Artigo Sobre Treino Nao Proficiente de Profissionais

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 1977, 10.

197-205 NUMBER 2 (SUMMER) 1977

ASSESSING AND TRAINING TEACHERS IN THE GENERALIZED


USE OF BEHAVIOR MODIFICATION WITH AUTISTIC CHILDREN'
ROBERT L. KOEGEL, DENNIS C. RUSSO, AND ARNOLD RINCOVER
UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA, SANTA BARBARA; THE JOHN F. KENNEDY
INSTITUTE AND THE JOHNS HOPKINS UNIVERSITY SCHOOL OF MEDICINE,
AND UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA, GREENSBORO
This study investigated the feasibility of developing reliable, valid criteria for measur-
ing and training the skills necessary to teach autistic children. The behaviors of 11 teach-
ers and 12 autistic children were recorded in a series of different teaching situations.
Teacher-training was initiated at different times for different teachers. The results
showed: (1) it was possible to assess empirically whether a teacher was correctly using
defined behavior-modification techniques; (2) generally, for any given session, systematic
improvement in the child's behavior did not occur unless the teacher working in that
session had been trained to use the techniques to a high criterion; (3) all 11 teachers
were rapidly trained to use these techniques; and (4) the teachers learned generalized
skills effective with a variety of children and target behaviors.
DESCRIPTORS: response generalization, behavior therapy, behavioral instruction,
contingency management, stimulus generalization, discrimination training, teacher train-
ing, teacher behavior, generalization, autistic children

In the past 10 years, a relatively large num- unable to provide instruction for such children.
ber of investigations have shown that behavior- The assumption generally made by school ad-
modification procedures can be effective in the ministrators is that the teachers are teaching cor-
treatment of autistic children (e.g., Hewett, rectly, and the children are unable to learn.
1965; Koegel and Rincover, 1974; Lovaas, As a result, it seems timely to assess whether
1966; Lovaas, Freitas, Nelson, and Whalen, or not teachers are (or even can) use the pro-
1967; Metz, 1965; Risley and Wolf, 1967; cedures that have been demonstrated in the lit-
Wolf, Risley, and Mees, 1964). In reviewing erature to be effective in teaching such children.
the literature, Kozloff (1975) and Lovaas and The present experiment was designed to: (1)
Koegel (1973) pointed out that: (1) behavior assess empirically whether or not a child's
modification has produced considerable treat- teacher is using behavior-modification proce-
ment gains in autistic children; and (2) these dures correctly; and (2) assess the necessity and
results have been replicated in numerous experi- feasibility of training teachers to teach autistic
mental laboratories. However, it is not at all cer- children effectively.
tain that any given teacher or therapist could
replicate these results (Lovaas, KoegeL, Simmons, 'This investigation was supported by the United
and Stevens-Long, 1973). This conclusion was States Public Health Service Research Grant MH
based primarily on the fact that there are cur- 11440 and MH28210 from the National Institute of
rently no published criteria for measuring Mental Health, and by State of California Research
whether or not a given individual is adept at Grant 42-00000-000832, Title VI-B of the United
States Elementary and Secondary Education Act. Many
using behavior-modification techniques with people assisted in this investigation. We are grateful
autistic children. Criteria for teacher certifica- for the help of Polly Dimalanta, Nancy Woodfield
tion in special education generally do not in- Everett, Glen Dunlap, Frank Schlosser, and Peggy
Pulleyn. We particularly appreciate the helpful sug-
clude a behavioral assessment of a teacher's gestions Laura Schreibman provided throughout this
ability to teach these children. Furthermore, most investigation. Reprints may be obtained from Robert
autistic children are still excluded from school L. Koegel, Social Process Research Institute, or Speech
Department, University of California, Santa Barbara,
programs on the grounds that the teachers are California 93106.
197
198 ROBERT L. KOEGEL, DENNIS C. RUSSO, and ARNOLD RINCOVER

