social, and cultural variables into the examination of var- context of adolescent mental health and development is
ious patterns of adolescent psychological growth and the needed, even within our own society. The majority of
processes influencing the formation of these patterns. empirical studies of adolescent mental health in the
Analysis of the reciprocal effects of these additional factors United States recognize the potential of contextual vari-
with psychological development is especially critical for ables for influencing the course of adolescent growth and
the study of adolescence because of the significant and adaptation. Contextual variables in these studies are sta-
rapid changes that occur in each of these arenas during tistically or methodologically controlled so that they do
this life phase. This approach demands a greater collab- not obscure the effect of psychological variables of interest.
oration and communication between psychologists and As a result, the vast majority of empirical research on
investigators of adolescence from the disciplines of soci- adolescent mental health is based on purposefully re-
ology, biology, medicine, psychiatry, and anthropology. stricted, homogeneous samples (e.g., White, middle-class
One example of an extensive research program that adolescents from nuclear families). Our knowledge of the
exemplifies this integrative approach is the work of so- variety of patterns of adaptation in adolescents from lower
ciologists Simmons, Blyth, and their colleagues (Blyth, socioeconomic statuses, of different ethnic backgrounds,
Simmons, & Carlton-Ford, 1983; Simmons, 1987; Sim- or from alternate family structures is, therefore, severely
mons & Blyth, 1987) investigating the influence of social limited. An important task for future research will be to
variables (e.g., school structure) and biological variables study the context of adolescents' lives in greater depth
(e.g., rate of pubertal development) on self-esteem. Their and diversity.
inclusion of these dimensions in studies describing ado- Developmental Features of Adolescent Mental Health
lescent adaptation has allowed them to highlight the in-
The study of adolescence is fundamentally the study of
change. Depending on the variable of interest, change or
3Though this definition supportsthe discoveryof a varietyof pat-
terns of mental health, werecognizethat, as withany definitionof mental development in adolescence is usually conceptualized ei-
health, cultural values strongly influence our descriptions of what is ther as a series of qualitative reorganizations of capacities
healthy and what is pathological. (e.g., stages of cognitive and moral development) and be-

202 February 1989 • American Psychologist


haviors or as a more continuous series of incremental Adolescent Mental Health in Family Context
changes (e.g., evolving renegotiations of power within the
family). Regardless of differences in conceptualization of The remaining portion of this article reviews research
the process of development, there is an increasing rec- that investigates adolescent mental health and develop-
ognition of the importance of temporal analyses in studies ment within the social context of the family, l~ychologists'
of adolescence. These temporal analyses focus on tWO changing views of conflict in families of adolescents are
aspects of development. Most basic is the developmental noted, as well as the impact of adolescent individual
status or level of an individual at a particular point in change and development on family relationships. Finally,
adolescence. The level of an adolescent's maturity on a the influence of family behaviors, parental development,
number of developmental indices, both psychological (e.g., and family relationships on adolescents' healthy devel-
level of ego development) and physical (e.g., pubertal ma- opment of autonomy and intimacy are discussed.
turity), has been shown to be empirically related to adap- Research on the familial context of adolescent de-
tive functioning and emotional well-being (Brooks-Gunn velopment is flourishing and is characterized by increasing
& Petersen, 1983; Noam et al., 1984). A second temporal conceptual clarity and empirical rigor (e.g., see Barber &
feature is the timing of development with respect to peers. Rollins, 1987). Although legitimate differences among
Developmental "timing" refers to whether a particular competing theoretical perspectives continue, there is
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

adolescent is "on time," late, or precocious in specific growing agreement on the significance of family ties, pa-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

