Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Rest-Activity Cycles in Childhood and

Adolescent Depression
ROSEANNE ARMITAGE, PH.D., ROBERT HOFFMANN, PH.D., GRAHAM EMSLIE, M.D.,
JEANNE RINTELMAN, B.S., JARRETTE MOORE, M.A., AND KELLY LEWIS, B.S.

ABSTRACT
Objective: To quantify circadian rhythms in rest-activity cycles in depressed children and adolescents. Method: Rest-
activity cycles were evaluated by actigraphy over five consecutive 24-hour periods in 100 children and adolescents,
including 59 outpatients with major depressive disorder (MDD) and 41 healthy normal controls. Total activity, total light
exposure, and time spent in light at more than 1,000 lux were averaged over the recording period for each participant.
Time series analysis was used to determine the amplitude and period length of circadian rhythms in rest-activity.
Results: Overall, adolescents with MDD had lower activity levels, damped circadian amplitude, and lower light exposure
and spent less time in bright light than healthy controls. Among children, those with MDD showed lower light exposure
and spent less time in bright light, but only depressed girls showed damped circadian amplitude. The sex differences
were substantially greater in the MDD group than in the normal control group. Conclusions: These results confirm
damped circadian rhythms in children and adolescents with MDD and highlight the influence of gender and age on these
measures. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry, 2004;43(6):761–769. Key Words: rest-activity cycles, circadian
rhythms, childhood depression, gender.

Sleep and biological rhythm abnormalities have been a sleep cycles and associated rhythms may be more char-
key focus of theories on major depressive disorder acteristic of MDD than either a consistent phase ad-
(MDD) in adults for more than two decades. Begin- vance or delay in circadian timing (Healy, 1987; Schulz
ning with the work of Goodwin et al. (1982), it was and Lund, 1985; Siever and Davis, 1985). The major-
suggested that the sleep, temperature, and cortisol ab- ity of more recent studies, however, have not provided
normalities evident in adults with MDD reflected a strong support for circadian abnormalities in tempera-
phase advance in the timing of circadian rhythms. Oth- ture or endogenous cortisol secretion in adults with
ers have suggested that instability or irregularity of MDD (cf. Monk, 1993). Moreover, only one study has
supported elevated cortisol at sleep onset in adolescents
with MDD (Dahl et al., 1991).
Accepted December 19, 2003.
With regard to sleep laboratory findings, depressed
From the Department of Psychiatry, The University of Texas Southwestern
Medical Center at Dallas. Drs. Armitage and Hoffmann are now at the Uni- adults show shorter rapid eye movement latencies, re-
versity of Michigan in Ann Arbor. duced slow-wave sleep, and increased sleep fragmenta-
This research was supported by NIMH grant MH56593 (R.A.) and was tion, all of which point to abnormalities in the timing
conducted at the Department of Psychiatry, University of Texas Southwestern
Medical Center at Dallas. The authors are grateful for the technical support of of the rapid eye movement-nonrapid eye movement
the Sleep Study Unit at University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center at sleep cycle (Armitage, 1995; Armitage et al., 1992,
Dallas, under the supervision of Darwynn D. Cole; for department support from 1993a,b, 1999; Kupfer et al., 1984a,b, 1990; Reynolds
Eric Nestler, M.D., Ph.D. (Chair); and for the secretarial support from Doris
Benson.
and Kupfer, 1987; Reynolds et al., 1990). The findings
Correspondence to Dr. Armitage, Director, Sleep and Chronophysiology in children and adolescents with MDD have been more
Laboratory, 2101 Commonwealth, Ann Arbor, MI 48105; e-mail: rosearmi@ equivocal. Although some studies have reported REM
umich.edu.
timing problems in early-onset MDD, prolonged sleep
0890-8567/04/4306–0761©2004 by the American Academy of Child
and Adolescent Psychiatry. latency in adolescents is a more consistent finding (Bir-
DOI: 10.1097/01.chi.0000122731.72597.4e maher and Heydl, 2001; Dahl, 1996).

J. AM. ACAD. CHILD ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY, 43:6, JUNE 2004 761


ARMITAGE ET AL.

