Facilitating The Transition To First Grade - The Nature of Transition and Research On Factors Affecting It

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 14

Facilitating the Abstract

Transition to First In this articlewe describethe natureof the first-


grade transitionand summarizeseveral studies
Grade: The Nature that have investigatedhow children'sschooling
proceeds over this period. Drawing on Begin-
of Transition and ning School Study data that include children's
marks and test scores plus informationabout
their parents and schools, we carriedout a lon-
Research on Factors gitudinal study of a large random sample (N =
790) of children who began first grade in Balti-
Affecting It more in 1982.Westudied effectson the transition
for childrenattending full-day ratherthan half-
day kindergarten,of living in differentkinds of
family arrangements,and of several other cir-
cumstances.We found that children with more
Doris R. Entwisle kindergarten,those whose families included co-
resident grandmothers,and those who did not
Karl L. Alexander change schools between kindergartenand first
JohnsHopkins University grade did betterover the transition,otherthings
being equal. We close with a list of implications
for practicebased on what is known about the
first-grade transitionand offer suggestions for
futureresearch.

In this article we provide an overview of the


nature and importance of the first-grade
transition. The first part describes the tran-
sition. We then summarize several quanti-
tative studies that have examined how the
circumstances of that transition affect stu-
dents' progress in school, drawing mainly
on data from the Beginning School Study in
Baltimore.

Nature of the First-Grade Transition


So far, surprisingly little is known about the
first-grade transition, yet how children ne-
gotiate the move to formal schooling has
important and long-lasting effects on their
school careers. In fact, this transition con-
The ElementarySchoolJournal stitutes a "critical period" for their aca-
Volume 98, Number 4
C 1998 by The University of Chicago. All rights reserved. demic and social development (Entwisle &
0013-5984/98/9804-0007$02.00 Alexander, 1989).

This content downloaded from 141.218.001.105 on August 04, 2016 16:46:23 PM


All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
352 THE ELEMENTARYSCHOOLJOURNAL

The Critical Period children-they have an unusual "response


The critical period idea has its roots in potential." Children's cognitive develop-
ethology (see Brauth, Hall, & Dooling 1991; ment proceeds rapidly over ages 5-8. For
Newport, 1991) and is defined as a life stage example, their memory span, general learn-
of limited duration where an unusual re- ing capacity, and speed of cognitive pro-
sponse potential of an organism is coupled cessing all develop rapidly during this pe-
with particular kinds of environmental riod (Varnhagen, Morrison, & Everall,
stimulation. A well-known example of a 1994). In Piagetian terms, they are shifting
critical period is imprinting in newborn from preoperational to concrete modes of
ducklings. For a period lasting just 24 operational thought, beginning to test ideas
hours, ducklings instinctively follow and of invariance and causality, and starting to
become attached to any large animal in their form rudimentary conceptions of time,
vicinity, including humans. They have the number, and logic. For example, children
innate propensity to follow (an "unusual re- are able to understand that the number of
sponse potential"), and in most cases the fe- objects in a group is not changed when the
male parent (a "particular kind of stimula- objects are rearranged in space. A dramatic
tion") is there to be followed, but this cognitive change occurs in their under-
sensitive period is of brief duration. standing of language at this stage because
The existence of "hard-wired" critical they reorganize their filing system for
periods in the development of children is words. At age 5, when asked to give a word
debatable, but critical period imagery is in response to "went," they say "home"-
nonetheless useful for thinking about early "went home." By age 8, when asked to give
schooling. First, events during the earliest a word in response to "went," they say "go"
years of school provide a "particular" kind ("went-go"), showing they have learned the
of stimulation: elementary schools are so- substitution privileges of these two verbs.
cially organized in ways children have not "Go" almost never follows "went" in a sen-
previously experienced and provide the tence, so children cannot have heard these
kind of stimulation society believes is essen- two words in contiguity, but they have de-
tial for children's development. An unre- duced how and when one of these words
lated adult (the teacher) is in control. Chil- can substitute for the other. "Went" and
dren meet in classrooms with desks, books, "go" have the same privileges of occurrence
blackboards, clocks, bells, maps, and often in speech or text-"they go home/they
plants, animals, and mineral specimens de- went home"-and children have deduced
signed for their instruction and remain this fact plus many others and use that in-
there for several hours each weekday. The formation to construct a general theory of
presence of other children who share the language.
same goals and who are at about the same The first two grades constitute a critical
level of competence provides a strong in- period, then, because the child's external
centive to do well because human beings and internal worlds are undergoing pro-
seek social approval (peers) and at the same found change at the same time. Over this pe-
time are profoundly rewarded by positive riod, when children are separated from
reinforcement from an authority figure their families and instructed in groups, their
(teacher). All these "particulars" provide an own capabilities are simultaneously chang-
arena rich in learning opportunities and a ing in ways that are propitious for learning
social context that is unique to this life to read and think. As memory span in-
stage. creases, for example, children find it easier
Second, the particular kinds of environ- to remember number series and so are able
mental stimulation provided in first grade to acquire the rudimentary concepts of
coincide with important internal changes in mathematics.

