Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 13

Journal of Retailing 87 (1, 2011) 46–58

Aesthetics and the online shopping environment: Understanding


consumer responses
Yong Jian Wang a,∗ , Michael S. Minor b,1 , Jie Wei c,2
a College of Business, Ohio University, Copeland Hall 534, Athens, OH 45701, United States
b College of Business Administration, The University of Texas-Pan American, 1201 West University Drive, Edinburg, TX 78539, United States
c NUS Business School, National University of Singapore, 1 Business Link Level 6 BIZ 2 Building, Singapore 117592, Singapore

Abstract
As the e-tail industry continues to grow with increasing competition witnessed, the study of how to entice and retain online customers has never
been as important as at present. Our hierarchical model examines how the two dimensions of web aesthetics, aesthetic formality and aesthetic appeal,
influence online consumers’ psychological reactions, including perceived service quality, satisfaction, and arousal, and how these psychological
changes, in turn, influence online consumers’ conative tendencies. The results indicate: (1) consumers’ cognitive, affective, and conative outcomes
can be significantly evoked by aesthetic stimuli; (2) the two dimensions of web aesthetics exhibit dissimilar patterns of influences; and (3) purchase
task significantly moderates consumers’ responses in terms of magnitude and direction. The study provides practical guidelines for properly
manipulating the two dimensions of web aesthetics based on consumers’ motivational orientations.
© 2010 New York University. Published by Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Keywords: e-tail; Web aesthetics; Satisfaction; Online service quality; Online consumer behavior; Environmental psychology

Introduction because of the stimulation effect of interesting websites (Ganesh


et al. 2010). Given the fact that the website is the primary
As part of the e-tail strategy, the issue of e-store design has interface for an e-tailer during consumers’ service encounter,
emerged as a major issue in online retailing. As Grewal and consumers may not be interested in continuing their online nav-
Levy (2007) pointed out, the impact of web design on online igation if they dislike the interface, although this e-tailer can
consumer behavior is a potential area in retailing research due actually offer benefits such as cheaper merchandise, analyti-
to the lack of in-depth investigation. Currently, many e-tailers cal assistance, and faster delivery. If we realize the degree to
adopted advanced web technologies, such as customized solu- which shoppers care about the shopping environment and how
tions and computer-aided analytical tools, to improve online the store atmospherics can significantly influence purchase deci-
service in order to win the ultimate competition. However, from a sions (e.g., Bitner 1992; Donovan et al. 1994; Spies, Hesse, and
marketing perspective, offering all the functional attributes may Loesch 1997), we believe that how to create an aesthetic website
not be enough for the creation of a consumer-oriented online that can let consumers enjoy their online shopping environment
shopping environment. Past research in the e-tail field has rec- is a critical research question in retailing. In this study, we focus
ognized the importance of attractive web design in enhancing on such an aesthetic approach to investigating e-tail website
shoppers’ online experience (Ganesh et al. 2010; Wolfinbarger design and consumer responses.
and Gilly 2003). Noticeably, recent findings revealed that many According to the American Heritage Dictionary of English
web surfers are motivated to conduct online shopping activities Language, aesthetics is “a conception of what is artistically valid
or beautiful”. The concept of aesthetics is also referred to as “the
philosophy of beauty” in the literature of arts (Dickie 1997). In
an online context, web aesthetics may represent how different
∗ Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 740 597 1987; fax: +1 740 597 2150. elements and attributes are combined to yield an impression of
E-mail addresses: wangy@ohio.edu (Y.J. Wang),
beauty.
msminor@hotmail.com (M.S. Minor), jwei@nus.edu.sg (J. Wei).
1 Tel.: +1 956 316 7135; fax: +1 956 381 2867. There are at least three reasons for the investigation of con-
2 Tel.: +65 6516 4418; fax: +65 6777 1296. sumer responses to web aesthetics. First, previous marketing

0022-4359/$ – see front matter © 2010 New York University. Published by Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.jretai.2010.09.002
Y.J. Wang et al. / Journal of Retailing 87 (1, 2011) 46–58 47

research has recognized the fundamental trend of consumers’ Stimulus Organism Response
online consumption style. The web is not merely used for util-
itarian purposes, such as looking for information and cheaper Affective:
Conative:
merchandise; it is increasingly becoming a place for recreational Perceived
Web
and entertaining experiences that can fulfill consumers’ hedo- Aesthetics: Satisfaction
Purchase
nic needs (Childers et al. 2001; Ganesh et al. 2010; Hartman Purchase Purchase
Task Task
et al. 2006). Thus, an artistically beautiful website that pleases Aesthetic
Formality
eyes may be considered superior to other less-appealing sites. Arousal Consultation
Investigation of web aesthetics will contribute to a deeper Aesthetic
inquiry into the hedonic needs of online consumers. Second, Appeal
Search on
a parsimonious measure of overall atmospherics has not been Other
well discussed, although previous studies suggested a signifi- Cognitive:
Websites
cant influence of overall atmospherics on consumers’ shopping Online
behavior (e.g., Donovan et al. 1994; Eroglu, Machleit, and Davis Service
Re-visit
Quality
2001; Hui, Dube, and Chebat 1997; Puccinelli et al. 2009).
Since atmospheric stimuli consist of a variety of elements, a
study of consumer responses to web aesthetics should examine Fig. 1. Research framework of consumer responses to web aesthetics.
whether and how different web elements, attributes, and tools, in
combination, impact consumers’ psychological and behavioral
reactions. Going beyond the investigation of the functionality of
individual atmospheric stimulus, web aesthetics offers a holistic site, and is akin to the aesthetic formality dimension, while
picture on the online shopping environment. Third, intellectual expressive aesthetics refers to the creativeness, fascination,
knowledge on web aesthetics has been limited to informa- and originality of a website, and corresponds to the aesthetic
tion system users, lacking application to consumers. Previous appeal dimension. The findings indicate that one dimension
research on human–computer interaction has generated substan- of web aesthetics is related to the utilitarian, economic, and
tial findings on the dimensionality, consistency, and fidelity of practical properties of design, whereas the other dimension is
web aesthetics (e.g., Park, Choi, and Kim 2004; Schenkman and reflected by the hedonic, attractive, and recreational attributes of
Jonsson 2000; Tractinsky 2004; Tractinsky et al. 2006). But the design.
significance of web aesthetics to online consumers and the role Based on the Stimulus-Organism-Response (S-O-R) frame-
of web aesthetics in consumers’ purchase/repurchase intentions, work (Mehrabian and Russell 1974), this study proposes a
loyalty behavior, and service switching behavior remain unclear. research framework to examine online consumers’ hierarchi-
Thus, research efforts on consumer responses to web aesthetics cal responses to web aesthetics. Taking into consideration the
seem essential. moderating role of purchase task, this study attempts to exam-
The purpose of the study is to investigate online consumers’ ine how the two dimensions of web aesthetics influence online
cognitive, affective, and conative responses to web aesthetics, consumers’ affective changes (satisfaction and arousal) and cog-
taking into consideration consumers’ motivational orientations. nitive reaction (perceived online service quality); and further,
After the introduction, the study focuses on the development of how these psychological changes, in turn, influence online con-
a research framework with key research hypotheses. The study sumers’ conative inclinations, including purchase, consultation,
continues with research methodology and analysis of results. search, and re-visit. The research framework is presented in
On the basis of the results, discussion of findings, research Fig. 1.
limitations, and recommendations for future research are Since consumer satisfaction is demonstrated by positive
provided. emotions (pleasure) during a disconfirmation of expectations
(Dawson, Bloch, and Ridgway 1990; Spreng, MacKenzie, and
Research framework and hypotheses Olshavsky 1996), we turn to the appraisal theory of emo-
tions to explain the effects of aesthetic formality and aesthetic
Previous research posited a bi-dimensional character of web appeal on satisfaction under task-oriented and task-free cir-
aesthetics. Schenkman and Jonsson (2000) revealed that visual cumstances. According to appraisal theory of emotions (Frijda
aesthetics of websites has two dimensions, aesthetic formality 1994), positive emotions can be a result of the match between
and aesthetic appeal. Aesthetic formality refers to the order, environmental stimuli and the goal of an individual. A mis-
legibility, and simplicity of a website, while aesthetic appeal match may result in negative emotions. For example, a dark
refers to the overall impressiveness of a website, which is environment can evoke positive emotions when an individ-
closely tied to the hedonic quality of the website. Consistent ual wants to sleep and negative emotions when he or she
with the findings of Schenkman and Jonsson (2000), Lavie wants to read. The theory also contends that in other condi-
and Tractinsky (2004) found two similar dimensions of web tions in which an individual does not possess a goal, positive
aesthetics. These two dimensions were respectively named clas- emotions are a result of the hedonic property of environ-
sical aesthetics and expressive aesthetics. Classical aesthetics mental stimuli, which are intrinsically preferred by human
refers to the organization, clearness, and symmetricity of a web- beings.
48 Y.J. Wang et al. / Journal of Retailing 87 (1, 2011) 46–58