areas of development. Brooks-Gunn and Ruble (1983) rental models, and reciprocal influences between adoles-
and Klein and Litt (1983) have shown that the sociocul- cents and their families. Not only do families affect ad-
rural meaning attributed to being "on time" or not with olescent development, but aspects of adolescent devel-
regard to aspects of pubertal development can have a sig- opment (e.g., puberty and cognitive changes) affect the
nificant impact on adolescents' self-images. In addition, life of the family.
cross-cultural studies show that timing of pubertal de-
velopment may vary not only by individuals but by coun-
Changing Views of Adolescent-Family Conflict
try. For example, the age of menarche may vary both A pervasive belief, stemming from the storm and
within a society and between societies. In the United stress theory of adolescence discussed earlier, is that
States the median age ofmenarche is 12.8 years for White severe ongoing family conflict should be expected during
Americans and 12.5 for Black Americans, and the range the adolescent years. We now know, from a variety of
for 95% of girls is 9.8 to 15.8 years (Eveleth, 1986). The studies using such diverse methods as epidemiological
median age of menarche is 14.2 years in Czechoslovakia surveys (Rutter, Graham, Chadwick, & Yule, 1976),
(Whiting, Burbank, &Ratner, 1986), 15.9 years among phone interviews (Monetemayor, 1983), and time sam-
the Kikuyu of Kenya (Worthman, 1986) and 18.0 years piing with electronic beepers (Csikszentmihalyi & Larson,
among the Bindi of New Guinea (Eveleth, 1986). 1984), that for most families with adolescent members,
Thus, current approaches to the study of adolescent serious conflict and disorganization are not characteristic
mental health and development no longer try to describe states (Hill, 1987). Conflicts do occur during the various
a unitary picture. Empirical descriptions of adaptation phases of adolescence, and they arise over mundane
are now seen as a function of an adolescent's particular issues, such as the family chores, curfews, eating practices,
point in a trajectory of development. This developmental dating, and personal appearance. However, the current
"point" is less often designated exclusively by age (early, evidence, drawn from various kinds of self-reports offered
middle, or late adolescence) but more and more often is by nonpatient adolescents and their parents, provides no
precisely delineated by each adolescent's developmental support for a picture of dramatic increase in family con-
level and timing with respect to a variety of biological flict from childhood to adolescence. In general, despite
and psychological variables and institutional settings. The the past dire predictions, the family is not "at risk" for
importance of accurately describing an adolescent's turmoil or disorganization during the adolescent years.
placement on several interacting developmental trajec- Indeed, detailed accounts of daily family life temper the
tories underscores the critical nature of longitudinal data more dramatic view of adolescent and family turmoil.
in adolescent research. Archives of longitudinal social Yet it would be a mistake to conclude that conflicts
science data, such as the Henry A. Murray Research between adolescents and their parents are insignificant
Center of Radcliffe College and the Institute of Human and rare except when there is family or individual
Development at Berkeley, greatly facilitate the study of psychopathology.
adolescent mental health by providing for reanalysis of Although there may not be striking quantitative
data on a variety of aspects of growth and adaptation shifts in amount, the loci of the conflicts do change. And
collected over several decades. 4 these very loci illuminate significant aspects of family life
during adolescence. By more clearly identifying the
sources of conflicts and how they shape, and are shaped
4Reflectingthe importance of effortsto investigate the develop- by, individual and family functioning, we can enhance
mental courseof mental health, the National Instituteof MentalHealth our knowledge of adolescent development within the
has recently funded the establishment of a national archive for longi-
tudinal mental health data at the Murray ResearchCenter of Radcliffe family, especially the range of individual and family di-
College. versity. In addition, such investigations can lead us closer

February 1989 • American Psychologist 203


to distinguishing between functional and dysfunctional her own behavior,.., parents' conventional perspectives...
conflict within the family (Cooper, 1988). may become increasingly maladaptive or inappropriate as ad-
olescents move into young adulthood. (Smetana, 1987, p. 7)
Impacts of the Adolescent's Biological and Cognitive
Development on Family Relationships Parents of adolescents