However, a closer examination of the EEG fre- Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Outpatient Clinic. Rest-activity
measurements were obtained from 100 children and adolescents, 8
quency structure in sleep and ultradian rhythms to 17 years of age, including 59 outpatients diagnosed with uni-
(shorter than 24 hours) has confirmed sleep abnormali- polar, nonpsychotic MDD, single or recurrent, and 41 healthy
ties in depressed adults, in children and adolescents normal controls (NCs). There were 28 females and 31 males in the
with MDD (Armitage et al., 2000a), and in those at sample of outpatients with MDD and 20 females and 21 males in
the sample of healthy NCs. All patients were symptomatic and
risk of, but not yet ill with, MDD (Fulton et al., 2000; unmedicated at the time of the study. Independent sleep disorders
Morehouse et al., 2002). These findings provide strong by history or polysomnography were exclusionary for all partici-
support for the dysregulation hypothesis of depression pants. Diagnostic and demographic information is shown in Table
1. Note that there were no significant differences in age or educa-
and that a more general breakdown in biological tion between groups.
rhythm in rest and activity underlies depression.
Diagnostic Procedures
Studies of motor activity rhythms have also consis-
tently reported blunted circadian amplitude in de- Patients for the study were scheduled for a full evaluation after a
telephone screening for inclusion and exclusion criteria. The study
pressed adults (cf. Goodwin and Jamieson, 1990) and was approved by the Institutional Review Board at the University of
in children and adolescents with nonseasonal depres- Texas Southwestern Medical Center at Dallas. Before the initial
sion (Teicher et al., 1993). The data from Teicher et al. interview, the study was explained and written informed consent
was obtained from the parent(s) and assent from the patient. In
(1993) go further to suggest that the ultradian compo- addition to a structured psychiatric interview, the initial evaluation
nents of locomotor rhythms are often more pro- included a medical review of systems, a physical examination, rou-
nounced than circadian rhythms and that these tine laboratory tests, and neurological examination. The evaluation
findings were evident in both children and adolescents was completed during a 3-week period.
At the initial visit, each patient and parent(s) were interviewed
with MDD. Damped circadian amplitude was evident separately using the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizo-
in adolescents but not in children. phrenia for School-Age Children–Present and Lifetime (K-SADS-
Interestingly, Glod et al. (1997) also reported PL), a revision of the K-SADS (Kaufman et al., 1997). The
K-SADS-PL is a semistructured DSM-IV–based diagnostic inter-
blunted locomotor rhythms in children and adolescents view to establish that the patient met DSM-IV criteria for MDD
with seasonal affective disorder, although this was not and to identify other concurrent and lifetime psychiatric disorders.
confirmed in adults with this disorder (Teicher et al., The final diagnoses were based on information from interviews of
the parent(s) and child. Additionally, depressive symptom severity
1997). We have argued previously that abnormalities was assessed using the Children’s Depression Rating Scale-Revised
in ultradian rest-activity cycles may underlie both the (Poznanski et al., 1985). While the child was being interviewed, a
sleep EEG and locomotor disturbances evident in separate interviewer obtained family history from the parent(s) us-
MDD but that the effects may be gender specific ing the Family History Diagnostic Interview. Patients completed
two self-report scales for depression and anxiety, the Weinberg
(Armitage and Hoffmann 2001; Armitage et al., Screening Affective Scale–Short Form providing information on a
2000a–c, 2001, 2002; Hoffmann et al., 2000). The patient’s perception of his/her problems based on 10 major symp-
sleep EEG frequency analyses do indicate a greater de- tom groups of depression (Weinberg and Emslie, 1988), and the
Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children (March et al., 1997).
gree of ultradian rhythm disturbance among females The Children’s Global Assessment Scale assessed overall function-
with MDD, from early childhood to later adulthood ing (Shaffer et al., 1985). Tanner upper and lower body maturation
but particularly in adolescence. These findings, coupled (1–5 scores) was self-assessed by participants using the “Typical
Progression of Pubertal Development Chart” adapted from Tanner
with the results from Teicher et al. (1993), suggest that (1962, 1978). Breast and pubic hair development was assessed for
locomotor rhythms in those with MDD should show girls and genital and pubic hair development was assessed for boys.
both age and gender effects. Note that Duke et al. (1980) have shown that children can reliably
The purpose of the current study was to evaluate self-rate sexual maturation using these standard pictures. A third
interview was conducted just before the sleep study to review psy-
rest-activity cycles and locomotor activity in 59 de- chiatric assessment and inventories. A Children’s Depression Rating
pressed children and 41 healthy controls. Of primary Scale-Revised score of 40 or more was required for entry into the
interest was an evaluation of gender differences and study. Further details on the clinical evaluation procedures are re-
ported elsewhere (Emslie et al., 2001).
age-related differences in both patients and controls. All NC children and adolescents underwent the same initial
psychiatric and medical evaluations as those with MDD and were
METHOD scheduled for the laboratory tour after the first interview.
Subjects Procedures
Participants were recruited through campus and community ad- All participants agreed to follow their usual school week estab-
vertisement, by word of mouth, or during an initial visit to the lished by sleep history with bed- and rise-times schedules through-

