Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

ZDM – Mathematics Education (2023) 55:359–369

https://doi.org/10.1007/s11858-022-01456-1

ORIGINAL PAPER

Studying while anxious: mathematics anxiety and the avoidance


of solving practice problems during exam preparation in college
calculus
Jalisha B. Jenifer1,2 · Susan C. Levine2 · Sian L. Beilock1,2

Accepted: 8 December 2022 / Published online: 21 December 2022


© FIZ Karlsruhe 2022

Abstract
Students who experience mathematics anxiety have long been suggested to engage in avoidance behaviors that negatively
impact their mathematics performance. However, little is known about how these avoidance behaviors manifest for highly
anxious students within the context of a mathematics course. Since the use of effortful study strategies has been shown to be
an important predictor of students’ academic outcomes, we investigated whether highly mathematics-anxious college students
would be less likely to utilize such strategies during exam preparation compared to their less-anxious peers. To measure
this, we asked college students (N = 72) enrolled in introductory Calculus to complete a battery of anxiety-related surveys
and to retrospectively report how they studied for the final exam in their Calculus course. Using theories of achievement
emotion as well as recent research showing a relation between mathematics anxiety and students’ planned exam preparation
behavior, we hypothesized that students experiencing mathematics anxiety would retrospectively report less engagement
with effortful study strategies during exam preparation compared to their less-anxious peers. In line with our hypothesis,
we found that solving practice problems was viewed by students as being one of the most effortful ways to prepare for a
mathematics examination, and that mathematics anxiety was negatively associated with the proportion of study time students
allocated to solving practice problems during exam preparation. These findings highlight the significant relation between
mathematics anxiety and students’ self-regulated exam preparation behaviors, marking it as an important topic to consider
when investigating ways to improve the mathematics performance of highly mathematics-anxious individuals.

Keywords Mathematics anxiety · Mathematics avoidance · Practice problems · Exam preparation · Study strategies

1 Introduction These negative feelings characterize what is now referred


to as mathematics anxiety, a domain-specific anxiety that
With rising demands for STEM professionals worldwide, has been shown to correlate strongly with poor mathemat-
many countries are making mathematics education a top ics performance in both children and adults (Foley et al., 6;
priority (Lacey & Wright, 24). One critical determinant of Hembree, 23; Ma, 33).
mathematics success that is becoming a more prominent Individuals who experience mathematics anxiety are
concern with educators and policymakers alike is how indi- often characterized as mathematically avoidant, with highly-
viduals feel about the subject (Corcoran, 6; Foley et al., 6). anxious students opting into fewer STEM courses and
Fortunately, decades of research have explored how feel- careers than their less-anxious peers (Daker et al, 6; Hem-
ings of “fear” about mathematics can impact one’s math- bree, 23). These avoidance behaviors have been theorized
ematics performance (Ashcraft & Kirk, 4; Dowker et al., 6). to mediate the relation between mathematics anxiety and
performance, as students who avoid mathematics are likely
to have less exposure to and practice with the mathematical
* Jalisha B. Jenifer concepts required for success in the subject (Carey et al.,
jjenifer@barnard.edu
2016; Ramierez et al., 24). However, most of the existing
1
Barnard College, Columbia University, NY, New York, USA literature on mathematics anxiety and avoidance focuses
2
Department of Psychology, University of Chicago, Chicago, on situations where individuals can opt out of engagement
USA with mathematics completely. As a result, little is known

13
Vol.:(0123456789)
360 J. B. Jenifer et al.

about how these avoidance tendencies manifest in situations difficulties for students in college-level courses. College
where it is impossible to completely avoid mathematics, students are often given autonomy over how to engage with
such as when students experiencing mathematics anxiety course material, including whether to utilize various study
are enrolled in a mathematics course. strategies when preparing for an upcoming examination
In the present study, we aimed to investigate whether the (Hartwig & Dunlosky, 6; Karpicke et al., 24). However,
tendency to avoid mathematics by students who experience there has been a recent surge in research focused on how
mathematics anxiety might impact their exam preparation study strategy selection relates to academic performance,
behaviors when studying for an exam in a college Calculus with a substantial body of literature supporting the benefits
course. Gaining an understanding of this relation is critical, of engaging in effortful, desirably difficult study behaviors
as students’ study behavior has been shown to be a signifi- such as self-testing to enhance learning and performance
cant predictor of their academic success (Dunlosky et al., (Bjork, 6; Bjork et al., 6; Roediger & Karpicke, 6, 23). In
6; Miyatsu et al., 23; Rohrer & Pashler, 33). Here, we used quantitative courses, self-testing typically refers to solving
theories of achievement emotion as well as previous findings practice problems that are similar to those one would expect
linking mathematics anxiety to the avoidance of mathemat- to occur on an exam (Tullis & Maddox, 6). This strategy,
ics-related effort to hypothesize that mathematics anxiety which usually involves students solving problems presented
would be associated with decreased engagement with study by the textbook or instructor, is more commonplace than
strategies deemed effortful by students. Such findings would other, potentially more beneficial forms of problem engage-
highlight a significant relation between mathematics anxi- ment such as the self-construction of problems or “prob-
ety and students’ self-regulated exam preparation behaviors, lem-posing” (Baumanns & Rott, 6; Watson & Mason, 33).
prompting further exploration of this phenomenon and its Nonetheless, engagement with self-testing strategies like
potential impact on student outcomes in mathematics. solving practice problems has been found to boost long
term retention and transfer of learned information as well
1.1 Mathematics anxiety, mathematics avoidance, as guide subsequent study behaviors, resulting in positive
& self‑regulated study behavior performance outcomes (Carrier & Pashler, 6; Richland et al.,
44; Soderstrom & Bjork, 24).
Although mathematics anxiety is defined in a variety of ways Despite the benefits of effortful study behaviors, how-
within the education and research literatures, one of the most ever, many students report using easier, sub-optimal strate-
common conceptualizations describes it as “a feeling of ten- gies that are likely to limit their overall learning (Hartwig &
sion, apprehension, or fear that interferes with mathemat- Dunlosky, 6; Karpicke et al., 24; Kornell & Bjork, 33). For
ics performance” (Ashcraft, 2). This interference has been example, students tend to under-use effortful strategies such
theorized to happen in multiple ways, including by leading as self-testing and over-use less strategies such as rereading
highly anxious students to experience in-the-moment wor- that can provide them with a false sense of material mastery
ries and ruminations about their likelihood of success that (Gurung et al., 24; Koriat & Bjork, 23; Wallace et al., 23).
limits the availability of working memory resources needed This type of decision-making can hinder students’ learn-
to solve mathematics problems in the moment (Ashcraft & ing and preparation for exams, marking the avoidance of
Kirk, 4; Beilock et al., 6; Ramirez et al., 33). However, math- effortful study strategies as being detrimental to students’
ematics anxiety has also been theorized to effect how stu- mathematics outcomes.
dents perform in mathematics by impacting the choices they
make regarding the subject (Buckley & Sullivan, 6; Hem- 1.2 Mathematics anxiety & Pekrun’s control value
bree, 23). For example, individuals who experience math- theory of achievement emotions
ematics anxiety often opt out of STEM courses and careers
at higher rates than their less-anxious peers (Daker et al, 6; Given the mathematics-avoidant tendencies mentioned
Hembree, 23). Higher levels of mathematics anxiety have earlier, students who experience mathematics anxiety may
also been linked to a reduction of in-class attention (Geary be particularly likely to forgo effortful study strategies and
et al., 23), mathematical processing (Ashcraft & Faust, 3; instead study via less effortful options. This idea is sup-
Pizzie & Kraemer, 23), and cognitive reflection (Maloney ported by theories of achievement emotions, or emotions
& Retanal, 24; Morsanyi et al., 33). Together, these findings tied directly to achievement activities or achievement out-
suggest that highly mathematics-anxious individuals possess comes. One such theory is Pekrun’s Control Value Theory,
a tendency to avoid and disengage with mathematics mate- which distinguishes between state achievement emotions,
rial in ways that are likely to have serious implications for which refers to momentary occurrences within a given
their mathematics outcomes. situation, and trait achievement emotions, which refers to
The mathematics-avoidant tendencies of highly mathe- habitual, recurring emotions that are typically experienced
matics-anxious individuals are particularly likely to pose in relation to achievement activities and outcomes (Pekrun,

