Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

901709

research-article2020
JDRXXX10.1177/0022034520901709Journal of Dental ResearchSocioeconomic Life Course Models and Oral Health

Research Reports: Clinical


Journal of Dental Research
2020, Vol. 99(3) 257­–263
Socioeconomic Life Course Models © International & American Associations
for Dental Research 2020

and Oral Health: A Longitudinal Analysis Article reuse guidelines:


sagepub.com/journals-permissions
DOI: 10.1177/0022034520901709
https://doi.org/10.1177/0022034520901709
journals.sagepub.com/home/jdr

R.K. Celeste1 , H.S. Eyjólfsdóttir2, C. Lennartsson2, and J. Fritzell2

Abstract
We compared socioeconomic life course models to decompose the direct and mediated effects of socioeconomic status (SES) in different
periods of life on late-life oral health. We used data from 2 longitudinal Swedish studies: the Level of Living Survey and the Swedish Panel
Study of Living Conditions of the Oldest Old. Two birth cohorts (older, 1925 to 1934; younger, 1944 to 1953) were followed between
1968 and 2011 with 6 waves. SES was measured with 4 indicators of SES and modeled as a latent variable. Self-reported oral health
was based on a tooth conditions question. Variables in the younger and older cohorts were grouped into 4 periods: childhood, young/
mid-adulthood, mid /late adulthood, late adulthood/life. We used structural equation modeling to fit the following into lagged-effects life
course models: 1) chain of risk, 2) sensitive period with late-life effect, 3) sensitive period with early- and late-life effects, 4) accumulation
of risks with cross-sectional effects, and 5) accumulation of risks. Chain of risk was incorporated into all models and combined with
accumulation, with cross-sectional effects yielding the best fit (older cohort: comparative fit index = 0.98, Tucker-Lewis index = 0.98,
root mean square error of approximation = 0.04, weighted root mean square residual = 1.51). For the older cohort, the chain of SES
from childhood → mid-adulthood → late adulthood → late life showed the following respective standardized coefficients: 053, 0.92, and
0.97. The total effect of childhood SES on late-life tooth loss (standardized coefficient: –0.23 for older cohort, –0.17 for younger cohort)
was mediated by previous tooth loss and SES. Cross-sectional effects of SES on tooth loss were observed throughout the life course, but
the strongest coefficients were at young/mid-adulthood (standardized coefficient: –0.41 for older cohort, –0.45 for younger cohort). SES
affects oral health cumulatively over the life course and through a chain of risks. Actions to improve socioeconomic conditions in early
life might have long-lasting effects on health if they help prevent people from becoming trapped in a chain of risks.

Keywords: life cycle stages, structural equation modeling, tooth loss, socioeconomic status, birth cohort, gerontology

Introduction life course epidemiology has developed several models that


incorporate early- and later-life SES (Kuh et al. 2003). The
According to the Global Burden of Disease study, untreated most common life course model is perhaps the accumulation
dental caries is the most prevalent condition in the world hypothesis, proposing that each exposure has a concurrent but
(Kassebaum et al. 2015), and the total costs of oral health dis- independent effect on health.
eases amounted to $544.41 billion in 2015, 67% of which was Regarding oral health, SES measured during childhood
due to tooth loss (Righolt et al. 2018). Treatment for oral con- (Poulton et al. 2002; Listl et al. 2014; Han and Khang 2017;
ditions accounts for 4.6% of global health expenditures, and Fantin et al. 2018; Listl et al. 2018; Vendrame et al. 2018) and
such expenditures are mostly made by high-income countries during adulthood (Tsakos et al. 2011; Schwendicke et al. 2015;
(Righolt et al. 2018). Seerig et al. 2015) has been consistently associated to preva-
In the life course approach to aging, the effects of early-life lent cases of tooth loss, chewing, or caries in adulthood.
social and biological factors on health later in life have been Additionally, a systematic review suggests that earlier life con-
examined extensively because of the increasing evidence that ditions, such as parental education or occupational status,
factors from early life are associated with a number of chronic
disease in later life (Pollitt et al. 2005). More recently, research
has acknowledged that early social factors are associated with 1
Department of Preventive and Social Dentistry, Federal University of
quality of life (Peres et al. 2009; Niedzwiedz et al. 2012). Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre, Brazil
2
Nonetheless, a “direct effect” has not been unequivocally Aging Research Center, Karolinska Institutet / Stockholm University,
shown because early-life socioeconomic effects are consis- Stockholm, Sweden
tently reduced after controlling for socioeconomic status (SES) A supplemental appendix to this article is available online.
in other periods of life (Pollitt et al. 2005). Most chronic dis-
Corresponding Author:
eases have their onset in middle and later adulthood, and the
R.K. Celeste, Department of Preventive and Social Dentistry, Federal
proposed mechanisms for such long latency effects reside in University of Rio Grande do Sul, Rua Ramiro Barcelos 2492, Porto
structural or functional alterations and metabolic maladapta- Alegre, Rio Grande do Sul 90035-033, Brazil.
tion to the nutritional environment (Kuh et al. 2003). Therefore, Email: roger.keller@ufrgs.br
258 Journal of Dental Research 99(3)