METHOD During each session, one teacher was seated


facing one child at a 50.8- by 76.2-cm table.
Subjects and Setting All the stimuli necessary to teach the behavior
Eleven teachers participated in this investiga- assigned, such as different color blocks, differ-
tion. Ten were under 30 yr of age. Four resided ent pictures, etc., and a supply of food reinforcers
in cities of Southern California; the other seven were present on the table.
in New Jersey. At the time of this study, nine
teachers had at least 2 yr of previous teaching Target Behaviors
experience with autistic children. None of the Target behaviors were selected for each child
11 teachers had any formal training in behavior according to the individual level of academic
modification but all had read in the area. All skill. Also, some target behaviors were assigned
the teachers were employed in special-education that had already been learned by a child, so that
classrooms. However, the selection was not ran- we could determine if the teacher could main-
dom, in that all of the teachers requested train- tain correct responding. A total of 27 target
ing, and all of them generally appeared eager behaviors was selected. Each was selected from
to participate. No teacher was denied partici- one of the following categories: self-help skills
pation. (e.g., shoe-tying); arithmetic skills (e.g., 1"
Twelve autistic children also participated. versus "2"); writing skills (e.g., tracing letters);
They were randomly selected either from a par- picture labelling (e.g., "bear" versus "horse");
ticipating teacher's classroom, or from one of abstract language skills (e.g., "big versus
the training sites. Five of the children were en- "small"); speech skills (e.g., verbal imitation-
rolled in an experimental classroom at the Uni- "Say 'Mama' ").
versity of California at Santa Barbara, four were
enrolled in a pilot classroom for autistic children Teacher-Training Procedure
in Orcutt, California, the other three were en- The teachers first were asked to read a train-
rolled in the Princeton Child Development Insti- ing manual that described examples of correct
tute in Princeton, New Jersey. The children and incorrect use of five categories of behavior-
ranged in chronological age from 5 to 13 yr. modification procedures. Each teacher also was
Ten were reported to be untestable on standard- shown videotapes illustrating correct and incor-
ized IQ tests; the other two had reported IQ rect use of each of the procedures. The teacher
scores of 28 and 32. The results of standardized then attempted to teach a child a new target
social maturity tests placed all of the children behavior. About once every 5 min, the trainer
between 2 and 4 yr. All the children had been would interrupt the session and provide feedback
excluded from existing public school classrooms, as to whether or not the teacher was conducting
and had been diagnosed as autistic by agencies the teaching according to the operational defini-
not associated with this study. They engaged in tions described below. If errors occurred in any
little appropriate play or social behavior and had category, the trainer (either an author or a uni-
almost no appropriate speech. Six children were versity student familiar with these procedures)
essentially mute, and the others were primarily so informed the teacher, and then modelled the
echolalic. They evidenced little, if any, self-help procedure according to its operational definition.
behaviors (e.g., feeding themselves, dressing Feedback was very brief, usually taking only a
themselves, etc.), and frequently engaged in tan- few seconds, in order to produce minimal inter-
trum and self-stimulatory behaviors. In general, ference with the session. For example, the trainer
they were probably representative of children in might say, "You are using good prompts, but
the more severe half, with regard to severity of your SDs are too long. Try saying, 'Touch red.'"
autism. Approximately once every half hour, the teach-
BEHAVIOR MODIFICATION WITH AUTISTIC CHILDREN 199
ers were given more elaborate feedback. These tasks assigned both before and after training
sessions continued until the trainer was subjec- were always different from those used during
tively satisfied with the teacher's performance. training. During each session, we simultaneously
All the teachers completed training within less measured the correctness of the teacher's proce-
than 25 hr (usually either five days of 5 hr per dures and the learning of the child to assess
day, or 10 days of 2.5 hr per day). None of the whether changes in the children's behavior were
teachers reported either training schedule to be a function of changes in the teacher's behavior
excessively demanding. and whether the training produced the teachers'
generalized use of behavior-modification proce-
Design dures across (autistic) children and tasks.
The design of this experiment was a modified
multiresponse baseline (Birnbrauer, Peterson, Assessment of Teacher Use of Behavior
and Solnick, 1974). For a predetermined number Modification Procedures
of baseline sessions (1, 3, or 4 sessions) each In each session, at least one observer recorded
teacher attempted to teach one to four randomly data on the teachers' use of five aspects of be-
assigned target behaviors. In each session, one havior-modification procedures. Each observer,
teacher worked with one child on one behavior. whether familiar or naive with respect to the
During successive sessions, a teacher might be conditions of the experiment, had completed at
assigned to a different child and/or a different least one college level course in behavior modi-
target behavior. After training, the teacher again fication. The observers were given the following
attempted to teach one to three behaviors. The instructions:

Table 1
The assignment of target behaviors (B) and children (Ch) for each session of each
teacher in this experiment.
After
Base Training
Teacher 1 Ch1B1 Ch3B2 Ch1B3
Teacher 2 Ch2B4 Ch3B5 ChB6
Teacher 3 ChloB7 ChlB5 Ch9B | Ch11B3 Ch8B1o Ch9B7
Teacher 4 Ch2B,6 Ch2B8 Ch1B5
Teacher 5 Ch6B1l Ch7B19 Ch6B1o Ch71B2 Ch9B8 Ch8B21 Ch6B1O
Teacher 6 Ch3B17 Ch2Bl8 Ch2B5
Teacher 7 Ch4B1l Ch5B,2 Ch4B13 Ch8Bl4 Ch8B15 Ch4B8 Ch5B5
Teacher 8 ChB22 Ch1B23 Ch1B24
Teacher 9 Chl2B7 Ch2B7 Ch2B7 Gh12B7 Ch,2B7 Chl2B7
Teacher 10 Ch3B25 ChLB26 Ch3B23
Teacher 11 Ch3B27 Ch3B8 Ch2B1o
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Task
200 ROBERT L. KOEGEL, DENNIS C. RUSSO, and ARNOLD RINCOVER

2. Consequences should be contingent, that is, re-


SCORING INSTRUCTIONS inforcers only for correct responses, and extinc-
Observe a session for 30 seconds, then record tion only for incorrect responses.
during the next 30 seconds. Use the definitions 3. The consequation should be unambiguous. A
given below to make a decision about the ade- "No" said with a smile, or a "Good boy" given
quacy of each aspect being assessed. If you do with a frown are ambiguous.
not see a procedure being used in a given period,
4. Consequation should be consistent. Reinforc-
leave the appropriate space blank. ers should follow each correct response, during
acquisition. Similarly, punishment, if used,
SDs: The Teacher's Instructions to the Child should follow every instance of bizarre or inap-
1. The SD should be clear and discriminable, that propriate behavior.
is, it should stand apart from anything else that 5. Consequences should be effective. They should
the teacher says. A good SD has a distinct onset be selected individually for each child. Reinforc-
and a distinct offset. ers should be something the child will approach,
2. The SD should be appropriate to the task. If and punishers something the child will avoid.
the teacher is teaching the child to point to a red
card, he should not mistakenly say, "Point to the Discrete Trials
blue card", or "Hand me the red card". 1. Each trial must have a distinct onset and off-
3. The SD should be consistent with that given set, and a discrete intertrial interval. An inter-
on the previous trial. Exception: the SD can ap- trial interval consists of any noticeable period of
propriately vary when a discrimination task is time in which no on-task responding is required
being taught, and the child has reached criterion of the child.
on each of the individual stimulus items.
4. The SD should be uninterrupted. During every 30-sec recording interval, ob-
servers scored each of the five categories as "cor-
5. When the SD is presented, the child should be
attending. The child should not be engaging in rect" (fulfilled all aspects of the definition of the
off task behavior. The child should be sitting procedure for all of the trials occurring in that
quietly, not engaged in any overtly disruptive be-interval), "incorrect" (did not fulfill the defini-
havior when the verbal component of the SD is tion in some way during any trial(s) within the
presented. The child should be looking either at interval), or "NA" (not applicable-procedure
the task at hand or at the teacher. was not observed in the 30-sec period).
The observers simply made a check mark in
Prompts: Guiding the Child the appropriate box for each of the five proce-
to Respond Correctly dures. An overall index of the teacher's behavior
1. The prompt must be effective, that is, it must was then computed for each session. First, the
evoke a correct response. number of "correct" 30-sec intervals was totalled
for each procedure (summing across categories).
Shaping: Reinforcing Successive Second, the number of correct plus incorrect
Approximations to the Target Behavior intervals was totalled. Then, the first total was
1. Each reinforced response should be at least as divided by the second, and multiplied by 100
good as the last one (score shaping with refer- to give a percentage. The average of the five
ence only to responses actually observed during
the scoring period). percentages was computed in order to give an
overall index of the teacher's behavior during
Consequences: The Teacher's Behavior each session.
after the Child Responds
Measurements of the Child's Performance
1. Consequences should be delivered immedi-
ately after the response. Immediately is defined In each session, data were recorded continu-
as within three seconds. ously on the correctness of the child's perform-
BEHAVIOR MODIFICATION WITH AUTISTIC CHILDREN 201