13
Studying while anxious: mathematics anxiety and the avoidance of solving practice problems… 361

44). The control-value theory also highlights two groups of that when preparing for a mathematics exam, advanced high
appraisals as being relevant for emotions in achievement set- school students ranked solving practice problems as being
tings: (1) perceived control over achievement activities and the most effortful study strategy, and greater amounts of
their outcomes, and (2) perceived value of these activities mathematics anxiety were associated with smaller alloca-
and outcomes (Pekrun, 44). The theory specifically suggests tions of study time to solving practice problems during
that students who experience achievement-related anxiety exam preparation (Jenifer et al., 23). Additionally, the study
are likely to have high value for the subject but minimal per- found that students who experience mathematics anxiety
ceived control. This combination of appraisals is theorized also reported a likelihood of prioritizing easier practice
to create feelings of uncertainty for students regarding the problems over harder ones during study sessions, and that
utility of actions designed to prevent academic failure from students’ planned study behaviors partially mediated the
occurring, ultimately shaping the types of behaviors they are association between mathematics anxiety and exam perfor-
willing to engage in. mance. Together, these findings showcase the important role
Based on the control-value theory, it seems plausible to students’ effort-related exam preparation behaviors play in
predict that students experiencing trait mathematics anxiety their mathematics outcomes and suggest that mathematics
would differ in how they prepare for a mathematics exam anxiety may be a critical factor in this relation.
compared to their less-anxious peers. While mathematics However, while students in the aforementioned study
anxiety has been suggested to exist in both “state” and “trait” were asked to report their study behaviors prospectively, no
forms throughout the literature, it is trait mathematics anxi- study to date has investigated highly mathematics-anxious
ety that is viewed as having an impact through its effect on students’ actual study behaviors either during or after a
learning-related choices and is therefore likely to impact study session/examination. Such a test is critical, as it is
students’ exam preparation behaviors (Buckley & Sullivan, unknown whether students’ planned engagement with effort-
6). Additionally, while trait mathematics anxiety typically ful study strategies reflects their actual engagement. Relat-
correlates with other forms of anxiety such as test anxiety edly, students’ use of individual study strategies has been
and general trait anxiety, it is also distinguishable from these suggested to vary across study sessions (Janes et al., 6) and
other forms of anxiety, with different measures of math- the accuracy of students’ predictions regarding their use of
ematics anxiety correlating more highly with one another individual study strategies has been found to vary across
than with test and trait anxiety (Dew et al., 6; Hembree, 23; strategies (Blasiman et al., 6). It’s possible, then, that while
Kazelskis et al., 6). Thus, despite its relation to other forms highly mathematically anxious students report less planned
of anxiety, trait mathematics anxiety appears to exist as a engagement with solving practice problems compared to
unique contributor to students’ mathematic decision-making their less-anxious peers, they actually end up using the strat-
and performance. egy to a degree similar to their peers after they commence
Since control-value theory specifically suggests that exam preparation. Such a finding would suggest that there is
students who experience achievement-related anxiety are no relation between mathematics anxiety and students actual
likely to experience uncertainty regarding the utility of study behavior, which could have significant implications for
particular actions designed to prevent failure, it’s possible future work designed to improve the mathematics outcomes
that anxious students would forgo effortful study strategies of highly mathematics-anxious individuals. The current
and instead study via strategies that require less cognitive study was therefore designed to test this relation.
effort and are less likely to lead them to “labor in vain”
(Bae et al., 5). In line with this thinking, findings of experi-
mental mood research suggest that the activation of nega- 1.3 The present study
tive emotions such as anxiety minimizes the use of flexible,
creative learning strategies and instead facilitates the use of The goal of the current study was to investigate the rela-
rigid learning strategies such as rehearsal (Isen, 24; Pekrun, tion between mathematics anxiety and college students’
44). Additionally, negative emotions are assumed to hinder retrospectively-reported exam preparation behaviors. This
students’ self-regulation of learning which requires the use study follows up on previously published research, which
of metacognitive skills (Wolters, 24). Mathematics anxiety found that greater mathematics anxiety was associated with
may therefore reduce students’ likelihood of optimizing their a reduction in planned engagement with solving practice
exam preparation behaviors, to the detriment of their subse- problems during exam preparation for advanced-level high
quent mathematics exam performance. school students (Jenifer et al., 23). Given existing limita-
Findings from recent research also support the prediction tions surrounding students’ planned estimates of their study
that students who experience mathematics anxiety may be behavior, the current study aimed to determine whether
particularly likely to avoid effortful study strategies in math- previously found patterns with planned study behavior hold
ematics (Ashcraft, 2). In fact, results from one study found when study behaviors are reported retrospectively.

