Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/322740095

Cross-sectional study assessing the addition of contrast sensitivity to visual


acuity when testing for fitness to drive

Article in BMJ Open · January 2018


DOI: 10.1136/bmjopen-2017-018546

CITATIONS READS

12 191

4 authors, including:

Bernard Favrat Francois Xavier Borruat


University of Lausanne University of Lausanne
236 PUBLICATIONS 3,739 CITATIONS 198 PUBLICATIONS 2,765 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Paul Vaucher
University of Applied Sciences and Arts Western Switzerland
126 PUBLICATIONS 1,907 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

OCAB - Osteopathic Care and Applied Biomechanics View project

Iron deficiency and blood donation (FerDon) View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Paul Vaucher on 29 January 2018.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Downloaded from http://bmjopen.bmj.com/ on January 27, 2018 - Published by group.bmj.com

Open Access Research

Cross-sectional study assessing the


addition of contrast sensitivity to visual
acuity when testing for fitness to drive
Lucie Spreng,1 Bernard Favrat,2,3 François-Xavier Borruat,4 Paul Vaucher3,5

To cite: Spreng L, Favrat B, ABSTRACT


Borruat F-X, et al. Cross- Strengths and limitations of this study
Objectives The aim of this study is to quantify the
sectional study assessing the importance of loss of contrast sensitivity (CS) and its
addition of contrast sensitivity ► Participants of 70 years of age or older were drivers
relationship to loss of visual acuity (VA), driving restrictions
to visual acuity when testing from the general population. We did not concentrate
and daytime, on-road driving evaluations in drivers aged
for fitness to drive. BMJ Open on those with known health issues or those that had
2018;8:e018546. doi:10.1136/ 70+.
known driving history.
bmjopen-2017-018546 Design A predictive cross-sectional study.
► Drivers were free to speak openly about their health
Setting Volunteer participants to a drivers’ refresher
► Prepublication history for and driving experiences, given data were collected
course for adults aged 70+ delivered by the Swiss
this paper is available online. anonymously and there was no threat for their
Automobile Club in western Switzerland from 2011 to
To view these files, please visit licence.
2013.
the journal online (http://dx.doi. ► Observer bias cannot be totally excluded, given
org/10.1136/bmjopen-2017- Participants 162 drivers, male and female, aged 70 years
information on visual performance and driving
018546). or older.
restrictions was collected by the same researcher.
Clinical predictors We used a vision screener to estimate
► The on-road driving tests were done during the day;
Received 6 July 2017 VA and the The Mars Letter Contrast Sensitivity Test to test
Revised 21 November 2017
we cannot therefore exclude that performances
CS.
Accepted 30 November 2017 would be different with lower luminance.
Outcomes We asked drivers to report whether they
found five driving restrictions useful for their condition;
restrict driving to known roads, avoid driving on highways,
avoid driving in the dark, avoid driving in dense traffic 2015, 1797 drivers of 70 years of age or more
and avoid driving in fog. All participants also underwent a were involved in a road accident with severe
standardised on-road evaluation carried out by a driving
injury or death (0.02% of the population).2
instructor.
Results Moderate to severe loss of CS for at least one eye
This population has been shown to be more
was frequent (21.0% (95% CI 15.0% to 28.1%)) and often at risk. Accidents involving aged drivers are
1 isolated from a loss of VA (11/162 cases had a VA ≥0.8 much more likely to result in hospital admis-
Doctoral School of Medicine,
Faculty of Medicine and decimal and a CS of ≤1.5 log(CS); 6.8% (95% CI 3.4% to sions or fatalities due to such drivers’ own
Biology, University of Lausanne, 11.8%)). Drivers were more likely (R2=0.116, P=0.004) health vulnerabilities.3–6 In other words,
Lausanne, Switzerland to report a belief that self-imposed driving restrictions older drivers and health issues is more a
2
Department of Ambulatory would be useful if they had reduced CS in at least one eye. public health concern for their own secu-
Care and Community Medicine, Daytime evaluation of driving performance seems limited rity rather than for others’.7 8 Being able to
University of Lausanne, in its ability to correctly identify difficulties related to CS
Lausanne, Switzerland
drive a vehicle is, however, very important
loss (VA: R2=0.004, P=0.454; CS: R2=0.006, P=0.332).
3
Unit of Psychology and Traffic for the autonomy, the self-esteem and the
Conclusion CS loss is common for older drivers.
Medicine, University Centre quality of life of the elderly.1 It is linked to
Screening CS and referring for cataract surgery even
of Legal Medicine Lausanne-
in the absence of VA loss could help maintain mobility. a lower mortality risk.9 Without this ability,
Geneva, University Hospital of
Reduced CS and moderate reduction of VA were both poor the resulting loss of independence induces
Lausanne (CHUV), Lausanne,
Switzerland predictors of daytime on-road driving performances in this a higher risk of isolation, depression and
4
Ophthalmology Department, research study. associated functional impairment.9–11
Jules-Gonin Eye Hospital, In Switzerland, non-professional drivers
University of Lausanne, who are 70 years of age or older have to
Lausanne, Switzerland
5 INTRODUCTION undergo a compulsory medical assessment
aR&D, School of Health
Sciences Fribourg, University Within an ageing population, the ques- of their fitness to drive every 2 years.12 The
of Applied Sciences and Arts tion of whether elderly people should be law specifies that the physician is responsible
Western Switzerland (HES-SO), managed differently with regard to their of verifying the absence of psychological,
Fribourg, Switzerland driving licence is becoming more and more neurological, musculoskeletal, cardiovas-
Correspondence to relevant. The literature predicts that by cular, respiratory or metabolic (diabetes)
Dr Paul Vaucher; the year 2020, 20% of licensed drivers will disorders that could affect driving. Like
paul.vaucher@hes-so.ch be older than 65 years.1 In Switzerland, in in many other countries, the legislation