(more or less cross-sectionally) or if it had


an effect in adulthood in incident cases .
Therefore, the aim of this study was to
compare different socioeconomic life course
models with longitudinal data to decompose
the direct and mediated effects of SES in
different periods of life on oral health in late
life.

Methods
We used data from 2 longitudinal Swedish
panel studies that are linked at the individ-
ual level. The Level of Living Survey began
Figure 1. Measurement model tested in confirmatory factor analysis. in 1968 and is based on a representative
sample of the Swedish population aged 15
to 75 y. Subsequent follow-up interview
might be associated with periodontal health (Schuch et al. waves were in 1974, 1981, 1991, 2000, and 2010 (Fritzell et al.
2017). Studies that assess social mobility and tooth loss, how- 2007). When individuals reached the upper age limit, they
ever, tend to show a more diverse picture. While some of these were invited to continue in the Swedish Panel Study of Living
studies showed that current adulthood SES (Pearce et al. 2004; Conditions of the Oldest Old (Lennartsson et al. 2014), which
Bernabé et al. 2011; Bernabé et al. 2012; Astrom et al. 2015; has completed 5 waves starting in 1992 and from which we
Delgado-Angulo and Bernabe 2015; Schwendicke et al. 2015) used data from 2002 and 2011. Both surveys have mainly been
is a more important predictor of oral health, others reported a conducted through face-to-face interviews, with no clinical
stronger effect of childhood SES (Poulton et al. 2002; Pearce oral health data. The present study followed the STROBE
et al. 2009) or similar effects for downward and upward social guidelines.
mobility (Peres et al. 2011; Han and Khang 2017). Studies that For this work, 2 birth cohorts (1925 to 1934, n = 923; 1944
used structural equation modeling (SEM) to decompose early to 1953, n = 1,195) were followed between 1968 (further
and adult SES effects on tooth loss showed no direct effect (Lu details are given elsewhere: Celeste and Fritzell 2018) and
et al. 2011; Broadbent et al. 2016) or a smaller direct effect of 2011 (Swedish Panel Study of Living Conditions of the Oldest
early-life SES as compared with adult-life SES on oral health Old) or 2010 (Level of Living Survey). With these cohorts, we
(Bernabé et al. 2012; Vendrame et al. 2018). were able to assess incidence of losing all or almost all teeth in
There are theoretical and methodological challenges to esti- different age groups from 15 to 24 y to 77 to 86 y and evaluate
mating direct and indirect effects of early-life SES without the cohort effects. Each cohort has 6 measurements over the
some degree of collinearity with SES later in life. Most studies life course. For analytic purposes, those 6 measurements were
use traditional regression methods to estimate independent organized in the following 4 periods: T1) retrospective child-
direct effects and therefore cannot appropriately estimate indi- hood and early-life conditions (data were collected in 1968),
rect effects in the presence of intermediate confounding factors T2) current young adulthood (merging interview waves 1968
(De Stavola et al. 2015). The use of SEM has been advocated and 1974), T3) current mid-adulthood (merging interview
when there is an a priori theory that can be used to test relations waves 1981 and 1992), and T4) current late adulthood or late
(Baker and Gibson 2014). In the presence of repeated measures life (merging interview waves 2000/2002 and 2010/2011).
of the exposure and the outcome, SEM is a particularly appro- Variables from different waves were combined in latent vari-
priate method to decompose effects that go through a chain. ables as shown in Figure 1 and explained as follows.
For example, SES in childhood is hypothesized to have an Self-reported oral health was obtained from the following
effect on the outcome in 1) the cumulative model, 2) the sensi- question: “Which of the following alternatives best describes
tive/critical model, 3) the so-called trajectories model (upward the condition of your teeth?” The 5 response alternatives were
mobility assumes poverty in early life), and 4) the chain-of- as follows: 1, no teeth or mere remains; 2, dentures, whole or
risks model (assuming an indirect triggering effect via adult- part; 3, own teeth but in bad condition, many missing; 4, own
life poverty). Finally, another important problem resides in the teeth but many fillings or bridgework; 5, own teeth in good
fact that almost all oral health life course studies used prevalent condition, no or few fillings.
outcomes in adulthood (Listl et al. 2018), while a longitudinal Childhood SES was created as a compound/latent score for
study on oral health life course showed no associations of SEM from 4 observed variables as described in a previous work
early-life SES with changes in oral health from age 65 to 70 y (Lennartsson et al. 2018). Information on SES in childhood was
(Astrom et al. 2015; Gülcan et al. 2015). Because tooth loss is collected retrospectively at the time of the first interview, in
a cumulative outcome, without repeated measures, it is impos- 1968. Mothers and fathers’ levels of education for both cohorts
sible to know if SES in childhood had an effect in childhood were divided into 5 ordinal categories, with compulsory
Socioeconomic Life Course Models and Oral Health 259