762 J. AM. ACAD. CHILD ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY, 43:6, JUNE 2004


REST-ACTIVITY CYCLES

TABLE 1
Demographic and Clinical Features of the Sample by Diagnostic Group and Gender
MDD F MDD M NC F NC M
No. 28 31 21 20
Age overall mean 12.3 ± 2.9 12.3 ± 3.0 12.4 ± 3.1 12.4 ± 2.5
≤12 yr 9.9 ± 1.4 9.5 ± 1.3 9.9 ± 1.7 10.3 ± 1.4
≥13 yr 15.0 ± 1.4 15.5 ± 1.6 14.9 ± 1.9 14.5 ± 1.3
Tannera
A 2.9 ± 1.5 2.7 ± 1.5 3.5 ± 1.5 3.0 ± 1.6
B 2.8 ± 1.6 2.8 ± 1.7 3.3 ± 1.5 2.9 ± 1.6
FGAS 61.4 ± 10.4 64.5 ± 11.9 91.7 ± 2.6 91.5 ± 6.0
CGAS 53.0 ± 7.3 52.5 ± 6.1 90.7 ± 4.1 90.7 ± 5.4
MASC 54.2 ± 19.8 48.8 ± 16.1 33.1 ± 13.6 30.6 ± 14.7
CDRS 58.9 ± 11.1 57.1 ± 7.2 19.0 ± 2.3 18.4 ± 2.0
BPRS 33.0 ± 9.0 34.4 ± 8.4 2.4 ± 3.1 2.4 ± 2.8
No. of episodes 1.4 ± 0.7 1.4 ± 0.6 — —
Age at onset of 1st episode 11.1 ± 3.2 11.3 ± 3.3 — —
Length of current episode 18.3 ± 7.1 18.5 ± 10.2 — —
Suicide attempts 0 0 — —
Suicidal ideation 2.3 ± 0.8 2.3 ± 0.6 — —
Family history (%)
Maternal MDD 64.3 45.2
Paternal MDD 17.9 12.9
Comorbid illness, no. (%) 17 (60.2) 16 (51.6)
GAD 1 2
OCD 0 1
Dysthymia 7 4
ODD 0 1
Phobia 0 1
Enuresis 1 0
ADHD 8 7
Note: MDD F = depressed females; MDD M = depressed males; NC F = normal control females; NC M = normal control
males; FGAS = Family Global Assessment Scale; CGAS = Children’s Global Assessment Scale; MASC = Multidimensional
Anxiety Scale for Children; CDRS = Children’s Depression Rating Scale; BPRS = Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale; MDD =
major depressive disorder; GAD = general anxiety disorder; OCD = obsessive-compulsive disorder; ODD = oppositional
defiant disorder; ADHD = attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
a
Tanner A is based on breast development for girls and genitalia for boys. Tanner B assesses development of pubic hair.

out the study. They were informed that a deviation of more than subjects were instructed not to wear any clothing that covered up
one half hour would result in elimination from the study. With the the watch face at any time during the recording period.
exception of two children and three adolescents, all participants The technical reliability of the actigraph was very high, with little
were tested Monday through Friday during the regular school year loss of data. Subjects were compliant with the actigraphy proce-
outside of holiday and vacation schedules. Actigraphs (Actiwatch-L, dures, resulting in the loss of very few data points. Of the 100 study
Mini-Mitter) were worn throughout the week, and sleep/wake dia- participants, complete 5-day actigraphy data were available for 95
ries were collected daily during the home recording period. Acti- participants. For the remaining five subjects, 3 complete days of
graphs were set to begin at noon and end at 9 A.M. on the last data were available. The additional days were excluded from analy-
morning of the recording. The actigraphs measured the number of sis. The 24-hour blocks with missing data due to participants re-
movements that exceeded 0.01g (gravitation force per minute of moving the watch were excluded from the analysis.
recording). In addition, a photoconductive cell recorded light level
exposure, measured in lux. All actigraphs were calibrated before
Data Analysis
recording to ensure comparability across subjects. Thresholds, sen-
sitivities, scaling, and epoch lengths were held constant across all Data were downloaded from the actigraphs at the end of the
individual recordings. Actigraphs were not removed while shower- recording period. Initial data analyses were conducted with on-
ing or bathing. Subjects were asked to note in the diaries whether board Mini-Mitter software, computing the average activity count
the actigraphs were removed, for how long, and for what purpose per epoch in the light and dark periods. Total daily light exposure
and to record any travel periods less than 15 minutes. Finally, was also computed for each day of recording, along with average

J. AM. ACAD. CHILD ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY, 43:6, JUNE 2004 763


ARMITAGE ET AL.