Spreng L, et al. BMJ Open 2018;8:e018546. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2017-018546 1


Downloaded from http://bmjopen.bmj.com/ on January 27, 2018 - Published by group.bmj.com

Open Access

also specifies that drivers’ vision and hearing must be METHODS


checked.4 12 Design
For the elderly, health decline and visual impairments We designed an exploratory, cross-sectional, prag-
are the most common causes that lead to driving cessa- matic study combining the clinical and on-road evalu-
tion.13–16 Older drivers have difficulties adapting their ation of older drivers attending a refresher course in
vision to low light conditions, recognising on-road Switzerland.
and roadside objects in low light conditions and recov-
ering from glare.17 Loss of visual acuity (VA) has been Population
shown to be associated with the avoidance of driving From 2011 to 2013, all residents aged 70 years or
long distances or in unfamiliar areas. Loss of contrast more (n=16 858) of four regions of Vaud, Switzerland
sensitivity (CS) is, on the other hand, associated with received an invitation to participate in a refresher
the avoidance of driving at night or in conditions course in driving competencies, provided by the Swiss
of reduced luminosity.18 At age 75+, 39.8% of female Automobile Club. Of these, 1004 participated (5.1%
drivers and 28.8% of male drivers report restricting of all concerned senior drivers) and were invited to
their driving due to problems with their eyesight.19 voluntarily and anonymously be screened for medical
Drivers who think their vision has worsened will restrict conditions within the Guarding Aged drivers against
their driving most, even if their binocular vision is not accidents project (GarAge). There was a 44.9% accep-
affected.19 Those that have a binocular CS loss <1.4 tance rate. Measurements of CS were added in October
log(CS) are 2.5 (95% CI 1.2 to 3.0) times more likely to 2012. To be included in this study, participants had to
cease driving altogether.20 hold a valid Swiss driving licence, be community resi-
High contrast VA is the most commonly used dents of the Canton of Vaud in Switzerland, be aged
measure of vision in assessing fitness to drive, but there 70 years or older, and have CS evaluated separately for
are other measures of vision such as CS that are more each eye.
discriminating. The true value of asking aged drivers
Visual acuity
to sacrifice their mobility because they have reduced
Monocular and binocular distance VA were measured
vision is unknown, and there is no existing evidence
using a vision screener called Visiotest (manufacturer
that supports the belief that screening VA under
Essilor). This device has been shown to have a sensi-
photopic conditions improves road safety.10 13 21–23
tivity of 100% and a specificity of 88.1%27 to detect
A population-based survey has shown that 2.6% of
vision deficiency compared with an examination of
those currently driving have a VA lower than the legal
vision carried out by an optometrist. Luminosity was
limit (<0.5 decimal) without them being more at risk
set to 320 cd/m2 with optotype contrast close to 100%.
of having accidents.24 It is commonly admitted that
All tests were carried out with both eyes open. We first
screening for VA alone is inappropriate for assessing
tested the VA of the left eye, then the right and finally
full visual function;10 it is also, at best, a very weak
both eyes simultaneously. All tests were carried out with
means of predicting road accidents,6 24 and is also a
optical corrections for vision at a distance—that is, to
bad predictor of night driving ability.14 For some ocular
say, patients’ glasses.
pathologies, cataracts, for example, CS loss might be
more influential than VA as a contributory cause of Contrast sensitivity
accidents.6 24 Furthermore, drivers with a monocular The Mars Letter Contrast Sensitivity Test was used to eval-
or binocular reduction of CS are more likely to be uate CS.28–30 It consists of three cardboard charts with
involved in a collision that is their fault than are drivers 48 letters each. From one letter to the next, there was a
with reduced VA.25 For the moment, European guide- decrease in contrast of a constant factor of 0.04 log units.
lines and legislations provide norms and indications The chart is held 50 cm away from the eyes. The MARS
for VA, ≥0.5 decimal; visual field, ≥120° ; no diplopia; chart has the advantage over the Pelli-Robson chart of
no important deterioration of twilight vision and no being smaller, more portable and more easily uniformly
major increase in glare sensitivity. But little is known illuminated.28 This test is at least as reliable or more so
regarding the usefulness of also screening for loss of than the Pelli-Robson test.28–30 In our study, we decided
CS, which could provide more insight into the patient’s to set the threshold for detecting moderate to severe
ability to drive at night. This would make it possible to contrast loss for CS at <1.5 log(CS).31 32
restrict driving to daylight conditions rather than have
people cease driving—measures that have been shown Ancillary tests
to be effective in reducing risks.26 In addition to VA and CS, we also evaluated horizontal
The aim of this study is to: (1) estimate the preva- visual fields; visual processing, using the useful field of
lence of drivers aged 70+ who are concerned by CS loss, view (UFOV);33 overall cognitive function, using the
(2) evaluate the strength of the association between Montreal Cognitive Assessment;34 executive function and
loss of VA and CS and (3) estimate the added value of visuospatial search, using the Trail Making Task (parts A
measuring CS over VA when trying to evaluate self-regu- and B)35 and motor function, using the Timed Up-and-Go
lations of driving or on-road driving performance. Test.36