MARCH 1998

This content downloaded from 141.218.001.105 on August 04, 2016 16:46:23 PM


All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
FIRSTGRADE 353

This ideal coupling of internal and ex- children's behavior change a great deal over
ternal conditions for children's learning be- the primary years. Also, social development
gins to change as children approach the up- accelerates over ages 6-8 partly because
per elementary grades. For one thing, cognitive development accelerates. Six- and
although there has been surprisingly little seven-year-olds are becoming independent
effort to understand children's attitudes to- from their families: they learn how to find
ward school, their enthusiasm for school di- their way about the neighborhood, monitor
minishes as they age. Older children rate their own activities to some extent, and op-
themselves as less smart, less good, and less erate away from the family, at least during
hardworking than do younger children and the school day. Unlike parents, teachers,
are less likely to perceive themselves as able who are the "significant adult other" during
(Blumenfeld, Pintrich, & Hamilton, 1986; the school day, respond to children's social
Blumenfeld, Pintrich, Meece, & Wessels, class and ethnicity. They constantly com-
1982). Purkey (1970) likewise reported that pare children against one another, if not al-
children's self-perceptions become more ways in terms of physical coordination or
negative between the second and sixth physical attractiveness, certainly in terms of
grades, and data for children in Baltimore mental quickness, cooperativeness, and sa-
also show this. In first grade, 62% of Begin- voir faire. Children who slip easily into the
ning School Study children said they "like student role enhance their own develop-
school a lot," but by sixth grade, only 29% ment. A child who has the temperament
liked school a lot. Small wonder that the and inclination to fit in well gets better
first few years of school make such a differ- marks and gains more on standardized tests
ence. in the early grades than does a child who
The critical period idea is useful mainly has fewer of those qualities (Alexander &
because it directs attention to important is-
Entwisle, 1988).
sues that have been overlooked in contex- But "fitting in" is a two-way street. Teach-
tual studies of education. First, because chil-
ers' own social origins influence how they re-
dren's cognitive skills are increasing
act to students. Other things being equal,
rapidly, the primary grades are a special
time. Second, developmental research has higher-status teachers tend to rate poor and/
or minority-group children lower than they
focused much more on changes within the
rate other children in terms of maturity and
child than on the social environments sur-
classroom behavior. These teachers also hold
rounding the child (Bronfenbrenner & lower expectations for poor or minority-
Crouter, 1983), even though school learning
obviously does not depend wholly on the group students. Children's gains on stan-
child. Grades 1 and 2 also provide learning dardized tests and marks in first grade are
conditions unlike those children experience depressed by such teacher disaffection
earlier or later because most elementary (Alexander, Entwisle, & Thompson, 1987). In
schools, even those in depressed urban ar- short, social distance between teachers and
eas, go to great lengths to provide an emo- their first-grade students can lead students to
achieve less: it generates inequality of edu-
tionally supportive environment at this
time. In later grades, this kind of sensitivity cational outcomes. On average, a random
to individual children and their needs is less sample of African-American first graders in
evident. Baltimore in 1982 received marks about
one-fifth of a grade lower than their white
Fitting In classmates did, with children statistically
The transition into full-time schooling equated across a wide range of other char-
drastically reconfigures the child'ssocial roles acteristics including their standardized test
because social roles and expectations for scores when they began first grade and

This content downloaded from 141.218.001.105 on August 04, 2016 16:46:23 PM


All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
354 THE ELEMENTARYSCHOOLJOURNAL

their economic backgrounds (Entwisle & Entwisle & Alexander, 1988), and later on
Alexander, 1988). these children are more likely to discon-
Still another part of "fitting in" is the cu-tinue high school (Consortium of Longitu-
mulative file that schools keep for each dinal Studies, 1983; Ensminger &
child. Records of marks, school problems, Slusarcick, 1992). The early grades are
and test data are accumulated in a folder "critical" then in the ordinary sense of the
that provides a paper trail following the word because whether or not children meet
child from first grade upward. The second- challenges at that stage has serious and
grade teacher is privy to all the information long-lasting consequences as far in the fu-
collected over first grade, the third-grade ture as investigators have searched (e.g.,
teacher is privy to all the information col- Alexander et al., 1994; Ensminger &
lected in the first two grades, and so on. The Slusarcick, 1992; Entwisle & Hayduk, 1988;
setting is ripe for self-fulfilling prophecies Kerckhoff, 1993; Pedersen, Faucher, &
and for labeling. Teachers' expectations in Eaton, 1978). How well children do in the
the early grades, which are shaped in part primary grades probably matters more for
by these dossiers, can affect students' school their future success than does their school
performance as much as 4-9 years later, performance at any other time.
even taking students' later ability level into The cumulativeness of school perfor-
account (Entwisle & Hayduk, 1988). mance has never been in doubt, but the ac-
tual data to support this linkage are sur-
Cumulative Nature of Schooling
prisingly sparse. Still, by the end of third
"Fitting in" is important for children be- grade, achievement test scores are fairly sta-
ginning school. In a study of fourth graders ble and the quality of children's perfor-
the effects of children's personal qualities mance is usually a good indicator of future
on achievement in that year were small, but school performance. In the standardization
carryover effects from the effects of the sample for the California Achievement Test
same child's personal qualities on first- (CAT) battery (McGraw-Hill, 1979), for in-
grade achievement were substantial stance, grade 3 reading scores obtained in
(Alexander, Entwisle, & Dauber, 1993). A the fall correlate .79 with the grade 3 scores
good rating in terms of classroom behavior the following spring; the similar correlation
by the first-grade teacher not only led chil- for the mathematics scores is .83. These cor-
dren to do well in that first year but actually relations are close to the reliability of the
affected the gains children made in grade 4 test, so they are at a practical ceiling. In
more than did the rating of the grade 4 Husen's (1969) large cross-national study,
teacher. First-grade performance is "criti- both intelligence scores and teachers' rat-
cal" because the cumulative nature of the ings in third grade were strong predictors
curriculum makes it hard for a child to of children's long-term educational careers.
achieve at a high level in fourth grade with- Kraus (1973), who followed children in
out achieving at a high level in earlier New York City for over 20 years,
similarly
grades. found that the most significant predictor of
The early administrative placements in adult status was the score obtained on
school that reflect social structure in the third-grade reading achievement tests.
larger society also have long-term conse- Pope, Lehrer, and Stevens (1980) likewise
quences. Compared to other children, and reported a correlation of .50 between a read-
if all else is equal, males, minority group ing test in kindergarten and children's read-
members, and/or children of low economic ing achievement in grade 5; Weller, Schnit-
status more often get low marks and/or fail tjer, and Tuten (1992) reported a correlation
a grade in elementary school (Alexander, of .57 between a reading readiness test
Entwisle, & Dauber, 1994; Bianchi, 1984; given at the start of first grade and scores

