Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Journal of Psychosomatic Research 62 (2007) 631 – 639

Emotional reactions following exposure to idealized bodies predict


unhealthy body change attitudes and behaviors in women and men
Sara Cahill, Alexander J. Mussap4
School of Psychology, Deakin University, Melbourne, Australia
Received 28 November 2005

Abstract
Objective: We explored the extent to which changes in and muscular male models, and visual analogue scales were used
emotional states following exposure to images of idealized bodies to measure changes in postexposure state body dissatisfaction,
predict unhealthy body change attitudes and behaviors in women anger, anxiety, and depression. Results: Postexposure increases in
and men, and whether particular psychological traits mediate state anger, anxiety, depression, and body dissatisfaction corre-
these effects. Method: One hundred thirty-three women and 93 lated with drive for thinness and disordered eating symptomatol-
men were assessed for unhealthy attitudes and behaviors related ogy in women, while postexposure increases in state body
to body weight and muscles using the Eating Disorder Inventory- dissatisfaction correlated with muscle development in men.
2 (EDI-2), the Obligatory Exercise Questionnaire, and the Analyses revealed that internalization and body comparison
strategies to increase muscles subscale of the Body Change mediated these relationships, with trait body dissatisfaction, trait
Inventory. Psychological traits assessed included body dissat- depression, self-esteem, and self-concept/identity confusion serv-
isfaction (EDI-2), internalization of the thin/athletic ideal (Socio- ing as mediators for women only. Conclusion: These results are
cultural Attitudes Towards Appearance Questionnaire-3), body indicative of gender differences in: (a) reactions to idealized
comparison (Body Comparison Scale), self-esteem (Rosenberg bodies; (b) psychological traits that predispose individuals to
Self-Esteem Inventory), depression (Beck Depression Inventory- experience these reactions; and (c) types of body change behavior
II), and identity confusion (Self-Concept Clarity Scale). Partic- that are associated with these reactions.
ipants were then exposed to photographs of thin female models D 2007 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Body image; Disordered eating; Exercise; Muscle development; Gender; Media; Unhealthy body change

Introduction the results of correlational studies suggesting that a history


of exposure to the media predicts level of body dissat-
According to the tripartite influence model, sociocultural isfaction, weight concerns, and disordered eating sympto-
pressure from peers, family, and the media to conform to matology in women, and body dissatisfaction, muscle
unrealistic physical ideals can contribute to negative body concern, and symptoms of unhealthy muscle development
image and unhealthy body change attitudes and behaviors in men [7–11], and experimental studies suggesting that
[1]; the thin ideal is implicated in drive for thinness and even short-term exposure to images of idealized bodies can
disordered eating in women [2,3], and the athletic/muscular negatively impact on body satisfaction and mood in both
ideal is implicated in drive for muscularity, compulsive women and men [12–19].
overexercising, overeating, and use of chemical supplemen- To account for individual differences in unhealthy body
tation in men [4–6]. Consistent with a role for the media are change attitudes and behaviors (especially given that
idealized bodies are almost ubiquitous in Western soci-
eties) [20], it has been suggested that certain psycholo-
4 Corresponding author. School of Psychology, Deakin University, gical traits predispose individuals to react emotionally to
221 Burwood Highway, Melbourne 3125, Australia. images of idealized bodies, and that unhealthy body
E-mail address: mussap@deakin.edu.au (A.J. Mussap). change reflects an accumulated impact of these emotional

0022-3999/07/$ – see front matter D 2007 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.jpsychores.2006.11.001
632 S. Cahill, A.J. Mussap / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 62 (2007) 631 – 639