light exposure and the amount of time in light above a 1,000-lux dren and adolescents was greater in the MDD group.
level across the recording period.
The raw activity data per minute (time series) were also exported
Statistical analyses indicated that these effects were
to SAS and subjected to Fourier-based spectral analysis for each strongly moderated by gender. ANOVA revealed a sig-
individual subject to determine the circadian amplitude or strength nificant gender by diagnostic group by age interaction
of the 24-hour rhythms. Relative circadian amplitude was also com- for total activity (F7,92 = 2.2; p < .04) and for activity
puted (100 × amplitude/mean daily activity) to ensure that ampli-
tude estimates were not biased by total activity (Teicher, et al., level during the light phase (F7,92 = 2.8; p < .02) but
1993). All data were coded for group (NC or MDD), gender, and not during the dark phase (F7,92 = 1.1; p < .35).
age, using an arbitrary cut point of 12 to contrast children and The three-way interaction for total daily activity is
adolescents. Average Tanner scores (upper body + lower body/2)
were also used to evaluate maturational age by gender by group illustrated in Figure 1. Male children with MDD
interaction. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) evaluated statistical dif- showed more total activity than the other groups of the
ferences, testing the group by gender by age interactions first, and same age range but differed significantly only from
simple interactions and main effects only if three-way interactions
were not significant. Least-squares multiple comparisons contrasted
MDD girls (p < .03). Moreover, the differences in total
differences between individual means only if a significant overall activity between the age groups were most dramatic in
ANOVA effect was obtained to protect against type I errors. the MDD males; adolescents had significantly lower
activity than children (p < .001). By contrast, the dif-
RESULTS
ference between the child and adolescent groups were
not significant for NC females (p < .23), NC males (p <
Using an age cut point of 12, there were 15 girls and .37), or MDD females (p < .22), accounting for the
11 boys in the 8- to 12-year-old MDD group. There three-way interaction. Results were similar for activity
were 10 girls and 11 boys in the 8- to 12-year-old NC in the light phase.
group. The adolescent group had 13 girls and 14 boys Activity in the dark phase did show a main effect for
with MDD and 10 NC girls and 10 NC boys all in the age (F1,92 = 4.0; p < .05). Means indicated that activity
13- to 17-year-old age range. during the dark was higher in adolescents than in chil-
dren, in all groups, as is evident in Table 2. Note that
Activity Levels the minimal and maximal values were lower in the
The means and standard deviations for all the activ- MDD groups for all activity measures.
ity measures are shown in Table 2 by group and age but
collapsed across gender. The means indicate substantial Light Exposure
age-related differences in activity, with lower total ac- The means and standard deviations for average daily
tivity and lower activity in the light phase in the ado- light level and the average time spent above a 1,000-lux
lescents but with higher activity in the dark phase. threshold are shown in Table 2. Average light levels
Further, the difference in total activity between chil- were lower in both children and adolescents with

TABLE 2
Means (and Standard Deviations) of Actigraphy Measures by Diagnostic Group (NC Versus MDD) and Age Group
NC MDD
Children Adolescents Children Adolescents
a
Activity total 18,313.6 (9,378.2) 14,461.4 (8,464.7) 18,888.2 (9,936.4) 11,626.7 (7,380.8)
Avg. no. in light 386.0 (191.9) 288.2 (169.1) 389.9 (212.9) 236.7 (153.7)
Avg. no. in dark 25.2 (27.5) 38.2 (28.5) 28.5 (30.9) 40.6 (33.3)
Light avg.b (lux) 847.9 (758.6) 832.7 (1,162.9) 597.9 (647.1) 408.1 (586.1)
Timec (min) 99.6 (90.4) 81.4 (67.5) 64.0 (44.6) 50.3 (50.1)
Circadian period (h) 24.30 (0.30) 24.09 (0.36) 23.98 (0.38) 24.18 (0.32)
Amplituded 2,865.3 (2,784.6) 1,467.8 (1,335.9) 3,311.5 (3,038.5) 987.1 (1,757.1)
a
Total number of events more than criterion × 103.
b
Average time at more than 1,000 lux.
c
Average daily time spent at 1,000 or more lux.
d
Power (area under the curve) × 104.