MARCH 1998

This content downloaded from 141.218.001.105 on August 04, 2016 16:46:23 PM


All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
FIRSTGRADE 355

on tenth-grade reading and math tests (see press peers, and to forecast others' reactions
also Butler, Marsh, Sheppard, & Sheppard, as well as on their ability to read and do
1985). arithmetic. Feedback comes from many
sources, and much of it is evaluative. To be
As for marks, the child's first marks in first
grade strongly forecast marks throughout el- successful, they must learn to differentiate
ementary school and in some ways are an carefully among evaluators according to
even more reliable bellwether of future per- age and social status. Praise from some
formance than test scores because marks are classmates does not carry the same weight
sensitive to the child's gender, ethnicity, and as praise from others. It makes a difference
economic background. African-American whether one's "best friend" is the most or
students in Baltimore got significantly lower least popular child in the class.
reading and math marks than whites did, The compulsory nature of schooling
starting with their first mark in first grade, means that success is no longer guaranteed.
even though their beginning test scores and Adults are free to move-from one house to
home resources were not significantly differ- another, one job to another, one spouse to
ent from whites' (Entwisle & Alexander, another, or one hobby to another. A key
1988). consequence of this freedom is that adults
can improve their odds of being successful.
The Social Environment in First Grade If promotion at the present job is refused,
The life transition marked by the begin- they can move to another job; if they are
ning of children's formal schooling has no unhappy with their spouse, divorce is an
generally accepted name. However, in con- option. In fact, adults frequently respond to
sidering moral education Durkheim (1973) failure by trying another venue. Elementary
emphasized this developmental stage: school children do not have that luxury,
"One can distinguish two stages in child- however, because they are harnessed to a
hood: The first taking place almost entirely role at an institution that they must stick
within the family ... the second, in elemen- with, like it or not. Legally compelled to at-
tary school, when the child ... is initiated tend school, they usually have no choice
into a larger environment" (pp. 17-18). Per- about which school they attend, which
haps for this reason the significance of the teacher(s) they get, or what they will do
transition has attracted little attention, and while in school.
analyses of its consequences are few and far The shock of the first-grade transition is
between. Children's new social environ- hard to appreciate. One way for adults to
ments in first grade, however, challenge imagine it is to recall their own reactions to
them in many ways. being placed in a total institution, for ex-
Over the first-grade transition the basis ample, how they felt upon becoming an in-
of rewardschanges. When children leave the patient in a hospital, or starting a new job,
protective circle of the family, they are rated or going into the army. Hospital admission
according to how well they perform com- may be the best analogy. First graders, like
pared to others, whereas in the family they hospital patients, lose control over life's
were evaluated mainly in terms of how well small daily routines of eating or elimina-
they did with respect to their own record. tion. They have little control over when
As 4-year-olds they were bigger and more they can get a drink of water or use the
capable than when they were 3, for instance, bathroom. First graders, like patients, can-
and so they were always rated positively. not leave the room without getting permis-
Furthermore, success in school supposedly sion. First graders take a prescribed curric-
depends on academic performance, but ulum (course of treatment), move through
children soon discover that they are rated a preordained daily schedule (patients are
on their ability to please the teacher, to im- awakened when nursing shifts change), and

This content downloaded from 141.218.001.105 on August 04, 2016 16:46:23 PM


All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
356 THE ELEMENTARYSCHOOLJOURNAL

are evaluated (poked or questioned) ac- ter 5 years in school this difference had
cording to criteria they do not understand. grown to over 60 points (see Entwisle et al.,
First-grade children quickly learn that 1997).
they are being evaluated but are often con-
fused about what matters (Dornbusch & Facilitating the First-Grade
Scott, 1975). As Finn (1972, p. 395) noted: Transition: Research Review
"In school [the child] first discovers that not In this section of the article we review a se-
all students receive the same reactions from ries of quantitative studies that offer evi-
the teacher, the principal or from others. At dence about resources that help children ne-
this age, the reactions to him are not colored gotiate the first-grade transition. Except for
by his achievement record so much per- Head Start, most of the findings come from
haps, as by his sex, color, physical appear- the Beginning School Study (BSS), a longi-
ance, or his exhibiting proper-that is, doc- tudinal project in Baltimore that has fol-
ile-behavior." Children get marks in topics lowed several hundred randomly selected
like "language" without knowing exactly African-American and white students from
what "language" is supposed to be. They the time they started first grade in 1982 up
may be placed in the lowest reading group to the present. This archive is rich in school
merely by chance (Entwisle, Alexander, & outcome information (test scores, marks, re-
Olson, 1997), and their marks may even de- tention status, absences, etc.) and in infor-
pend on what an older sibling has done mation related to parents' and children's
(Seaver, 1973). Small wonder that on enter- characteristics. It differs from most other
ing "big school," many children are seri- studies of children's early development in
ously threatened and fear failure for the first that it contains measures of school context
time. (school integration status, socioeconomic
Also, children are necessarily evaluated status levels of school, teachers' ethnicity,
with respect to their classmates. This means etc.) and is large enough to join the contex-
that the possibility of an unfavorable com- tual and individual information about stu-
parison is outside of children's control be- dents in multivariate models. Full details on
cause it depends on how talented their the statistical analyses in BSS studies cited
classmates happen to be. Elementary here are omitted but are available in articles
schools enroll students who tend to be published elsewhere but cited in this article.
much alike, so small differences among chil-
dren are magnified. The absolute differ- Family Structure Effects:
ences among the top 15 students in an arith- Entwisle et al. (1997)
metic class may be small, but these Many studies show that children in
differences loom large in the eyes of chil- single-parent families do not do as well in
dren, teachers, and parents. school as children in two-parent families ei-
The most credible evidence supporting ther in terms of their standardized test
the seriousness of the first-grade transition scores or in terms of adapting to the student
is that relatively small differences at this role. Prominent among the reasons pro-
time in children's performance and adjust- posed to explain this linkage is the dearth
ment to school not only persist but enlarge of economic resources in single-parent
in subsequent years. Differences on stan- homes. But an additional explanation is the
dardized tests of reading (the California lack of "another pair of hands," that is, the
Achievement Test) for Baltimore children time and effort a second parent could con-
from "high" and "low" socioeconomic tribute in helping the child adjust to school
groups when they began school were about and do well there. The BSS archive contains
20 points. This difference amounts to about some information as to how these two ex-
half a standard deviation in first grade. Af- planations account for the school perfor-