Figure 2. Four structural models of life course socioeconomic status on tooth loss: t1, childhood; t2, 1968/1974; t3, 1981/1991; t4, 2000/2010.

education as the lowest education (0) and university education then dichotomized with the median value. Childhood SES was
as the highest (4). Childhood financial hardship was measured categorized in lower, middle, and higher SES.
dichotomously (yes = 0, no = 1), and the father’s social class Social trajectories were created by combining 2 periods:
during the respondent’s childhood (first 16 y) was classified as childhood SES with 1 adulthood SES—the first from child-
manual worker (0) or nonmanual worker (1). hood to wave 1974, the second to wave 1991, and the third to
Adulthood SES was also created as a compound/latent wave 2010/2011. Having childhood SES and 1 adulthood SES
score for SEM. Two variables in each survey year were com- score, we generate 4 categories: 0 = persistently lower, 1 =
bined: the individual’s occupational social class and an downwardly mobile, 2 = upwardly mobile, and 3 = persistently
economic-based measure. The occupational social class, fol- higher.
lowing the official Swedish socioeconomic classification, was
divided into the following 4 groups: 0 = unskilled manual
workers, 1 = skilled manual workers–lower nonmanuals I Measurement Model: Latent Variables
(small farmers and self-employed people without employees), Our measurement model was composed of 3 latent oral health
2 = lower nonmanuals II (farmers with extensive land and/or variables and 4 latent SES variables (Fig. 1). The first oral
employees and self-employed people with 1 to 19 employees), health latent variable combined the 5-point scale from 1968
and 3 = higher nonmanuals (academic professionals and self- and 1974, while the second combined years 1981 and 1991 and
employed people with ≥20 employees). The economic mea- the third 2000/2002 and 2010/2011. Although oral health does
sure (cash margin) stemmed from the following question: “If a not fit the definition of a latent variable, this procedure reduced
situation suddenly arose where you had to come up with 2,000 collinearity and random error from self-reported outcome. The
SEK [Swedish crowns], could you manage it?” Response alter- childhood and adulthood latent variables were composed as
natives, in order of severity of economic hardship, were as fol- described earlier.
lows: 0 = no; 1 = yes—by bank loan, help from friends, or
relatives; 2 = yes—by myself or with help from family. The
amount of SEK has been adjusted for inflation since the first Structural Model
edition of the survey (1968), so it represents approximately the We tested 4 distinct socioeconomic life course models follow-
same amount of money each year. In 2011, the requested ing the original life course theory (Kuh et al. 2003). However,
amount was 14,000 SEK (US ~$1,600). given previous studies (Lennartsson et al. 2018; Vendrame
et al. 2018), we incorporated in all models chains of lagged
effects for repeated measures of disease and risk factors (Fig.
Social Trajectories
2). We named those models as follows: M1) sensitive period
Individual raw scores of adulthood and childhood SES were with late-life effect, M2) sensitive period with early- and late-
obtained by summing the variables in each period of life and life effects, M3) accumulation of risks with lagged effects, and
260 Journal of Dental Research 99(3)

Table 1. Incidence of Losing All or Almost All Teeth by Social Trajectories and Socioeconomic Status in Childhood in 2 Swedish Cohorts.