reactions over a lifetime [1,21]. In the present study, an Method


experimental design was employed to identify which
psychological states are most reactive to short-term Participants
exposure to idealized bodies, the extent to which these
reactions are similar for females and males, and whether Participants were a self-selected sample of 93 males (age:
certain psychological traits mediate the relationship mean=22.34, S.D.=4.21) and 133 females (age: mean=22.42,
between state reactivity and long-term symptoms of un- S.D.=4.59), predominantly Australian students of Deakin
healthy body change. Identifying relevant psychological University. An invitation to participate was announced during
factors would not only enhance our understanding of the lectures across a range of faculties and classes. No reward for
sociocultural origins of unhealthy body change but also participation was offered. There is evidence to suggest that
facilitate the identification and early intervention of university exposes both females and males to peer pres-
individuals who are most vulnerable to sociocultural sure on appearance that can impact on their body image, self-
pressures on body image. esteem, and adoption of unhealthy body change attitudes
The psychological factors in the present study were chosen and behaviors [27].
on the basis of sociocultural models and the results of
previous research. In the dual-pathway model [22], socio- Materials and procedure
cultural pressures on appearance (e.g., via the media),
coupled with internalization of the physical ideal (e.g., the Two versions of a questionnaire were produced: one for
thin ideal), lead to body dissatisfaction, which in turn women (with photographs of thin female models) and one
contributes both to body change behaviors (dieting in the for men (with photographs of muscular male models). Both
case of dissatisfaction with weight) and to negative affect versions contained the same scales, completed in the order
associated with failure to achieve and maintain the internal- presented below. Participants were given only a broad
ized ideal. Together, dieting and negative affect promote overview of the study; hypotheses were not discussed.
unhealthy body change attitudes and behaviors characteristic
of disordered eating. If the dual-pathway model is correct, Body mass index (BMI)
women with elevated trait body dissatisfaction, who internal- BMI (for women: mean=22.3, S.D.=3.8; for men:
ize the thin ideal, value and frequently engage in body mean=23.5, S.D.=2.9) was calculated from self-reported
comparisons, and experience depressed mood, would be weight and height [BMI=weight (kg)/height (m)2]. Apply-
vulnerable to experiencing negative emotional reactions to ing National Institutes of Health guidelines, 7.5% of females
thin models. Since individuals with an unclear body image and 3.2% of males were bunderweightQ (BMIb18.5); 80.5%
would be most susceptible to sociocultural pressures, it is also of females and 68.8% of males were of bnormalQ weight
expected that elevated trait identity confusion and diminished (BMI=18.5–24.9); 7.5% of females and 23.7% of males
trait self-esteem would be associated with negative emotional were boverweightQ (BMI=25.0–29.9); and 4.5% of females
reactions to idealized bodies. Research has confirmed each of and 4.3% of males were bobeseQ (BMIz30.0).
these relationships in terms of vulnerability to experiencing
decreases in state body satisfaction and increases in anger,
Trait measures
anxiety, and depression in women following exposure to thin
female models [12,21,23–25].
Body dissatisfaction
On the basis of these results on females, together with
The body dissatisfaction subscale (DISS; for women:
emerging evidence of similar negative emotional reactions to
a=.86, mean=3.0, S.D.=0.8; for men: a=.76, mean=2.6,
idealized muscular bodies by males [11,17,26], it was
S.D.=0.6) of the Eating Disorder Inventory-2 (EDI-2) [28]
hypothesized that viewing idealized same-sex bodies would
was used. Participants responded to statements such as
result in increased state body dissatisfaction, depression,
bI think my hips are too big.Q Three additional items were
anxiety, and anger in both women and men. If these emo-
included from the Body Change Inventory (BCI) [5] to assess
tional reactions are of clinical relevance, then they should
male-relevant concerns, such as bI think that the size/width of
also correlate with unhealthy body change attitudes and
my shoulders is just right.Q All EDI-2 and BCI responses were
behaviors; in the present study, we evaluated drive for
scored between 1 (never) and 5 (always).
thinness, symptoms of bulimia, obligatory exercise, and
participation in strategies to increase muscles. Most impor-
tantly, if the psychological traits identified above actually Internalization
predispose individuals to experiencing negative emotional The TV–magazine and athlete subscales of the Socio-
reactions, then any relationship obtained between change in cultural Attitudes Towards Appearance Questionnaire-3
emotional state and unhealthy body change should be [29] (INTERN; for women: a=.84, mean=2.9, S.D.=0.8;
mediated by trait body dissatisfaction, internalization of the for men: a=.90, mean=2.6, S.D.=0.9) were used to assess
thin/muscular ideal, body comparison, self-esteem, depres- agreement with appearance-related information. Responses
sion, and identity confusion. were scored between 1 (never) and 5 (always).
S. Cahill, A.J. Mussap / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 62 (2007) 631 – 639 633

Comparison thinner.Q Drive for thinness is a fundamental characteristic of


The muscular and weight subscales of the Body disordered eating and constitutes a primary diagnostic
Comparison Scale [30] (COMP; for women: a=.84, criterion for diagnosis with an eating disorder [28].
mean=2.6, S.D.=0.7; for men: a=.88, mean=2.3, S.D.=0.8)
were used to assess the frequency of comparing body
regions relevant to weight and musculature. Responses were Body change attitudes and behaviors relevant to
scored between 1 (never) and 5 (always). body/muscle development

Obligatory exercise
Self-esteem
The Obligatory Exercise Questionnaire [34] (EXER;
The 10-item Rosenberg Self-Esteem
P Inventory [31] for women: a=.75, mean=2.5, S.D.=0.8; for men: a=.82,
(SELFEST; for women:
P a=.90, =36.5, S.D.=6.6;
mean=2.4, S.D.=0.7) was used to assess the emotional
for men: a=.91, =39.7, S.D.=6.5) was used, with
aspects of exercising, exercise frequency and intensity, and
responses scored between 0 (strongly disagree) and 3
preoccupation with exercise using statements such as
(strongly agree).
bI exercise even when I am advised against it (by a doctor,
friend, etc).Q
Beck Depression Inventory (BDI)
P The 21-item BDI-II [32] (BDI; for P women: a=.92,
=11.6, S.D.=9.7; for men: a=.92, =8.8, S.D.=8.0) Strategies to increase muscles
was used. The sample did not display a distribution of The revised strategies to increase muscles subscale
depression substantially different from that of US university (MUSC; for women: a=.75, mean=2.1, S.D.=0.7; for men:
samples [32]; on the basis of cutoff score guidelines for the a=.78, mean=2.6, S.D.=0.8) of the BCI [35] assessed the
BDI-II, approximately 15% of the female sample and 11% level of engagement in activities designed to increase
of the male sample were within the moderate to severe range muscle size using statements such as bI take food supple-
for depression, with most (67% and 73% for females and ments (such as vitamins, protein drinks, or diet pills) to
males, respectively) being within the minimal risk range. increase my muscles.Q

Identity confusion State visual analogue scales (VAS)