MARCH 1998

This content downloaded from 141.218.001.105 on August 04, 2016 16:46:23 PM


All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
FIRSTGRADE 357

mance of children from single-parent fami- We interpret these patterns as showing that
lies. differences in economic resources are the
Entwisle et al.'s (1997) analysis centered major reason that children in two-parent
on the gains children made in reading com- families did better than children in single-
prehension over the first year of elementary parent families. But they also showed that
school as measured by the California children in single-parent families did better
Achievement Test. The BSS children were if their mothers shared a residence than if
divided into three groups according to fam- they lived alone, and this effect was not tied
ily type: (1) two-parent families, (2) one- to economic resources. In fact, the economic
parent families in which single mothers resources available to both single-parent
shared a residence with at least one other groups were much the same.
adult, and (3) single mothers who were the Why did children in mother-other fam-
only adults in the household. The notion ilies do better? The data show that as they
here is that in one-parent families where started first grade they had better workhab-
mothers are the only adult, both economic its and better readingmarksthan children in
resources and time are in relatively short mother-alone families, with test scores con-
supply, whereas in one-parent families who trolled. In addition, their beginning scores
share residences, the "extra hands" may be were a little higher, and this initial advan-
there even though economic resources are tage also helped their first-year records look
short. better. Children in mother-other families
As would be expected from national were better able than children of other sin-
studies (McLanahan & Sandefur, 1994; Zill, gle parents to make the transition into full-
1996), BSS children from two-parent fami- time schooling.
lies started school with higher scores on Looking further into the characteristics
standardized tests of reading comprehen- of the mother-other families, we found that
sion and math concepts than children in a minority (40%) consisted of mother-
single-parent families (McGraw-Hill, 1979). grandmother combinations, whereas in al-
However, by the end of the first year, chil- most all the other families the mother
dren in families where mothers shared a shared a residence with other relatives (sis-
residence (i.e., "mother-other" families) had ters, aunts, etc.). Only the children in the
gained the same amount in reading com- "grandmother" families had significantly
prehension as those in two-parent families, better work habits. Children in families
but both groups had gained significantly where the additional adult was not a grand-
more than children in mother-alone fami- mother had work habits like those of chil-
lies. These gains in children's reading test dren in mother-alone families. (Work habits
scores were evaluated with statistical con- involve paying attention, finishing assign-
trols to equate the three family groups ac- ments, etc.) We suspect, but cannot prove,
cording to children's test scores when they that children who resided with grandmoth-
began first grade, race, gender, family size, ers as well as with mothers benefited from
mother's age, kindergarten attendance, "another pair of hands" attached to a per-
school type attended, and several other son who helped monitor and supervise
variables that tapped parents' psychologi- them over the preschool years. This surmise
cal and economic resources. The test score agrees with Furstenberg's (1976) finding
advantage of the children in mother-other that, in families where single mothers
families over those in mother-alone families shared a residence with grandmothers, chil-
held up under this full set of controls, but dren had better cognitive scores in the pre-
the advantage of children in two-parent school period than did children who lived
families faded when parents' psychological with mothers only, and it also agrees with
and economic resources were controlled. Kellam, Branch, Agrawal, and Ensminger's

This content downloaded from 141.218.001.105 on August 04, 2016 16:46:23 PM


All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
358 THE ELEMENTARYSCHOOLJOURNAL

(1975) research indicating that children who first grade than their control-group coun-
lived with grandmothers adapted better to terparts (see Woodhead, 1988).
first grade than those who lived in other Doing better in first grade is critical be-
kinds of single-parent families. cause it can have substantial effects on the
likelihood of being retained. In the BSS, for
Head Start example, children who lived in mother-
Early evaluations of Head Start concen- grandmother families had better work hab-
trated on changes in children's IQ, and the its than other children did when they
findings in this regard were disappointing. started first grade--an advantage that soon
Children who had been allocated randomly faded like the Head Start children's IQ ad-
to Head Start gained about 8 IQ points more vantage-but the mother-grandmother
than control-group children when tested in children were almost never retained in first
first grade, but this advantage disappeared grade. The BSS analyses showed that the
by the end of third grade. Nevertheless, positive effects of residing in a mother-
follow-up data showed that over their en- grandmother family were also transient.
tire school careers, the Head Start children Nevertheless, over the first 5 years of
had been held back (retained) less often and school, children in the mother-grandmother
had been assigned to special education less group had a cumulative retention rate of
often than their control-group counterparts. only 27% compared to 50% for children liv-
Because the early Head Start studies were ing in other types of single-parent homes.
small (the largest involved about 100 chil- We suspect, although we cannot know for
dren), the long-term advantages for Head certain, that the long-term advantage favor-
Start children of remaining "on time" in ing Head Start children in terms of retention
school and in mainstream classes were not rates likewise came about because of the
apparent when the studies were evaluated small advantage they had in first grade.
one at a time. Still, these two consequences
of Head Start, which became apparent Kindergarten Experience: Entwisle,
when data from several studies were Alexander, Cadigan, and Pallas (1987)
pooled (Lazar & Darlington, 1982), were Although in the second half of the twen-
enormously important for children's later tieth century public kindergartens have be-
lives. Head Start children were more likely come virtually universal in the United
to graduate from high school, and after they States, not much is known about their ef-
left high school, they were more likely to be fects on children's subsequent educational
employed (Consortium of Longitudinal careers. Kindergarten could benefit children
Studies, 1983, pp. 443 ff.). for two main reasons. The first, derived
Because data are lacking from either from a cognitive perspective, is that attend-
teachers or the schools that Head Start stu- ing kindergarten could teach children cog-
dents attended, there is no way to deter- nitive skills or enhance their cognitive de-
mine exactly how the Head Start program velopment. For example, children who
helped children avoid retention or special learn letters and sounds in kindergarten
education. Still, in the BSS and every other may learn to read more easily in first grade.
study of retention known to us that cities The second perspective draws on a life-
year-by-year retention rates, these rates are course approach. It hypothesizes that, by
highest in first grade, next highest in second providing anticipatory socialization and
grade, and so on. It seems likely that the role rehearsal, kindergarten could help chil-
temporary elevation of Head Start chil- dren adjust to the student role. For first
dren's IQs and the other transient positive graders who have attended kindergarten,
effects on their marks and achievement test first grade is not a "new" experience in that
scores enabled these children to do better in they have to some extent become accus-