1968 to 1974 1981 to 1991 2000 to 2011

% n P Value % n P Value % n P Value

Trajectories from childhood to last survey year


Cohort: 1925 to 1934 (mean age) 38 to 44 y 51 to 61 y 70 to 81 y
Persistently lower 12.0 192 0.01 4.8 146 0.04 17.2 122 0.04
Downwardly mobile 12.7 55 7.8 64 12.5 56
Upwardly mobile 7.8 257 1.9 157 6.6 91
Persistently higher 3.7 241 1.2 162 6.3 95
Total 7.9 745 3.2 529 11.0 364
Cohort: 1944 to 1953 (mean age) 20 to 26 y 33 to 43 y 52 to 63 y
Persistently lower 3.1 322 0.01 0.4 244 >.99 2.8 215 0.36
Downwardly mobile 0.5 188 0.6 158 1.4 143
Upwardly mobile 0.8 260 0.4 236 1.5 134
Persistently higher 0.3 364 0.7 297 0.5 198
Total 1.2 1,134 0.5 935 1.6 690
Socioeconomic status in childhood
Cohort: 1925 to 1934 (mean age) 38 to 44 y 51 to 61 y 70 to 81 y
Lower 9.6 449 0.02 3.2 312 0.69 13.1 214 0.74
Middle 7.0 185 3.7 134 3.5 85
Higher 2.7 111 2.1 96 14.9 67
Total 7.9 745 3.1 542 11.2 366
Cohort: 1944 to 1953 (mean age) 20 to 26 y 33 to 43 y 52 to 63 y
Lower 2.1 583 0.02 0.4 487 0.86 2.3 349 0.06
Middle 0.4 272 0.9 234 1.8 166
Higher 0.4 280 0.4 234 0.0 175
Total 1.2 1,135 0.5 955 1.6 690

People with all teeth lost were removed from the baseline year. P values are for trend for socioeconomic status in childhood.

M4) accumulation of risks with cross-sectional effects. Age and 19.9 y of age. In the older cohort, 26.1% of the individuals
and sex were included as independent exogenous variables reported financial difficulties during childhood versus 9.1% in
with direct effects on baseline measures. the younger cohort. In the older and younger cohorts, 43.6%
and 34.9% had deceased before the last wave (2010/2011).
Among those still alive, there were missing data for 6.5% and
Statistical Analyses 7.4% of respondents in the older and younger cohorts.
For descriptive purposes, crude prevalence and incidence of Low childhood SES was associated with a greater incidence
losing all or almost all teeth are presented as percentages for of losing all or almost all teeth between any 2 consecutive sur-
childhood SES and social trajectories (Table 1 and Appendix vey years. However, the incidence was statistically significant
Table 1). We collapsed self-reported tooth conditions catego- only between the first and second waves (1968 to 1974) in both
ries 1 and 2 (score = 1) and categories 3, 4, and 5 (score = 0). cohorts (Table 1). Between the years 1968 and 1974, the group
Then, new cases of people with all or almost all teeth lost could of lower childhood SES for the older cohort (age, 38 to 44 y)
be identified between every 2 waves. had a 9.6% incidence of losing all or almost all teeth; the mid-
In conducting SEM, the first step was to produce an identi- dle childhood SES group, 7.0%; and the higher childhood SES
fied measurement model to test in a confirmatory factor analy- group, 2.7% (P = 0.04, chi-square test for trend). When this
sis (see Appendix Table 2). This model evaluates how latent cohort was followed between 1981 and 1991 (age, 51 to 61 y),
constructs perform in terms of measurement validity. We then the respective incidences of losing all or almost all teeth were
tested the fit of the structural models described earlier. 3.2%, 3.7%, and 2.1% (P = 0.69, chi-square test for trend), and
Due to loss of follow-up, we adopted a pairwise approach in when they were followed between 2000 and 2011 (age, 70 to
SEM with the Mplus 7.11 software, and other descriptive anal- 81 y), the incidences were 13.1%, 3.5%, and 14.9% (P = 0.74,
yses were performed with Stata 13.1 (StataCorp). chi-square test for trend). The prevalence of losing all or almost
all teeth was associated with childhood SES in all waves
(Appendix Table 1).
Results
There were no sex differences in childhood SES in the
The response rate at baseline (1968) was 93.3% and 96.8% for younger (P = 0.86) or older (P = 0.38) cohort. However, there
the older and younger cohorts with analytical samples of 923 was a significant association between the sexes in terms of life
and 1,195, respectively. At baseline, the older and younger course trajectories in both cohorts (P < 0.01). We found a
cohorts had 50% and 51% women and were on average 38.5 higher proportion of women among those persistently in lower
Socioeconomic Life Course Models and Oral Health 261

Table 2. Standardized Estimated Effects (Standardized Coefficients) of Life Course Structural Models in 2 Swedish Cohorts.