2 Spreng L, et al. BMJ Open 2018;8:e018546. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2017-018546


Downloaded from http://bmjopen.bmj.com/ on January 27, 2018 - Published by group.bmj.com

Open Access

Driving history and self-imposed driving restrictions For comparability and to assure linearity for regression
Participants were questioned on any accidents they may analysis, both VA and CS were used on their log scale (ie,
have had in the past and their average weekly distance log(minimum angle of resolution) and log(CS)).18 Asso-
driven. To investigate relevant driving restriction strate- ciations between VA and CS were analysed for all tested
gies, we reviewed the literature and identified 13 ques- eyes adjusting for a lack of independence of values coming
tions related to tactical or strategic compensations that from the same individuals, using mixed-effect linear
were reported by older drivers. These were reformulated regression. To test associations with self-imposed driving
so that drivers could report their perceived usefulness to restrictions and the on-road evaluation, we used the value
their own, respective, situations, using a five-step Likert observed for both eyes, for the worst eye or the absolute
scale (ie, essential, very useful, useful, slightly useful and difference of values between both eyes as independent
useless). We then used data from 445 drivers to derive variables. We then used linear regression analysis to test
and test the psychometric values of questions related to associations with and without adjustment for possible
driving cessation. Item reduction was carried out using confounders (ie, cognitive deficits, motor deficits, age
polychoric, correlation-iterated principal factor anal- and gender). To verify the assumption of linearity, we used
ysis with orthogonal rotation. Items with a uniqueness likelihood ratio tests to compare models using dependent
value of 0.6 or more were excluded and the analysis was variables as a continuous variable instead of dichotomised
run over again. Principal component analysis revealed ordinal variables. We compared the added value of using
a single factor with an eigenvalue of 2.62; the second CS to VA alone using a likelihood ratio test between both
factor had an eigenvalue of 0.19. The five items for models. P<0.05 meant that CS improved the prediction of
this factor were: restrict driving to known roads, avoid driving restrictions or on-road performances.
driving on highways, avoid driving in the dark, avoid Given most outcome measures were expected to be
driving in dense traffic and avoid driving in fog. The highly correlated, no adjustments were made for multiple
internal coherence of these items was acceptable (Cron- testing. Only subjects with full data were included in the
bach’s alpha=0.680). The driving restriction score was analysis. All statistics were planned prior to analysis and
obtained by adding up the values of each individual were run using STATA V.14.0 (Stata Statistical Software:
question about the usefulness of these specific driving Release V.14.0, StataCorp).
restriction strategies. The score therefore ranges from
0 to 20 with high scores corresponding to increased Ethical standards
self-restriction. All participants provided their written, informed consent
to participate. It was performed in accordance with the
On-road driving evaluation ethical standards of the 2008 amended Declaration of
The procedure has been previously described in two Helsinki (Seoul).
publications.35 37 Twelve driving instructors, blinded to
the clinical assessment, were engaged in the study. Each
RESULTS
participant drove once, with one of the instructors, on
Population description
a 45 min route on the open road, including urban and
The average acceptance rate over all four regions to
rural sections, secondary and principal roads and high-
attend the refreshing course was of 5.1%. Between
ways, simple and complex intersections, ‘roundabouts’
(circular intersections with changing on-road priorities),
traffic signals and complex lane selections. Instructors
reported their ‘gestalt’ evaluation of driving perfor-
mance as ‘good’ or ‘sufficient’ for the following criteria:
respecting road regulations, handling the vehicle, speed
adaptation, correct positioning on the road, comfort,
behaviour towards other road users, observation and
anticipation. Driving competencies were summarised as
excellent (no lapse), good (lapses reported for one or
two items), moderate (lapses reported for three to five
items) or poor (lapses reported for six to eight items).
This scoring method identified a unique dimension
(eigenvalue=5.1) and had a good fit to an overall trait
(R1c=12.2, df=14, P=0.565).

Statistical methods
Significance level was set at α=0.05 and all estimates are
provided with 95% CIs. Sample size was estimated with a
power of 80% to detect a determinant of correlation (R2) Figure 1 Participant flow chart. CS, contrast sensitivity;
of 0.25. This required a minimum of 124 participants. GarAge, Guarding Aged drivers against accidents project .