MARCH 1998

This content downloaded from 141.218.001.105 on August 04, 2016 16:46:23 PM


All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
FIRSTGRADE 359

tomed to being separated from home, work- attendance positively affected cognitive
ing in groups, and following instructions. outcomes early in the first grade: fall CAT
The two types of benefits-cognitive and scores in both the verbal and the math do-
socioemotional-need not be mutually ex- mains and first-quarter marks in reading
clusive, of course. and math were all higher for children who
One BSS study (Entwisle et al., 1987) fo- had attended full-day kindergarten. There
cused on how the amount of kindergarten were only borderline effects on the fourth-
experience (full-day, half-day, or no kinder- quarter mark in reading. The number of ab-
garten) affected BSS children's standard- sences for children with more kindergarten
ized achievement test scores in verbal com- experience was significantly lower, but
prehension and math concepts at the time there were no significant effects on personal
they beganfirst grade and also on children's maturity, conduct marks, or peer popular-
test gains over the first year. It examined, in ity, and for 10 other "socioemotional" vari-
addition, how the amount of kindergarten ables there was only one borderline effect
impinged children's socioemotional status that involved children's fall expectations in
in first grade, such things as their personal math.
maturity and marks in conduct. Finally, it Despite the complexity of the analyses,
examined how kindergarten experience af- they tell a straightforward story. Aside from
fected the marks that teachers gave first- the number of absences, kindergarten af-
grade children in reading and math, con- fected cognitive variables exclusively: CAT
trolling for their achievement test scores scores in the fall and spring, and first-
and the socioemotional indicators. Teach- quarter and fourth-quarter marks in read-
ers' marks could be sensitive to children's ing and math. Effects of kindergarten on fall
kindergarten background, either because CAT scores, fall marks, and end-of-year
kindergarten could teach children specific marks were positive. There were no signifi-
subject matter (cognitive effects) or because cant effects for the affective/socioemotional
it socializes children into the student role outcomes.
(socioemotional effects). Effects of kindergarten on the cognitive
The analyses used the amount of kin- outcomes were generally stronger for
dergarten as a predictor of various first- African-American than for white children.
grade outcomes, net of children's prekin- For African Americans, the amount of kin-
dergarten experience and net of children's dergarten had significant positive effects on
race, gender, initial test scores in reading fall CAT scores in reading comprehension
and math, teacher's rating of child's per- and on first marks (for boys, on the math
sonal maturity and conduct, their parents' mark; for girls, on both reading and math
characteristics, and several other control marks). There were also borderline effects
variables. The cognitive outcomes evalu- for African-American boys on the math
ated were children's CAT scores in reading CAT and the end-of-year reading mark. For
comprehension and math concepts at the whites, the amount of kindergarten had
beginning and end of the first-grade year only borderline effects on girls' fall CAT
and their marks in reading and math during scores and boys' end-of-year marks in read-
first grade. The affective or socioemotional ing. For white girls, more kindergarten was
outcomes that were evaluated were the stu- associated with fewer days absent (border-
dents' first- and fourth-quarter marks in line), and for African-American girls this ef-
conduct; personal maturity; expectations fect was fully significant.
for their own performance in reading, math, The major findings of this study are
and conduct early and late in the year; and clear when it comes to choosing between
their popularity with peers. the "cognitive" and the "socialization" per-
The amount of children's kindergarten spectives as the explanation for kindergar-

This content downloaded from 141.218.001.105 on August 04, 2016 16:46:23 PM


All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
360 THE ELEMENTARYSCHOOLJOURNAL

ten effects because it is mainly the cognitive model to examine factors that contributed
outcomes-CAT test scores in reading com- to unexpectedly large gains on standard-
prehension and math concepts and marks ized tests of reading comprehension made
in both reading and mathematics early in by some children in the BSS sample.
the year-that were affected by the amount To focus on "exceptional cases"-be
of children's kindergarten experience. The they students, classrooms, or schools
evidence is not consistent with a socializa- (Alexander, McPartland, & Cook, 1981;
tion hypothesis because the amount of kin- Klitgaard & Hall, 1977)-entails a twofold
dergarten did not affect children's expecta- challenge. The first is to identify an appro-
tions, marks in conduct, personal maturity, priate set of exceptional performers. The
or peer popularity. The evidence shows a second is to uncover the characteristics that
cognitive "spurt" early in the year for chil- distinguish the exceptional performers
dren who attended kindergarten, especially from their unexceptional counterparts. The
for African Americans. This advantage was clues identified sometimes provide useful
short-lived, however, because the total ef- insights because the process of achievement
fect of the amount of kindergarten on could differ for the average and the excep-
spring CAT scores was negligible. The tran- tional child. More important to note, how-
sient nature of kindergarten effects resem- ever, is that success is not necessarily the
bles the advantages conferred by Head Start obverse of failure. Children may suffer from
or by the child's residence in a mother- many problems-hearing or vision prob-
grandmother family. lems, difficulties posed by coming from a
The findings of this study agree with dialect-speaking home, immaturity, lack of
prior research comparing children who parental encouragement, excessive ab-
have attended kindergarten with those who sences, and so on-but ameliorating one or
have not. In these studies, achievement and more of these problems, even if it is possi-
mental maturity are prominently men- ble, will not necessarily lead to outstanding
tioned as being affected by kindergarten, achievement.
but effects are not long-lasting, and no ef- In this study, which focused exclusively
fects are reported for personality measures on why some children performed excep-
(Williams, 1973). The BSS study provides a tionally well in first grade, we used a tele-
more robust test of the cognitive status ver- scoped version of an earlier model to pre-
sus socialization explanations for effects of dict gains in scores on verbal tests. The
kindergarten than previous research does, predictor variables fell into four categories:
however, because the sample is large and the child's personal characteristics, perfor-
diverse and because many other variables mance measures of the child, parent's char-
are controlled, including the amount of chil- acteristics, and teachers' characteristics.
dren's prekindergarten experience and their Streamlining the model and grouping the
parents' education level. independent variables focuses attention on
whether there are distinctive resources that
Children Who Do Exceptionally Well in
support exceptionally high achievement by
First Grade: Pallas, Entwisle, some students during their first year of for-
Alexander, and Cadigan (1987) mal schooling.
Children's progress in the early years of A series of logistic regression analyses
school is thought to be important for their left us with an uncomplicated view of some
later achievement, but most sociological re- complex results. Exceptionalgrowth in ver-
search skirts an inclusive accounting of the bal performance across first grade was as-
social and personal processes that underlie sociated prominently with indicators of the
early achievement. The study reported in students' psychological maturity or tempera-
this article used a modified school process ment and with some characteristicsof teach-