Cohort: 1925 to 1934 Cohort: 1944 to 1953

Outcome Variable: Independent Variable SPL SPEL AL AC SPL SPEL AL AC

Tooth loss in young/mid-adulthood


Female 0.04 0.04 0.01 −0.21 0.13 0.13 0.12 –0.02
Age in 1968 0.17 0.17 0.17 0.17 0.21 0.18 0.20 0.19
SES in childhood −0.48 −0.47 −0.48 −0.41
SES in young/mid-adulthood −0.41 −0.45
Tooth loss in mid-/late adulthood
Tooth loss in young/mid-adulthood 0.94 0.91 0.91 0.93 0.88 0.94 0.82 0.94
SES in childhood −0.09 0.12
SES in young/mid-adulthood −0.11 −0.03
SES in mid-/late adulthood −0.05 0.13
Tooth loss in late adulthood/life
Tooth loss in mid-/late adulthood 0.86 0.87 0.87 0.88 0.79 0.76 0.77 0.77
SES in childhood −0.50 −0.06 −0.35 −0.10
SES in mid-/late adulthood −0.06 −0.10
SES in late adulthood/life −0.06 −0.10
SES childhood
Age in 1968 −0.02 0.02 0.02 −0.03 −0.04 −0.05 −0.05 −0.06
SES in young/mid-adulthood
SES in childhood 0.59 0.71 0.66 0.53 0.59 0.67 0.65 0.57
Female −0.56 −0.56 −0.55 −0.56 −0.35 −0.36 −0.36 −0.36
SES in mid-/late adulthood
SES in young/mid-adulthood 0.99 0.96 0.96 0.96 0.91 0.88 0.89 0.87
SES in late adulthood/life
SES in mid-/late adulthood 0.97 0.97 0.97 0.97 0.90 0.89 0.88 0.89
Fit indices
CFI 0.94 0.98 0.98 0.98 0.92 0.95 0.95 0.96
TLI 0.93 0.97 0.97 0.98 0.91 0.94 0.94 0.95
RMSEA 0.08 0.05 0.05 0.04 0.07 0.06 0.05 0.05
WRMR 2.68 1.62 1.60 1.51 2.35 1.80 1.79 1.68

Bold indicates statistical significance at P < 0.05.


AC, accumulation: with cross-sectional effects; AL, accumulation: with lagged effects; CFI, comparative fit index; RMSEA, root mean square error of
approximation; SES, socioeconomic status; SPE, sensitive period: early- and late-life effects; SPL, sensitive period: late-life effects; TLI, Tucker-Lewis
Index; WRMR, weighted root mean square residual.

SES or downward mobility in the younger cohort (54.9% and similar patterns in both cohorts. SES in early life and SES in
54.6%, respectively) and the older cohort (60.6% and 59.4%). young to mid-adulthood had stronger effects than SES in late
The group with persistently higher SES and the upward mobil- adulthood and SES in late life.
ity category consisted of only 44% women in both cohorts.
In both cohorts, accumulation with cross-sectional effects
(M4) was the model with the best fit indices (Table 2). For the Discussion
older cohort, the values of root mean square error of approxi- Our results showed that both versions of the accumulation
mation, comparative fit index, Tucker-Lewis index, and model (cross-sectional and lagged) presented good statistical
weighted root mean square residual were 0.05, 0.98, 0.97, and fit, but the lagged-effect model might be theoretically more
1.62, respectively. In the period 1968 to 1974, the coefficient plausible. The sensitive period with early- and late-life effect
(β) linking SES to tooth loss was −0.41 (P < 0.01); the period cannot be completely ruled out, and future studies may test
1981 to 1991, −0.05 (P = 0.08); and the period 2000 to 2010, combinations of models. In fact, it seems likely that the models
−0.06 (P = 0.11). The sensitive period with late-life effects play a role simultaneously. Although SES has a cumulative
(M1) showed an unacceptable model fit, which improved with effect on oral diseases over the life course, its strongest effect
the addition of early-life effects (M2). All effects of childhood was observed early in life, creating a socioeconomic gap that
SES on early- and late-life tooth loss were mediated by adult- seems to be pervasive throughout the life course (Celeste and
hood SES. For models M3 and M4, no plausible modification Fritzell 2018). This confirms other studies in which the sensi-
indices were suggested by the software to improve the model, tive period and accumulation models were regarded as accept-
so no additional associations were added. able models (Poulton et al. 2002; Pearce et al. 2009; Peres et al.
Direct, indirect, and total effects of each SES measure on 2011; Han and Khang 2017; Vendrame et al. 2018); however, it
tooth loss in the last wave are presented in Table 3, showing refutes the hypothesis that early-life SES has a direct effect late
262 Journal of Dental Research 99(3)

Table 3. Standardized Coefficients for Total, Direct, and Indirect Effects of SES over 4 Periods of the Life Course on Tooth Loss in 2 Swedish
Cohorts.