Although there are theoretical and intuitive reasons to must shift from viewing multi-faceted and personal issues as
believe that aspects of adolescent development influence conventional and legitimately subject to their authority, to
granting the adolescent (or young adult) increasing personal ju-
family relationships, it is a biological aspect of adoles- risdiction over these issues. (Smetana, 1988a, p. 333)
cence, puberty, that has been most intensively investigated
from this perspective. Paralleling an increasing interest Through such cognitive changes, adolescent devel-
in biological determinants of social relations in adoles- opment may lead to transformations in family relation-
cence (e.g., Brooks-Gunn, Petersen, & Eichorn, 1985; ships. On the surface are mundane, often ubiquitous,
Brooks-Gunn & Warren, in press; Inhoff-Germain et al., conflicts over the use of the car, choice of friends, or eve-
1988), recent reports delineate impacts of pubertal mat- ning activities. But beneath these seemingly concrete dif-
uration on parent-adolescent relationships (Steinberg & ferences may be deeper ones: the adolescent's increasingly
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Hill, 1978; Steinberg, 1988; Hill, Holmbeck, Marlow, varied conceptions of roles, authority, expectations, and
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Green, & Lynch, 1985a, 1985b; Holmbeck & Hill, 1988). responsibilities. Basic realignments in family relationships
A number of general conclusions can be drawn. Pubertal and new views of parental authority and of family roles
maturation differentially influences parent-adolescent and rules may be necessary to permit continued growth
relationships, independent of effects that might be con- of the adolescent and family, rather than an impasse re-
nected with the adolescent's chronological age. In the early suiting from sustained opposition by the adolescent or
and middle phases of the pubertal cycle, there is tran- parents' reluctance or inability to appreciate new per-
siently increasing conflict, distance, and dissatisfaction in spectives from their changing son or daughter. There re-
parent-child relations, an effect particularly apparent with main important questions about how family relationships
mothers. Although many of these findings are based on specifically change during such realignments of perspec-
adolescents' reports of family relations (Steinberg, 1988), tives. These questions are being pursued in ongoing lon-
analyses of observed family interaction sequences provide gitudinal family studies based on direct observation of
similar findings (Holmbeck & Hill, 1988; Steinberg & families and using scoring systems sensitive to the extent
Hill, 1978). In addition, a recent short-term longitudinal of constraining and enabling (Hauser et al., 1984; Sme-
study offers the intriguing suggestion that girls' maturation tana, 1987) and cognitive and alfective support and con-
may be accelerated by conflict and distance with their flict (Powers et al., 1983; Powers, 1988) expressed by par-
parents, particularly with mothers (Steinberg, 1988). ents and adolescents toward one another.
Important cognitive changes are also occurring dur-
ing adolescence: Family Contributions to the Adolescent's Development
For the first time in their lives, some adolescents are able to of Autonomy and Intimacy
employ formal operational thought.., take the role of others A third group of studies delineates family influences on
• .think in terms of possibilities, alternatives, and principles,
.
aspects of adolescent development. Two aspects of ado-
and they may have the cognitiveequipment to think about family lescent psychosocial development are especially impor-
issues and situations independent of their own immersion in tant: autonomy and intimacy. Earlier formulations
them. (Hill & Holmbeck, 1986, p. 176)
equated autonomy with detachment from parents, with
Such cognitive and social-cognitive developments have freedom from parental attachments and influence (Hill
been studied extensively. Yet it is only recently that they & Holmbeck, 1986; Steinberg & Silverberg, 1986). But
have been linked to transformations in family relation- there is now strong theoretical and empirical support for
ships. In her studies of family conflicts, Smetana (1985, an alternative view, one that does not assume major psy-
1988a, 1988b) has shown that an increasing "gap" be- chological or behavioral disruption in family relations as
tween parent and child reasoning and social judgment underlying or reflecting adolescent autonomy. Instead,
emerges in adolescence. Children are more likely to reason current studies address the interplay between transfor-
about personal items (e.g., friendships, content of cor- mations in autonomy and transformations in family re-
respondence, and recreational activities) in terms of per- lationships. One direction of work considers how varied
sonal choice, but parents of children at these ages do not advances and deflections in autonomous development are
demonstrate parallel changes. Smetana's results suggest influenced by family relationships.
that Baumrind (1968, 1987) has pointed to potential
in studying adolescent-parent relationships it is important to connections between parenting style and adolescent au-
examine the coordination of adolescents' and parents' perspee- tonomy through her conceptualizations of prototypes of
fives. . . . Children viewthemselvesas increasinglyemancipated parental authority. Traditional parents "value a sense of
from their parents' conventionalperspectivesduring adolescence. continuity and order more than innovation and risk-tak-
Torn between the necessity of maintaining the family social sys- ing. They accept the pattern of understanding and value
tem and allowing the child increasing personal jurisdiction over judgements that has been worked out over time by pre-