MARCH 1998

This content downloaded from 141.218.001.105 on August 04, 2016 16:46:23 PM


All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
FIRSTGRADE 361

ers. Other noncognitive measures of stu- Also, students of teachers who reported
dents, most notably gender and race, and high levels of conflict with parents or school
parent/family background variables, such administrators were more likely to do better
as the parents' expectations or story read- than expected. Conflict may be healthy and
ing, did not shed light on why some stu- constructive if it arises from teachers' strug-
dents made large verbal gains and others gles to do well.
did not. For example, children's marks did In sum, the resources that helped children
not discriminate between students who do exceptionally well in terms of gains on
were exceptional performers and other chil- standardized tests in reading comprehension
dren's standardized reading test scores. were their own psychological assets and their
(This early BSS study did not include family teachers' feelings about work and the work-
structure-one-parent versus two-parent- place. These latter resources are "contextual,"
among the predictor variables.) that is, they are found in the classroom/
Personal maturity distinguished children school environment. Most research so far on
with good prospects for outstanding verbal the first-grade transition has investigated the
growth in the first grade from those whose individual rather than the contextual factors
gains in performance were only average. The that explain schooling.
various items in this scale-being enthusi-
astic and being able to concentrate, among Implications for Research
others-suggest ways in which personality and Practice
factors could contribute to learning, either di- 1. More purely descriptive research is
rectly or by way of the teachers' positive re- needed on the transition into kindergarten
actions to such traits. Higher ratings of per- (Ladd & Price, 1987) and into first grade
sonal maturity also distinguished children (Entwisle & Alexander, 1989, 1993;
with relatively little ability who were pro- Reynolds, 1991, 1994; Reynolds &
moted at the end of first grade from other Bezruczko, 1993), especially for ethnically
children who had comparable ability but and socioeconomically diverse samples.
were held back (Cadigan, Entwisle, The BSS data, for example, show that full-
Alexander, & Pallas, 1988). day kindergarten confers more benefits for
The "teacher variables" that were sig- African-American than for white children.
nificant (and had the largest effects) were 2. Strong efforts are needed to get all
school climate as perceived by the teacher children into preschools. About 95% of U.S.
and the teacher's work-relatedconflict. The children attend kindergarten. In terms of
climate scale was the sum of responses to center-based care before kindergarten,
four items such as "For most faculty, teach- Head Start enrolls about 40% of eligible
ing here is [very unpleasant, very pleas- 4-year-olds and 20% of 3-year-olds, but
ant]." The work-related conflict scale was even with Head Start and all other center-
the sum of six items, five of which were based programs combined, only about 60%
adapted from a scale on teachers' stress of 4-year-olds in families with incomes un-
(Pettigrew & Wolfe, 1982). For example, one der $15,000 were involved in preschools in
item was "Trying to resolve conflicts be- 1991, and children whose parents were
tween parents and the school is a difficult dropouts were least likely to be enrolled
and frustrating part of my job." (U.S. Department of Education, 1994). The
Positive school climates promoted strik- Head Start evaluations (Barnett, 1996; Con-
ingly high growth in the verbal perfor- sortium of Longitudinal Studies, 1983;
mance of these first graders. An increase of Lazar & Darlington, 1982) and the BSS data
1 standard deviation in the climate measure leave no doubt that schooling prior to first
raised the probability of a high gain in the grade confers enormous benefits. These
achievement of students by almost 10%. benefits derive from improvements in chil-

This content downloaded from 141.218.001.105 on August 04, 2016 16:46:23 PM


All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
362 THE ELEMENTARYSCHOOLJOURNAL