Cohort: 1925 to 1934 Cohort: 1944 to 1953

Outcome: Tooth Loss in Late Adulthood/Life Total Effects Direct Effects Indirect Effects Total Effects Direct Effects Indirect Effects

M1: Sensitive period I—late-life effects


SES in childhood −0.50 −0.50 −0.35 −0.35
M2: Sensitive period II—early- and late-life effects
SES in childhood −0.51 −0.06 −0.45 −0.35 −0.10 −0.25
M3: Accumulation of risks—with lagged effects
SES in childhood −0.47 −0.47 −0.33 −0.33
SES in young/mid-adulthood −0.15 −0.15 −0.10 −0.10
SES in mid-/late adulthood −0.06 −0.06 −0.10 −0.10
M4: Accumulation of risks—with cross-sectional effects
SES in childhood −0.23 −0.23 −0.18 −0.18
SES in young/mid-adulthood −0.43 −0.43 −0.32 −0.32
SES in mid-/late adulthood −0.10 −0.10 0.01 0.01
SES in late adulthood/life −0.06 −0.06 −0.10 −0.10

Bold indicates statistical significance at P < 0.05.


SES, socioeconomic status.

in life. Unique to our study is that we managed to compare health leading to lower SES) was not incorporated into the life
models with repeated data for the exposure and outcome. course models; additionally, as with any observational study, it
Because of this, the restrictive sensitive period model (M1) did cannot ensure causality, only temporality. There is some evi-
not have a good fit. M2 allowed for childhood SES to have dence of reverse causation between psychological distress and
effects early and late in life, and it showed that childhood income (Darin-Mattsson et al. 2018), but such an issue seems
effects on oral health later in life were mainly indirect. less likely in terms of oral health. Finally, the use of retrospec-
In all models, a chain of risks had to be incorporated to tive childhood data suffers from recall bias, despite the fact that
yield acceptable fits. This highlights the importance of a good parental education is rather objective information, and eco-
start in life because the effect of lower SES in childhood on nomic hardship was collected when participants were young
later-life oral health was mainly mediated through adulthood (34 to 43 y and 15 to 24 y). A strength is that our analyses were
SES. This confirms studies that showed a strong effect of chain based on a large and representative sample of the general popu-
of risks in Sweden (Darin-Mattsson et al. 2018; Lennartsson lation with, as far as we are aware, the longest follow-up period
et al. 2018) and other countries (Vendrame et al. 2018). In soci- with oral health data and with high response rates. Therefore,
ology, such a chain has been described as the Matthew effect we were able to reduce sampling bias and test life course mod-
(Merton 1988), or the accumulation of disadvantage. Two els appropriately. These models were originally proposed for
strong chains were described in this study: 1) SES early in life long-latency chronic diseases, while dental caries affects people
leads to a similar SES later in life, and 2) oral health status at younger ages and has shorter latency repeatedly over time.
early in life leads to similar oral health later in life. While it is Our findings show that SES affects oral health cumulatively
difficult to revert the oral health indicator, which is based on over the life course, and they highlight the importance of child-
tooth loss, it is not impossible to break the SES chain. Our SES hood SES as an indirect factor for oral health later in life. A
score in adult life did not include education, because this var- good start in life seems important to prevent people from
ied very little during adulthood, but parents’ education was becoming trapped in a chain of risks; nonetheless, preventive
included in childhood SES. If parents’ education can be con- action in all periods of life might improve oral health and
sidered a good proxy for children’s early-life education, then decrease socioeconomic inequalities in oral health.
this shows the importance of investments in this issue. Indeed,
a randomized intervention mainly based on education in very Author Contributions
early life showed important effects in preventing several R.K. Celeste, contributed to conception, design, data analysis, and
important diseases (Campbell et al. 2014). interpretation, drafted the manuscript; H.S. Eyjólfsdóttir, contrib-
The strengths and limitations of this study should be taken uted to conception, data acquisition, and interpretation, critically
into account when interpreting the results. First, a limitation is revised the manuscript; C. Lennartsson, contributed to conception,
the self-reported measures of oral health. Although they have data acquisition, and analysis, critically revised the manuscript;
been validated (Pitiphat et al. 2002), there is some measurement J. Fritzell, contributed to conception, design, data acquisition,
error. To partially account for this, oral health outcomes were analysis, and interpretation, critically revised the manuscript. All
considered as latent variables representing major periods of life. authors gave final approval and agree to be accountable for all
Second, in spite of being plausible, reverse causation (poor oral aspects of the work.
Socioeconomic Life Course Models and Oral Health 263