13
362 J. B. Jenifer et al.

In the current study, we specifically asked first-year col- American, 21% Asian, 1% Native American, and 6% other
lege students to retrospectively report how they studied for (an additional 7% chose not to report their race). 15% of
the final exam in their introductory Calculus course. Our the students in our sample identified as Hispanic or Latino.
decision to focus the present study on college-age students
was motivated by the desire to test whether previous findings 2.2 Materials and procedure
would replicate in a non-advanced academic setting. Addi-
tionally, we specifically focused our investigation on stu- Students received an email from the university’s mathemat-
dents enrolled in introductory Calculus because this course ics department inviting them to participate in our study
often serves as a gatekeeper for students attempting to pur- survey at the conclusion of their course. The online survey
sue STEM majors and careers. The current study therefore was administered through the Qualtrics survey platform. All
contributes to the mathematics anxiety literature by testing study procedures were approved via the Social and Behav-
the robustness of the link between mathematics anxiety and ioral Sciences Institutional Review Board of the university
the avoidance of effortful study strategies identified in previ- where the study was conducted (IRB17-1248).
ous work while also helping to expand the generalizability of
those findings to students enrolled in college courses. 2.2.1 Mathematics anxiety
Building on the significant findings from the previous
research, we were specifically interested in examining the Mathematics anxiety was measured using the shortened
following: Mathematics Anxiety Rating Scale (sMARS; Alexander &
Martray, 1), which is a shortened version of the 98-item
• R1: Do college students view solving practice problems MARS (Richardson & Suinn, 43). Students responded to
as one of the most effortful ways to study for a mathemat- questions about how anxious they would feel in different
ics examination? mathematics-related situations (e.g., “signing up for a math-
ematics course”, “studying for a mathematics test”) on a
o H1: College students will rank solving practice
5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 5 (very
problems as being one of the most effortful ways to
much).1 The scale was found to be highly reliable (α = 0.95)
prepare for a mathematics exam.
and all analyses were conducted on the average of the items
• R2: Is mathematics anxiety related to college students’ for each student.
engagement with solving practice problems during exam
preparation? 2.2.2 Trait & test anxiety
o H2: Mathematics anxiety will be negatively related
Given the significant relation found in previous research
to college students’ engagement with solving prac-
between mathematics anxiety and other forms of anxiety,
tice problems during exam preparation.
we also measured participant’s trait anxiety and test anxiety
• R3: Does engagement with solving practice problems to include as covariates in our models. Trait anxiety was
during exam preparation mediate the typically-found assessed using the trait component of the State-Trait Anxiety
relation between mathematics anxiety and mathematics Inventory (Spielberger, 44) in which participants rated how
performance? frequently they experienced feelings of anxiety and calmness
(e.g., “I feel nervous and restless” and “I make decisions
o H3: Practice problem engagement will partially
easily”). Test anxiety was assessed using the Test Anxiety
mediate the negative relation between mathematics
Inventory (Spielberger, 43) in which participants rated how
anxiety and mathematics performance.
anxious they feel in 20 test-related situations (e.g., “dur-
ing tests I feel very tense” and “I feel confident and relaxed
while taking tests”). In both measures, items were scored
2 Method on a 1–4 scale and reverse coded where appropriate. Scores
were summed for a composite measure ranging from 20 to
2.1 Participants 80, with a higher value indicating higher level of trait or
test anxiety. Both scales were found to be highly reliable
All students enrolled in a section of an introductory Calculus (αtest anxiety = 0.96, αtrait anxiety = 0.93).
course (Elementary Calculus or Calculus I) at a midwest-
ern university were invited to participate in our study. The
response rate to our survey was 10%, resulting in a total 1
Due to experimenter error, only 24 of the 25 questions for the
sample size of 72 first-year students (Female = 69%; mean sMARS were administered (Question 13 was accidentally omitted
age = 18.36). Our sample was 50% White, 15% African from the survey).