204 February 1989 • American Psychologist


vious generations" (Baumrind, 1987, p. 111). Adolescents terest for at least two reasons. First, it suggests that au-
from such families have been described as more attached thoritative parents may be functioning at higher levels of
to their parents, more conforming, and more achievement ego development. In addition, we begin to see cues about
oriented (Baumrind, 1987). Adolescents from these set- the specific behaviors shown by these parents in ongoing
tings may bypass major conflicts in their teenage years. family interactions.
Authoritarian-restrictive parents, according to Results from Grotevant and Cooper's work (1986;
Baumrind, press toward the shaping, controlling, and Cooper, Grotevant, & Condon, 1983), also based on ob-
evaluating of their adolescent in line with usually fixed served family interactions, underscore how individuality
standards, derived from religious or higher secular au- and connectedness in family relationships are linked with
thorities. Obedience is a virtue. "They do not willingly adolescent identity exploration and perspective taking.
share power and responsibility evenly with their adoles- Adolescents expressing high levels of identity exploration
cents, and in this sense violate the implicit contract in had fathers who expressed mutuality and separateness
which parental authority is exercised to benefit primarily and mothers who were aware of clear boundaries between
the child rather than the parent" (Baumrind, 1987, p. them and their teenagers. These adolescents were mem-
111). Related to the authoritarian orientation is the more bers of families that flourished when their differences were
punitive one, in which harsh discipline is favored to curb examined within the context of connectedness. In con-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

willfulness (autonomy strivings). This last pattern is as- trast~ adolescents with minimal levels of identity forma-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

sociated with major deflections in autonomous develop- tion and perspective taking were found in families that
ment and problematic adolescent behaviors (Baumrind, blurred boundaries between members and avoided dis-
1987). agreements.
In contrast, authoritative parents, according to In our research (Hauser et al., 1984; Powers et al.,
Baumrind, do not regard their standards or themselves 1983), we also find strong ties between parents' affective
as divinely inspired or infallible. They are supportive, responsiveness (acceptance, empathy, and support) and
committed, and show differentiated responsiveness. Con- adolescent ego development. The meaning of these cross-
text and individual differences guide their support for au- sectional findings is illuminated by recent longitudinal
tonomous expression and emphasis on "disciplined con- analyses suggesting links between these family interactions
formity" (Baumrind, in press). They are affectively re- at age 14 and subsequent ego development trajectories
sponsive (loving, supportive, and committed) and (Hanser, Powers, et al., 1987). Both mothers and fathers
cognitively responsive (providing a stimulating and chal- were least enabling toward those adolescents who sub-
lenging environment; Baumrind, 1987). In short, "'the sequently showed arrested ego development. On the other
authoritative parent combines limit-setting with negoti- hand, parents expressed more cognitive and affective en-
ation, thus encouraging the child's contribution to the abling toward adolescents who later showed trajectories
discipline process" (Grotevant & Cooper, 1986, p. 84). of progressive development. Thus, family discussions oc-
Although Baumrind's published findings to date involve curling in middle adolescence already show indications
familial antecedents of competent early and middle regarding which adolescents will exhibit arrested or un-
childhood functioning, results from other studies support stable development in their subsequent years.
the conclusion that authoritative parenting also predicts These sets of observations highlight the significance
greater autonomy in adolescents (Hill, 1980, 1987). De- of specific kinds of family relationships in the unfolding
spite this clear association between a particular kind of of adolescent autonomous development (identity explo-
parenting and adolescent development, there is an im- ration, perspective taking, and ego development). We
portant qualification that must be kept in mind. In the cannot yet, however, draw holistic pictures of adolescent
absence of longitudinal studies that deal with parenting families, be they "individuated," "enabling" or "sup-
in the preadolescent years, we cannot know whether these portive" characterizations. Differentiated relationships
connections represent parental contributions to auton- between dyads of family members are characteristic of
o m y or parents' responsiveness to their more autonomous these findings. Each of these dyads contributes in different
adolescents. W h e n we have access to data from Baum- ways to the development of adolescent autonomy. This
rind's wave of adolescent observations, we will be in a caveat is not meant to rule out the eventual distinguishing
better position to understand the extent to which these of "family types" that may be most effective in promoting
parenting practices function as antecedents of autono- varying paths of autonomy development. However, right
mous development. now more is to be gained from understanding the fine
Other pertinent findings about possible parent con- texture of family relations than from "smoothing" this
tributions to development of autonomy describe the im- texture in order to arrive at a readily comprehensible
portance of parent ego development (Hauser et al., 1984; account of the role of this complex social system in shap-
Hauser, Houlihan, ctal., in press). Parents at higher levels ing these paths.
of ego development (with greater awareness of self and A second significant strand of adolescent develop-
increasing appreciation of individual differences among ment involves intimate and close relationships. Adoles-
people, Loevinger 1976) express more explaining, cu- cents experience distinctive dilemmas around maintain-
riosity, and problem solving in family discussions with ing meaningful family and peer attachments in the midst
their adolescents and spouses. This observation is of in- of rapid pubertal, cognitive, and environmental change