Acknowledgments Kuh D, Ben-Shlomo Y, Lynch J, Hallqvist J, Power C. 2003. Life course epide-
miology. J Epidemiol Community Health. 57(10):778–783.
This work was supported by grants from FORTE (2012:1950 and Lennartsson C, Agahi N, Hols-Salén L, Kelfve S, Kåreholt I, Lundberg O,
2016-07206) and NordForsk (74637). R.K.C. holds a PQ-2 Parker MG, Thorslund M. 2014. Data resource profile: the Swedish Panel
Study of Living Conditions of the Oldest Old (SWEOLD). Int J Epidemiol.
Fellowship from CNPq (310978/2016-5). The authors declare no 43(3):731–738.
potential conflicts of interest with respect to the authorship and/or Lennartsson C, Eyjólfsdóttir HS, Celeste RK, Fritzell J. 2018. Social class and
publication of this article. infirmity: the role of social class over the life-course. SSM Popul Health.
4:169–177.
Listl S, Broadbent JM, Thomson WM, Stock C, Shen J, Steele J, Wildman J,
ORCID iD Heilmann A, Watt RG, Tsakos G, et al. 2018. Childhood socioeconomic
conditions and teeth in older adulthood: evidence from SHARE wave 5.
R.K. Celeste https://orcid.org/0000-0002-2468-6655 Community Dent Oral Epidemiol. 46(1):78–87.
Listl S, Watt RG, Tsakos G. 2014. Early life conditions, adverse life events
and chewing ability at middle and later adulthood. Am J Public Health.
References 104(5):e55–e61.
Astrom AN, Ekback G, Lie SA, Ordell S. 2015. Life-course social influences on Lu HX, Wong MCM, Lo ECM, McGrath C. 2011. Trends in oral health from
tooth loss and oral attitudes among older people: evidence from a prospec- childhood to early adulthood: a life course approach. Community Dent Oral
tive cohort study. Eur J Oral Sci. 123(1):30–38. Epidemiol. 39(4):352–360.
Baker SR, Gibson BG. 2014. Social oral epidemi(olog)2y where next: one small Merton RK. 1988. The Matthew effect in science, II: cumulative advantage and
step or one giant leap? Community Dent Oral Epidemiol. 42(6):481–494. the symbolism of intellectual property. Isis. 79(4):606–623.
Bernabé E, Suominen AL, Nordblad A, Vehkalahti MM, Hausen H, Knuuttila Niedzwiedz CL, Katikireddi SV, Pell JP, Mitchell R. 2012. Life course socio-
M, Kivimäki M, Watt RG, Sheiham A, Tsakos G. 2011. Education level economic position and quality of life in adulthood: a systematic review of
and oral health in Finnish adults: evidence from different lifecourse models. life course models. BMC Public Health. 12:628.
J Clin Periodontol. 38(1):25–32. Pearce MS, Steele JG, Mason J, Walls AWG, Parker L. 2004. Do circum-
Bernabé E, Watt RG, Sheiham A, Suominen AL, Vehkalahti MM, Nordblad A, stances in early life contribute to tooth retention in middle age? J Dent Res.
Uutela A, Kivimäki M, Tsakos G. 2012. Childhood socioeconomic posi- 83(7):562–566.
tion, adult sense of coherence and tooth retention. Community Dent Oral Pearce MS, Thomson WM, Walls AWG, Steele JG. 2009. Lifecourse socio-
Epidemiol. 40(1):46–52. economic mobility and oral health in middle age. J Dent Res. 88(10):938–
Broadbent JM, Zeng J, Foster Page LA, Baker SR, Ramrakha S, Thomson WM. 941.
2016. Oral health–related beliefs, behaviors, and outcomes through the life Peres KG, Peres MA, Araujo CL, Menezes AM, Hallal PC. 2009. Social and
course. J Dent Res. 95(7):808–813. dental status along the life course and oral health impacts in adolescents: a
Campbell F, Conti G, Heckman JJ, Moon SH, Pinto R, Pungello E, Pan Y. population-based birth cohort. Health Qual Life Outcomes. 7:95.
2014. Early childhood investments substantially boost adult health. Science. Peres MA, Peres KG, Thomson WM, Broadbent JM, Gigante DP, Horta BL.
343(6178):1478–1485. 2011. The influence of family income trajectories from birth to adulthood