The aesthetic formality dimension contains the patterns in because their experience is enriched by the highly appealing
which the details of information are presented in the online shop- atmospherics.
ping environment. The structure of information is related to the
H1a. Regardless of purchase tasks are pursued or not, aesthetic
distribution of attribute levels across the products listed (Lurie
formality of websites increases satisfaction.
2004). The organization, legibility, and clearness of the task-
relevant content determine the readability of the websites (Hall H1b. When purchase tasks are pursued (vs. not pursued), aes-
and Hanna 2004), and are directly relevant to the achievement of thetic appeal of websites decreases (vs. increases) satisfaction.
the online shopping goals (Eroglu et al. 2001). Since consumers
Berlyne’s (1960) behaviorism paradigm has been used to
with online purchase tasks have to exploit online information
describe the appreciators’ arousal in response to a visual work
about products and services to complete the purchase task in
of art. An increase in arousal results from the visual stimuli that
a timely manner, a well-organized, legible, and easy-to-follow
provoke temporary conflict in the memory. To quantitatively
web environment facilitates consumers’ information processing
explain arousal, Mehrabian and Russell (1974) posited that the
and goal-attainment. In light of the appraisal theory of emotions,
amount of arousal in an environment is positively related to the
a match between high aesthetic formality and the purchase task
information rate received by an individual, and the information
results in consumer satisfaction. Congruent with the argument,
rate is higher when the object presented is novel, surprising, or
Szymanski and Hise (2000) found that online consumers with
complex. Since higher complexity indicates lower aesthetic for-
a purchase task are more satisfied with a website if the website
mality of a website and higher novelty reflects higher aesthetic
is more organized. On the other hand, when online consumers
appeal, higher arousal tends to be generated in response to an e-
browse an e-tail website without any purchase task, the pres-
tail website with lower aesthetic formality and higher aesthetic
ence of an organized, legible, and easy-to-follow website may
appeal.
also lead to consumers’ positive emotions because the prefer-
ence for order and certainty can be attributed to intrinsic human H2a. Regardless of whether purchase tasks are pursued or not,
needs (Maslow 1970). Recent empirical studies on the online aesthetic formality of websites decreases arousal.
environment also support a positive effect of aesthetic formal-
H2b. Regardless of whether purchase tasks are pursued or not,
ity on the satisfaction by revealing that perceived complexity
aesthetic appeal of websites increases arousal.
of a website has a negative impact on pleasure (e.g., Huang
2003; Vilnai-Yavetz and Rafaeli 2006). Thus, whether online A large number of empirical studies in marketing over-
consumers pursue purchase tasks or not, a positive effect of a whelmingly supported a positive relationship between positive
website’s aesthetic formality on online consumers’ satisfaction emotion and purchase in both store and online environments
can be expected. (e.g., Donovan et al. 1994; Menon and Kahn 2002; Sherman,
In view of the appraisal theory of emotions, higher aes- Mathur, and Smith 1997; Spies et al. 1997). Remarkably,
thetic appeal of a website, the hedonic property of the website, the positive emotions elicited by atmospheric stimuli may
may result in higher pleasure for those who browse the website lead to unplanned purchase by consumers who do not pur-
with no purchase task, because art and aesthetic advancement sue any purchase task at the time of service encounter (Beatty
can be linked to intrinsic human preference (Maslow 1970). In and Ferrell 1998). Therefore, we expect that, whether con-
Lindgaard and Dudek’s (2003) study, satisfaction of online view- sumers pursue purchase tasks or not, satisfaction with web
ers comes from highly appealing websites, regardless of whether aesthetics will have a positive impact on the propensity to
the websites are useful to them. By recruiting participants with- purchase.
out a concrete online task, Van der Heijden (2003) also found In both store and online environments, emotional changes
that online visual attractiveness, which is defined as the degree have been found to influence consumers’ actions other than
to which a person believes that a website is aesthetically appeal- purchase. These actions include communicating and interacting
ing to the eyes, is positively associated with users’ enjoyment. with other consumers, browsing related sites and product cat-
However, in the study of store atmosphere, Kaltcheva and Weitz egories, and participating in promotion activities (Menon and
(2006) suggested that atmospheric stimuli that impede the shop- Kahn 2002; Ridgway, Dawson, and Bloch 1989). Perceptual
ping goal evoke negative emotions. In Kaltcheva and Weitz’s control theory (Hershberger 1989) helps to explain how con-
(2006) study, a store’s appealing atmospherics, such as satu- sumer satisfaction with an environment influences their conative
rated color or music, decrease the pleasure of shoppers engaged tendencies under different motivational orientations. The expla-
in completing their shopping tasks, though these appealing ele- nation of goal-directed volitional behavior may describe the
ments increase the pleasure of recreational shoppers. In view conative change of consumers who pursue purchase tasks during
of the appraisal theory of emotions, the negative emotions of the encounter with environmental stimuli (e.g., web aesthet-
shoppers result from the mismatch of the stimuli and their shop- ics). Perceptual control theory posits that, when an individual
ping goal, because the appealing atmospherics are irrelevant starts pursuing a goal, influential environmental disturbances
to the shopping task and distract information processing and may provide negative feedback, which subsequently produces
decision making. Previous findings lead us to expect that task- a new round of actions to better adapt to the environment.
oriented consumers may feel unpleasant because the appealing Overall, negative feedback from the environment leads an indi-
atmospherics require a higher degree of effort to complete the vidual to adjust his or her volitional behavior at times. Thus,
shopping tasks, while task-free consumers may feel pleasant if the environment does not provide any negative feedback,
Y.J. Wang et al. / Journal of Retailing 87 (1, 2011) 46–58 49