Spreng L, et al. BMJ Open 2018;8:e018546. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2017-018546 3


Downloaded from http://bmjopen.bmj.com/ on January 27, 2018 - Published by group.bmj.com

Open Access

Table 1 Description of the population


All participants Both eyes with log(CS) ≥1.5 Worst eye with log(CS) <1.5
(n=162) (n=128, 79%) (n=34, 21%)
Median Median Median
Mean (SD) (p5–p95) Mean (SD) (p5–p95) Mean (SD) (p5–p95) P value*
Age 76.1 (4.73) 75.2 (70.8–84.9) 75.9 (4.64) 74.9 (70.8–84.9) 77.0 (5.0) 75.6 (70.6–88.7) 0.195
Functional mobility
TUG test (s)† 9.49 (2.65) 9.0 (6.8–15.1) 9.16 (2.32) 8.8 (6.3–13.1) 10.7 (3.38) 10.1 (6.1–16.3) 0.002
Cognitive state
MoCa (points)‡ 26.5 (2.63) 27 (21–30) 26.5 (2.53) 27 (21–30) 26.24 (2.99) 27 (19–29) 0.561
Driving
Distance (km)/ 238 (224) 200 (60–500) 234 (239) 195 (60–500) 253 (159) 225 (40–550) 0.668
week
Restriction score 2.3 (2.78) 1 (0–8) 1.88 (2.46) 1 (0–6) 3.68 (3.45) 3 (0–12) <0.001
Vision
VA bilateral§ 1.02 (0.20) 1.0 (0.6–1.2) 1.06 (0.17) 1.2 (0.8–1.2) 0.88 (0.24) 1.0 (0.4–1.2) <0.001
VA best eye§ 0.96 (0.22) 1.0 (0.6–1.2) 0.99 (0.21) 1.0 (0.6–1.2) 0.85 (2.05) 0.8 (0.6–1.2) <0.001
VA worst eye§ 0.72 (0.31) 0.8 (0.2–1.2) 0.79 (0.27) 0.8 (0.4–1.2) 0.46 (0.31) 0.4 (0.0–1.0) <0.001
Visual field¶ 183 (16) 180 (160–200) 185 (14) 180 (160–200) 179 (23) 180 (140–200) 0.059
CS bilateral** 1.72 (0.06) 1.72 (1.6–1.76) 1.73 (0.04) 1.74 (1.68–1.76) 1.66 (0.09) 1.68 (1.48–1.76) <0.001
CS best eye** 1.69 (0.06) 1.72 (1.56–1.76) 1.73 (0.04) 1.74 (1.68–1.76) 1.66 (0.09) 1.68 (1.48–1.76) <0.001
CS worst eye** 1.57 (0.25) 1.64 (1.24–1.72) 1.66 (0.06) 1.68 (1.52–1.72) 1.25 (0.38) 1.44 (0.04–1.48) <0.001
*Student’s t-test comparing groups with or without contrast sensitivity loss.
†If ≥13.5 s: functional mobility difficulties. One person could not do the test and was not assessed.
‡If <26 points: cognitive difficulties.
§In decimal.
¶Visual field in degrees.
**Expressed in log(CS): considered as normal if ≥1.5 log(CS) and restricted if <1.5 log(CS).
CS, contrast sensitivity; MoCa, Montreal Cognitive Assessment; p5, fifth percentile; p95, 95th percentile; TUG, Timed Up-and-Go Test; VA,
visual acuity.

October 2012 and September 2013, 174 older drivers Prevalence of moderate or severe CS loss
volunteered to participate in the GarAge study; complete Twenty-one per cent (95% CI 15.0% to 28.1%) of drivers
data proved available for 162 of them (figure 1). Visual had a moderate or severe CS loss for at least one eye. Twelve
tests were partially available for seven of the nine drivers cases having a severe loss (7.4%; 95% CI 3.9% to 12.6%).
who were excluded. Five had a VA of 0.8 decimal or more We also observed two cases (1.2%; 95% CI 0.1% to 4.4%)
for each of their eyes, one had 0.6 decimal for both eyes of CS loss in both eyes. The two cases we observed with
and one had 0.6 decimal for one eye and 1.0 decimal for binocular CS loss were a 82-year-old male driver who had
the other. The majority of participants were men (66.7%, severe binocular CS loss (log(CS)=1.28) without having
n=108). One-third (35.4%) had had an incomplete educa- lost his VA (0.8 decimals), and a 78-year-old female driver
tion history (<12 years of schooling). Forty-three of the with a moderate CS loss of 1.48 log(CS) but a VA of 1.0
drivers (26.5%) had been involved in an accident during decimals. Both drivers had no cognitive deficits and were
the previous 2 years; eight drivers had had to file a claim given an excellent daytime on-road driving evaluation and
on their vehicle insurance and only one accident caused both reported only slightly limiting their night driving.
injury. Driving instructors considered 46.4% of drivers We also observed that drivers with low CS tended to also
to have excellent driving competencies, 30.0% to have have slower functional mobility, identified more self-im-
good competencies, 16.3% to have moderate competen- posed driving restrictions as being useful and had lower
cies and 7.3% to have poor competencies. Having good VA (table 1).
or moderate competencies did not prevent drivers from
considering self-imposed driving restrictions useful; only Associations between VA and CS
35% (56 persons) reported all five restrictions as useless When screening for vision, measuring VA alone had us
to them. An important number of participants had a take into account 39.7% of the variance we observed
health condition that could affect driving performances when measuring CS (P<0.001). One-third of the 39 tested
(n=85, 52.5%). Further details on participating drivers eyes that had a log(CS)<1.5 had a VA of 0.8 decimal or
are provided in table 1. better. Adding CS would have made it possible to detect

4 Spreng L, et al. BMJ Open 2018;8:e018546. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2017-018546


Downloaded from http://bmjopen.bmj.com/ on January 27, 2018 - Published by group.bmj.com

Open Access

Association and added value of CS for predicting daytime on-


road driving performance
Daytime driving performance was unrelated to CS or VA
loss (figure 2; table 3).