13
Studying while anxious: mathematics anxiety and the avoidance of solving practice problems… 363

2.2.3 Study behavior questionnaire 2.2.4 Calculus course grade

Students were asked to retrospectively reflect on their use Students’ final letter grade for their Calculus course was
of particular study strategies using a modified version of obtained from the university’s registrar office at the end of
the questionnaire utilized in prior research (Jenifer et al., the semester. Letter grades were numerically transformed
23).The items of the questionnaire were modeled after for all analyses.
those used other study strategy and avoidance behavior
questionnaires (Midgley et al., 6; Pintrich, 6). Addition-
ally, students were asked about their study behavior in 3 Results
relation to six specific study strategies that are similar to
the strategies students typically self-report as being those 3.1 Descriptive statistics
they use the most when studying (e.g. Karpicke et al., 24).
Students were simply presented with a list of strategies Means and standard deviations for all study variables can be
and were not given examples of the strategies. Students found in Table 1. Zero-order correlations between all study
responded to the questionnaire prior to receiving their final variables are reported in Table 2. We report significant cor-
exam and course grades. The questionnaire included three relations that were pertinent to our research questions in the
items focused on effortful study strategy use; these three remainder of this section. All analyses were conducted using
items are described in greater detail below. R (version 4.0.2).
Rank ordering of study strategies by effort. To identify
the study strategies that students viewed as requiring the 3.1.1 Student ranking of effortful study strategies
most effort, we asked the following question: “Rank the
following strategies in order from the strategy that requires In rank order from most to least work, students listed solving
the most work (1) to the least work (6).” Students then practice problems as being the most effortful study strat-
rank-ordered the following set of study strategies: reading egy, followed by reading textbook section(s) for the first
textbook section(s) for the first time, re-reading textbook time, reviewing homework solutions, reading examples of
section(s), reviewing homework solutions, solving prac- solved problems, reviewing notes, and re-reading textbook
tice problems, reading examples of solved problems, and section(s) (Table 1). More than half of the students (78%)
reviewing your notes. ranked solving practice problems as the most or the sec-
Allocation of study time across various strategies. Stu- ond-most effortful strategy. Mathematics anxiety was not
dents were also asked to report how they allocated their significantly related to students’ effort rankings, suggesting
study time across various study strategies with the follow- that both high and low mathematics-anxious students view
ing question: “When preparing for your mathematics final solving practice problems as being an effortful study strategy
exam, what percentage of your test preparation was spent (Table 2).
doing the following activities:: (a) reading textbook sec-
tions for the first time, (b) re-reading textbook section(s), 3.1.2 Student use of specific study strategies
(c) reviewing homework solutions, (d) solving practice
problems, (e) reading examples of solved problems, (f) When asked to report how they allocated their study time
reviewing your notes?” Students reported their allocations across multiple study strategies, students reported allocating
using a sliding bar to indicate the percentage of study time the largest proportion of their study time to solving prac-
they allocated to each strategy. Students’ total study time tice problems, followed by reviewing their notes, reviewing
allocation was constrained to equal 100% across the six homework solutions, reading examples of solved problems,
options. re-reading textbook sections, and reading textbook sections
Prioritization of difficulty during strategy use. Stu- for the first time (Table 1).
dents were asked to report how they would prioritize dif-
ficulty when solving practice problems with the following 3.2 Mathematics anxiety & effort avoidance
question: “When studying for your mathematics exam,
how much did you prioritize (or spend time on) solving Given students’ ranking of solving practice problems
easy practice problems in comparison to solving hard as being the most effortful study strategy, we next tested
practice problems?” Students reported their answers on whether mathematics anxiety was negatively related to stu-
a scale from 1 to 7, with 1 corresponding to prioritizing dents’ engagement with solving practice problems when
easier problems and 7 corresponding to prioritizing harder preparing for their final exam. Mathematics anxiety was
problems. negatively related to the proportion of total study time
students reported devoting to solving practice problems,

13
364 J. B. Jenifer et al.

Table 1  Means (SDs) for all Measure Mean SD Range


measures
1 Mathematics anxiety (α = .95) 2.38 0.73 1.17–4.38
2 Effort Ranking: Reading the textbook for the first time 3.51 1.78 1–6
3 Effort Ranking: Re-reading textbook sections 4.11 1.33 1–6
4 Effort Ranking: Reviewing homework solutions 3.78 1.47 1–6
5 Effort Ranking: Solving practice problems 1.80 1.28 1–6
6 Effort Ranking: Reading examples of solved problems 3.79 1.49 1–6
7 Effort Ranking: Reviewing notes 4.03 1.74 1–6
8 Time Allocation: Reading the textbook for the first time 4.39 7.02 0–29
9 Time Allocation: Re-reading textbook sections 9.47 14.32 0–65
10 Time Allocation: Reviewing homework solutions 16.07 13.22 0–58
11 Time Allocation: Solving practice problems 35.21 21.02 0–100
12 Time Allocation: Reading examples of solved problems 14.78 15.39 0–80
13 Time Allocation: Reviewing notes 20.08 16.51 0–75
14 Effort prioritization 4.68 1.13 1–7
15 Test anxiety (α = .96) 40.44 14.62 20–79
16 Trait anxiety (α = .93) 45.79 12.33 21–73
17 Course grade 2.33 1.61 1–8

Calculus course letter grades were numerically transformed for analyses using the following scale: 1 = A,
2 = A-, 3 = B + , 4 = B, 5 = B-, 6 = C + , 7 = C, 8 = C-, 9 = D, 10 = F

r(70) = − 0.27, p = 0.02 This relation remained marginally p = 0.61, or their prioritization of effortful problems during
significant after controlling for students’ test and trait anxi- solving practice problems, r(67) = − 0.04, p = 0.73. However,
ety, β = − 0.23, t = − 1.72, p = 0.09.2 Figure 1 depicts how we note that our results may be impacted by the skewed dis-
high and low mathematics-anxious students allocated their tribution of Calculus course grades in our sample where the
study time, with mathematics anxiety divided by a median mean grade was approximately an A (Table 1).
split (median split was used here solely for illustrative pur-
poses; all analyses were conducted using the full continuous
measure of mathematics anxiety). We also tested whether 4 Discussion
mathematics anxiety was negatively related to students’ pri-
oritization of effort when solving practice problems during Students’ ability to prepare for and perform on examinations
studying. We did not find evidence to support the idea that is critical for their academic success. However, research sug-
mathematics anxious students are less likely to prioritize gests that many students are do not optimally regulate their
harder practice problems compared to their less-anxious use of study strategies, and often avoid effortful strategies
peers, r(67) = − 0.07, p = 0.52. known to be effective for student learning (Culler et al., 6;
Karpicke et al., 24; Kornell & Bjork, 33). In the current
3.3 Relation to mathematics outcomes study, we hypothesized that individual differences in math-
ematics anxiety would relate to students’ use of effortful
An additional aim of the current study was to test whether study strategies when preparing for the final exam in a math-
mathematics anxious students’ use of effortful study strat- ematics course, and that this relation would impact students’
egies predicted their mathematics outcomes as measured mathematics performance.
by the final grade they received in their Calculus course. Our results partially support these hypotheses by reveal-
Surprisingly, we did not find a significant relation between ing that (a) college students view solving practice problems
students’ mathematics anxiety and their course grade, as being one of the most effortful ways to prepare for a math-
r(70) = 0.10, p = 0.39. Students’ Calculus course grades were ematics exam, and (b) students experiencing mathematics
also not significantly related to the proportion of study time anxiety retrospectively report allocating smaller proportions
they allocated to solving practice problems, r(70) = − 0.06, of their study time to the effortful study strategy of solv-
ing practice problems compared to their less anxious peers.
These findings not only provide additional evidence for the
2
Neither test anxiety nor trait anxiety were significant predictors in link between mathematics anxiety and the avoidance of
the model, βtest = -.10, t = -0.61, p = .55; βtrait = -.03, t = 0.18, p = .86. effortful study strategies found in prior work on high school