dren's cognitive status that ease the first- dictability from grade 1 to grades 2 or 3. A
grade transition. Children who do better in key implication of the increased variance in
first grade do better in the later grades later grades is that effects of early interven-
(Alexander et al., 1994). The lack of facilities tions may be underestimated if students are
for the schooling of disadvantaged children not followed long enough.
prior to kindergarten is a major way that 7. Staying in the same school, staying
differential tracks for students of various with same peers, and minimizing the num-
family income levels take an early hold on ber of other life transitions like household
children. In other words, inequities in early moves or marital disruptions probably also
schooling resources linked to families' so- help children make the transition into first
cioeconomic status are a major source of so- grade. More research is needed on effects of
cioeconomic status differences in children's multiple transitions.
later school careers. 8. Research is also needed on how the
3. Full-day kindergarten can help chil- number of early transitions can be reduced,
dren from disadvantaged backgrounds do even by keeping children in the same school
better in first grade, but many of the least ad- for kindergarten and first grade.
vantaged children do not attend public kin- 9. Much more research is called for on
dergartens. Parents need to be informed of how marks affect early school outcomes.
the importance of kindergarten "full day." Marks have strong effects on test score gains
4. More research is needed on the first- over first grade, even after test scores at the
grade transition because relatively small beginning of the year and many other vari-
differences at this time in children's perfor- ables are taken into account. Because marks
mance and adjustment to school not only are reinforcing, and because marks set par-
persist but enlarge. Differences on stan- ents' and children's expectations, they are
dardized tests of reading comprehension critical for the early schooling process (see
(California Achievement Test) for Begin- Entwisle et al. 1997).
ning School Study children in Baltimore 10. The large number of different marks
from high versuis low socioeconomic first graders receive and the vague defini-
groups when they began school are about tion of many kinds of early marks seem un-
20 points, for example, a difference of about warranted. The BSS first graders were
half a standard deviation. By the end of el- marked in 30 subjects, for example, and in
ementary school (beginning grade 6), how- an earlier study Entwisle and Hayduk
ever, this difference grew to about 1.5 stan- (1981) found that the definitions of some
dard deviations. first-grade subjects varied widely from one
5. Children whose families have more teacher to the next. Some teachers included
resources show more growth on standard- spelling as part of language, for example,
ized tests, but the yearly gains that children whereas others did not.
make grow smaller over the elementary Although this agenda may seem daunt-
years. Because increments in test scores are ing, the likelihood of benefits from research
much greater at the time of the first-grade on the first-grade transition is high. The pre-
transition than later, benefits of interven- school period has captured researchers'
tions designed to help offset social inequi- attention mainly because of Head Start pro-
ties affecting children in the early years are grams for 3- and 4-year-olds, but kindergar-
likely to be greater than for interventions ten programs for 5-year-olds are important
later on. too. In the case of both, researchers have al-
6. The variance across test scores inevi- most totally neglected contextual effects. The
tably gets larger over time. Therefore, the time has come to remedy this oversight and
predictability from first-grade scores to to look much more carefully at school con-
scores in grades 5 or 6 is better than the pre- texts in first grade.

MARCH 1998

This content downloaded from 141.218.001.105 on August 04, 2016 16:46:23 PM


All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
FIRSTGRADE 363

Note self-perceptions of ability in elementary


classrooms. Elementary School Journal, 82,
401-420.
Data collection for this research was sup- Brauth, S. E., Hall, W. S., & Dooling, R. J. (1991).
ported by the W. T. Grant Foundation Grant No. Plasticity of development. Cambridge, MA:
83079682 and National Institute of Child Health MIT Press.
and Development Grant No. 1 R01 16302. The Bronfenbrenner, U., & Crouter, A. C. (1983). The
analysis was supported by National Science evolution of environmental models in devel-
Foundation Grant No. SES 8510535 and National opment research. In P. Mussen (Ed.), Hand-
Institute of Child Health and Development bookof child psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 359-415).
Grants No. 1 R01 21044, 5 R01 23738, and 5 R01 New York: Wiley.
23943. We thank the children, parents, and teach- Butler, S. R., Marsh, H. W., Sheppard, M. J., &
ers who gave us such splendid cooperation in all
Sheppard, J. L. (1985). Seven-year longitudi-
phases of this research. nal study of the early prediction of reading
achievement. Journal of Educational Psychol-
ogy, 77, 349-361.
Cadigan, D., Entwisle, D. R., Alexander, K. L., &
References Pallas, A. M. (1988). First-grade retention
among low-achieving students: A search for
Alexander, K. L., & Entwisle, D. R. (1988). significant predictors. Merrill-Palmer Quar-
Achievement in the first two years of school. terly, 34, 71-88.
Patterns and processes. Monographsof the So- Consortium of Longitudinal Studies. (1983). As
in Child the twig is bent: Lasting effect of preschoolpro-
ciety for Research Development 53(2,
[Serial No. 218]). grams. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Alexander, K. L., Entwisle, D. &
R., Dauber, S. L. Dornbusch, S. M., & Scott, W. R. (1975). Evalua-
(1993). First grade classroom behavior: Its
tion and the exerciseof authority.San Francisco:
short- and long-term consequences for Jossey-Bass.
school performance. Child Development, 64, Durkheim, E. (1973). On morality and society: Se-
801-814. lected writings. Chicago: University of Chi-
Alexander, K. L., Entwisle, D. R., & Dauber, S. L. cago Press.
(1994). On the success of failure:A reassessment Ensminger, M. E., & Slusarcick, A. L. (1992).
the retention in the Paths to high school graduation or dropout:
of effects of primarygrades.
A longitudinal study of a first-grade cohort.
Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University
Press. Sociology of Education,65, 95-113.
Alexander, K. L., Entwisle, D. R., & Thompson, Entwisle, D. R., & Alexander, K. L. (1988). Fac-
M. S. (1987). School performance, status re- tors affecting achievement test scores and
lations and the structure of sentiment: Bring- marks received by black and white first grad-
ing the teacher back in. American Sociological ers. ElementarySchoolJournal,88, 449-471.
Review, 52, 665-682. Entwisle, D. R., & Alexander, K. L. (1989). Early
Alexander, K. L., McPartland, J., & Cook, M. schooling as a "critical period" phenomenon.
(1981). Using standardized test performance In K. Namboodiri & R. Corwin (Eds.), Soci-
in school effects research. In R. G. Corwin ology of educationand socialization(pp. 27-55).
(Ed.), Researchin sociology of educationand so- Greenwich, CT: JAI.
cialization (Vol. 2, pp. 1-33). Greenwich, CT: Entwisle, D. R., & Alexander, K. L. (1993). Entry
JAI. into schools: The beginning school transition
Barnett, W. S. (1996). Long-term effects of early and educational stratification in the United
childhood care and education on disadvan- States. In Annual Review of Sociology (Vol. 19,
taged children's cognitive development and pp. 401-423). Palo Alto, CA: Annual Re-
school success. Future of Children,5, 25-50. views, Inc.
Bianchi, S. M. (1984). Children's progress Entwisle, D. R., Alexander, K. L., Cadigan, D., &
through school: A research note. Sociologyof Pallas, A. M. (1987). Kindergarten experi-
Education,57, 184-192. ence: Cognitive effects or socialization?
Blumenfeld, P.C., Pintrich, P.R., & Hamilton, American Educational Research Journal, 24,
V. L. (1986). Children's concepts of ability, ef- 337-364.
fort and conduct. American Educational Re- Entwisle, D. R., Alexander, K. L., & Olson, L. S.
searchJournal,23, 95-104. (1997). Children, schools and inequality: Boul-
Blumenfeld, P. C., Pintrich, P. R., Meece, J., & der, CO: Westview.
Wessels, K. (1982). The formation and role of Entwisle, D. R., & Hayduk, L.A. (1981). Aca-