DISCUSSION
In our study, one driver aged 70+ out of five (21%) had
a loss of CS (<1.5 log(CS)) in at least one eye. Further-
more, our results suggest that 6.8% of all drivers aged
70+ start experiencing difficulties in low illumination
without their VA being affected in normal light condi-
tions (VA ≥0.5 decimal). Our study confirmed the clear
association between CS loss and the perceived utility of
driving restrictions even if the added value of using CS
instead of VA was not significant. The feeling of impair-
ment was more related to only one eye being affected or
to having an important difference between the two eyes
than to binocular impairment. Our results also confirmed
that loss of CS had no association to daytime on-road
driving performance. Our results therefore reveal two
important facts about vision loss and driving. First, many
people consider restrictions as being useful when their
loss only concerns one eye and is not believed to affect
their driving performance. Second, standard on-road
driving evaluations have important limitations, given that
they cannot reveal the difficulties experienced in more
difficult situations with less light as reported by drivers.
This suggests that standard on-road tests should not be
considered as a gold standard for assessing fitness to drive
Figure 2 Driving performances between drivers with normal
contrast sensitivity (CS) and those with low CS. (A) On-road unless these tests are adapted to the conditions in which
driving score ranges from 0 to 8 with higher scores indicating difficulties might arise.
more errors. (B) Driving restriction score ranges from 0 to 20 One of the strengths of this study is that it studies drivers
with higher scores indicating higher levels of restriction. that had not been selected for medical reasons or due to
the fact that they had experienced driving difficulties. This
provides better indication on the usefulness of medical
unsuspected vision deficits in 11 drivers (6.8%; 95% CI screening procedures targeting all drivers. Considering
3.4% to 11.8%). the worst eye also improved our understanding of what
causes people to consider driving restrictions. The limita-
Association and added value of CS for predicting the tions of this study are the limited number of participants
perceived utility of self-imposed driving restrictions with severe vision impairment and the limited precision
Drivers were more likely (R2=0.116, P=0.004) to report of our vision screening tests. Given very few drivers in our
finding driving restrictions potentially useful if they had at sample had important CS loss in both eyes, we cannot
least one eye that had reduced CS (figure 2; table 2). This exclude that such conditions would also affect daytime
on-road evaluations. Furthermore, some participants
was particularly the case for avoiding driving in the dark
certainly wore glasses that did not optimally correct their
(R2=0.056, P=0.022) and in fog (R2=0.083, P=0.001). The
refraction abnormalities. The Mars test is performed at
only significant association for VA, on the other hand, was
reading distance (50 cm), whereas VA is tested at distance.
between binocular loss and avoiding driving in the dark
Therefore, some discrepancies between the results from
(R2=0.065, P=0.010). The observed added value of using each test could arise from refraction problems instead of
CS instead of VA was, however, not significant (P=0.345). indicating a true lack of association between VA and CS.
We also observed that drivers who recognised the useful- Finally, we did not collect information about past cata-
ness of any form of driving restriction were 3.1 times ract operations or present diagnosis of cataract, making it
more likely to have moderate CS loss for at least one eye difficult to document patterns in access to surgery.
(CS ≤1.5 log(CS)). The association between loss of CS and When comparing our results to the existing literature,
recognising the usefulness of driving restrictions remained other studies have found that driving with a loss of CS
significant even after adjustment for cognitive decline, or a loss of VA leads to drivers tending to report diffi-
motor function, age and gender. culties experienced while driving.15 18 24 38 39 Difficulties

Spreng L, et al. BMJ Open 2018;8:e018546. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2017-018546 5


Downloaded from http://bmjopen.bmj.com/ on January 27, 2018 - Published by group.bmj.com