13
Table 2  Zero-order correlations
Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16

1. Mathematics anxiety
2. Effort Ranking: Reading the textbook for the first time .19
3. Effort Ranking: Re-reading textbook sections .09 .54***
4. Effort Ranking: Reviewing homework solutions − .02 − .46*** − .37**
5. Effort Ranking: Solving practice problems .08 − .23 − .33** .04
6. Effort Ranking: Reading examples of solved problems − .15 − .54*** − .46*** .00 .02
7. Effort Ranking: Reviewing notes − .18 − .43*** − .37** − .11 − .28* .01
8. Time Allocation: Reading the textbook (­ 1st time) .03 − .18 − .06 .08 .14 .10 − .03
9. Time Allocation: Re-reading textbook − .08 − .32** − .29* .20 .07 .12 .22 − .02
10. Time Allocation: Reviewing homework solutions .15 − .07 .11 − .21 .10 .08 .03 .18 − .24*
11. Time Allocation: Solving practice problems − .27* − .01 − .04 − .04 − .15 .13 .07 − .15 − .31** − .20
.
12. Time Allocation: Reading examples of solved problems .14 .28* .07 .02 .10 − .39*** − .10 − .19 02 − .25* − .46***
Studying while anxious: mathematics anxiety and the avoidance of solving practice problems…

13. Time Allocation: Reviewing Notes .16 .16 .17 − .01 − .11 .00 − .20 − .18 − .30* − .18 − .35** − .08
14. Effort Prioritization − .08 .04 − .05 .28* − .12 − .15 − .03 − .07 − .02 − .18 .05 .21 − .05
15. Test anxiety .49*** .26* .19 − .13 .08 − .09 − .27* − .01 .01 .17 − .20 .15 − .03 − .05
16. Trait anxiety 0.26* .12 .12 − .03 .08 − .02 − .23 .00 .02 .06 − .10 .08 − .02 .07 .66***
17. Course grade 0.10 .11 .01 − .07 .09 − .10 − .05 − .18 − .03 − .12 − .06 .25* .04 − .04 .13 − .02

N = 72. *p < .05; **p < .01;***p < .001

13
365
366 J. B. Jenifer et al.

Fig. 1  Proportion of time low

Proportion of Time Allocated to Study Strategies


60%
and high mathematics-anxious Solving practice problems
students reported allocating to
various study strategies. For Reviewing your notes
illustrative purposes, mathemat-
ics anxiety was divided into low 40% Reviewing homework solutions
and high levels using a median 40%
split (all analyses use the con- Reading examples of solved
tinuous measure of mathematics 31% problems
anxiety) Re-reading textbook section(s)

21% Reading textbook section(s) for


19% 18% the first time
20% 17%
15%
13%
10% 9%

5% 4%

0%
Low Math Anxious High Math Anxious
Student's Mathematics Anxiety

students (Jenifer et al., 23), but also expands the general- strategies that often accompany the activation of negative
izability of these findings to college students enrolled in emotions (Isen, 24; Pekrun, 44; Wolters, 24). However, pre-
introductory college courses. Additionally, we found that the vious research also suggests that these avoidance behaviors
relation between mathematics anxiety and students’ engage- may be related to how costly effortful mathematics engage-
ment with solving practice problems remained marginally ment is perceived to be (Ashcraft & Kirk, 4; Park et al., 43;
significant after controlling for students’ test and trait anxi- Choe et al., 6). Relatedly, research exploring the negative
ety, highlighting the important role domain-specific anxie- relationship between mathematics anxiety and mathematics
ties play in students’ self-regulated study behaviors. performance suggests that solving practice problems might
Surprisingly, however, we did not find an association be harder for mathematics anxious individuals, leading to
between mathematics anxiety and students’ course grades, their disengagement with the subject (Ashcraft & Faust,
nor did we find an association between students’ use of 3; Chang et al., 6; Geary et al., 23; Lyons & Beilock, 23;
effortful study strategies and students’ course grades. These Maloney & Retanal, 24; Morsanyi et al., 33; Pizzie & Krae-
findings differ from studies linking mathematics anxiety to mer, 23; Ramirez et al., 33; Lyons & Beilock, 23). Thus, it
poor mathematics outcomes (Barroso et al., 6; Hembree, 23; may be important to consider highly mathematics-anxious
Levine & Pantoja, 6; Ma, 33) and desirably difficult learning students’ perceptions of cost when attempting to understand
behaviors to positive academic outcomes (Dunlosky et al., their mathematics-related decision-making.
6; Miyatsu et al., 23; Rohrer & Pashler, 33). It is likely that
the opt-in nature of our survey in addition to our decision to 4.2 Limitations & future research
measure student performance via course grades rather than
by exam performance significantly impacted our ability to Despite the strength of our findings, the current study is
detect these relations, though more research is needed to limited by various factors. One serious limitations of our
fully understand the factors that influenced this outcome. study is the fact that our small sample results from a low
response rate to our survey. Additionally, while we didn’t
4.1 Mathematics anxiety and study strategy collect performance data for students who did not agree to
selection participate in the study, our sample likely consists of pri-
marily higher-performing students given the opt-in nature
Why would mathematics anxious students avoid solving of the survey (which may also explain the truncated range
practice problems when preparing for an exam? As previ- of course grades found for participants in our study, see
ously mentioned, the control-value theory suggests that this Table 1). Future research should attempt to recruit a greater
behavior may be related to anxious students’ uncertainty number of students from a range of ability levels and per-
regarding the utility of particular actions designed to pre- form more rigorous analyses to confirm the robustness of
vent failure. It’s therefore possible that anxious students these findings.
decide to forgo effortful study strategies that require the use The current study also measured mathematics anxiety
of metacognitive skills and instead use the rigid learning after the final exam for the course rather than before or