consumers with purchase tasks will continue their purchase processing. Mehrabian and Russell (1974) also reported that
tasks until the goal is achieved. Consumers will take alterna- higher arousal weakens concentration. In their experiments, high
tive actions that counterbalance the environmental stimuli only arousal elicited by environmental stimuli significantly decreased
if negative feedback is provided. The explanation of volitional the participants’ desire to work or to solve problems. Thus, we
behavior implies that consumers who pursue purchase tasks expect that, when arousal increases, online consumers who pur-
tend to explore for alternatives or additional information only sue no purchase task will have a higher tendency to search and
if negative emotions are evoked based on the negative feedback buy because of bodily activation, while those who need to com-
from the website. Therefore, when task-oriented online con- plete their purchase tasks will experience difficulty in continuing
sumers feel dissatisfied with the shopping environment, they the central purchase tasks because of the need for additional
will be likely to consult customer service or search on other information.
websites.
H4a. When purchase tasks are pursued, arousal by web aes-
When there is no purchase task in mind, consumers’ cona-
thetics decreases the propensity to purchase, and increases the
tive reactions to web aesthetics can be characterized as “elicited
propensities to consult customer service and/or search on other
output” described by perceptual control theory (Hershberger
websites.
1989). In this circumstance, online consumers’ positive emo-
tions evoked by the aesthetic stimuli may initiate fun- and H4b. When purchase tasks are pursued, arousal by web aes-
variety-seeking behavior, such as search on the web. Also, pos- thetics increases the propensities to purchase, consult customer
itive emotions may lead to a higher tendency to interact with service, and/or search on other websites.
customer service. This is consistent with previous findings that
Web aesthetics has been considered an important element
the positive emotions elicited by atmospheric stimuli increases
of online service quality, and found to correlate with other
further exploration and interaction when recreational consumers
elements, such as ease of use and perceived security (Yoo
encounter a novel or hedonic environment (e.g., Menon and
and Donthu 2001). In information systems research, Lavie and
Kahn 2002; Ridgway et al. 1989).
Tractinsky (2004) found that both of the two dimensions of
H3a. When purchase tasks are pursued, satisfaction with web web aesthetics are positively associated with perceived website
aesthetics increases the propensity to purchase, and decreases usability, the quality of websites perceived by users. The find-
the propensities to consult customer service and/or search on ing implies that aesthetic formality and aesthetic appeal may
other websites. both have positive impacts on perceived online service quality.
Further, previous research highlighted the impact of perceived
H3b. When purchase tasks are not pursued, satisfaction with service quality on consumers’ patronage (repeated re-visit) of
web aesthetics increases the propensities to purchase, consult specific stores (e.g., Pan and Zinkhan 2006; Sirohi, McLaughlin,
customer service and/or search on other websites. and Wittink 1998; Zeithaml, Berry, and Parasuraman 1996). In
addition, Baker et al. (2002) and Grewal et al. (2003) showed
Previous marketing studies showed that consumers’ purchase
that consumers’ evaluations of the retail atmosphere is crucial
tendency is not necessarily high when they are aroused. A num-
in determining their re-visit intentions, even after they visit the
ber of studies found that the effect of arousal on purchase is
store for recreational purposes. We extend the paradigm to the
positive (e.g., Baker, Levy, and Grewal 1992; Donovan and
web and propose:
Rossiter 1982; Sherman et al. 1997), but some others sup-
ported an opposite direction of the effect (e.g., Menon and Kahn H5a. Aesthetic formality and aesthetic appeal of websites both
2002; Milliman 1982). Kaltcheva and Weitz (2006) suggested increase perceived online service quality.
that purchase task moderates the arousal-behavior relationship.
H5b. Perceived online service quality based on web aesthetics
They found that higher arousal decreases purchase intentions
increases the propensity to re-visit a website.
of task-oriented shoppers, but increases purchase intentions of
recreational shoppers. Involvement theory contends that consumers with a pur-
An aroused state can be demonstrated as mental excitement, chase task tend to have higher involvement with utilitarian
which activates muscular activities and accelerates bodily move- information of products and services because of personal rele-
ments (Mehrabian and Russell 1974). Thus, higher arousal will vance (Zaichkowsky 1985). On the other hand, when consumers
generally make individuals more active. But it is noteworthy encounter a shopping environment without a purchase task, their
that high arousal is closely interrelated with the feelings of anx- involvement is more hedonically oriented (Laurent and Kapferer
iety and stress during critical decision making (Mehrabian and 1985). Higher involvement increases interest in the involved
Russell 1974). High anxiety and stress during information pro- fields and a greater effort to acquire information (Celsi and
cessing and decision making can lead to alternative-seeking Olson 1988). More importantly, consumers’ motivational ori-
behavior, rather than focusing on deliberation and decision entations determine their visual attention patterns. According to
making (Pham 1996; Sanbonmatsu and Kardes 1988). As a visual search theory (Theeuwes 1992), visual selection during
result, high arousal may lead to the suspension of the cen- task involvement is goal-driven, in which individuals attend to
tral task. Sanbonmatsu and Kardes (1988) found that highly and process utilitarian information to achieve the goals, whereas
aroused individuals in these situations tend to look for periph- visual selection without a task is stimulus-driven, with attention
eral cues rather than to concentrate on central information and information processing focusing on the hedonic proper-
50 Y.J. Wang et al. / Journal of Retailing 87 (1, 2011) 46–58

ties in the surroundings. Aesthetic formality helps to determine propensity to measure conation has been widely adopted by
whether product and service information can be effectively and previous marketing research (e.g., Dabholkar, Shepherd, and
efficiently used to facilitate the purchase tasks, and thus, may Thorpe 2000; Zeithaml et al. 1996). In this study, each of
have a stronger effect on the cognitive, affective, and conative the behavioral variables is measured by three items. One of
responses when consumers pursue purchase tasks. On the other the items is assessed by a seven-point Likert-type scale and
hand, aesthetic appeal, often demonstrated by decorative and the other two are based on seven-point semantic differential
hedonic features through colors, graphics, and images, helps scales.
to establish the hedonic quality of a website, and thus, may
have a stronger effect on the cognitive, affective, and cona- Quasi-experimental procedures
tive responses when consumers are task-free. Accordingly, we
propose: A 2 × 2 factorial design was employed in the quasi-
experiment. Four e-tail websites, online furniture stores A, B,
H6. When purchase tasks are pursued (vs. not pursued), C, and D, were created. The four websites offered identical
aesthetic formality has a stronger (vs. weaker) influence on con- products as well as customer services. The four e-tail stores
sumers’ cognitive, affective, and conative responses than does were designed into high formality/high appeal (online store A),
aesthetic appeal. low formality/high appeal (online store B), high formality/low
appeal (online store C), and low formality/low appeal (online
Research methodology store D), respectively. The web design utilized previous find-
ings on aesthetic attributes (e.g., Lavie and Tractinsky 2004;
Measures Schenkman and Jonsson 2000). Aesthetic formality was pre-
sented by organized and systematic layout of items, legible font
This study involves nine latent variables as indicated in the type and font size, and clear text/background color combination,
research framework. On the basis of previous findings on the whereas aesthetic appeal was presented by using larger pictures
dimensionality of web aesthetics (e.g., Lavie and Tractinsky of the listed items, setting images into constant motion or blink-
2004; Schenkman and Jonsson 2000), aesthetic formality is mea- ing, and adding decorative graphics. The use of movement to
sured by the organization, order, and legibility of a website, and manipulate aesthetic appeal followed practical guidelines in suc-
aesthetic appeal is measured by the fascination, creativeness, and cessfully creating appeal in a two-dimensional interface, implied
impressiveness of a website as perceived by consumers. Seven- by real movement theory (Metallinos 1996). We used four sim-
point semantic differential scales were used. In a pilot study ulated e-tail websites in a quasi-experiment for two purposes:
based on a convenient sample of 140 consumers, we conducted (1) to obtain normal distribution of the values for perceived
confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) to test the dimensionality of aesthetic formality and perceived aesthetic appeal and (2) to
the two aesthetic dimensions. The result yielded a two-factor mitigate the impact of other intervening variables, such as the
model with good model fit (GFI = 0.90) and factor loadings price of the products, that threaten the internal validity of the
above 0.70. dependent relationships.
Satisfaction is measured by five items that describe con- The moderating role of purchase task was manipulated by
sumers’ immediate feelings about a website. Five pairs of recruiting two groups of participants, one group with purchase
opposite adjectives are used: satisfied versus dissatisfied, pleased task and the other group with no purchase task. For the group
versus displeased, contented versus frustrated, delighted versus with no purchase task, participants were simply asked to browse
terrible, and pleasant versus unpleasant. These five items are the e-tail website. For the group with purchase task, participants
based on existing transaction-specific measures of customer sat- were asked to select a particular piece of furniture (a three-seat
isfaction (e.g., Eroglu and Machleit 1990; Spreng et al. 1996). upholstered sofa with wood frame, priced below $1,700) on the
Arousal is measured by five pairs of adjective states: aroused ver- e-tail website, before exposed to the questionnaire. This method
sus unaroused, wide-awake versus sleepy, excited versus calm, of motivational manipulation has been widely used in quasi-
stimulated versus relaxed, and frenzied versus sluggish. The five experiments in marketing research (e.g., Celsi and Olson 1988;
items are selected from existing measures of phasic arousal (e.g., MacKenzie and Spreng 1992).
Donovan et al. 1994; Mehrabian and Russell 1974; Menon and
Kahn 2002). Satisfaction and arousal measures are assessed by Participants
seven-point semantic differential scales. Perceived online ser-
vice quality is measured by six items selected from Yang, Jun, In order to ensure a random sample of real consumers, we
and Peterson’s (2004) online service quality construct. The mea- recruited participants via a mall-intercept method suggested
sure of online service quality consists of six items: reliability, by Bush and Hair (1985). Trained instructors randomly inter-
responsiveness, competence, ease of use, security, and prod- cepted shoppers in a shopping mall, and interested shoppers
uct portfolio. The perceived online service quality measure is were invited to the experiment. A total of 400 participants were
assessed by seven-point Likert-type scales with higher values recruited. 200 were in the group with purchase task and 200 were
indicating more positive responses. in the group with no purchase task. Each of the four simulated
This study used self-reported inclination to engage in a behav- e-tail websites was viewed by 50 participants in each group. We
ior to measure the four conative responses. Using behavioral used reverse-coding to screen off “lip service” responses. In the
Y.J. Wang et al. / Journal of Retailing 87 (1, 2011) 46–58 51