The 12-item Self-Concept P Clarity Scale [33] (IDEN-
CON; P for women: a=.89, =35.4, S.D.=8.2; for men: A series of VAS was used to assess psychological
a=.89, =33.1, S.D.=8.6) was used, with responses scored states. Four questions of the following form were
between 0 (strongly disagree) and 4 (strong agree). presented: bRight now, how ________ do you feel?Q
The words/phrases inserted in each case were bsatisfied
Body change attitudes and behaviors relevant to with you body,Q bdepressed,Q banxious,Q or bangry.Q
disordered eating Immediately below each question was a horizontal line
100 mm in length anchored with the phrase not at all on
Bulimia the left and very satisfied, very depressed, very anxious,
The bulimia subscale (BUL; for women: a=.78, or very angry on the right. Participants indicated their
mean=2.1, S.D.=0.6; for men: a=.72, mean=1.7, S.D.=0.5) response to each VAS item by placing a mark on the line
of the EDI-2 was used to assess the level of agreement with with a pen. The level of agreement with each VAS item
statements such as bI eat moderately in front of others and was scored by measuring the length of the left half of the
stuff myself when they are gone.Q Alternating binging and bisected line.
purging distinguishes bulimia nervosa from nonspecific VAS items were presented twice to participants:
disordered eating [28].1 immediately preceding exposure to photographs of female
and male models (see below), and immediately following
Drive for thinness exposure to these photographs. VAS items have been
The drive for thinness subscale (DFT; for women: a=.89, shown to be sensitive enough to register differences
mean=2.9, S.D.=0.8; for men: a=.82, mean=1.9, S.D.=0.7) before and after brief interventions, and to have good
of the EDI-2 was used to assess the level of agreement with convergent validity with various body dissatisfaction and
statements such as bI am preoccupied with the desire to be mood scales [36].

Photographs of thin female models and muscular


1
male models
Although the EDI-2 is normally scored on a 6-point scale, for
consistency, all scales that measured unhealthy body change attitudes and
behaviors were converted to 5-point scales scored between 1 (always) and 5 After the first set (preexposure) of VAS measures had
(never). been taken, six photographs of female and male models,
634 S. Cahill, A.J. Mussap / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 62 (2007) 631 – 639

Table 1
Means and paired-samples t tests of preexposure versus postexposure VAS scores
Women [mean (S.D.)] Men [mean (S.D.)]
State VAS Preexposure Postexposure t Preexposure Postexposure t
Anger 16.20 (21.60) 18.85 (22.90) 2.184 14.94 (21.05) 16.60 (20.56) 1.49
Anxiety 29.49 (27.54) 27.77 (25.40) 1.28 26.67 (27.49) 25.37 (26.62) 0.99
Depression 27.19 (40.73) 27.76 (27.79) 0.21 19.56 (26.48) 21.52 (25.89) 1.49
Satisfaction 46.77 (26.23) 42.34 (27.97) 3.2744 58.59 (25.53) 54.82 (27.35) 2.454
4 Significant at Pb.05.
44 Significant at Pb.01.

sourced from the media, were presented two to a page muscular status of the bodies depicted (see Results section).
(participants were exposed only to models of the same sex These ratings also ensured that participants attended and
as themselves). Female models were dressed in bikinis, engaged in an aesthetic evaluation of each image. Specif-
while male models were topless and dressed in shorts. All ically, each photograph was accompanied by two questions:
female models were visibly thin (the thinnest models we (a) bHow well does she/he compare to your ideal body?Q,
could source), and all male models were visibly lean and which was scored between 1 (she is much too thin/his
muscular bfitnessQ models. The photographs were sourced muscles are much too small) and 5 (she is much too fat/his
from the Internet, and participants’ own ratings of these muscles are much to large), and (b) bHow much would you
photographs were used to confirm the idealized thin/ like to look like her/him?Q, which was scored between 1 (not

Table 2
Measures of unhealthy body change regressed on each preexposure VAS score in Step 1 and on each postexposure VAS score in Step 2 (data for women are
shown)
DV VAS Step R R2 DR 2 B b t r sr 2
BUL Anger (1) Preexposure .10 .01 .02 0.00 0.09 0.61 .10 0.00
(2) Postexposure .17 .03 0.01 0.23 1.60 .16 0.02
Anxiety (1) Preexposure .2344 .05 .054 0.00 0.09 0.59 .2344 0.00
(2) Postexposure .324 .10 0.01 0.39 2.574 .3144 0.04
Depression (1) Preexposure .3044 .09 .044 0.00 0.14 1.28 .3044 0.01
(2) Postexposure .364 .13 0.01 0.25 2.404 .3444 0.04
Satisfaction (1) Preexposure .3944 .15 .034 0.00 0.15 1.02 .3944 0.01
(2) Postexposure .434 .18 0.01 0.29 2.044 .4244 0.03
DFT Anger (1) Preexposure .01 .00 .0944 0.02 0.39 2.764 .01 0.05
(2) Postexposure .2944 .09 0.02 0.49 3.4944 .184 0.08
Anxiety (1) Preexposure .02 .00 .0944 0.01 0.43 2.884 .02 0.06
(2) Postexposure .3144 .09 0.02 0.55 3.6444 .194 0.09
Depression (1) Preexposure .2444 .06 .1044 0.00 0.03 0.26 .2444 0.00
(2) Postexposure .4044 .16 0.01 0.42 4.0144 .4044 0.10
Satisfaction (1) Preexposure .5544 .30 .054 0.01 0.22 1.71 .5544 0.01
(2) Postexposure .5944 .35 0.01 0.40 3.094 .5844 0.05
MUSC Anger (1) Preexposure .184 .03 .01 0.00 0.02 0.12 .184 0.00
(2) Postexposure .21 .05 0.01 0.20 1.38 .214 0.01
Anxiety (1) Preexposure .174 .03 .00 0.00 0.10 0.64 .174 0.00
(2) Postexposure .17 .03 0.00 0.09 0.55 .174 0.00
Depression (1) Preexposure .07 .01 .02 0.00 0.05 0.46 .07 0.00
(2) Postexposure .17 .03 0.01 0.20 1.77 .174 0.02
Satisfaction (1) Preexposure .16 .03 .00 0.00 0.13 0.82 .16 0.00
(2) Postexposure .16 .03 0.00 0.04 0.24 .15 0.00
EXER Anger (1) Preexposure .00 .00 .01 0.00 0.12 0.81 .00 0.00
(2) Postexposure .09 .01 0.01 0.15 1.02 .05 0.01
Anxiety (1) Preexposure .08 .01 .00 0.00 0.03 0.21 .08 0.00
(2) Postexposure .09 .01 0.00 0.06 0.35 .08 0.00
Depression (1) Preexposure .07 .00 .01 0.00 0.01 0.12 .07 0.00
(2) Postexposure .12 .02 0.00 0.13 1.12 .12 0.01
Satisfaction (1) Preexposure .214 .05 .00 0.00 0.15 1.00 .214 0.01
(2) Postexposure .22 .05 0.00 0.77 0.45 .204 0.00
DV, dependent variable.
4 Significant at P b.05.
44 Significant at P b.01.
S. Cahill, A.J. Mussap / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 62 (2007) 631 – 639 635