February 1989 • American Psychologist 205


(Selman, in press). Although adolescents are discovering pioneering papers, Cooper and her colleagues (Cooper &
new and divergent perspectives from family members and, Ayers-Lopez, 1985; Cooper & Grotevant, 1987) mapped
at times, peers, continuing meaningful supportive rela- links between family and peer relationships. One relevant
tionships with these very same people can be profoundly finding is that adolescents who experience individuated
valuable for developing adolescents. Detailed under- relationships (involving qualities of separateness and mu-
standing of transformations in adolescent relationships, tuality) within their families are also more likely to engage
and the influences of family life on these bonds, is in its in more perspective taking, a valuable competence in ac-
very beginnings. Gilligan (1987) pointed out tual social relationships. Longitudinal observations, to-
gether with more intensive observations of friendships in
problems of disconnection in adolescence: As the balance of field settings (such as Gottman's 1983 study of children),
power between child and parent shifts with the child's coming are likely to provide us with fuller understanding of how
of age, so too the experienceand meaningof connectionchanges. family relationships may shape adolescent peer relation-
What constitutes attachment in early childhood does not con-
stitute attachment in adolescence, given the sexual changes of ships in their strength, depth, and breadth.
puberty and the growth of subjective and reflexive thought. Conclusion
(p. 80)
At present, psychologists are not in a position to precisely
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

define "normal" or "'healthy" adolescence. We are in the


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

She articulated two penetrating questions: "What are the


analogues in adolescence to the responsive engagement midst of determining a variety of profiles of healthy de-
psychologists find so striking in infancy and early child- velopment during these years. Individual differences with
hood? What constitutes genuine connection in the ado- respect to adolescents' behavior and inner states may vary
lescent years?" (Gilligan, 1987, p. 80). dramatically. Adolescents at the same age can be at strik-
Once we focus on significant adolescent attachments, ingiy different developmental levels in a variety of
we can see several possible links to family influences. First, sphereswphysical, social, and psychological. One key to
there is the issue, raised by White and her colleagues assessing adolescent mental health may rest on evaluating
(White, Speisman, & Costos, 1983), of transformations an adolescent's performance and experience in each of
in family relationships. Investigators of pubertal change these particular spheres, rather than trying to gauge his
have also attended to this issue, pointing out how family or her health through comparison with a unitary idea of
relationships change with the onset of puberty and how adolescent development.
family relationships themselves may contribute to pu- The issues raised in this article underscore the need
bertal change (Steinberg, 1988). But there is a further for longitudinal research as the "royal road" for identi-
question, taken up by Smetana: How do parent-child fying individual differences in rate and sequence of de-
relations change in light of the often rapid advances oc- velopment and the impact of these differing paths of de-
curring in the adolescent's self-reflective capacities, lead- velopment on adolescent mental health. It is recognized
ing to new-found questions, challenges, and doubts? More that future research programs must include more variety
specifically, what family features facilitate adolescent in the adolescents and families studledmnot enough is
growth, promoting greater differentiation while not jeop- known about varieties of adaptive functioning in Black,
ardizing, or rupturing, enduring family ties? Preliminary Hispanic, Native American, and other groups of adoles-
responses to this fundamental question cite the impor- cents in our culture. Finally, we have emphasized the
tance of interactions that permit conflict between mem- importance of including social contexts in assessing in-
bers in a context of support (Baumrind, 1987; Powers, dividual adolescent adaptation and growth through our
1988; Powers et al., 1983), acceptance, and active under- discussion of the family context of adolescent develop-
standing from parents (Hauser et al., 1984; Hauser, Hou- ment.
lihan, et al., 1987) and parental expressions of individ-
uality and connectedness (Grotevant & Cooper, 1986). REFERENCF~
These are qualities of family life that provide maximal Bandura,A. (1964). The stormydecade:Factor fiction?Psychology in
responsiveness during a challenging and potentially vul- the Schools, 1, 224-231.
nerable time in the life of the adolescent member. Certain Barber, B. K., & Rollins,B. C. (1987). Parent-adolescentrelationships
[Special issue].Family Perspectives,21(4).
parenting styles (e.g., authoritative) may be exquisitely Baumrind,D. (1968). Authoritarianvs. authoritativeparentalcontrol.
well matched for adolescent challenge, while others, such Adolescence, 3, 255-272.
as authoritarian and punitive, may dismally fail during Baumrind, D. (1987). Developmentalperspectiveson adolescentrisk-
this period. What we do not know yet is whether and taking in contemporaryAmerica. In C. E. Irwin (Ed.), Adolescent
how these parenting styles may evolve during adolescence social behavior and health (pp. 93-125). San Francisco:Jossey-Bass.
Baumrind, D. (in press). Rearingcompetentchildren. In W. Damon
and whether unique parenting forms emerge in the teen- (Ed.), New directionsfor child development: Child development today
age years. and tomorrow. San Francisco:Jossey-Bass.
A second family influence on relationship develop- Block,J. (1971). Lives through time. Berkeley,CA: BancroftBooks.
ment involves "carry over" from the family environment Blos, P. (1962). On adolescence: A psychoanalytic interpretation. New
York:Free Press.
to the peer group. Children and adolescents differ in their Blyth,D., Simmons,R., & Carlton-Ford,S. (1983). The adjustmentof
skills at negotiating peer relationships---in initiating, di- earlyadolescentsto schooltransitions.Journal of Early Adolescence,
versifying, sustaining, and deepening such ties. In two 3, 105-120.