questionnaire, the three semantic differential scales (aesthetic Table 1


formality, aesthetic appeal, satisfaction) were reverse-coded Construct validity and reliability.a
every other row. The reported values of the reverse-coded items Construct Cronbach α R2 GFIb
were converted back and compared with those of the regular Aesthetic formality 0.95/0.95 0.89/0.88 N/A
items. Cases showing fundamentally inconsistent patterns (more Aesthetic appeal 0.95/0.92 0.77/0.80 N/A
than 50% items with opposite directions) were dropped. This left Satisfaction 0.95/0.93 0.81/0.83 0.88/0.91
258 valid cases remained in the sample, 133 with purchase task Arousal 0.92/0.91 0.70/0.72 0.93/0.91
and 125 without purchase task. The demographic description of Online service quality 0.91/0.92 0.73/0.79 0.96/0.94
Purchase 0.91/0.91 0.79/0.79 N/A
the remaining participants is reported in Appendix A. The demo- Consultation 0.91/0.94 0.77/0.84 N/A
graphic characteristics of the sample reflect a pattern similar to Search 0.92/0.91 0.80/0.79 N/A
that of the internet users in the United States in terms of age, Re-visit 0.96/0.93 0.92/0.81 N/A
sex, and household income (Department of Commerce 2004). a The values are reported separately based on the group with purchase task

and the group without purchase task.


Statistical analysis b N/A: GFI value cannot be estimated due to 0◦ of freedom.

Our data were processed by LISREL 8.54 in tracing the


hypothesized structural relations. The evaluation of the mea-
Manipulation checks were conducted. Based on summated val-
surement model was first conducted to estimate the relationships
ues, we found that the participants who used websites A and
between the nine latent constructs and their indicators, followed
C reported higher perceived aesthetic formality than those who
by the evaluation of the hypothesized structure model examining
used websites B and D (M = 5.75 vs. 2.87, F (1, 256) = 119.52,
the relationships between the latent constructs.
p < 0.01). Meanwhile, participants who used websites A and
In the evaluation of the measurement model, the nine con-
B reported higher perceived aesthetic appeal than those who
structs were subjected to validity and reliability tests. To ensure
used websites C and D (M = 6.10 vs. 3.73, F (1, 256) = 135.39,
the validity of measures, the study began with construct valid-
p < 0.01). To compare consumers’ cognitive, affective, and
ity of each measure using confirmatory factor analysis (CFA),
conative responses as a result of the aesthetic effects, the
and then discriminant validity between the measures. Construct
means of the seven endogenous variables are provided in
validity was assessed upon three criteria: model fit, factor load-
Fig. 2.
ings of measurement items, and average variance extracted (R2 ).
The direct and indirect paths in the hypothesized model
Compared to Hu and Bentler’s (1999) 0.90 threshold for Good-
using the sample with purchase tasks are described in Fig. 3.
ness of Fit Index (GFI), the constructs demonstrated acceptable
On the basis of the combined results, when online con-
model fit, respectively. The factor loadings of all items in the nine
sumers pursue their purchase tasks, perceived aesthetic formality
constructs are reasonably high (all above 0.50) with significant
shows stronger impacts on consumers’ cognitive, affective,
t values (Anderson and Gerbing 1988). R2 values of the nine
and conative responses than does perceived aesthetic appeal.
constructs all exceed the suggested threshold of 0.50 (Fornell
Thus, all the hypotheses relevant to task-oriented consumers
and Larcker 1981).
are supported except that the satisfaction–consultation path in
Following Anderson and Gerbing’s (1988) method for testing
H3a and the arousal-search path in H4a are not statistically
discriminant validity, the constructs were paired up and sub-
significant.
jected to two models of confirmatory factor analysis. The first
The results of the direct and indirect paths in the hypothesized
model allows the covariance between the two constructs to be
model for task-free consumers are reported in Fig. 4. Indicated
unconstrained, and the second model constrains the covariance
by pair comparisons of the direct and indirect path coeffi-
between the two constructs. The χ2 values of the constrained
cients, for online consumers without a purchase task, perceived
and unconstrained models are compared. We found that discrim-
aesthetic appeal has stronger effects on consumers’ cognitive,
inant validity exists between all the paired constructs since the
affective, and conative responses than does perceived aesthetic
χ2 difference is significant for all the comparisons (χ2 = 3.84,
formality. Therefore, all the hypotheses relevant to task-free
d.f. = 1, p = 0.05).
consumers are supported.
The reliability analysis of the constructs was conducted
by calculating the Cronbach α for each construct. The
Cronbach α values for the nine constructs far exceed the Discussion
recommended critical point of 0.70 (Nunnally 1978), indi-
cating good internal-consistency reliability of the constructs. Our findings help to illustrate the effects of the two dimen-
The results of validity and reliability tests are reported in sions of web aesthetics on the cognitive, affective, and conative
Table 1. responses of online consumers, taking into consideration con-
sumers’ motivational orientations. The findings yield new
Results insights affecting a number of research topics in e-tail, such as e-
satisfaction, information search, online patronage behavior, and
In our study, four experimental e-tail websites were created so on. Several key notes may be highlighted for researchers and
to manipulate the two aesthetic dimensions in the experiments. practitioners.
52 Y.J. Wang et al. / Journal of Retailing 87 (1, 2011) 46–58

Fig. 2. Comparison of mean scores under aesthetic manipulations. FML, aesthetic formality; APL, aesthetic appeal; SAT, satisfaction; ARO, arousal; SVQ, online
service quality; PUR, purchase; CON, consultation; SCH, search; REV, re-visit.