Open Access

Table 2 Advantages of CS over VA for determining driving restriction


CS VA LR test between models*
2 2
Adjusted R † P value‡ Adjusted R † P value‡ P value
Both eyes
Driving when dark 0.045 0.065 0.065 0.010 0.750
Driving on the highway 0.095 0.609 0.097 0.438 0.999
Driving under foggy conditions 0.012 0.834 0.013 0.631 0.999
Driving on unknown roads 0.057 0.565 0.057 0.667 0.999
Driving in dense traffic 0.090 0.897 0.096 0.287 0.979
Driving restriction score 0.069 0.653 0.080 0.151 0.857
Worst eye
Driving when dark 0.056 0.022 0.037 0.147 0.778
Driving on the highway 0.096 0.528 0.097 0.447 0.999
Driving under foggy conditions 0.083 0.001 0.018 0.312 0.090
Driving on unknown roads 0.066 0.185 0.061 0.354 0.988
Driving in dense traffic 0.103 0.122 0.094 0.390 0.943
Driving restriction score 0.116 0.004 0.078 0.204 0.345
Difference between the eyes
Driving when dark 0.050 0.032 0.044 0.448 0.985
Driving on the highway 0.091 0.738 0.096 0.728 0.992
Driving under foggy conditions 0.080 0.001 0.012 0.339 0.077
Driving on unknown roads 0.066 0.104 0.056 0.303 0.945
Driving in dense traffic 0.099 0.116 0.089 0.336 0.942
Driving restriction score 0.113 0.004 0.072 0.271 0.292
*Best model over worst. P values under 0.05 would mean the test with the highest R2 is significantly better than the other visual test.
†Adjusted R2 for entire model. R2 corresponds to the test’s contribution in explaining restrictions and ranges from 0 to 1.
‡Significance of contribution of visual component.
CS, contrast sensitivity; LR, likelihood ratio; VA, visual acuity.

seem to arise especially in low illumination14 and increase decline, often deny experiencing or simply do not expe-
the probability of driving restrictions being required. rience any vision problems and therefore do not want
Restricting driving and avoiding driving at night, dawn or do not feel the need to have their driving practices
or dusk seem to be appropriate measures for reducing restricted.14 40 41 This is one of the situations in which a
risks.5 Men, more than women and people with cognitive deeper visual screening is needed. However, restricted

Table 3 Advantages of CS over VA for determining on-road driving score (0–3)*


LR test between
CS VA models†
R2‡ P value§ R2‡ P value§ P value
Both eyes
On-road driving score (0–3) 0.0001 0.895 0.00 0.946 0.9935
Worst eye
On-road driving score (0–3) 0.0062 0.332 0.0037 0.454 0.824
Difference between the eyes
On-road driving score (0–3) 0.059 0.411 0.057 0.568 0.999
*Excellent=0, Good=1, Moderate=2, Poor=3.
†P values under 0.05 mean that CS improved the prediction of on-road driving scores compared with VA alone.
‡Crude R2 were used instead of adjusted R2 given values were close to 0 and adjustment methods become inadequate. R2 corresponds to
the test’s contribution in explaining on-road driving scores and ranges from 0 to 1.
§Significance of contribution of visual component.
CS, contrast sensitivity; LR, likelihood ratio; VA, visual acuity.

6 Spreng L, et al. BMJ Open 2018;8:e018546. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2017-018546


Downloaded from http://bmjopen.bmj.com/ on January 27, 2018 - Published by group.bmj.com