Table 3
Measures of unhealthy body change regressed on each preexposure VAS score in Step 1 and on each postexposure VAS score in Step 2 (data for men
are shown)
DV VAS Step R R2 DR 2 B b t r sr2
BUL Anger (1) Preexposure .12 .02 .04 0.00 0.21 1.01 .12 0.01
(2) Postexposure .23 .05 0.00 0.38 1.86 .204 0.04
Anxiety (1) Preexposure .19 .03 .00 0.00 0.11 0.49 .184 0.00
(2) Postexposure .19 .04 0.00 0.08 0.35 .184 0.00
Depression (1) Preexposure .15 .02 .00 0.00 0.14 0.62 .15 0.00
(2) Postexposure .15 .02 0.00 0.02 0.07 .14 0.00
Satisfaction (1) Preexposure .15 .02 .01 0.00 0.33 1.71 .15 0.03
(2) Postexposure .19 .04 0.00 0.21 1.10 .07 0.01
DFT Anger (1) Preexposure .12 .02 .00 0.00 0.14 0.65 .12 0.00
(2) Postexposure .12 .02 0.00 0.02 0.08 .10 0.00
Anxiety (1) Preexposure .03 .00 .00 0.00 0.12 0.51 .03 0.00
(2) Postexposure .05 .00 0.00 0.10 0.42 .01 0.00
Depression (1) Preexposure .01 .00 .00 0.00 0.08 0.37 .01 0.00
(2) Postexposure .04 .00 0.00 0.08 0.36 .01 0.00
Satisfaction (1) Preexposure .18 .03 .01 0.00 0.00 0.01 .184 0.00
(2) Postexposure .21 .04 0.00 0.20 1.10 .214 0.01
MUSC Anger (1) Preexposure .18 .03 .01 0.00 0.02 0.09 .184 0.00
(2) Postexposure .21 .05 0.01 0.23 1.12 .214 0.01
Anxiety (1) Preexposure .09 .01 .02 0.01 0.18 0.77 .09 0.01
(2) Postexposure .16 .03 0.01 0.30 1.31 .14 0.02
Depression (1) Preexposure .09 .01 .00 0.01 0.21 0.93 .09 0.01
(2) Postexposure .11 .01 0.00 0.14 0.61 .05 0.00
Satisfaction (1) Preexposure .04 .00 .064 0.01 0.33 1.74 .04 0.03
(2) Postexposure .244 .06 0.01 0.44 2.314 .16 0.06
EXER Anger (1) Preexposure .20 .04 .02 0.00 0.01 0.07 .204 0.00
(2) Postexposure .24 .06 0.01 0.25 1.21 .2444 0.01
Anxiety (1) Preexposure .12 .01 .02 0.00 0.15 0.64 .12 0.00
(2) Postexposure .18 .03 0.01 0.30 1.31 .174 0.02
Depression (1) Preexposure .15 .02 .00 0.00 0.07 0.30 .15 0.00
(2) Postexposure .15 .03 0.00 0.09 0.41 .15 0.00
Satisfaction (1) Preexposure .09 .01 .03 0.01 0.18 0.94 .09 0.01
(2) Postexposure .20 .04 0.01 0.32 1.67 .174 0.03
DV, dependent variable.
4 Significant at Pb.05.
44 Significant at Pb.01.

at all) and 5 (extremely). Because the study was question- (skewness statistic/standard error of skewness) was N4.
naire-based, the timing of each component of the study and No multivariate outliers were identified.
the intervals between these components (preexposure items,
photographs, and postexposure items) were entirely under Aesthetic ratings of images of idealized bodies
the control of participants.
On the basis of the ratings given, women perceived
female models to be between ba little too thinQ and bjust
Results rightQ (women: mean=2.2, S.D.=0.5), while men perceived
male models to possess muscles that are between ba little
Data screening too largeQ and bjust rightQ (men: mean=3.6, S.D.=0.4). On
average, both female and male participants rated their desire
Female and male data were screened separately for to appear like the models as bmoderateQ (women: mean=2.8,
missing values (which were replaced with mean values S.D.=0.9; men: mean=2.6, S.D.=0.7; where 3=moderately).
for normally distributed variables and median values
for skewed variables). Scales were created from averaged Emotional reactions and unhealthy body change attitudes
responses to internally consistent items (item–total and behaviors
correlationN.3 and Cronbach’s aN.70; see Method sec-
tion). Univariate outliers (NF3 S.D. from the mean) A series of paired-samples t tests tested for postexposure
were identified and replaced with the value corre- changes in VAS score. Table 1 shows that both women and
sponding to 3 S.D. All scales were screened for viola- men reported significantly less state body satisfaction, and
tions of normality and were transformed when skewness women reported significantly more state anger. These results
636 S. Cahill, A.J. Mussap / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 62 (2007) 631 – 639