ance of the target behavior. On each trial, an of reliability percentages was identical for both
observer recorded whether the child's response the in vivo and videotape recordings.
was correct, incorrect, prompted, or an approxi- For child responses, one trainer and one naive
mation of the target behavior. If an approxima- observer independently and continuously re-
tion was performed, the observer noted whether corded the child's behavior in 24 (of 47) ran-
or not it was a closer approximation to the target domly selected sessions. The observers were in
behavior than the last reinforced response. If a agreement if, on a given trial, they both recorded
prompt was used to bring about a correct re- the response as correct, incorrect, prompted, or
sponse from the child, the observer noted a successive approximation. Reliability was cal-
whether the prompt was faded over the course culated for each session by dividing the number
of training. of agreements by the number of agreements plus
In addition to these trial-by-trial measures of disagreements in that session. The mean reliabil-
student improvement, we obtained a summary ity for these measures was 91.7%, with a range
measure of whether or not a child was learning of 84% to 100%. For the summary (improve-
in each session. A "+" was recorded if the child's ment versus no improvement) measure recorded
responding during the last 10 trials of the ses- in each of these 24 sessions, the observers were
sion was improved (i.e., a closer approximation, in complete agreement.
a reduced prompt, or a higher percentage of
correct responding) as compared to the first 10 RESULTS
trials of the session. Conversely, a "0" was re-
corded if responding during the last 10 trials Measurement of Teacher Behavior
showed no improvement, or a deterioration, Before and After Training
when compared to the first 10 trials. Data for all 11 teachers, showing their use
of behavior-modification procedures during base-
Reliability line and posttraining sessions, are presented in
Reliability of recording the teachers' use of Figure 1. Each point represents a composite
behavior-modification procedures was assessed score based on the average per cent correct use
by the following procedure. Two observers (usu- of the five categories of behavior-modification
ally one trainer and one naive observer) inde- procedures during a given session. The ordinate
pendently (data sheets shielded from each others' shows the per cent correct use of the behavior-
sight) recorded data for 24 of the 47 sessions modification procedures; sessions are presented
in this experiment. In addition, two naive ob- on the abscissa.
servers independently recorded data from 16 During the baseline sessions, all 11 teachers
videotaped sessions shown to them in a random- evidenced low percentages. The percentages for
ized order. An agreement or disagreement be- 10 of the 11 teachers were consistently below
tween observers was recorded for each of the 58%, and in two instances were as low as 0%.
five procedures in each 30-sec interval. An agree- The remaining teacher achieved scores of 63 %,
ment was counted if both observers marked the 91 %, and 71 % during three baseline sessions.
same square on their data sheets, and a disagree- In the posttraining sessions, the 11 teachers
ment was counted if only one observer marked scored between 90% and 100% correct use of
a given square. Reliability for each session was behavior-modification procedures for 25 of the
calculated by dividing number of agreements 26 sessions. For each teacher, this change repre-
(summing across categories) by the total number sents both a decrease in the frequency of incor-
of agreements plus disagreements for that ses- rect intervals, and an increase in the frequency
sion. The mean per cent agreement for these of correct intervals, the number of recording
sessions was 94.6%. The range (82% to 100%) intervals per session remaining constant. All 11
202 ROBERT L. KOEGEL, DENNIS C. RUSSO, and ARNOLD RINCOVER

After
Base After Training Base Training
100
to#TC
60
TEACHER 1
20 - 0
l-1r -1.
I I I I

100 0....

(I,
60
w TEACHER 2
201 0
-1.
I 11 I I
w
u
0 100
60
z TEACHER 3
Y 20
4 I I I1 I I~~~
w
I-- 100
U 60
w
TEACHER 4 10
0 20 0
U I
I I II I I I

I--
z 100
w
U
w 60
a. TEACHER 5
20_
I I I l1
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
100 lc
TASKS
60 L
0
TEACHER 6
20
L- 1--I
*
I I
I
I I I