206 February 1989 • American Psychologist


Bronks-Gunn, J., Petersen, A. C. (Eds.). (1983). Girls at puberty: Bio- D. (1984). Familial contexts of adolescent ego development. Child
logical and psychosocial perspectives. New York: Plenum. Development, 55, 195-213.
Brooks-Gunn, J., Petersen, A. C., & Eiehorn, D. (Eds,). (1985). Time Hill, J. P. (1980). The family. In M. Johnson (Ed.), Towardadolescence:
of maturation and psychosoeial functioning in adolescence [Special The middle school years. The Seventy-Ninth yearbook of the National
issue]. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 14, 149-264. Society for the Study of Education. Chicago: University of Chicago
Brooks-Gunn, J., & Ruble, D. N. (1983). The experience of menarebe Press.
from a developmental perspeet~. In J. Bronks-Gunn & A, C. Petersen Hill, J. P. (1987). Research on adolescents and their families: Past and
(Eds.), Girls at puberty: Biological and psychosocial perspectives (pp. prospect. In C. E. Irwin (Ed.), Adolescent social behavior and health
155-178). New York: Plenum. (pp. 13-31). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Brooks-Gunn, J., & Warren, M. P. (in press). The psychological signif- Hill, J. P., & Holmbeck, G. N. (1986). Attachment and autonomy during
icance of secondary sexual characteristics in 9- to 1l-year-old girls. adolescence. In G. W. Whitehurst (Ed.), Annals of child development
Child Development. (Vol. 3). Cn'eenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Burbank, V. (1988). Aboriginal adolescence:Maidenhood in an/atstralian Hill, J. P., Holmbcck, G. N., Marlow, L., Green, T. M., & Lynch,
community. New Brunswick: Rutgers University Press. M. E. (1985a). Menacheal status and parent-child relations in families
Cooper, C., Grotevant, H. D., & Condon, S. M. (1983). Individuality of seventh-grade girls. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 14, 301-
and eonnectedness in the family as a context for adoleseent identity 316.
and role-taking skills. In H. D, Grotevant & C. R, Cooper (Eds.), Hill, J. P,, Holmbeck, G. N., Madow, L., Green, I". M., & Lynch,
Adolescent development in thefamily (pp. 43-59). San Francisco: Jos- M. E. (1985b). Pubertal status and parent-child relations in families
sey.Bass. of seventh-grade boys. Journal of Early Adolescence, 5, 31-.44.
Cooper, C. S. (1988). Commentary: The role of conflict in adolescent- Holmbeck, G., & Hill, J. (1988), The role offamily conflict in adaptation
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

parent relationships. In M. Gunnar & C. Collins (Eds.), Development to menarche: Sequential analysis offamily interaction. Submitted for
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

during the transition to adolescence(pp. 181-187). Hillsdale, NJ: Erl- publication.