Celeste RK, Fritzell J. 2018. Do socioeconomic inequalities in pain, psycho- on adult oral health: findings from the 1982 Pelotas birth cohort. Am J
logical distress and oral health increase or decrease over the life course? Public Health. 101(4):730–736.
Evidence from Sweden over 43 years of follow-up. J Epidemiol Community Pitiphat W, Garcia RI, Douglass CW, Joshipura KJ. 2002. Validation of self-
Health. 72(2):160–167. reported oral health measures. J Public Health Dent. 62(2):122–128.
Darin-Mattsson A, Andel R, Celeste RK, Kåreholt I. 2018. Linking financial Pollitt RA, Rose KM, Kaufman JS. 2005. Evaluating the evidence for models of
hardship throughout the life-course with psychological distress in old age: life course socioeconomic factors and cardiovascular outcomes: a system-
sensitive period, accumulation of risks, and chain of risks hypotheses. Soc atic review. BMC Public Health. 5:7.
Sci Med. 201:111–119. Poulton R, Caspi A, Milne BJ, Thomson WM, Taylor A, Sears MR, Moffitt TE.
Delgado-Angulo EK, Bernabe E. 2015. Intergenerational mobility and 2002. Association between children’s experience of socioeconomic dis-
adult oral health in a British cohort. Community Dent Oral Epidemiol. advantage and adult health: a life-course study. Lancet. 360(9346):1640–
43(3):255–261. 1645.
De Stavola BL, Daniel RM, Ploubidis GB, Micali N. 2015. Mediation analy- Righolt AJ, Jevdjevic M, Marcenes W, Listl S. 2018. Global-, regional-, and
sis with intermediate confounding: structural equation modeling viewed country-level economic impacts of dental diseases in 2015. J Dent Res.
through the causal inference lens. Am J Epidemiol. 181(1):64–80. 97(5):501–507.
Fantin R, Delpierre C, Kelly-Irving M, Barboza Solís C. 2018. Early socio- Schuch HS, Peres KG, Singh A, Peres MA, Do LG. 2017. Socioeconomic
economic conditions and severe tooth loss in middle-aged Costa Ricans. position during life and periodontitis in adulthood: a systematic review.
Community Dent Oral Epidemiol. 46(2):178–184. Community Dent Oral Epidemiol. 45(3):201–208.
Fritzell J, Lennartsson C, Lundberg O. 2007. Health inequalities and welfare Schwendicke F, Dörfer CE, Schlattmann P, Page LF, Thomson WM, Paris S.
resources: findings and forecasts. In: Fritzell J, Lundberg O, editors. Health 2015. Socioeconomic inequality and caries: a systematic review and meta-
inequalities and welfare resources: continuity and change in Sweden. analysis. J Dent Res. 94(1):10–18.
Bristol (UK): The Policy Press. p. 199–207. Seerig LM, Nascimento GG, Peres MA, Horta BL, Demarco FF. 2015. Tooth
Gülcan F, Ekbäck G, Ordell S, Lie SA, Åstrøm AN. 2015. Inequality in oral loss in adults and income: systematic review and meta-analysis. J Dent.
health related to early and later life social conditions: a study of elderly in 43(9):1051–1059.
Norway and Sweden. BMC Oral Health. 15:20. Tsakos G, Demakakos P, Breeze E, Watt RG. 2011. Social gradients in oral
Han D-H, Khang Y-H. 2017. Lifecourse socioeconomic position indica- health in older adults: findings from the English longitudinal survey of
tors and tooth loss in Korean adults. Community Dent Oral Epidemiol. aging. Am J Public Health. 101(10):1892–1899.
45(1):74–83. Vendrame É, Goulart M de A, Hilgert JB, Hugo FN, Celeste RK. 2018.
Kassebaum NJ, Bernabe E, Dahiya M, Bhandari B, Murray CJ, Marcenes W. Decomposing early and adult life social position effects on oral health and
2015. Global burden of untreated caries: a systematic review and metare- chronic diseases in a cross-sectional study of Southern Brazil. Community
gression. J Dent Res. 94(5):650–658. Dent Oral Epidemiol. 46(6):601–607.

You might also like