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

TASKS
Fig. 1. The session-by-session per cent of correct use of behavior-modification procedures by teachers before
and after teacher training.
teachers showed considerable increases in their
correct use of behavior-modification procedures Measurement
of Children's Behavior
after the training program. Furthermore, after Before and After Teacher Training
training, teacher behavior generalized to new To determine whether correct performance of
tasks. the five techniques of behavior modification was
BEHAVIOR MODIFICATION WITH AUTISTIC CHILDREN 203
sufficient for teaching autistic children, we as- a given child during a given session is shown
sessed whether there was an increase in the on the ordinate, and blocks of 10 training trials
child's (unprompted) correct responses during are shown on the abscissa. Single vertical lines
each session. During 20 of the 21 baseline ses- separate different sessions; double vertical lines
sions, the children showed either no improve- signify the time at which the teacher was trained.
ment or a decrease in correct responding. In Since different behaviors were being taught to
marked contrast, after the teachers were trained, different children in each session, the number of
the children improved their level of correct re- trials varied (from 20 to 100 trials) over sessions.
sponding during each of 26 posttraining sessions. The first teacher (Teacher 3 in Figure 2) at-
Figure 2 shows all of the data for four repre- tempted to teach three different behaviors to
sentative teachers. Per cent correct responses three different children during the baseline ses-
performed on the particular task being taught to sions. Ih all three instances, the data are similar;

Baseline After Training


100 -
80-
60- TEACHER 3
40- Task2 Task 3 Task 4 Task 5 Tosk 6
20 -
0-
V I IF I I I I I I I I I I I I I I

C,)
w
Cf)
Baseline After Training
z
0
100-
80 - Tk
*P1*/
a-
CL)
w
n
60- 1l :hedule Faded
0 40-
-J
20- TI Task 2 Task 3 Task 4 Task 5 Task 6 TEACHER 9
I
0-
w
cr
0

z
ILI
w
w
a.

Baseline After Training


100- o

80-
60 - I
40-
TEACHER 7
20- TI T21 T3 I OSK T5 Task 6 Task 7

0-
TRIALS
Fig. 2. The per cent of correct responding by the children before and after teacher training. Per cent correct
responses are plotted for each block of 10 trials within a session.
204 ROBERT L. KOEGEL, DENNIS C. RUSSO, and ARNOLD RINCOVER
the children showed no improvement. In fact, correctly, their teaching was effective in produc-
during these sessions all children evidenced a ing gains in the children's responding. (2) The
lower percentage of correct responding at the training procedure was effective, for all teachers,
end of the session than at the beginning. The in increasing the correct use of the procedures.
results for the other three teachers during base- Further, after training, the teachers' correct use
line sessions are similar. Out of a total of 14 of these procedures generalized to new target
baseline sessions, 13 showed either no improve- behaviors and new children.
ment or a reduced level of correct responding by The training procedure was a package involv-
the end of the session. ing the use of modelling, feedback, and training
After the four teachers had been trained, the manuals. We did not do a component analysis
children's correct responding increased. Every and as such cannot be sure which components
child showed large increases in the per cent of contributed to each aspect of the results. It is
correct responses during each of 12 sessions con- possible that certain components influenced ac-
ducted after teacher training. In four sessions, the quisition of the techniques and others influenced
teachers introduced new prompt, shaping, or re- generalization. A component analysis of this or
inforcement-schedule fading techniques (marked any other teacher-training program might lead
in the figure by asterisks). Although temporary to interesting results (Bandura, 1976; Kazdin
drops in correct responding occurred in these and Moyer, 1976).
cases, each child quickly regained at least 90% There was no attempt to determine correla-
correct responding. tions between intermediate levels of teacher pro-
ficiency (e.g., 50%, 70%, etc.), and the rates of
Generalized Use of Behavior change in correct responding of the children. A
Modification Procedures 909% proficiency on the five procedures was al-
The teachers were not specifically trained to most always sufficient to ensure effective modifi-
teach the particular behaviors used in the post- cation of the behavior of these severely autistic
training assessment; these results show that the children. It may, however, be interesting to note
training procedures produced generalized correct that, during one of the baseline sessions for
applications of the procedures taught during Teacher 9, there was no improvement in child
training. In addition, it may be important to note performance with a high level of correct teacher
that posttraining Sessions 2 and 3 for two teach- performance. Thus, other categories of teacher
ers (Teachers 5 and 7) were conducted two and behavior may be important to measure and
four months after training, leading us to feel some categories may be more important than
optimistic about the durability of the results of others. Possibly some other variable, such as
training. consistency in teacher behavior across sessions,
may to some extent influence the results (note
that this teacher worked with the same child in
DISCUSSION every session). As Parsonsen, Baer, and Baer
The results showed the following. (1) The (1974) and Herbert and Baer (1972) pointed
assessment procedure reliably differentiated be- out, there can be value in improving the overall
tween teachers with respect to their correct use performance of someone who is already mod-
of defined behavior-modification procedures. erately competent.
Generally, when teachers failed to use the pro- The five categories measured and trained in
cedures correctly, then efforts to teach the chil- this investigation are not the only ones that
dren produced no measurable improvement. could be studied. Several investigators (e.g., Le-
Conversely, when the teachers showed consist- Laurin and Risley, 1972; Sailor and Taman,
ently high percentages of using the procedures 1972) have suggested that the physical config-
BEHAVIOR MODIFICATION WITH AUTISTIC CHILDREN 205