confirm that exposure to the models induced nontrivial significantly related to level of engagement in strategies to
psychological changes in the two groups. increase muscles (DR 2=.06, Pb.05), and none of the state
The hypothesis that individual differences in these VAS measures was related to unhealthy weight loss.
changes would be related to preexisting symptoms of
unhealthy body change was tested in a series of two-step Mediation analyses
hierarchical regression analyses. Each measure of unhealthy
body change was regressed on each preexposure state VAS A variant of the method of Baron and Kenny [37] was
measure in Step 1 and on each postexposure state VAS then employed to test which of the psychological traits
measure in Step 2. Significant additional variance predicted mediated the above relationships. A mediated path would
at Step 2 (i.e., significant DR 2 due to the inclusion of need to meet four conditions. First, a direct relationship
postexposure state measure) would support the presence of a must exist between a change in state VAS score and the
relationship between change in that psychological state and measure of unhealthy body change. This condition was
unhealthy body change. (The use of hierarchical analyses to tested in the hierarchical analyses described in the previous
measure change in a variable has important advantages over paragraph. Second, a change in state VAS score must
simply using difference scores [21,36].) correlate with a putative mediator (i.e., with the psycho-
The results of hierarchical analyses are presented in logical trait of interest). This condition was met if significant
Tables 2 and 3. For women, increased state anxiety and DR 2 values were obtained when each trait score was
depression, and decreased state body satisfaction following regressed on each preexposure state VAS score in Step 1
exposure to the same-sex models were significantly related and on each postexposure state VAS score in Step 2. Third,
to bulimia (DR 2=.05, .04, and .03, Pb.05, for state anxiety, the putative mediator must also correlate with the measure
depression, and body satisfaction, respectively) and drive of unhealthy body change in question (cf., Table 4). Finally,
for thinness (DR 2=.09, Pb.01; DR 2=.10, Pb.05; and traits that correlated both with change in the relevant state
DR 2=.05, Pb.05 for state anxiety, depression, and body VAS score (identified in Condition 2) and with the relevant
satisfaction, respectively). In addition, change in state anger measure of unhealthy body change (identified in Condition
was also significantly related to drive for thinness (DR 2=.09, 3) could be tested as mediators by determining whether their
Pb.05). Behaviors associated with body and muscle inclusion in the regression equation reduces to nonsignifi-
development (strategies to increase muscles, obligatory cance the direct path (of Condition 1) between change in
exercise) were unrelated to change in any of the state state VAS and unhealthy body change. To test this final
VAS scores. For men, decreased body satisfaction was condition, original hierarchical regressions were repeated as

Table 4
Separate correlations for females and males (Pearson’s r)
BMI BUL DFT MUSC EXER INTERN COMP DISS SELFEST BDI IDCONF
Women
Age .3444 .05 .08 .15 .13 .16 .19 .04 .204 .18 .2644
BMI – .12 .11 .04 .08 .06 .04 .36 .08 .04 .09
BUL – .6044 .2944 .2644 .4344 .4944 .4344 .4144 .3444 .3744
DFT – .3244 .4744 .6744 .6344 .6744 .4644 .2744 .3844
MUSC – .5144 .2844 .3444 .13 .2344 .184 .16
EXER – .3344 .3944 .15 .194 .09 .174
INTERN – .7244 .5044 .4144 .204 .2944
COMP – .5044 .4244 .2644 .3544
DISS – .5744 .3944 .3744
SELFEST – .6844 .6144
BDI – .6344
Men
Age .4044 .05 .14 .18 .10 .3144 .20 .12 .14 .12 .18
BMI – .02 .3444 .02 .13 .234 .10 .18 .214 .11 .214
BUL – .4144 .244 .3244 .18 .18 .224 .20 .18 .3444
DFT – .264 .3944 .2844 .3744 .4144 .04 .04 .15
MUSC – .5844 .4444 .5644 .20 .05 .01 .254
EXER – .4344 .5144 .15 .20 .06 .3244
INTERN – .7944 .4844 .3144 .17 .5344
COMP – .4144 .224 .07 .3944
DISS – .3444 .3044 .3544
SELFEST – .6944 .5344
BDI – .5044
4 Significant at P b.05.
44 Significant at P b.01.
S. Cahill, A.J. Mussap / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 62 (2007) 631 – 639 637