764 J. AM. ACAD. CHILD ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY, 43:6, JUNE 2004


REST-ACTIVITY CYCLES

group by age interaction was obtained for this measure


(F7,92 = 2.04; p < .06).
The average time spent in bright light showed simi-
lar results. Also seen in Table 2, the MDD group spent
roughly 30 minutes less on average in bright light than
NCs, regardless of age. ANOVA indicated significant
age by diagnostic group and gender by diagnostic
group interactions (F3,96 = 3.1, 2.7; p < .05, respec-
tively) but no significant three-way interaction (F7,92 =
1.6; p < .17). As seen with the average light level, NC
adolescent males showed slightly more (4 minutes)
time spent in bright light compared with children in
Fig. 1 Total daytime activity counts over 5 days in children (filled bars) this group. Although the means in Table 2 suggest
and adolescents (open bars) by gender and group. NC F = normal control
females; MDD F = depressed females; NC M = normal control males;
equivalent age-related differences in the daily time
MDD M = depressed males. spent in bright light in the MDD and NC groups, the
greatest difference between children and adolescents
was observed in the NC females. Girls in the NC group
MDD compared with controls. The between-group had an average of 90 minutes of bright light exposure,
differences are illustrated in Figure 2. Young girls with whereas adolescents in this group spent only 67 min-
MDD had lower light exposure than the other groups utes in bright light (data not shown). Nevertheless,
of children, whereas adolescent males in the NC group both male and female adolescents in the NC group still
had the highest level of exposure, confirmed by mul- had more light exposure than their MDD counterparts
tiple comparisons (p < .05). The NC males were also (p < .05). Once again, the minimal and maximal values
the only group to show more light exposure in the were lower in those with MDD than in the NCs for
adolescents than in the children, accounting for the both light variable groups.
nearly significant three-way interaction (p < .06). In-
terestingly, adolescent NC girls and girls and boys with Circadian Rhythm Measures
MDD were exposed mostly to indoor light levels. Only The means and standard deviations for the period
the adolescent NC males had sustained outdoor light length and the amplitude of circadian rest-activity
exposure. However, no significant gender by diagnostic rhythms are also shown in Table 2. Although children
with MDD had a circadian rhythm that was 21 min-
utes shorter than that of NC children (23 hours 59
minutes versus 24 hours 20 minutes), this difference
was not significant. The circadian period length did not
appear to differ by age or diagnostic group. Further,
ANOVA did not indicate significant main effects or
interactions for this variable (p > .23).
Collapsed across gender, the amplitude of circadian
rhythms was lower in adolescents with MDD com-
pared with NCs, as seen in Table 2. Children with
MDD had higher amplitude rhythms than controls;
however, a significant three-way interaction was ob-
tained for this measure (F7,91 = 3.0; p < .006). Relative
circadian amplitude adjusted for total activity also
showed a three-way interaction (F7,91 = 3.8; p < .002)
Fig. 2 Day light level (lux) averaged over 5 days in children (filled bars)
and is depicted in Figure 3. The means indicated that
and adolescents (open bars) by group and gender. NC F = normal control
females; MDD F = depressed females; NC M = normal control males; both female children and adolescents with MDD had
MDD M = depressed males. lower relative amplitude circadian rhythms than all

J. AM. ACAD. CHILD ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY, 43:6, JUNE 2004 765


ARMITAGE ET AL.

damped circadian amplitude compared with NCs (p <


.05).

Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Comorbidity


One further concern was the higher incidence of
comorbid attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder
(ADHD) in the children with MDD, particularly with
regard to the failure to find lower activity or damped
circadian amplitude in young boys with MDD. We
recomputed the analyses of the rest-activity measures
excluding the three adolescents and 12 children who
Fig. 3 Relative amplitude (power) of circadian (24-hour) rhythms in total had morbid ADHD. Removing these subjects had only
activity in children (filled bars) and adolescents (open bars) averaged by a small effect on results, reducing the overall probabil-
group and gender. Amplitude determined by time series analysis (based on
a fast Fourier transform) of activity count per minute throughout five ity due to the loss of degrees of freedom, but did not
consecutive 24-hour recording periods for each individual. NC F = normal alter the effect size. For relative circadian amplitude,
control females; MDD F = depressed females; NC M = normal control light exposure and activity levels, the resulting three-
males; MDD M = depressed males.
way interactions were (F7,85 = 2.7, 2.4; p < .03, 1.93;
p < .07, respectively). Even excluding the outpatients
with comorbid MDD, circadian amplitude was
other groups, whereas MDD boys showed higher cir-
damped in preteen and teenage girls and in teenage
cadian amplitude than any other group. Multiple com-
boys with MDD. Thus, the failure to find damped
parisons confirmed these differences (p < .05), except
amplitude rhythms in preteen depressed boys was not
when comparing NC boys with MDD boys. In addi-
due to comorbid ADHD.
tion, Figure 3 also illustrates the dramatic age differ-
ences in circadian amplitude, evident in all groups. The
difference between children and adolescents was, how- DISCUSSION
ever, most pronounced in the MDD males, with more
To summarize the overall findings, adolescents with
than 70% lower amplitude in the adolescents (p <
MDD had lower activity levels, damped circadian am-
.002), further contributing to the significant three-way
plitude, and lower light exposure and spent less time in
interaction.
bright light than healthy controls. Among preteens,
Thus, circadian rhythms and light exposure were
girls with MDD had lower light exposure, spent less
most abnormal in girls with MDD and evident even in
time in bright light than controls, and showed lower
preteens.
circadian amplitude. By contrast, preteen boys with
MDD had robust circadian amplitude. Most impor-
Chronological Versus Maturational Age tantly, the same results were obtained with relative cir-
To ensure that our arbitrary age cut point of 12 years cadian amplitude, adjusted for mean activity levels.
did not produce an artificial distinction between Thus, it is not just reduced activity that is evident in
groups, we also conducted an analysis of age using those with MDD. The results of the current study
average upper and lower body Tanner scores. The conform to those of Teicher et al. (1993), indicating
three-way interactions (group by gender by develop- that circadian amplitude was damped in adolescents
mental age) were evaluated for relative circadian am- with MDD. The Teicher et al. sample of control sub-
plitude, light exposure, and total activity. The outcome jects was substantially smaller, patients were 2.5 to 3.4
was strikingly similar to that obtained with chronolog- years older than controls, and neither potential gender
ical age (F7,69 = 3.1, 2.8, 2.4; p < .01, .02, .03, respec- effects nor light levels were evaluated. Nevertheless, the
tively), significantly lower circadian amplitude, light rest-activity cycle data in the current study are remark-
exposure, and total activity in prepubertal (Tanner 1 or ably similar to those shown in the Teicher et al. report.
2) girls with MDD (p < .05). Pubertal (Tanner 4 or 5) Damped circadian amplitude is likely to reflect weak
but not prepubertal boys with MDD also showed entrainment to a 24-hour day and/or reduced