Most of all, web aesthetics cannot be simply understood how consumers’ mind is affected by the bi-dimensional aesthetic
as “the nicer, the better”. It is a bi-dimensional concept and stimuli.
the stimulating effects of the two aesthetic dimensions are Speaking of the virtual shopping environment, marketing
intricate. Our findings subtly describe the dissimilar patterns research found that e-satisfaction is relevant to website design
that the two dimensions of web aesthetics display in the pro- (Evanschitzky et al. 2004; Szymanski and Hise 2000). However,
cess of online shopping. The dissimilarity includes not merely how do website design attributes matter in terms of consumer
the magnitude differences of their effects. It is noteworthy satisfaction remained a myth. Our findings can solve the puzzle
to mention that opposite directions of the effects of aes- for researchers and depict the function of visual design attributes
thetic formality and aesthetic appeal have been revealed. Our in consumer satisfaction. The findings indicate that the applica-
findings discover an interesting phenomenon that is against tion of high formality combined with high appeal as aesthetic
researchers’ and practitioners’ conventional understanding of attributes is not always ideal for e-tail web design aiming to
web aesthetics—although perceived aesthetic formality has a achieve consumer satisfaction. Web designers and web planners
positive influence on satisfaction, perceived aesthetic appeal has should be aware that, although a highly formalistic and highly
a negative influence on satisfaction when consumers pursue pur- appealing website may provide the utmost level of satisfaction
chase tasks. If we resort to the consumer reaction mechanism in for task-free online browsers, online consumers with purchase
which aesthetic formality facilitates shopping tasks but aesthetic tasks do not prefer such aesthetic attributes for task-centered
appeal interferes with the task completion, we can clearly depict online shopping.
Y.J. Wang et al. / Journal of Retailing 87 (1, 2011) 46–58 53

0.42* PUR
(4.81)

-0.34*
SAT (-3.85)
3 items
0.83*
(10.14)
-0.09
(-0.73)
-0.13* 5 items
FML (-2.28) CON
-0.61* 0.36*
(-7.28) (5.51)
-0.50*
3 items ARO (-5.26)
3 items
0.57*
(6.95)
0.15
5 items (1.64)
APL 0.81* SCH
(11.82)

0.38*
3 items (8.32) 3 items
SVQ

0.86*
(14.01)
6 items REV
Model Fit Indices:
2
=711.23, d.f.=513, p=0.00, GFI=0.76,
AGFI=0.72, RMSEA=0.05, CFI=0.98, NFI=0.96 3 items

Indirect Paths: 0.56* PUR


(7.04)
-0.25*
(-4.38)
-0.27*
(-3.54)
FML CON
0.23*
(3.56)
-0.50*
(-6.21)
APL 0.15*
SCH
(2.70)
0.70*
(10.87)
0.33*
* p<0.05, t values in the parentheses (7.97) REV

FML=Aesthetic Formality; APL=Aesthetic Appeal; SAT=Satisfaction; ARO=Arousal;


SVQ=Online Service Quality; PUR=Purchase; CON=Consultation; SCH=Search; REV=Re-visit.

Fig. 3. Hypothesized model for consumers with purchase task. FML, aesthetic formality; APL, aesthetic appeal; SAT, satisfaction; ARO, arousal; SVQ, online
service quality; PUR, purchase; CON, consultation; SCH, search; REV, re-visit.

There are also other remarkable dissimilar patterns of effects tasks are dependent on such cues. On the other hand, when
found. For instance, aesthetic formality has a negative influence consumers encounter a service environment without any imme-
on arousal while aesthetic appeal has a positive influence on diate shopping goal, high aesthetic appeal must be utilized as
arousal, due to information rate. All these various dissimilar the central factor in determining consumers’ evaluations and
patterns exhibited by the two aesthetic dimensions can illustrate emotions. For marketing researchers, the combination of the
web aesthetics as a potential “evil angel”, attractive, lovely, but results reveals that the moderation effect of motivational ori-
harmful when you do not understand its hidden nature. Our find- entation on consumers’ psychological and behavioral outcomes
ings further suggest e-tail web designers and web planners to is more complex than a linear effect as previously understood.
fully realize that, although both higher aesthetic formality and When consumers are task-free, their behavioral tendencies are
higher aesthetic appeal can present more “beautiful” percep- largely stimulated by environmental stimuli, and when con-
tions of the online environment, the impacts of the two aesthetic sumers engage in completing certain tasks, their behavior is
dimensions are far from being equally positive. goal-directed and volitional. The findings on the moderation
Moreover, to stay away from web design catastrophes, web effect of motivational orientation help to explain some of the
design strategies should take into account online consumers’ controversial findings in the literature, for example, the direc-
motivational orientations. Our findings demonstrate to e-tail tions of the arousal–purchase relationship (e.g., Donovan and
web designers and web planners that, high level of aesthetic Rossiter 1982; Menon and Kahn 2002; Milliman 1982; Sherman
formality must be provided for consumers whose utilitarian et al. 1997).
54 Y.J. Wang et al. / Journal of Retailing 87 (1, 2011) 46–58

0.63* PUR
(7.90)

0.34*
0.26* SAT (4.45)
3 items
(5.73)
0.50*
(7.13)
0.84*
(11.93)
5 items
FML CON
-0.43* 0.44*
(-6.42) (6.01)
0.40*
3 items ARO (4.67)
3 items
0.95*
(9.42)
0.50*

APL 0.34*
5 items (5.49)
SCH
(6.39)

0.76*
3 items (10.70)
3 items
SVQ

0.81*
(10.46)
6 items REV
Model Fit Indices:
2
=620.34, d.f.=513, p=0.00, GFI=0.77,
AGFI=0.74, RMSEA=0.04, CFI=0.99, NFI=0.97 3 items

Indirect Paths: 0.02 PUR


(0.31)
0.85*
(9.79)
-0.07
(-1.28)
FML CON
0.87*
(10.14)
-0.11
(-1.86)
APL 0.81*
SCH
(9.33)
0.27*
(5.80)
0.62*
* p<0.05, t values in the parentheses (8.44) REV

FML=Aesthetic Formality; APL=Aesthetic Appeal; SAT=Satisfaction; ARO=Arousal;


SVQ=Online Service Quality; PUR=Purchase; CON=Consultation; SCH=Search; REV=Re-visit.

Fig. 4. Hypothesized model for consumers without purchase task. FML, aesthetic formality; APL, aesthetic appeal; SAT, satisfaction; ARO, arousal; SVQ, online
service quality; PUR, purchase; CON, consultation; SCH, search; REV, re-visit.