13
Studying while anxious: mathematics anxiety and the avoidance of solving practice problems… 367

during students’ exam preparation. However, the focus of Author contributions JBJ developed the study concept. All authors
the current study was on “trait” mathematics anxiety, which contributed to the study design. JBJ collected the data, performed the
data analyses and drafted the manuscript. SCL and SLB provided criti-
is typically considered a relatively stable trait that does not cal revisions to the manuscript. All authors approved the final version
vary without significant intervention. Nonetheless, the tim- of the manuscript for submission.
ing of anxiety-related measurement should be a considera-
tion for future research. Data Availability The data that support the findings of this study are
available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.
Additionally, we were unable to obtain student grades
for the final exam that corresponded to student responses on
the study strategies survey. For this reason, student course
grades were used in place of final exam grades for our analy-
References
ses as a proxy for student performance. However, course
grades did not vary greatly across participants, as only let- Alexander, L., & Martray, C. (1989). The development of an abbrevi-
ter grades were administered and all students received a ated version of the mathematics anxiety rating scale. Measurement
grade between an A and a C-. As previously mentioned, our and Evaluation in Counseling and Development, 22(3), 143–150.
https://​doi.​org/​10.​1080/​07481​756.​1989.​12022​923
decision to measure student performance via course grades
Ashcraft, M. H. (2002). Mathematics anxiety: Personal, educational,
rather than by exam performance may have impacted our and cognitive consequences. Current Directions in Psychologi-
ability to detect the relations between mathematics anxi- cal Science, 11(5), 181–185. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1111/​1467-​8721.​
ety, students’ course grades, and students use of effortful 00196
Ashcraft, M. H., & Faust, M. W. (1994). Mathematics anxiety and
study strategies, especially since students’ course grades
mental arithmetic performance: An exploratory investigation.
are typically calculated using other non-performance met- Cognition & Emotion, 8(2), 97–125. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1080/​
rics such as student attendance and participation. Since 02699​93940​84089​31
students’ planned use of effortful study strategies has been Ashcraft, M. H., & Kirk, E. P. (2001). The relationships among work-
ing memory, math anxiety, and performance. Journal of Experi-
found to positively predict student exam performance and
mental Psychology: General, 130(2), 224–237. https://d​ oi.o​ rg/1​ 0.​
mediate the negative relation between mathematics anxiety 1037/​0096-​3445.​130.2.​224
and exam performance in prior research (Jenifer et al., 23), Bae, J., Hong, S. S., & Son, L. K. (2021). Prior failures, laboring in
future research should continue to test the robustness of this vain, and knowing when to give up: Incremental versus entity
theories. Metacognition and Learning, 16(2), 275–296. https://​
phenomenon by investigating the prevalence of mathematics
doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s11409-​020-​09253-5
avoidance behaviors and their consequences in classroom Barroso, C., Ganley, C. M., McGraw, A. L., Geer, E. A., Hart, S. A., &
settings using more proximal measures of performance. Daucourt, M. C. (2021). A meta-analysis of the relation between
math anxiety and math achievement. Psychological Bulletin,
147(2), 134–168. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1037/​bul00​00307
Baumanns, L., & Rott, B. (2020). Rethinking problem-posing situa-
5 Conclusion tions: A review, investigations in mathematics learning. Investi-
gations in Mathematics Learning. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1080/​19477​
503.​2020.​18415​01
A substantial body of literature exists supporting the benefits Beilock, S. L., Schaeffer, M. W., & Rozek, C. S. (2017). Understanding
of engaging in effortful, desirably difficult study behaviors and addressing performance anxiety. In A. J. Elliot, C. S. Dweck,
over less effortful options to enhance learning and perfor- & D. S. Yeager (Eds.), Handbook of Competence and Motivation:
mance (Bjork, 6; Bjork et al., 6; Roediger & Karpicke, 6, Theory and Application (2nd ed., pp. 155–174). Guilford Press.
Bjork, R. A. (1994). Memory and metamemory considerations in the.
23). However, the results from the current study suggest Metacognition Knowing about Knowing, 185, 7–2.
that students who experience mathematics anxiety allocate Bjork, R. A., Dunlosky, J., & Kornell, N. (2013). Self-regulated
smaller proportions of their study time to strategies deemed learning: Beliefs, techniques, and illusions. Annual Review
beneficial by the literature compared to their less-anxious of Psychology, 64, 417–444. https:// ​ d oi. ​ o rg/ ​ 1 0. ​ 1 146/ ​ a nnur​
ev-​psych-​113011-​143823
peers. These findings align with those from previous work, Blasiman, R. N., Dunlosky, J., & Rawson, K. A. (2017). The what, how
providing converging evidence in support of the idea that much, and when of study strategies: Comparing intended versus
mathematics anxiety relates to individuals’ effort-based actual study behaviour. Memory, 25(6), 784–792. https://​doi.​org/​
decision-making in consequential ways. The results of this 10.​1080/​09658​211.​2016.​12219​74
Buckley, S., & Sullivan, P. (2021). Reframing anxiety and uncertainty
study therefore mark this relation as an important future in the mathematics classroom. Mathematics Education Research
direction for work designed to improve the academic out- Journal. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s13394-​021-​00393-8
comes of highly mathematics-anxious individuals. Carey, E., Hill, F., Devine, A., & Szücs, D. (2016). The chicken or
the egg? The direction of the relationship between mathematics
Acknowledgements This work is supported by the National Science anxiety and mathematics performance. Frontiers in Psychology,
Foundation (Grant No. DGE-1144082 and DGE-1746045 to J.B.J.) 6, 1987. https://​doi.​org/​10.​3389/​fpsyg.​2015.​01987
and the Institute of Education Sciences (R305C050076 to J.B.J.). The Carrier, M., & Pashler, H. (1992). The influence of retrieval on reten-
opinions expressed are those of the authors and do not represent views tion. Memory & Cognition, 20(6), 633–642. https://​doi.​org/​10.​
of the funders. 3758/​BF032​02713

13
368 J. B. Jenifer et al.