three-step regressions, with each putative mediator included with strategies to increase muscles (accounting for 6% of
individually in the second step of the regression—between variance in VAS scores).
each preexposure state VAS score (included in the first step) These results are consistent with individual differences
and each postexposure state VAS score (included in the in psychological vulnerability to images of idealized
third step). bodies. To test this interpretation, a range of psychological
For women, the significant direct path between change in traits were tested as potential mediators of the relationships
state anxiety and bulimia (DR 2=.05, Pb.01) was reduced to between change in emotional state and unhealthy body
nonsignificance by the inclusion of internalization change. For women, internalization of the thin ideal
(DR 2=.01, P=.17) and comparison (DR 2=.02, P=.11); the mediated the relationships between increased state anger,
path between state depression and bulimia (DR 2=.04, anxiety, depression, and decreased body satisfaction
Pb.05) was reduced to nonsignificance by the inclusion of following exposure to thin models, and symptoms of
trait body dissatisfaction (DR 2=.01, P=.33), internalization unhealthy weight loss. For men, internalization of the
(DR 2=.01, P=.26), trait self-esteem (DR 2=.00, P=.57), trait athletic/muscular ideal mediated the relationship between
depression (DR 2=.01, P=.15), and trait identity confusion decreased state body satisfaction and participation in
(DR 2=.01, P=.21); the path between change in state body strategies to increase muscles. These relationships support
satisfaction and bulimia (DR 2=.03, Pb.05) was reduced to the dual-pathway model’s contention that exposure to
nonsignificance by the inclusion of trait body dissatisfaction media images of idealized bodies is not necessarily
(DR 2=.00, P=.38), comparison (DR 2=.01, P=.18), and sufficient to induce clinically relevant changes in psycho-
internalization (DR 2=.01, P=.38); and the path between logical state; the media images must be valued by the
change in state anger and drive for thinness (DR 2=0.09, individual [38]. That internalization was relevant not only
Pb.01) was reduced to nonsignificance by the inclusion of for women but also for men further suggests that the
comparison (DR 2=.01, P=.24).2 model is relevant to concerns with muscularity, as well to
For men, the only significant direct path obtained— as concerns with thinness.
between change in state body satisfaction and strategies to Frequency of body comparison was identified as a
increase muscles (DR 2=.06, Pb.05)—was reduced to non- mediator of the relationships between both increased state
significance only by the inclusion of internalization anxiety and decreased state body satisfaction and bulimic
(DR 2=.03, P=.09) and comparison (DR 2=.01, P=.20). symptoms in women, and also mediated the relationship
between decreased state body satisfaction in men and
strategies to increase muscles. These relationships suggest
Discussion that individuals predisposed to body comparisons are more
likely to psychologically engage with images of thin/
The proposition that emotional reactions to media images muscular bodies, thereby increasing the salience of these
of idealized bodies are relevant to the impact of sociocultural images. Previous research has found that body comparison
pressures on body image and unhealthy body change mediates the relationship between real-world media expo-
attitudes and behaviors was tested. As expected on the basis sure and disordered eating [39]. The present study extended
of previous research [13,21], exposure to thin female models this finding to men and the pursuit of muscularity.
significantly decreased state body satisfaction and increased In women, trait body dissatisfaction was found to
state anger in women, while exposure to muscular male mediate the relationships between increased state depression
models decreased state body satisfaction in men. These and both drive for thinness and bulimic symptomatology,
effects were obtained after only very brief exposures and after and trait depression mediated the relationship between
exposure to only six photographs—far less than an individual increased state depression and bulimic symptoms. Body
might witness from reading even a single fashion or sports dissatisfaction and depression are known to predict disor-
magazine. More importantly, individual differences in these dered eating in women [22,40]. The absence of mediating
emotional reactions were relevant to unhealthy body change effects involving these trait measures for men suggests that
attitudes and behaviors. For women, postexposure increases stable body dissatisfaction in men is not as relevant to male
in state anxiety and depression, and decreases in state body body change attitudes and behaviors as is malleability of
satisfaction, were associated with an elevated drive for state body dissatisfaction. Similar arguments have been
thinness (accounting for between 5% and 10% of variance made in the context of women and unhealthy attitudes and
in VAS scores) and with bulimic symptoms (accounting for behaviors associated with the desire for thinness and weight
between 2% and 5% of variance in VAS scores). For men, loss, where it has been found that unstable (rather than
postexposure change in state body satisfaction was associated simply negative) self-evaluations made by women following
exposure to thin bodies are predictive of disordered eating
2 symptomatology [41].
As noted by an anonymous reviewer, since trait depression correlated
with many other trait measures (self-esteem and body dissatisfaction in
Low trait self-esteem in women was identified as a
particular; see Table 4), it is possible that some of the abovementioned mediator of the relationship between increased state
effects were due to a common underlying influence of depression. depression and both drive for thinness and bulimic
638 S. Cahill, A.J. Mussap / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 62 (2007) 631 – 639