Table 2
Behavioral consequences of aesthetic manipulations.
Manipulation of aesthetic dimensions Possible behavioral consequences

Task-oriented consumers Task-free consumers

High formality/high appeal May stop processing product information and seek More likely to make impulsive purchase; more likely to
additional information; more likely to re-visit at a later time browse other websites; more likely to re-visit at a later time
(even if purchase is not made)
Low formality/high appeal More likely to discontinue the purchase task immediately More likely to explore the website in detail and browse
and look for help, or switch to another website; lower other websites; may make impulsive purchase; may re-visit
chance of re-visit at a later time
High formality/low appeal More likely to continue focusing on the purchase task until More likely to leave soon; lower chance of re-visit
it is completed, without hesitation, additional consultation,
or search; higher chance of re-visit
Low formality/low appeal More likely to discontinue the purchase task to seek more More likely to leave immediately and not return
information in all possible ways, including checking other
websites; less likely to visit again
Y.J. Wang et al. / Journal of Retailing 87 (1, 2011) 46–58 55

In respect to the ultimate influence of web aesthetics, online Marketing research often follows a cognition → affect →
consumers’ likely behavioral consequences from the four types conation route to examine the hierarchy of effects of market-
of aesthetic manipulations are summarized in Table 2. In con- ing messages. In the present study, the sequential relationship
sequence, how aesthetics is applied and manipulated on an of cognition and affect has not been scrutinized. This study con-
e-tail website depends on the consideration of consumers’ psy- sidered affect and cognition two components of the organism
chological reactions as well as their motivational orientations. stage in the S-O-R framework and assumed that consumers’
E-tailors can manipulate web aesthetics to achieve most favor- cognitive and affective responses are induced by web aesthet-
able outcomes. For example, more transaction-based sites with ics during online service encounter. In marketing research,
the aim of mitigating customer hesitation and promoting imme- it has been argued that consumers’ perceived service qual-
diate sales should consider an e-tail website that features high ity will influence their satisfaction in the subsequent stage,
aesthetic formality and high aesthetic appeal for task-free online or vice versa, based on different service evaluation models
consumers, while an e-tail website featuring high aesthetic for- (Brady et al. 2005). Therefore, future research may examine
mality and low aesthetic appeal may be best to target regular the sequential order of perceived service quality and sat-
online shoppers with prioritized purchase tasks. Websites which isfaction to see if perceived service quality is a mediator
are more directed to relationship-building through repetitive vis- between aesthetic stimuli and satisfaction under certain situa-
its and click rates should feature high aesthetic formality and tions.
high aesthetic appeal for both task-oriented and task-free online The findings of this study indicate that a website may
consumers. Consumer-oriented e-tailers should dynamically uti- simultaneously evoke positive and negative affective responses
lize the two aesthetic dimensions and strategically implement of an individual. For example, when consumers pursue
them through interface design customization. Certainly, the purchase tasks, aesthetic formality positively influences satis-
value of aesthetic design customization of e-tail websites has faction while aesthetic appeal negatively influences satisfaction.
been highlighted (Mathwick, Wagner, and Unni 2010), and our Under this simultaneous and contradictory emotional con-
findings further provide the methods in how to customize web dition, consumers are said to be in ambivalence. Although
aesthetics with the objective of maximizing e-tailers’ customer the present study renders some implications in the under-
equity. standing of ambivalence in response to environmental stimuli,
Overall, the contribution of this study to retailing include: future research may seek thorough answers to the ques-
(1) it made clear how consumers respond to aesthetics stepwise; tion.
(2) it illustrated the complex effects of the aesthetic stimuli; This study has only focused on visual web aesthetics. How-
(3) it linked environmental stimuli with motivational orienta- ever, a growing number of e-tail websites are equipped with
tions in understanding consumer preferences; and (4) it provided multi-media. A large number of websites offer background
strategies in personalizing web aesthetics to reach optimal out- music as well as various instructional sounds and button tones.
comes for the e-tailers. Akin to those demonstrated in the e-tail The wide application of audio stimuli raises a research ques-
environment, aesthetic stimuli in other physical settings may tion: how can consumers’ aesthetic experience through both
also show a similar pattern of influences on individuals’ psy- visual and aural channels influence their behavior? Do visual
chological and behavioral responses. Therefore, in a broader and audio stimuli display the same pattern in terms of their
sense, the findings suggest that aesthetic appearance should psychological effects? The study of the combination of visual
match the intended tasks in a given physical environment. This and audio aesthetic stimuli requires a higher level of elabora-
is of particular importance for professional designers, archi- tion because of the complexity of aesthetic stimuli. Further,
tects, and engineers in maximizing the effectiveness of their a physical store environment may have ambient, design, and
design outcomes. For example, appealing features demonstrated social factors and all these factors create dynamic multi-sensory
via lights, odors, music, and decorations should be applied in stimuli (Baker 1986; Baker et al. 1992). Thus, the effects
areas where people are not engaged in work-related tasks, but of broader sources of aesthetic stimuli in a physical envi-
are not recommended for areas where work-related tasks are ronment on consumers’ cognitive, affective, and behavioral
performed. responses can be extremely intricate. Certainly, this will pro-
vide challenging opportunities for future research in the area of
Limitations and future research opportunities servicescape.
Last but not least, we only used one type of goods, furni-
The research model is subject to modifications or improve- ture, in the experimental websites. Previous studies in the e-tail
ments in various ways. The interaction effect of the two area have found that online consumers’ focuses and concerns
dimensions of web aesthetics has not been reviewed in the liter- during online shopping may differ on the basis of product type
ature nor examined in the hypothesized model. The interaction (e.g., search/experience/credence goods) (Bhatnagar and Ghose
of perceived aesthetic formality and perceived aesthetic appeal 2004; Hsieh, Chiu, and Chiang 2005; Weathers, Sharma, and
may have a significant impact on online consumers’ satisfaction, Wood 2007). Given that furniture is a type of hedonic, durable,
arousal, and perceived online service quality. Future research experience-based, and specialty product, future research should
should include the interaction effect in the structural model so utilize other types of merchandise to explore more nuanced
that a more accurate illustration of the effects of web aesthetics effects of web aesthetics based on consumers’ information pro-
will be presented. cessing patterns.
56 Y.J. Wang et al. / Journal of Retailing 87 (1, 2011) 46–58

Total (n = 258) Purchasetaskpursued(n = 133) No purchase task pursued (n = 125)

N % N % N %

Age
Below 20 5 1.9 3 2.2 2 1.6
20–29 112 43.4 45 33.8 67 53.6
30–39 52 20.2 28 21.1 24 19.2
40–49 54 20.9 35 26.3 19 15.2
50–59 21 8.1 13 9.8 8 6.4
60 and above 4 1.6 1 0.7 3 2.4
Undisclosed 10 3.9 8 6.0 2 1.6
Sex
Male 101 39.1 54 40.6 47 37.6
Female 154 59.7 76 57.1 78 62.4
Undisclosed 3 1.2 3 2.3 0 0.0
Marital status
Single 120 46.5 47 35.3 73 58.4
Married 85 32.9 56 42.1 29 23.2
Widowed 24 9.3 15 11.3 9 7.2
Divorced or separated 10 3.9 4 3.0 6 4.8
Undisclosed 19 7.4 11 8.3 8 6.4
Ethnicity
Caucasian heritage 176 68.2 89 66.9 87 69.6
Hispanic/Latino heritage 30 11.6 13 9.8 17 13.6
African heritage 21 8.1 13 9.8 8 6.4
Asian heritage 22 8.5 16 12.0 6 4.8
Native American heritage 2 0.8 0 0.0 2 1.6
Other heritage 1 0.4 0 0.0 1 0.8
Undisclosed 6 2.3 2 1.5 4 3.2
Education
High school 47 18.2 20 15.0 27 21.6
Undergraduate 170 65.9 94 70.7 76 60.8
Graduate 29 11.2 10 7.5 19 15.2
Undisclosed 12 4.7 9 6.8 3 2.4
Annual household income
Below 10,000 14 5.4 4 3.0 10 8.0
10,000–39,999 39 15.1 19 14.2 20 16.0
40,000–99,999 121 46.9 71 53.3 50 40.0
100,000–149,999 33 12.8 13 9.8 20 16.0
150,000–499,000 20 7.7 7 5.3 13 10.4
500,000 and above 7 2.7 3 2.3 4 3.2
Undisclosed 24 9.4 16 12.0 8 6.4
Time spent online per week
<1 h 6 2.3 3 2.3 3 2.4
1–2 h 17 6.6 7 5.3 10 8.0
2–5 h 44 17.1 20 15.0 24 19.2
5–10 h 91 35.3 59 44.4 32 25.6
10–20 h 57 22.1 23 17.3 34 27.2
More than 20 h 29 11.2 17 12.8 12 9.6
Undisclosed 14 5.4 4 3.0 10 8.0
Frequency of online shopping
Rarely 50 19.4 29 21.8 21 16.8
Several times a year 139 53.9 71 53.4 68 54.4
Several times a month 51 19.8 23 17.3 28 22.4
Several times a week 9 3.5 4 3.0 5 4.0
At least once a day 4 1.6 2 1.5 2 1.6
Undisclosed 5 1.9 4 3.0 1 0.8