Chang, H., Sprute, L., Maloney, E. A., Beilock, S. L., & Berman, M. G. Karpicke, J. D., Butler, A. C., & Roediger, H. L., III. (2009). Metacog-
(2017). Simple arithmetic: Not so simple for highly mathematics nitive strategies in student learning: Do students practise retrieval
anxious individuals. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, when they study on their own? Memory, 17(4), 471–479. https://​
12(12), 1940–1949. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1093/​scan/​nsx121 doi.​org/​10.​1080/​09658​21080​26470​09
Choe, K. W., Jenifer, J. B., Rozek, C. S., Berman, M. G., & Beilock, Kazelskis, R., Reeves, C., Kersh, M. E., Bailey, G., Cole, K., Larmon,
S. L. (2019). Calculated avoidance: Mathematics anxiety predicts M., & Holliday, D. C. (2000). Mathematics anxiety and test anxi-
mathematics avoidance in effort-based decision-making. Science ety: Separate constructs? The Journal of Experimental Education,
Advances, 5(11), 1–10. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1126/​sciadv.​aay10​62 68(2), 137–146. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1080/​00220​97000​95984​99
Corcoran, R. P., Cheung, A. C., Kim, E., & Xie, C. (2018). Effective Koriat, A., & Bjork, R. A. (2005). Illusions of competence in moni-
universal school-based social and emotional learning programs for toring one’s knowledge during study. Journal of Experimental
improving academic achievement: A systematic review and meta- Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31(2), 187–194.
analysis of 50 years of research. Educational Research Review, 25, https://​doi.​org/​10.​1037/​0278-​7393.​31.2.​187
56–72. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/j.​edurev.​2017.​12.​001 Kornell, N., & Bjork, R. A. (2008). Optimising self-regulated study:
Culler, R. E., & Holahan, C. J. (1980). Test anxiety and academic The benefits—and costs—of dropping flashcards. Memory, 16(2),
performance: The effects of study-related behaviors. Journal of 125–136. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1080/​09658​21070​17638​99
Educational Psychology, 72(1), 16–20. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1037/​ Lacey, T. A., & Wright, B. (2009). Employment outlook:
0022-​0663.​72.1.​16 2008–18-occupational employment projections to 2018. Monthly
Daker, R. J., Gattas, S. U., Sokolowski, H. M., Green, A. E., & Lyons, Lab. Rev., 132, 82.
I. M. (2021). First-year students’ mathematics anxiety predicts Levine, S. C., & Pantoja, N. (2021). Development of children’s math-
STEM avoidance and underperformance throughout university, ematics attitudes: Gender differences, key socializers, and inter-
independently of mathematics ability. NPJ Science of Learning, vention approaches. Developmental Review, 62, 100997. https://​
6(1), 1–13. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1038/​s41539-​021-​00095-7 doi.​org/​10.​1016/j.​dr.​2021.​100997
Dew, K. H., Galassi, J. P., & Galassi, M. D. (1984). Math anxiety: Lyons, I. M., & Beilock, S. L. (2012). When mathematics hurts: math-
Relation with situational test anxiety, performance, physiological ematics anxiety predicts pain network activation in anticipation
arousal, and math avoidance behavior. Journal of Counseling Psy- of doing mathematics. PLoS ONE. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1371/​journ​
chology, 31(4), 580. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1037/​0022-​0167.​31.4.​580 al.​pone.​00480​76
Dowker, A., Sarkar, A., & Looi, C. Y. (2016). Mathematics anxiety: Ma, X. (1999). A meta-analysis of the relationship between anxiety
What have we learned in 60 years? Frontiers in Psychology, 7, toward mathematics and achievement in mathematics. Journal
508. https://​doi.​org/​10.​3389/​fpsyg.​2016.​00508 for Research in Mathematics Education, 30(5), 520–540. https://​
Dunlosky, J., Rawson, K. A., Marsh, E. J., Nathan, M. J., & Willing- doi.​org/​10.​2307/​749772
ham, D. T. (2013). Improving students’ learning with effective Maloney, E. A., & Retanal, F. (2020). Higher mathematics anxious
learning techniques: Promising directions from cognitive and edu- people have a lower need for cognition and are less reflective in
cational psychology. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, their thinking. Acta Psychologica, 202, 102939. https://​doi.​org/​
14(1), 4–58. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1177/​15291​00612​453266 10.​1016/j.​actpsy.​2019.​102939
Foley, A. E., Herts, J. B., Borgonovi, F., Guerriero, S., Levine, S. C., Midgley, C., Maehr, M. L., Hruda, L. Z., Anderman, E., Anderman,
& Beilock, S. L. (2017). The math anxiety-performance link: A L., Freeman, K. E., & Urdan, T. (2000). Manual for the Patterns
global phenomenon. Current Directions in Psychological Science, of Adaptive Learning Scales. University of Michigan.
26(1), 52–58. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1177/​09637​21416​672463 Miyatsu, T., Nguyen, K., & McDaniel, M. A. (2018). Five popular
Geary, D. C., Hoard, M. K., Nugent, L., & Scofield, J. E. (2021). In- study strategies: Their pitfalls and optimal implementations. Per-
class attention, spatial ability, and mathematics anxiety predict spectives on Psychological Science, 13(3), 390–407.
across-grade gains in adolescents’ mathematics achievement. Morsanyi, K., Busdraghi, C., & Primi, C. (2014). Mathematic anxiety
Journal of Educational Psychology, 113(4), 754–769. https://​doi.​ is linked to reduced cognitive reflection: A potential road from
org/​10.​1037/​edu00​00487 discomfort in the mathematics classroom to susceptibility to
Gurung, R. A. R., Weidert, J., & Jeske, A. (2012). Focusing on How biases. Behavioral and Brain Functions. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1186/​
Students Study. Journal of the Scholarship of Teaching and Learn- 1744-​9081-​10-​31
ing, 10(1), 28–35. Retrieved from https://​schol​arwor​ks.​iu.​edu/​ National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. (1980). An Agenda for
journ​als/​index.​php/​josotl/​artic​le/​view/​1734 Action: Recommendations for School Mathematics of the 1980s.
Hartwig, M. K., & Dunlosky, J. (2012). Study strategies of college Reston.
students: Are self-testing and scheduling related to achievement? Park, D., Ramirez, G., & Beilock, S. L. (2014). The role of expressive
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19(1), 126–134. https://​doi.​org/​ writing in mathematics anxiety. Journal of Experimental Psychol-
10.​3758/​s13423-​011-​0181-y ogy: Applied, 20(2), 103.
Hembree, R. (1990). The nature, effects, and relief of mathematics Pekrun, R. (2006). The control-value theory of achievement emo-
anxiety. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 21(1), tions: Assumptions, corollaries, and implications for educational
33–46. https://​doi.​org/​10.​5951/​jrese​mathe​duc.​21.1.​0033 research and practice. Educational Psychology Review, 18, 315–
Isen, A. M. (2000). Some perspectives on positive affect and self- 341. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10648-​006-​9029-9
regulation. Psychological Inquiry, 11(3), 184–187. http://​www.​ Pintrich, P. R. (1991). A manual for the use of the Motivated Strategies
jstor.​org/​stable/​14498​00 for Learning Questionnaire (MSLQ).
Janes, J. L., Dunlosky, J., & Rawson, K. A. (2018). How do students Pizzie, R. G., & Kraemer, D. J. M. (2017). Avoiding math on a rapid
use self-testing across multiple study sessions when preparing timescale: Emotional responsivity and anxious attention in math
for a high-stakes exam? Journal of Applied Research in Memory anxiety. Brain and Cognition, 118, 100–107. https://​doi.​org/​10.​
and Cognition, 7(2), 230–240. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/j.​jarmac.​ 1016/j.​bandc.​2017.​08.​004
2017.​11.​003 Ramirez, G., Chang, H., Maloney, E. A., Levine, S. C., & Beilock, S.
Jenifer, J. B., Rozek, C. S., Levine, S. C., & Beilock, S. L. (2022). L. (2016). On the relationship between math anxiety and math
Effort(less) exam preparation: Math anxiety predicts the avoid- achievement in early elementary school: The role of problem solv-
ance of effortful study strategies. Journal of Experimental Psy- ing strategies. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 141,
chology: General. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1037/​xge00​01202 83–100. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/j.​jecp.​2015.​07.​014