symptomatology. Trait self-esteem is known to be associ- It is also impossible to dismiss the influence on the
ated with body dissatisfaction and symptoms of disordered results of demand characteristics. The body change meas-
eating [23]—an observation that is consistent with the idea ures related to weight and muscles provided participants
that self-esteem reflects, at least in part, an individual’s with sufficient information to conclude that we expected
evaluation of one’s own appearance. It has also been
negative emotional reactions to the models. However, it is
suggested that individuals with low self-esteem may attempt
unlikely that this could have produced the complex pattern
to increase their self-esteem by striving for social accept-
of relationships obtained between emotional reactions and
ance, perhaps by endeavoring to meet culturally mandated
standards of physical appearance [42]. The results of the unhealthy body change attitudes and behaviors—relation-
present study lend partial support to this idea. Trait identity ships that were mediated by specific psychological traits.
confusion was also found to mediate the relationship In conclusion, there is a multidimensional and gendered
between increased state depression and bulimic symptoms. relationship between exposure to media images of idealized
This is consistent with the proposition that women with an bodies, psychological reactions to these images, and
unstable or unclear self-concept may be more inclined to attitudes and behaviors that reflect a desire to conform to
look at culturally prescribed ideals to find their sense of these images. While the media’s effects on men appear to be
identity [42]. limited to their current sense of body satisfaction and appear
Given the variety of mediators identified in the present to contribute to (or interact with) their desire to increase
study, it would be worthwhile for future research to assess muscles, the media’s effects on women influence their
other factors that meet the basic prerequisites for mediation current sense of anger, anxiety, depression, and body
(that they correlate both with emotional reactivity and with satisfaction, and appear to contribute to (or interact with)
measures of unhealthy body change). Such factors include their desire to decrease weight. Thus, the results not only
sexual orientation and gender–role orientation [43–45], fear confirm that men and women differ in their body image
of negative appearance evaluation [46], obsessiveness and concerns, attitudes, and body change behaviors [51] but also
perfectionism [47], impulsivity [48], and participation in point to differences in the psychological processes that
sporting activities [49]. determine the effects of media exposure on these concerns,
As to limitations, it is important to note that the study attitudes, and behaviors.
possessed only limited ecological validity (cf., Thompson et
al. [1]); a brief and isolated exposure to idealized bodies
does not replicate a lifetime of repeated exposure to such Acknowledgments
bodies, which one experiences in the real world, nor does it
simulate the positive outcomes (success, happiness, popular- This research was supported by a grant from the
ity, etc.) that the media associate with these bodies. This Australian Research Council.
limitation is exacerbated by having the timing of each
component of the study and the intervals between these
components under the control of participants. Furthermore,
the use of VAS scores, while ideally suited to the short References
exposure durations involved in this study (because VAS
[1] Thompson JK, Heinberg LJ, Altabe M, Tantleff-Dunn S.
scales are unmarked), does not permit interpretation of the
Exacting beauty. Washington (DC)7 American Psychological Associ-
clinical significance of changes in psychological state. Thus, ation, 1999.
while causal relationships between changes in state VAS [2] van den Berg P, Thompson JK, Obremski-Brandon K, Coovert M.
scores and exposure to same-sex models can be stated with The tripartite influence model of body image and eating disturb-
some degree of certainty due to the experimental design ance: a covariance structure modeling investigation testing the
mediational role of appearance comparison. J Psychosom Res
employed, the implications of these effects on unhealthy
2002;53:1007 – 20.
body change behaviors must be made cautiously—the [3] Cash TF, Brown TA. Body image in anorexia nervosa and
absence of longitudinal data; the use of an adult sample bulimia nervosa: a review of the literature. Behav Modif 1987;11:
where any evidence of unhealthy body change preexists; the 487 – 521.
short exposure durations employed; the short-lived effects [4] Olivardia R, Pope HG, Hudson JI. Muscle dysmorphia in
male weightlifters: a case–control study. Am J Psychiatry 2000;157:
of exposure measured in the study; and the lack of clinically
1291 – 6.
validated measures of state change make it impossible [5] Ricciardelli LA, McCabe MP. Psychometric evaluation of the Body
to confirm that it is an accumulation of emotional reactions Change Inventory: an assessment instrument for adolescent boys and
to idealized bodies that contributes to the development girls. Eat Behav 2002;2:1 – 15.
of unhealthy body change [50]. To do so would require [6] Smolak L, Murnen SK, Thompson JK. Sociocultural influences and
muscle building in early adolescent boys. Psychology of Men and
further research to confirm that the inherent instability of
Masculinity 2005;6:227 – 39.
psychological states actually precedes the development of [7] Cusumano DL, Thompson JK. Body image and body shape ideals in
negative body image and unhealthy body change attitudes magazines: exposure, awareness and internalization. Sex Roles
and behaviors. 1997;37:701 – 21.
S. Cahill, A.J. Mussap / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 62 (2007) 631 – 639 639