uration of the teaching environment may be an LeLaurin, K. and Risley, T. The organization of
important variable influencing learning. In our day-care environments: "zone" versus "man-to-
man" staff assignments. Journal of Applied Be-
own laboratory, we have investigated various havior Analysis, 1972, 5, 225-232.
procedures that are effective for teaching a group Lovaas, 0. I. A program for the establishment of
of autistic children in a classroom setting (Koegel speech in psychotic children. In J. K. Wing (Ed),
Early childhood autism: clinical, educational and
and Rincover, 1974). As these or other new social aspects. London: Pergamon Press, 1966.
functional relationships are discovered, then new Pp. 115-144.
categories should also be included in the assess- Lovaas, 0. I., Freitas, L., Nelson, K., and Whalen, C.
ment and training procedures. The contribution The establishment of imitation and its use for the
establishment of complex behavior in schizo-
that the present study offers at this time is not phrenic children. Behaviour Research and Ther-
in providing an all-inclusive list of procedures apy, 1967, 5, 171-181.
to measure but, rather, in suggesting that it is Lovaas, 0. I. and Koegel, R. L. Behavior modifica-
tion with autistic children. In C. Thoresen (Ed),
important to measure carefully both the behav- Behavior modification in education. Chicago:
ior of the teacher and the behavior of the child University of Chicago Press, 1973. Pp. 230-258.
in order to discover functional relationships Lovaas, 0. I., Koegel, R. L., Simmons, J. Q., and Ste-
vens-Long, J. Some generalization and follow-
between the two. up measures on autistic children in behavior ther-
apy. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1973,
REFERENCES 6, 131-165.
Metz, J. R. Conditioning generalized imitation in
Bandura, A. Effecting change through participant autistic children. Journal of Experimental Child
modeling. In J. D. Krumboltz and C. E. Thoresen Psychology, 1965, 2, 389-399.
(Eds), Counseling methods. New York: Holt, Parsonson, B. S., Baer, A. M., and Baer, D. M. The
Rinehart & Winston, 1976. Pp. 248-265. application of generalized correct social contin-
Birnbrauer, J. S., Peterson, C. P., and Solneck, J. V. gencies: an evaluation of a training program.
The design and interpretation of studies of single Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1974, 7,
subjects. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 427-437.
1974, 79, 191-203. Risley, T. and Wolf, M. Establishing functional
Hewett, F. M. Teaching speech to an autistic child speech in echolalic children. Behaviour Research
through operant conditioning. American Journal and Therapy, 1967, 5, 73-88.
of Orthopsychiatry, 1965, 35, 927-936. Sailor, W. and Taman, T. Stimulus factors in the
Kazdin, A. K. and Moyer, W. Training teachers to training of prepositional usage in three autistic
use behavior modification. In S. Yen and R. Mc- children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
Intire (Eds), Teaching behavior modification. Kal- 1972, 5, 183-192.
amazoo, Michigan: Behaviordelia, 1976. Pp. 171- Wolf, M., Risley, T., and Mees, H. Application of
200. operant conditioning procedures to the behavior
Koegel, R. L. and Rincover, A. Treatment of psy- problems of an autistic child. Behaviour Research
chotic children in a classroom environment: I. and Therapy, 1964,1, 305-312.
Learning in a large group. Journal of Applied
Behavior Analysis, 1974, 7, 45-59.
Kozloff, M. Educating children with learning and Received 27 October 1975.
behavior problems. New York: John Wiley and (Final acceptance 17 May 1976.)
Sons, 1975.

You might also like