766 J. AM. ACAD. CHILD ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY, 43:6, JUNE 2004


REST-ACTIVITY CYCLES

exposure to zeitgebers (time cues and entrainment) are more dramatic in those with MDD (Armitage,
(Teicher et al., 1993). Because the master circadian 1995; Armitage and Hoffmann, 2001; Armitage et al.,
clock is strongly driven by light (Czeisler et al., 1989) 1999, 2000b,c).
and given the low light levels that appear to accompany
damped amplitude, it suggests that increasing light ex- Limitations
posure will enhance circadian amplitude. Additional
There are limitations to this study that need to be
clinical implications of these results are discussed below.
considered when interpreting data, most notably
Our findings are also in agreement with those of
sample size. Splitting the groups by age and gender left
Glod et al. (1997) who reported blunted circadian am-
only 10 to 17 subjects per cell. There is some concern
plitude but no phase disruption in children with sea-
that the findings may not generalize to a larger sample.
sonal affective disorder, although their study did not
Thus, it will be necessary to replicate our findings.
assess gender. Consistent with the dysregulation hy-
Such efforts are currently underway.
pothesis of depression (Siever and Davis, 1985), we did
In addition, it is also necessary to determine whether
not find evidence of a phase advance in circadian timing.
the rest-activity abnormalities obtained in this study
The strong influence of gender obtained in the cur-
correlate with the amplitude and phase of other circa-
rent study is also consistent with our previous reports
dian measures such as temperature and cortisol. A con-
on sleep data. The gender differences in the MDD
stant routine or forced desynchrony paradigm would
group were substantially larger than those obtained in
allow an assessment of the interaction of circadian and
healthy controls overall. We have previously reported
wake-dependent processes, including mood. Because
larger gender differences in adults with MDD com-
recent work has demonstrated that circadian phase also
pared with controls on several quantitative EEG mea-
affects mood regulation (Boivin et al., 1997), it would
sures during sleep (Armitage and Hoffmann, 2001;
be of significance to include repeated mood assessments
Armitage et al., 1999, 2000b,c). Further, we have also
in further studies of circadian rhythms in depression. It
reported a greater degree of ultradian rhythm distur-
will undoubtedly be necessary to manipulate light ex-
bance in females with MDD, in line with increased risk
posure and rest-activity cycles, including sleep-wake
of MDD among women (Armitage and Hoffmann,
cycles to evaluate the clinical impact directly of these
2001) and with studies of continuing biological vul-
measures in depression and to further elucidate the
nerability to recurrence that is greater in women. The
mechanisms responsible for damped amplitude rest-
data from the current study extend these gender dif-
activity cycles.
ferences to circadian rest-activity cycles. Even 8- to
An additional limitation to this study is our inability
12-year-old girls with MDD show damped circadian
to fully assess the influence of comorbidity on rest-
amplitude, whereas damped circadian rhythms were
activity cycles. Although we reanalyzed our data ex-
only evident in the adolescent boys with MDD.
cluding those with comorbid ADHD and found very
Interestingly, it is believed that the increased risk of
similar results to full analysis, it is still possible that this
MDD in females does not occur until after puberty,
and other comorbid illness could contribute to rest-
with equivalent risk of MDD in prepubertal boys and
activity cycle parameters. Nonetheless, comorbid illness
girls (Frank and Young, 2000; Kessler, 2000; Parry,
was not found to differentiate between the genders.
2000). Our data suggest that the developmental time
Thus, comorbidity cannot account for the significant
course of biological rhythm abnormalities is different in
group by gender effects obtained in this study.
boys and girls with MDD. Damped circadian rest-
activity cycles are evident earlier in females than in
males with MDD. It is not clear, however, how (or Clinical Implications
whether) these biological abnormalities relate to the The current study indicates that damped circadian
risk of MDD or to the clinical course of illness. A study amplitude is characteristic of teenagers with MDD and
of children at risk of MDD and longitudinal evalua- of preteen depressed girls. Because relative circadian
tions of those already ill are necessary to address these amplitude was also found to be damped, it appears
issues. Regardless of the outcome, the current study unlikely that increasing total activity alone would nor-
supports our previous findings that gender differences malize circadian amplitude. The regularity of daytime