Open Access

driving has also been shown to reduce risks to the condition affects their driving. This remains, however,
minimum for those experiencing cognitive decline.42 We controversial given vision status was poorly associated to
conclude that recommendations to reduce driving in the driving performances for both VA and CS in this research
dark require at least the measurement of CS or the testing study. Further research is needed to evaluate the impact
of VA in low illumination.14 This has been recommended of vision screening on road accidents.
by the Deutsche Ophthalmologische Gesellschaft since
2008 when evaluating fitness to drive.43 Existing on-road Acknowledgements We thank Frederique Margot for calling all the participants
and setting up the appointments, the 12 driving instructors who assessed driving
tests, mainly done in high illumination, are not suited performance, Sylvie Hautle and the Swiss Automobile Club for organising the
for detecting difficulties related to loss of CS. If on-road refresher courses and letting us use their facilities, and Dave Brooks (ELCS) for
evaluation is to be considered as a reference standard for editing our English.
fitness to drive, it might be necessary to adapt the driving Contributors PV and BF applied for and obtained the grants for this study. PV
tests and also assess performances in places with low lumi- wrote the protocol, managed the staff, recruited participants and monitored data
nosity. In clinical settings, it might be relevant to test CS collection. BF was the principal investigator. LS and PV ran the analysis. LS, BF,
F-XB and PV interpreted the results; LS and PV wrote the manuscript; all authors
using free, computer-based solutions or by simply ques- approved the final draft.
tioning the drivers concerned.44 The vision and night
Funding This study was financed by a grant accorded for research in 2011 by
driving questionnaire (VND-Q) was developed for this the Department of Medicine and Community Health of the University Hospital of
purpose and includes nine simple questions that drivers Lausanne, Switzerland.
can answer prior to their medical examination.45 46 Disclaimer The funder had no influence on the design, the collection, the analysis
We must not forget that a decrease in CS and/or VA47 and the interpretation of data; on the writing of the manuscript or on the decision to
can be the major symptom of several ocular disorders, submit the manuscript for publication.
like cataracts, which affect half of adults by the time they Competing interests None declared.
reach 75 years of age.6 Many people can live a long time Patient consent Obtained.
with a cataract before a diagnosis is made and surgery Ethics approval The protocol was approved by the official state ethics committee
is performed.22 Unfortunately, drivers with a cataract or for the Canton of Vaud (www.cer-vd.ch) under the reference CE 157/2011.
cataracts, even involving only one eye, have 2.5 times Provenance and peer review Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.
more risk of causing a car crash than those without a Data sharing statement Data is made available on request to the corresponding
cataract.6 22 47 This is why general practitioners should be author or francois.mooser@hefr.ch. It is not made publicly available for
very vigilant when patients talk about difficulties experi- confidentiality to be maintained. Health data could be linked and merged to
enced when driving at night or under foggy conditions. existing non-anonymised databases including those held by automobile insurance
companies.
Impaired vision should always be investigated even if
Open Access This is an Open Access article distributed in accordance with the
patients are reluctant, because it can often be corrected
Creative Commons Attribution Non Commercial (CC BY-NC 4.0) license, which
with either medical or surgical management, with subse- permits others to distribute, remix, adapt, build upon this work non-commercially,
quent improvement of visual function.6 24 Following cata- and license their derivative works on different terms, provided the original work is
ract surgery, patients can often even drive again at night properly cited and the use is non-commercial. See: http://creativecommons.org/
licenses/by-nc/4.0/
and/or under foggy conditions with a decreased risk of
being involved in a car crash.6 Adequate management © Article author(s) (or their employer(s) unless otherwise stated in the text of the
article) 2018. All rights reserved. No commercial use is permitted unless otherwise
might avoid unneeded self-restriction or, even worse, expressly granted.
unneeded driving cessation.
When assessing fitness to drive, the role of vision
screening is more about targeting appropriate driving
restrictions than recommending driving cessation.26 REFERENCES
Some drivers expose themselves to excessive risks by 1. Adler G, Rottunda S. Older adults' perspectives on driving cessation.
J Aging Stud 2006;20:227–35.
driving in the dark. On the other hand, many drivers, 2. Niemann S, Achermann Stürmer Y, Bianchi G, et al. STATUS 2016:
especially women, tend to unnecessarily restrict their statistique des accidents non professionnels et du niveau de sécurité
en Suisse; circulation routière, sport, habitat et loisirs. bpa 2016.
driving due to vision deficits in one eye alone.19 Referring 3. Ryan GA, Legge M, Rosman D. Age related changes in drivers' crash
patients to ophthalmologists, occupational therapists or risk and crash type. Accid Anal Prev 1998;30:379–87.
4. Wagner JT, Müri RM, Nef T, et al. Cognition and driving in older
driving instructors to compensate appropriately for vision persons. Swiss Med Wkly 2011;140:w13136.
loss in one eye could help some drivers overcome their 5. Owsley C, Stalvey BT, Phillips JM. The efficacy of an educational
fear of driving. intervention in promoting self-regulation among high-risk older
drivers. Accid Anal Prev 2003;35:393–400.
6. Owsley C, Stalvey BT, Wells J, et al. Visual risk factors for crash
involvement in older drivers with cataract. Arch Ophthalmol
2001;119:881–7.
CONCLUSION 7. Langford J, Bohensky M, Koppel S, et al. Do older drivers pose a risk
Loss of CS could concern up to one older driver out to other road users? Traffic Inj Prev 2008;9:181–9.
of five and often occurs without affecting VA. Those 8. Braver ER, Trempel RE. Are older drivers actually at higher
risk of involvement in collisions resulting in deaths or non-fatal
with CS loss tend to be more likely to avoid driving at injuries among their passengers and other road users? Inj Prev
night, in bad weather or under foggy conditions. Visual 2004;10:27–32.
9. Marottoli RA, Glass TA, Glass TA, et al. Consequences of driving
screening including CS would help detect ophthalmic cessation: decreased out-of-home activity levels. J Gerontol B
diseases earlier and refer patients for surgery before their Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2000;55:S334–40.

Spreng L, et al. BMJ Open 2018;8:e018546. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2017-018546 7


Downloaded from http://bmjopen.bmj.com/ on January 27, 2018 - Published by group.bmj.com