13
Studying while anxious: mathematics anxiety and the avoidance of solving practice problems… 369

Ramirez, G., Shaw, S. T., & Maloney, E. A. (2018). Math anxiety: Spielberger, C. D. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Inventory STAI
Past research, promising interventions, and a new interpretation (Form Y). Mind Garden.
framework. Educational Psychologist, 53(3), 145–164. https://d​ oi.​ Tullis, J. G., & Maddox, G. B. (2020). Self-reported use of retrieval
org/​10.​1080/​00461​520.​2018.​14473​84 practice varies across age and domain. Metacognition and Learn-
Richardson, F. C., & Suinn, R. M. (1972). The mathematics anxiety ing, 15(2), 129–154. https://d​ oi.o​ rg/1​ 0.1​ 007/s​ 11409-0​ 20-0​ 9223-x
rating scale: Psychometric data. Journal of Counseling Psychol- Wallace, A. S., Elliot, A. J., & Rogge, R. D. (2022). Spontaneous use of
ogy., 19(6), 551. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1037/​h0033​456 retrieval and rereading: Relation to achievement goals and exam
Richland, L. E., Kornell, N., & Kao, L. S. (2009). The pretesting effect: performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 114(6), 1412.
Do unsuccessful retrieval attempts enhance learning? Journal of https://​doi.​org/​10.​1037/​edu00​00757
Experimental Psychology: Applied, 15(3), 243–257. https://​doi.​ Watson, A., & Mason, J. (2006). Mathematics as a constructive activ-
org/​10.​1037/​a0016​496 ity: Learners generating examples. Routledge. https://​doi.​org/​10.​
Roediger, H. L., III., & Karpicke, J. D. (2006). Test-enhanced learning: 4324/​97814​10613​714
Taking memory tests improves long-term retention. Psychologi- Wolters, C. A. (2003). Regulation of motivation: Evaluating an under-
cal Science, 17(3), 249–255. https://d​ oi.o​ rg/1​ 0.1​ 111/j.1​ 467-9​ 280.​ emphasized aspect of self-regulated learning. Educational Psy-
2006.​01693.x chologist, 38(4), 189–205. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1207/​S1532​6985E​
Roediger, H. L., III., & Karpicke, J. D. (2018). Reflections on the resur- P3804_1
gence of interest in the testing effect. Perspectives on Psychologi-
cal Science, 13(2), 236–241. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1177/​17456​91617​ Publisher's Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to
718873 jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rohrer, D., & Pashler, H. (2010). Recent research on human learn-
ing challenges conventional instructional strategies. Educational Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds
Researcher, 39(5), 406–412. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1073/​pnas.​18085​ exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the
89116 author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted
Soderstrom, N. C., & Bjork, R. A. (2014). Testing facilitates the regula- manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of
tion of subsequent study time. Journal of Memory and Language, such publishing agreement and applicable law.
73, 99–115. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/j.​jml.​2014.​03.​003
Spielberger, C. D. (1980). Test anxiety inventory: Preliminary Profes-
sional Manual. Consulting Psychologist Press.

13

You might also like