baum. lnhoff-Germain, G., Arnold, G., Nottleman, E., Susman, E., Cutler, G.,
Cooper, C. R., & Ayers-Lopez, S. (1985). Family and peer systems in & Chrousos, G. (1988). Relations between hormone levels and ob-
early adolescence: New models of the role of relationships in devel- servational measures of aggressive behavior of adolescents in family
opment. Journal of Early Adolescence, 5, 9-21. interactions. Developmental Psychology, 24, 129-149.
Cooper, C, R., & Grotevant, H. D. (1987). Gender issues at the interface Irwin, C. E. (1987). Editor's notes. In C. E, Irwin (Ed.), Adolescent
of family exparience and adolescents" friendship and dating identity. social behavior and health (pp. 1-12). San Francisco- Jossey-Bas~
Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 16, 247-264. Klein, J. R., & Litt, I. (1983). Mcnarche and dysmenorrhea. In J. Brooks-
Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Larson, R. (1984). Being adolescent. New York: Gunn & A. C. Petersen (Eds.), Girls at puberty: Biological and psy-
Basle Books. chosocialperspectives (pp. 73-88), New York: Plenum.
Douvan, E., & Adelson, J. (1966). The adolescent experience. New York: Locvinger, J. (1976). Ego development. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Wiley. Montemayor, R. (1983). Parents and adolescents in conflict: All families
Erikson, E. H. (1959). Identity and the life cycle. Psychological Issues, some of the time and some families most ofthe time. Journal of Early
1, 1-171. Adolescence, 3, 83-103.
Erikson, E. H. (1966). The challenge of youth. New York: Anchor Pa- Noam, G. G., Hauser, S. T., Santostefano, S., Garrison, W., Jacobson,
perback. A. M., Powers, S. I., & Mead, M. (1984). Ego devclopmont and psy-
Erikson, E. H. (1968). Identity: Youth and crisis. New York: Norton. chopathoingy: A study of hospitalized adolescents. Child Development,
Eveleth, P. B. (1986). Timing of menarehe: Secular trend and population 55, 184-194.
differences. In J. B. Lancaster & B. A. Hamburg (Eds.), School.age Offer, D. (1969). The psychological world of the teenager: A study of
pregnancy and parenthood: Biosocial dimensions (pp. 39-52). Haw- normal adolescent boys. New York: Basic Books.
thorne, NY: Aldine de Gruyter. Offer, D. (1987). In defense of adolescents. Journal of the American
Freud, A. (1946). The ego and mechanisms of defense (C. Baines, Trans.). Medical Association, 25 7, 3407-3408.
New York: International Universities Press. Offer, D., & Offer, J. (1975). From teenage to young manhood: A psy-
Freud, A. (1958). Adolescence. In Psychoanalytic study of the child (Vol. chological study. New York: Basic Books.
13). New York: International Uniw-~ities Press. Offe~ D., Ostrov, E., & Howard, I. (1981). The adolescent:A psychological
Gilligan, C. (1987). Adolescent development reconsidered. In C. Irwin self-portrait. New York: Basic Books.
(Ed.), Adolescent social behaviorand health (pp. 63-92). San Francisco: Offer, D., & Sabshin, M. (1984). Normality and the life cycle. New York:
Jossey-Bass: Basic Books.
Gottman, J. M. (1983). How children beeome friends. Monographs of Petersen, A. C. (1988). Adolescent development. Annual Review of Psy-
the Society for Research in Child Development, 48 (3, Serial 1"4o.201). chology, 39, 583-60%
Powers, S. I. (1988). Moral judgment development in the family. Journal
Grotevant, H. D., & Cooper, C. R. (1986). lndividuation in family re-
of Moral Education, 17, 209-219.
lationships. Human Development, 29, 82- iO0. Powers, S. I. (in press). Individual development through the lifespan:
Hauser, S., Jacobson, A. M., Miiley, J., Wertlieb, D., Hershowitz, R., Family constellations of development, meaning, and behavior. In K.
Woifsdorf, J., & Lavori, P. (in press). Ego trajectories and adjustment Kreppner & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Family systems throughout the life-
to diabetes: Longitudinal studies of diabetic and acutely ill patients. span. Hilisdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
In L. Feagens, W. Ray, & E. Susman (Eds.), Emotion and cognition Powers, S. I., Hanser, S. T., & Jacobson, A. (1985, April). Parent and
in child and adolescent health and development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erl- adolescent longitudinal trajectoriesof ego development. Paper presented
baum. at the meetings of the Society for Research in Child Development,
Hauser, S. T., Houlihan, L, Po~ers, S. I., Jacobson, A. M,, Noam, G., Toronto.
Weiss-Perry, B., & Follansbce, D. (1987). Interaction sequences in Powers, S. I., Hauser, S. T., Schwartz, J., Noam, G. G., & Jacobson, A.
families of psychiatrically hospitalized and non-patient adolescents. (1983). Adolescent ego development and family interaction: A strue-
Psychiatry, 50. 308-319. rural--developmental perspective. In H. D. Grotevant & C. R. Cooper
Hauscr, S. T., Houlihan, J., "Powers, S. I., Jaeobson, A. M., Noam, (Eds.), Adolescent development in the family. San Francisco: Jossey-
G. G., Weiss-Perry, B., Follansbee, D., & Book, B. K. (in press). Ad- Bass.
olescent ego development within the family: Family styles and family Rutter, M., Graham, P., Chadwick, O., & Yule, W. (1976). Adolescent
sequences. International Journal of Behavioral Development. turmoil: fact or fiction? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
Hauser, S. T., Powers, S. I., Noam, G. G., & Bowlds, M. K. (1987). 17, 35-56.
Family interiors of adolescent ego development trajectories. Family Selman, R. (in press). Fostering intimacy and autonomy. In W. Damon
Perspectives. 21, 263-282. (Ed.), Child development today and tomorrow (pp. 409--435). San
Hanser, S. T., Powers,S., Noam, G., Jaeobson, A., Weiss,B., & FoUansbee, Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