This content downloaded from 141.218.001.105 on August 04, 2016 16:46:23 PM


All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
364 THE ELEMENTARYSCHOOLJOURNAL

demic expectations and the school attain- first-grade teachers on children's subsequent
ment of young children. Sociology of Educa- adult status. Harvard EducationalReview, 48,
tion, 54, 34-50. 1-31.
Entwisle, D. R., & Hayduk, L. A. (1988). Lasting Pettigrew, L. S., & Wolfe, G. E. (1982). Validating
effects of elementary school. Sociologyof Edu- measures of teacher stress. American Educa-
cation, 61, 147-159. tional ResearchJournal,19, 373-396.
Finn, J. D. (1972). Expectations and the educa- Pope, J., Lehrer, B., & Stevens, J. (1980). A mul-
tional environment. Review of EducationalRe- tiphasic reading screening procedure. Journal
search,42, 387-409. of LearningDisabilities, 13, 98-102.
Furstenberg, J. F. (1976). Unplanned parenthood: Purkey, W. W. (1970). Self-concept and school
The social consequencesof teenage childbearing. achievement. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-
New York: Free Press. Hall.
Husen, T. (1969). Talent, opportunity and career. Reynolds, A. J. (1991). Early schooling of chil-
Stockholm: Almqvist & Wiksell. dren at risk. American Educational Research
Kellam, S. G., Branch, J., Agrawal, K., & Journal,28, 393-422.
Ensminger, M. E. (1975). Mental healthand go- Reynolds, A. J. (1994). Effects of a preschool plus
ing to school: The Woodlawnprogramof assess- follow-on intervention for children at risk.
ment, early intervention, and evaluation. Chi- DevelopmentalPsychology, 30, 787-804.
cago: University of Chicago Press. Reynolds, A. J., & Bezruczko, N. (1993). School
Kerckhoff, A. C. (1993). Diverging pathways:Social adjustment of children at risk through fourth
structure and career deflections. New York: grade. Merrill-PalmerQuarterly,39, 457-480.
Cambridge Press. Seaver, W. B. (1973). Effects of naturally-induced
Klitgaard, R. E., & Hall, G. R. (1977). A statistical teacher expectancies. Journal of Personality
search for unusually effective schools. In W. and Social Psychology,28, 333-342.
Fairley & F. Mosteller (Eds.), Statistics and U.S. Department of Education. (1994). Access to
public policy. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. early childhood programs for children at risk
Kraus, P. E. (1973). Yesterday'schildren.New York: (NCES 93-372). Washington, DC: National
Wiley. Center for Education Statistics.
Ladd, G. W., & Price, J. M. (1987). Predicting chil- Varnhagen, C. K., Morrison, F. J., & Everall, R.
dren's social and school adjustment follow- (1994). Age and schooling effects in story re-
ing the transition from preschool to kinder- call and story production. DevelopmentalPsy-
garten. Child Development,58, 1168-1189. chology, 30, 969-979.
Lazar, I., & Darlington, R. (1982). Lasting effects Weller, L. D., Schnittjer, C. J., & Tuten, B. A.
of early education. A report from the Con- (1992). Predicting achievement in grades
sortium for Longitudinal Studies. Mono- three through ten using the Metropolitan
graphs of the Society for Researchin Child De- Readiness Test. Journal of Research in Child-
velopment,47(2-3). hood Education,6, 121-129.
McGraw-Hill. (1979). CaliforniaAchievementTest: Williams, R. R. (1973). The influence of kinder-
Technicalbulletin 1, Forms C and D, levels 10- garten experience on growth and develop-
19. Monterey, CA: McGraw-Hill. ment of pupils who have had kindergarten
McLanahan, S. S., & Sandefur, G. (1994). Growing compared with pupils who have not had kin-
up with a single parent: Whathurts, what helps. dergarten experience (Doctoral dissertation,
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. University of Georgia, 1973). DissertationAb-
Newport, E. L. (1991). Contrasting conceptions stracts International,34, 4976A.
of the critical period for language. In S. Carey Woodhead, M. (1988). When psychology informs
& R. Gelman (Eds.), Theepigenesisof the mind: public policy. The case of early childhood in-
Essays on biology and cognition (pp. 111-130). tervention. American Psychologist, 43, 443-
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 454.
Pallas, A. M., Entwisle, D. R., Alexander, K. L., & Zill, N. (1996). Family change and student
Cadigan, D. (1987). Children who do excep- achievement: What we have learned, what it
tionally well in first grade. Sociology of Edu- means for schools. In A. Booth & J. Dunn
cation, 60, 257-271. (Eds.), Family-school links: How do they affect
Pedersen, E., Faucher, T. A., & Eaton, W. W. educationaloutcomes?(pp. 139-174). Mahwah,
(1978). A new perspective on the effects of NJ: Erlbaum.

MARCH 1998

This content downloaded from 141.218.001.105 on August 04, 2016 16:46:23 PM


All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).

You might also like