[8] Botta RA. For your health? The relationship between magazine [30] Fisher E, Dunn M, Thompson JK. Social comparison and body image:
reading and adolescents’ body image and eating disturbances. Sex an investigation of body comparison processes using multidimen-
Roles 2003;48:389 – 99. sional scaling. J Soc Clin Psychol 2002;21:566 – 79.
[9] Harrison K. Television viewing, fat stereotypical, body shape stand- [31] Rosenberg M. Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton (NJ)7
ards, and eating disorder symptomatology in grade school children. Princeton University Press, 1965.
Communic Res 2000;27:617 – 40. [32] Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK. Manual for the Beck Depression
[10] Botta RA. Television images and adolescent girls’ body image Inventory, 2nd ed. San Antonio7 Psychological, 1996.
disturbance. J Commun 1999;49:22 – 41. [33] Campbell JD, Trapnell PD, Heine SJ, Katz IM, Lavallee LF, Lehman
[11] Hatoum IJ, Belle D. Mags and abs: media consumption and bodily DR. Self-concept clarity: measurement, personality correlates and
concerns in men. Sex Roles 2004;51:397 – 407. cultural boundaries. J Pers Soc Psychol 1996;70:141 – 56.
[12] Groesz LM, Levine MP, Murnen SK. The effect of experimental [34] Thompson J, Pasman L. The Obligatory Exercise Questionnaire.
presentation of thin media images on body satisfaction: a meta- Behavior Therapist 1991;14:137.
analytic review. Int J Eat Disord 2002;21:1 – 16. [35] Garner DM, Olmstead M, Polivy J. Development and validation of a
[13] Monteath SA, McCabe MP. The influence of societal factors on multi-dimensional Eating Disorder Inventory for anorexia nervosa and
female body image. J Soc Psychol 1997;137:708 – 27. bulimia. Int J Eat Disord 1983;2:15 – 34.
[14] Henderson-King E, Henderson-King D. Media effects on women’s [36] Cronbach LJ, Furby L. How should we measure bchangeQ—or should
body esteem: social and individual difference factors. J Appl Soc we? Psychol Bull 1970;74:68 – 80.
Psychol 1997;27:399 – 417. [37] Baron RM, Kenny DA. The moderator–mediator variable distinction
[15] Posovac HD, Posovac SS, Posovac EJ. Exposure to media images of in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical
female attractiveness and concern with body weight among young considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol 1986;51:1173 – 82.
women. Sex Roles 1998;38:187 – 201. [38] Thompson JK, Stice E. Thin-ideal internalization: mounting evidence
[16] Levine MP, Smolak L, Hayden H. The relation of sociocultural factors for a new risk factor for body-image disturbance and eating pathology.
to eating attitudes and behaviours among middle school girls. J of Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2001;10:181 – 3.
Early Adolescence 1994;14:472 – 91. [39] Stice E, Shaw HE. Adverse effects of the media portrayed thin-ideal
[17] Lorenzen LA, Grieve FG, Thomas A. Exposure to muscular on women and linkages to bulimic symptomatology. J Soc Clin
male models decreases men’s body satisfaction. Sex Roles Psychol 1994;13:288 – 308.
2004;51:743 – 8. [40] Polivy J, Herman CP. Causes of eating disorders. Annu Rev Psychol
[18] Grogan S, Williams Z, Conner M. The effects of viewing same 2002;53:187 – 213.
gender photographs on body esteem. Psychol Women Q [41] Waller G, Barnes J. Preconscious processing of body image cues
1996;20:569 – 75. Impact on body percept and concept. J Psychosom Res 2002;
[19] Agliata D, Tantleff-Dunn S. The impact of media exposure on males’ 53:1037 – 41.
body image. J Soc Clin Psychol 2004;23:7 – 22. [42] Stice E. Review of the evidence for a sociocultural model of bulimia
[20] Hoek JW, van Hoeken D. Review of prevalence and incidence of nervosa and an exploration of the mechanisms of action. Clin Psychol
eating disorders. Int J Eat Disord 2003;34:383 – 96. Rev 1994;14:633 – 61.
[21] Durkin SJ, Paxton SJ. Predictors of vulnerability to reduced body [43] Conner M, Johnson C, Grogan S. Gender, sexuality, body image and
satisfaction and psychological well-being in response to exposure to eating behaviours. J Health Psychol 2004;9:505 – 15.
idealized female media images in adolescent girls. J Psychosom Res [44] Borchert J, Heinberg L. Gender schema and gender role discrepancy
2002;53:995 – 1005. as correlates of body image. J Psychol 1996;130:547 – 59.
[22] Stice E, Agras WS. Predicting onset and cessation bulimic behaviors [45] Hospers HJ, Jansen A. Why homosexuality is a risk factor for eating
during adolescence: a longitudinal grouping analysis. Behav Ther disorders in males. J Soc Clin Psychol 2005;24:1188 – 201.
1998;29:257 – 76. [46] Lundgren JD, Anderson DA, Thompson JK. Fear of negative
[23] Abell SC, Richards MH. The relationship between body shape appearance evaluation scale: psychometric properties and validation.
satisfaction and self-esteem: an investigation of gender and class Eat Behav 2004;5:75 – 84.
differences. J Youth Adolescence 1996;25:691 – 703. [47] Maida DM, Lee Armonstrong S. The classification of muscle
[24] Tiggemann M, McGill B. The role of social comparison in the effect dysmorphia. International Journal of Men’s Health 2005;4:73 – 91.
of magazine advertisements on women’s mood and body dissat- [48] Fernandez-Aranda F, Aitken A, Badia A, Gimenez L, Solano R,
isfaction. J Soc Clin Psychol 2004;23:23 – 44. Collier D, Treasure J, Vallejo J. Personality and psychopathological
[25] Posovac HD, Posovac HD, Weigel RG. Reducing the impact of media traits of males with an eating disorder. Eur Eat Disord Rev 2004;
images on women at risk for body image disturbance: three targeted 12:367 – 74.
interventions. J Soc Clin Psychol 2001;20:324 – 40. [49] Cafri G, van den Berg P, Thompson J. Pursuit of muscularity
[26] Agliata D, Tantleff-Dunn S. The impact of media exposure on males’ in adolescent boys: relations among biopsychosocial variables
body image. J Soc Clin Psychol 2004;23:7 – 22. and clinical outcomes. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 2006;
[27] Zalta AK, Keel PK. Peer influence on bulimic symptoms in college 35:283 – 91.
students. J Abnorm Psychol 2006;115:185 – 9. [50] Hargreaves D, Tiggemann M. Longer-term implications
[28] Garner DM. Eating Disorder Inventory-2 professional manual. Odessa of responsiveness to bthin-idealQ television: support for a cumulative
(FL)7 Psychological Assessment Resources, 1991. hypothesis of body image disturbance? Eur Eat Disord Rev
[29] Thompson JK, van den Berg P, Roehrig M, Guarda AS, Heinberg LJ. 2003;11:465 – 77.
The Sociocultural Attitudes Towards Appearance Scale-3 (SATAQ-3): [51] Cohane GH, Pope HG. Body image in boys: a review of the literature.
development and validation. Int J Eat Disord 2004;35:293 – 305. Int J Eat Disord 2001;29:373 – 9.

You might also like