February 1989 • American Psychologist 207


Simmons, R. G. (1987). Social transition and adolescent development. Steinberg, L. D., & Hill J. E (1978). Patterns of family interaction as a
In C. E. Irwin (Ed.), Adolescent social behavior and health. San Fran- function of age, the onset of puberty, and formal thinking. Develop-
cisco: Jossey-Bass. mental Psychology, 14, 683--684.
Simmons, R. G., & Blyth, D. A. (1987). Moving into adolescence: The Steinberg, L., & Silverberg, S. (1986). The vicissitudes of autonomy in
impact of pubertal change and school context, New York: Aldine. adolescence. Child Development, 5 Z 841-85 i.
Smetana, J. G. (1985, April). Family rules, conventions, and adolescent-
parent conflict. Paper presented at the biennial meetings of the Society White, K. M., Speisman, J. C., & Costos, D. (1983). Young adults and
for Research in Child Development, Toronto. their parents: Individuation to mutuality. In H. D. Grotevant &
Smetana, J, G. (1987, April). Parentfactors in family relations in ado- C. R. Cooper (Eds.), Adolescent development in the family (pp. 61-
lescence. Paper presented at the biennial meetings of the Society for 67). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Research in Child Development, Baltimore. Whiting, J. W. M., Burbank, V. K., &Ratner, M. S. (1986). The duration
Smctana, J. G. (1988a). Concepts of self and social conventions: Ado- of maidenhood across cultures. In L B. Lancaster & B. A. Hamburg
lescents' and parents' reasoning about hypothetical and actual family (Eds,), School-age pregnancy and parenthood." Biosocial dimensions
conflicts. In M. R. Gunner & W. A. Collins (Eds.), 21st Minnesota (pp. 39-52). Hawthorne, NY: Aldine de Gruytes.
Symposium on Child Psychology(pp. 79-122). Hillsdale, N J: Eflbaum.
Smetana, J. G. (1988b). Adolescents' and parents' conceptions of parental Worthman, C. M. (1986). Developmental dysynchrony as normative
authority. Child Development, 59, 321-335. experience: Kikuyu adolescents. In J. B. Lancaster & B. A. Hamburg
Steinberg, L. (1988). Reciprocal relations between parent-child distance (Eds.), School-agepregnancy and parenthood: Biosocial dimensions
and pubertal maturation. Developmental Psychologg, 24, 122-128. (pp. 95-109). Hawthorne, NY: Aldine de Gruytes.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

208 F e b r u a r y 1989 • A m e r i c a n Psychologist

You might also like