J. AM. ACAD. CHILD ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY, 43:6, JUNE 2004 767


ARMITAGE ET AL.

and nighttime schedules, of the amount of light expo- Dahl RE (1996), The regulation of sleep and arousal: development and
psychopathology. Dev Psychopathol 8:3–27
sure, and of the timing of these events is more likely Dahl RE, Ryan ND, Puig-Antich J et al. (1991), 24-hour cortisol measures
to have an impact on the strength of rhythms. The in adolescents with major depression: a controlled study. Biol Psychiatry
30:25–36
outcome of this study suggests that behavioral and Duke PM, Litt IF, Gross RT (1980), Adolescents’ self-assessment of sexual
chronobiological intervention may be of benefit in maturation. Pediatrics 66:918–920
Emslie GJ, Armitage R, Weinberg WA, Rush AJ, Mayes TL, Hoffmann RF
early-onset depression. Regularized sleep-wake sched- (2001), Sleep polysomnography as a predictor of recurrence in children
ules, structured daytime activity, and increased light and adolescents with major depressive disorder. Int J Neuropsychophar-
exposure may serve to strengthen entrainment and to macol 4:159–168
Frank E, Young E (2000), Pubertal changes and adolescent challenges. Why
stabilize sleep-wake cycles. Ultimately, improving sleep do rates of depression rise precipitously for girls between ages 10 and 15
and circadian rhythm regulation should the biological years? In: Gender and Its Effects on Psychopathology, Frank E, ed. Wash-
ington, DC: American Psychiatric Press, pp 85–102
risk of depression, although any direct clinical benefit Fulton MK, Armitage R, Rush AJ (2000), Sleep electroencephalographic
remains to be established. coherence abnormalities in individuals at high risk for depression: a pilot
study. Biol Psychiatry 47:618–625
Glod CA, Teicher MH, Polcari A, McGreenery CE, Ito Y (1997), Circadian
rest-activity disturbances in children with seasonal affective disorder. J
Disclosure: Dr. Armitage currently has two RO1 (MH54593 and Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 36:188–195
MH061515). She currently serves on advisory boards for Pfizer and Goodwin FK, Jamieson KR (1990), Manic-Depressive Illness. New York:
Takeda Pharmaceuticals. Oxford University Press
Goodwin FK, Wirz-Justice A, Wehr T (1982), Evidence that the patho-
physiology of depression and the mechanism of action of antidepressant
drugs involve alterations in circadian rhythms. Adv Biochem Psychophar-
REFERENCES macol 31:1–11
Healy D (1987), Rhythm and blues. Neurochemical, neuropharmacological
Armitage R (1995), Microarchitectural findings in sleep EEG in depression: and neuropsychological implications of a hypothesis of circadian
diagnostic implications. Biol Psychiatry 37:72–84 rhythm dysfunction in the affective disorders. Psychopharmacology
Armitage R, Emslie GJ, Hoffmann RF et al. (2000a), Ultradian rhythms 93:271–285
and temporal coherence in sleep EEG in depressed children and ado- Hoffmann R, Hendrickse W, Rush AJ, Armitage R (2000), Slow-wave
lescents. Biol Psychiatry 47:338–350 activity during NREM sleep in men with schizophrenia and major
Armitage R, Emslie GJ, Hoffmann RF, Rintelmann J, Rush AJ (2001), depressive disorders. Psychiatry Res 95:215–225
Delta sleep EEG in depressed adolescent females and healthy controls. Kaufman J, Birmaher B, Brent D et al. (1997), Schedule for Affective
J Affect Disord 63:139–148 Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children–Present and Life-
Armitage R, Hoffmann RF (2001), Sleep EEG, depression and gender. Sleep time Version (K-SADS-PL). Initial reliability and validity data. J Am
Med Rev 5:237–246 Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 36:980–988
Armitage R, Hoffmann RF, Emslie GJ, Weinberg WA, Mayes TL, Rush AJ Kessler RC (2000), Gender differences in major depression: epidemiological
(2002), Sleep microarchitecture as a predictor of recurrence in children findings. In: Gender and Its Effects on Psychopathology, Frank E, ed.
and adolescents with depression. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 5:217–228 Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press, pp 61–84
Armitage R, Hoffmann R, Fitch T, Trivedi M, Rush AJ (2000b), Temporal Kupfer DJ, Frank E, McEachran AB, Grochocinski VJ (1990), Delta sleep
characteristics of delta activity during NREM sleep in depressed ratio: a biological correlate of early recurrence in unipolar affective
outpatients and healthy adults: group and sex effects. Sleep 23:607– disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 47:1100–1105
617 Kupfer DJ, Ulrich RF, Coble PA et al. (1984a), Application of an auto-
Armitage R, Hoffmann RF, Rush AJ (1999), Biological rhythm disturbance mated REM and slow wave sleep analysis: I. Normal and depressed
in depression: temporal coherence of ultradian sleep EEG rhythms. subjects. Psychiatry Res 13:325–334
Psychol Med 29:1435–1448 Kupfer DJ, Ulrich RF, Coble PA et al. (1984b), Application of an auto-
Armitage R, Hoffman R, Trivedi M, Rush AJ (2000c), Slow-wave activity mated REM and slow wave sleep analysis. II. Testing the assumptions
in NREM sleep: sex and age effects in depressed outpatients and healthy of the two-process model of sleep regulation in normal and depressed
controls. Psychiatry Res 95:201–213 subjects. Psychiatry Res 13:335–343
Armitage R, Hudson A, Trivedi M, Rush AJ (1993a), Sex differences in the March JS, Parker JDA, Sullivan K, Stallings P, Conners CK (1997), The
distribution of EEG frequencies during sleep: unipolar depressed out- Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children (MASC): factor structure,
patients. J Affect Disord 34:121–129 reliability, and validity. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 36:554–565
Armitage R, Roffwarg HP, Rush AJ (1993b), Digital period analysis of Monk TH (1993), Biological rhythms and depressive disorders. In: Biology
EEG in depression: periodicity, coherence, and interhemispheric rela- of Depressive Disorders, Part A: A Systems Perspective, Mann JJ, Kupfer
tionships during sleep. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry DJ, eds. New York: Plenum Press, pp 109–122
17:363–372 Morehouse RL, Kusumakar V, Kutcher SP, LeBlanc J, Armitage R (2002),
Armitage R, Roffwarg HP, Rush AJ, Calhoun JS, Purdy DG, Giles DE Temporal coherence in ultradian sleep EEG rhythms in a never-
(1992), Digital period analysis of sleep EEG in depression. Biol Psychia- depressed, high-risk cohort of female adolescents. Biol Psychiatry
try 31:52–68 51:446–456
Birmaher B, Heydl P (2001), Biological studies in depressed children and Parry BL (2000), Hormonal basis of mood disorders in women. In: Gender
adolescents. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 4:149–157 and Its Effects on Psychopathology, Frank E, ed. Washington, DC: Ameri-
Boivin DB, Czeisler CA, Dijk D-J et al. (1997), Complex interaction of the can Psychiatric Press, pp 3–21
sleep-wake cycle and circadian phase modulates mood in healthy sub- Poznanski EO, Freeman LN, Mokros HB (1985), Children’s Depression
jects. Arch Gen Psychiatry 54:145–152 Rating Scale – Revised (September 1984) Psychopharmacol Bull 21:979–
Czeisler CA, Kronauer RE, Allan JS et al. (1989), Bright light induction of 989
strong (type 0) resetting of the human circadian pacemaker. Science Reynolds CF III, Kupfer DJ (1987), Sleep research in affective illness: state
244:1328–1333 of the art circa 1987. Sleep 10:199–215