Appendix A. Description of participants Baker, Julie (1986), “The Role of the Environment in Marketing Services: The
Consumer Perspective,” in The Services Challenge: Integrating for Compet-
itive Advantage, Czepiel John A., Congram Carole A. and Shanahan James,
References
eds. Chicago, IL: American Marketing Association.
Anderson, James C. and David W. Gerbing (1988), “Structural Equation Mod- Baker, Julie, Michael Levy and Dhruv Grewal (1992), “An Experimental
eling in Practice: A Review and Recommended Two-Step Approach,” Approach to Making Retail Store Environmental Decisions,” Journal of
Psychological Bulletin, 103 (3), 411–23. Retailing, 68 (4), 445–60.
Y.J. Wang et al. / Journal of Retailing 87 (1, 2011) 46–58 57

Baker, Julie, A. Parasuraman, Dhruv Grewal and Glenn B. Voss (2002), “The and Behavioural Intention,” Behaviour and Information Technology, 23 (3),
Influence of Multiple Store Environment Cues on Perceived Merchandise 183–95.
Value and Patronage Intentions,” Journal of Marketing, 66 (2), 120–41. Hartman, Jonathan B., Soyeon Shim, Bonnie Barber and Matthew O’Brien
Beatty, Sharon E. and M. Elizabeth Ferrell (1998), “Impulse Buying: Modeling (2006), “Adolescents’ Utilitarian and Hedonic Web Consumption Behavior:
Its Precursors,” Journal of Retailing, 74 (2), 169–91. Hierarchical Influence of Personal Values and Innovativeness,” Psychology
Berlyne, Daniel E. (1960), Conflict, Arousal, and Curiosity, New York: McGraw- and Marketing, 23 (10), 813–39.
Hill. Hershberger, Wayne A. (1989), Volitional Action: Conation and Control, Ams-
Bhatnagar, Amit and Sanjoy Ghose (2004), “Online Information Search Termi- terdam, The Netherlands: North-Holland.
nation Patterns across Product Categories and Consumer Demographics,” Hsieh, Yi-Ching, Hung-Chang Chiu and Mei-Yi Chiang (2005), “Maintaining a
Journal of Retailing, 80 (3), 221–8. Committed Online Customer: A Study across Search-Experience-Credence
Bitner, Mary J. (1992), “Servicescapes: The Impact of Physical Surroundings Products,” Journal of Retailing, 81 (1), 75–82.
on Customers and Employees,” Journal of Marketing, 56 (2), 57–71. Hu, Li-Tze and Peter M. Bentler (1999), “Cutoff Criteria for Fit Indices in
Brady, Michael K., Gary A. Knight, J. Joseph Cronin, G. Tomas, M. Hult Covariance Structure Analysis: Conventional Criteria versus New Alterna-
and Bruce D. Keillor (2005), “Removing the Contextual Lens: A Multi- tives,” Structural Equation Modeling, 6 (1), 1–55.
national, Multi-Setting Comparison of Service Evaluation Models,” Journal Huang, Ming-Hui (2003), “Modeling Virtual Exploratory and Shopping
of Retailing, 81 (3), 215–30. Dynamics: An Environmental Psychology Approach,” Information and
Bush, Alan J. and Joseph F. Hair (1985), “An Assessment of the Mall Intercept as Management, 41 (1), 39–47.
a Data Collection Method,” Journal of Marketing Research, 22 (2), 158–67. Hui, Michael K., Laurette Dube and Jean-Charles Chebat (1997), “The Impact
Celsi, Richard L. and Jerry C. Olson (1988), “The Role of Involvement in Atten- of Music on Consumers’ Reactions to Waiting for Services,” Journal of
tion and Comprehension Processes,” Journal of Consumer Research, 15 (2), Retailing, 73 (1), 87–104.
210–24. Kaltcheva, Velitchka D. and Barton A. Weitz (2006), “When Should a Retailer
Childers, Terry L., Christopher L. Carr, Joann Peck and Stephen Carson (2001), Create an Exciting Store Environment?,” Journal of Marketing, 70 (1),
“Hedonic and Utilitarian Motivations for Online Retail Shopping Behavior,” 107–18.
Journal of Retailing, 77 (4), 511–35. Laurent, Gilles and Jean-Noel Kapferer (1985), “Measuring Consumer Involve-
Dabholkar, Pratibha A., C. David Shepherd and Dayle I. Thorpe (2000), “A ment Profiles,” Journal of Marketing Research, 22 (1), 41–53.
Comprehensive Framework for Service Quality: An Investigation of Critical Lavie, Talia and Noam Tractinsky (2004), “Assessing Dimensions of Perceived
Conceptual and Measurement Issues through a Longitudinal Study,” Journal Visual Aesthetics of Web Sites,” International Journal of Human-Computer
of Retailing, 76 (2), 139–73. Studies, 60 (3), 269–98.
Dawson, Scott, Peter H. Bloch and Nancy M. Ridgway (1990), “Shopping Lindgaard, Gitte and Cathy Dudek (2003), “What Is This Evasive Beast We Call
Motives, Emotional States, and Retail Outcomes,” Journal of Retailing, 66 User Satisfaction?,” Interacting with Computers, 15 (3), 429–52.
(4), 408–27. Lurie, Nicholas H. (2004), “Decision Making in Information-Rich Environ-
Department of Commerce (2004), “A Nation Online: Entering the ments: The Role of Information Structure,” Journal of Consumer Research,
Broadband Age,” available at http://www.ntia.doc.gov/reports/anol/ 30 (4), 473–86.
NationOnlineBroadband04.htm. MacKenzie, Scott B. and Richard A. Spreng (1992), “How Does Motivation
Dickie, George (1997), Introduction to Aesthetics: An Analytic Approach, New Moderate the Impact of Central and Peripheral Processing on Brand Atti-
York: Oxford University Press. tudes and Intentions?,” Journal of Consumer Research, 18 (4), 519–2.
Donovan, Robert J. and John R. Rossiter (1982), “Store Atmosphere: An Envi- Maslow, Abraham H. (1970), Motivation and Personality, New York: Harper
ronmental Psychology Approach,” Journal of Retailing, 58 (1), 34–57. and Row.
Donovan, Robert J., John R. Rossiter, Gillian Marcoolyn and Andrew Nesdale Mathwick, Charla, Janet Wagner and Ramaprasad Unni (2010), “Computer-
(1994), “Store Atmosphere and Purchasing Behavior,” Journal of Retailing, Mediated Customization Tendency (CMCT) and the Adaptive e-Service
70 (3), 283–94. Experience,” Journal of Retailing, 86 (1), 11–2.
Eroglu, Sevgin A. and Karen A. Machleit (1990), “An Empirical Study of Retail Mehrabian, Alpert and James A. Russell (1974), An Approach to Environmental
Crowding: Antecedents and Consequences,” Journal of Retailing, 66 (2), Psychology, Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
201–2. Menon, Satya and Barbara Kahn (2002), “Cross-Category Effects of Induced
Eroglu, Sevgin A., Karen A. Machleit and Lenita M. Davis (2001), “Atmospheric Arousal and Pleasure on the Internet Shopping Experience,” Journal of
Qualities of Online Retailing: A Conceptual Model and Implications,” Jour- Retailing, 78 (1), 31–40.
nal of Business Research, 54 (2), 177–84. Metallinos, Nikos (1996), Television Aesthetics: Perceptual, Cognitive, and
Evanschitzky, Heiner, Gopalkrishnan R. Iyer, Josef Hessea and Dieter Ahlerta Compositional Bases, Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
(2004), “E-Satisfaction: A Re-Examination,” Journal of Retailing, 80 (3), Milliman, Ronald E. (1982), “Using Background Music to Affect the Behavior
239–47. of Supermarket Shoppers,” Journal of Marketing, 46 (3), 86–91.
Fornell, Claes and David F. Larcker (1981), “Evaluating Structural Equation Nunnally, Jum C. (1978), Psychometric Theory, New York: McGraw-Hill.
Models with Unobservable Variables and Measurement Error,” Journal of Pan, Yue and George M. Zinkhan (2006), “Determinants of Retail Patronage: A
Marketing Research, 18 (1), 39–50. Meta-Analytical Perspective,” Journal of Retailing, 82 (3), 229–43.
Frijda, Nico H. (1994), “Emotions Require Cognitions, Even If Simple Ones,” in Park, Su-e, Dongsung Choi and Jinwoo Kim (2004), “Critical Factors for the
The Nature of Emotion: Fundamental Questions, Paul Ekman and Richard Aesthetic Fidelity of Web Pages: Empirical Studies with Professional Web
Davidson, eds. New York: Oxford University Press Designers and Users,” Interacting with Computers, 16 (2), 351–76.
Ganesh, Jaishankar, Kristy E. Reynolds, Michael Luckett and Nadia Pomirleanu Pham, Michel T. (1996), “Cue Representation and Selection Effects of Arousal
(2010), “Online Shopper Motivations, and e-Store Attributes: An Examina- on Persuasion,” Journal of Consumer Research, 22 (4), 373–87.
tion of Online Patronage Behavior and Shopper Typologies,” Journal of Puccinelli, Nancy M., Ronald C. Goodstein, Dhruv Grewal, Robert Price, Priya
Retailing, 86 (1), 106–15. Raghubir and David Stewart (2009), “Customer Experience Management in
Grewal, Dhruv, Julie Baker, Michael Levy and Glenn B. Voss (2003), “The Retailing: Understanding the Buying Process,” Journal of Retailing, 85 (1),
Effects of Wait Expectations and Store Atmosphere Evaluations on Patron- 15–30.
age Intentions in Service-Intensive Retail Stores,” Journal of Retailing, 79 Ridgway, Nancy M., Scott A. Dawson and Peter H. Bloch (1989), “Pleasure
(4), 259–68. and Arousal in the Marketplace: Interpersonal Differences in Approach-
Grewal, Dhruv and Michael Levy (2007), “Retailing Research: Past, Present, Avoidance Responses,” Marketing Letters, 1 (2), 139–47.
and Future,” Journal of Retailing, 83 (4), 447–64. Sanbonmatsu, David M. and Frank R. Kardes (1988), “The Effects of Phys-
Hall, Richard H. and Patrick Hanna (2004), “The Impact of Web Page Text- iological Arousal on Information Processing and Persuasion,” Journal of
Background Colour Combinations on Readability, Retention, Aesthetics Consumer Research, 15 (3), 379–85.
58 Y.J. Wang et al. / Journal of Retailing 87 (1, 2011) 46–58