Open Access

10. Desapriya E, Harjee R, Brubacher J, et al. Vision screening of older 30. Khambhiphant B, Tulvatana W, Busayarat M. The new numbers
drivers for preventing road traffic injuries and fatalities. Cochrane contrast sensitivity chart for contrast sensitivity measurement. J
Database Syst Rev 2014;2:CD006252. Optom 2011;4:128–33.
11. Anstey KJ, Wood J, Lord S, et al. Cognitive, sensory and physical 31. Green KA, McGwin G, Owsley C. Associations between visual,
factors enabling driving safety in older adults. Clin Psychol Rev hearing, and dual sensory impairments and history of motor
2005;25:45–65. vehicle collision involvement of older drivers. J Am Geriatr Soc
12. Merour A, Favrat B, Borruat F. Exigences visuelles pour la conduite. 2013;61:252–7.
Rev Med Suisse 2014;10:2252–7. 32. Owsley C, Ball K, McGwin G, et al. Visual processing impairment
13. Owsley C, McGwin G. Vision impairment and driving. Surv and risk of motor vehicle crash among older adults. JAMA
Ophthalmol 1999;43:535–50. 1998;279:1083–8.
14. Gruber N, Mosimann UP, Müri RM, et al. Vision and night driving 33. Edwards JD, Ross LA, Wadley VG, et al. The useful field of view
abilities of elderly drivers. Traffic Inj Prev 2013;14:477–85. test: normative data for older adults. Arch Clin Neuropsychol
15. Gilhotra JS, Mitchell P, Ivers R, et al. Impaired vision and other 2006;21:275–86.
factors associated with driving cessation in the elderly: the blue 34. Bernstein IH, Lacritz L, Barlow CE, et al. Psychometric evaluation of
mountains eye study. Clin Exp Ophthalmol 2001;29:104–7. the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) in three diverse samples.
16. Dellinger AM, Sehgal M, Sleet DA, et al. Driving cessation: what older Clin Neuropsychol 2011;25:119–26.
former drivers tell us. J Am Geriatr Soc 2001;49:431–5. 35. Vaucher P, Herzig D, Cardoso I, et al. The trail making test as a
17. Desapriya E, Wijeratne H, Subzwari S, et al. Vision screening of older screening instrument for driving performance in older drivers; a
drivers for preventing road traffic injuries and fatalities. Cochrane translational research. BMC Geriatr 2014;14:123.
Database Syst Rev 2011:CD006252. 36. Hofheinz M, Mibs M. The prognostic validity of the timed up and
18. Freeman EE, Muñoz B, Turano KA, et al. Measures of visual function go test with a dual task for predicting the risk of falls in the elderly.
and their association with driving modification in older adults. Invest Gerontol Geriatr Med 2016;2:233372141663779.
Ophthalmol Vis Sci 2006;47:514–20. 37. Vaucher P, Cardoso I, Veldstra JL, et al. A neuropsychological
19. Ragland DR, Satariano WA, MacLeod KE. Reasons given by instrument measuring age-related cerebral decline in older drivers:
older people for limitation or avoidance of driving. Gerontologist development, reliability, and validity of MedDrive. Front Hum
2004;44:237–44. Neurosci 2014;8:772.
20. Freeman EE, Muñoz B, Turano KA, et al. Measures of visual 38. McGwin G, Chapman V, Owsley C. Visual risk factors for driving
function and time to driving cessation in older adults. Optom Vis Sci difficulty among older drivers. Accid Anal Prev 2000;32:735–44.
2005;82:765–73. 39. Lyman JM, McGwin G, Sims RV. Factors related to driving difficulty
21. Owsley C, Wood JM, McGwin G. A roadmap for interpreting the and habits in older drivers. Accid Anal Prev 2001;33:413–21.
literature on vision and driving. Surv Ophthalmol 2015;60:250–62. 40. Sandlin D, McGwin G, Owsley C. Association between vision
22. Anstey KJ, Eramudugolla R, Ross LA, et al. Road safety in an aging impairment and driving exposure in older adults aged 70 years
population: risk factors, assessment, interventions, and future and over: a population-based examination. Acta Ophthalmol
directions. Int Psychogeriatr 2016;28:349–56. 2014;92:e207–e212.
23. Owsley C, McGwin G. Vision and driving. Vision Res 41. West CG, Gildengorin G, Haegerstrom-Portnoy G, et al. Vision
2010;50:2348–61. and driving self-restriction in older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc
24. Keeffe JE, Jin CF, Weih LM, et al. Vision impairment and older 2003;51:1348–55.
drivers: who's driving? Br J Ophthalmol 2002;86:1118–21. 42. Man-Son-Hing M, Marshall SC, Molnar FJ, et al. Systematic review
25. Cross JM, McGwin G, Rubin GS, et al. Visual and medical risk of driving risk and the efficacy of compensatory strategies in persons
factors for motor vehicle collision involvement among older drivers. with dementia. J Am Geriatr Soc 2007;55:878–84.
Br J Ophthalmol 2009;93:400–4. 43. Deutsche Ophtalmologische Gesellschaft. DOG-Kongress 2008
26. O'Byrne C, Naughton A, O’Neill D. Is driver licensing restriction for Rückblick. 2008 http://www.dog.org/?cat=115 (accessed 16 Jan
age-related medical conditions an effective mechanism to improve 2017).
driver safety without unduly impairing mobility? Eur Geriatr Med 44. Bach M. The Freiburg visual acuity test--automatic measurement of
2015;6:541–4. visual acuity. Optom Vis Sci 1996;73:49–53.
27. Molina-Torres MJ, Crespo MS, Francés AT, et al. Diagnosis accuracy 45. Kimlin JA, Black AA, Djaja N, et al. Development and validation of
of two vision screeners for visual health surveillance of workers who a vision and night driving questionnaire. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt
use video display terminals. J Occup Health 2016;58:444–51. 2016;36:465–76.
28. Dougherty BE, Flom RE, Bullimore MA. An evaluation of the mars 46. Kimlin JA. Night driving and assessment of mesopic vision for older
letter contrast sensitivity test. Optom Vis Sci 2005;82:970–5. adults: Queensland University of Technology, 2016. Brisbane.
29. Haymes SA, Roberts KF, Cruess AF, et al. The letter contrast 47. Owsley C, Stalvey B, Wells J, et al. Older drivers and cataract:
sensitivity test: clinical evaluation of a new design. Invest Ophthalmol driving habits and crash risk. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci
Vis Sci 2006;47:2739–45. 1999;54:M203–11.

8 Spreng L, et al. BMJ Open 2018;8:e018546. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2017-018546

View publication stats

You might also like