768 J. AM. ACAD. CHILD ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY, 43:6, JUNE 2004


REST-ACTIVITY CYCLES

Reynolds CF III, Kupfer DJ, Thase ME et al. (1990), Sleep, gender, and Tanner JM (1962), Growth at Adolescence, 2nd ed. Springfield, IL: Charles
depression: an analysis of gender effects on the electroencephalographic C Thomas
sleep of 302 depressed outpatients. Biol Psychiatry 28:673–684 Tanner JM (1978), Fetus Into Man. Cambridge: Harvard University Press
Schulz H, Lund R (1985), On the origin of early REM episodes in the sleep Teicher MH, Glod CA, Harper D et al. (1993), Locomotor activity in
of depressed patients: a comparison of three hypotheses. Psychiatry Res depressed children and adolescents: I. Circadian dysregulation. J Am
16:65–77 Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 32:760–769
Shaffer D, Gould MS, Brasic J et al. (1985), A children’s global assessment Teicher MH, Glod CA, Magnus E et al. (1997), Circadian rest-activity
scale (CGAS) (for children 4 to 16 years of age). Psychopharmacol Bull disturbances in seasonal affective disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 54:124–
21:747–748 130
Siever LJ, Davis KL (1985), Overview: toward a dysregulation hypothesis of Weinberg WA, Emslie GJ (1988), Weinberg Screening Affective Scale
depression. Am J Psychiatry 142:1017–1031 (WSAS and WSAS-SF). J Child Neurol 3:294–296

J. AM. ACAD. CHILD ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY, 43:6, JUNE 2004 769

You might also like