Schenkman, Bo N. and Fredrik U. Jonsson (2000), “Aesthetics and Preferences Van der Heijden, Hans (2003), “Factors Influencing the Usage of Websites: the
of Web Pages,” Behaviour and Information Technology, 19 (5), 367–7. Case of a Generic Portal in the Netherlands,” Information and Management,
Sherman, Lawrence J., Anil Mathur and Ruth B. Smith (1997), “Store Envi- 40 (6), 541–9.
ronment and Consumer Purchase Behavior: Mediating Role of Consumer Vilnai-Yavetz, Iris and Anat Rafaeli (2006), “Aesthetics and Professionalism of
Emotions,” Psychology and Marketing, 14 (4), 361–78. Virtual Servicescapes,” Journal of Service Research, 8 (3), 245–59.
Sirohi, Niren, Edward W. McLaughlin and Dick R. Wittink (1998), “A Model Weathers, Danny, Subhash Sharma and Stacy L. Wood (2007), “Effects of
of Consumer Perceptions and Store Loyalty Intentions for a Supermarket Online Communication Practices on Consumer Perceptions of Performance
Retailer,” Journal of Retailing, 74 (2), 223–45. Uncertainty for Search and Experience Goods,” Journal of Retailing, 83 (4),
Spies, Kordelia, Friedrich Hesse and Kerstin Loesch (1997), “Store Atmosphere, 393–401.
Mood and Purchasing Behavior,” International Journal of Research in Mar- Wolfinbarger, Mary and Mary C. Gilly (2003), “eTailQ: Dimensionalizing,
keting, 14 (1), 1–17. Measuring and Predicting eTail Quality,” Journal of Retailing, 79 (3),
Spreng, Richard A., Scoff B. MacKenzie and Richard W. Olshavsky (1996), “A 183–98.
Reexamination of the Determinants of Consumer Satisfaction,” Journal of Yang, Zhilin, Minjoon Jun and Robin T. Peterson (2004), “Measuring Customer
Marketing, 60 (3), 15–32. Perceived Online Service Quality,” International Journal of Operations and
Szymanski, David M. and Richard T. Hise (2000), “E-Satisfaction: An Initial Production Management, 24 (11), 1149–74.
Examination,” Journal of Retailing, 76 (3), 309–22. Yoo, Boonghee and Naveen Donthu (2001), “Developing a Scale to Measure
Theeuwes, Jan (1992), “Cross-Dimensional Perceptual Selectivity,” Perception the Perceived Quality of Internet Shopping Sites (SITEQUAL)”,” Quarterly
and Psychophysics, 50 (2), 184–93. Journal of Electronic Commerce, 2 (1), 31–47.
Tractinsky, Noam (2004), “A Few Notes on the Study of Beauty in HCI,” Human- Zaichkowsky, Judith L. (1985), “Measuring the Involvement Construct,” Journal
Computer Interaction, 19 (4), 351–7. of Consumer Research, 12 (3), 341–52.
Tractinsky, Noam, Avivit Cokhavi, Moti Kirschenbaum and Tal Sharfi (2006), Zeithaml, Valarie A., Leonard L. Berry and A. Parasuraman (1996), “The
“Evaluating the Consistency of Immediate Aesthetic Perceptions of Web Behavioral Consequences of Service Quality,” Journal of Marketing, 60 (2),
Pages,” International Journal of Human–Computer Studies, 64 (11), 31–46.
1071–83.

You might also like