Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

Body Image 6 (2009) 221–227

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Body Image
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/bodyimage

Characteristics of muscle dysmorphia in male football, weight training,


and competitive natural and non-natural bodybuilding samples
Timothy Baghurst *, Cathy Lirgg
Department of Health Science, Kinesiology, Recreation, and Dance, University of Arkansas, Fayetteville, AR 72701, United States

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Article history: The purpose of this study was to identify differences in traits associated with muscle dysmorphia
Received 8 October 2008 between collegiate football players (n = 66), weight trainers for physique (n = 115), competitive non-
Received in revised form 5 March 2009 natural bodybuilders (n = 47), and competitive natural bodybuilders (n = 65). All participants completed
Accepted 20 March 2009
demographic questionnaires in addition to the Muscle Dysmorphia Inventory (Rhea, Lantz, & Cornelius,
2004). Results revealed a significant main effect for group, and post hoc tests found that the non-natural
Keywords: bodybuilding group did not score significantly higher than the natural bodybuilding group on any
Muscle dysmorphia
subscale except for Pharmacological Use. Both the non-natural and natural bodybuilding groups scored
Males
significantly higher than those that weight trained for physique on the Dietary Behavior and Supplement
Body image
Football Use subscales. The collegiate football players scored lowest on all subscales of the Muscle Dysmorphia
Drive for muscularity Inventory except for Physique Protection where they scored highest. Findings are discussed with future
Muscle Dysmorphia Inventory research expounded.
Natural bodybuilding ß 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Non-natural bodybuilding
Steroids

Introduction power and renounces stereotypic feminine weakness (Wesely,


2001). Thus, women who possess a muscular body are a threat to
Over the last several decades there has been a growing focus on how a male interprets his masculinity, as masculinity is driven by a
body image (Alexander, 2003), as evidenced by a 731% rise in the fear of being feminine or less than what a man should be
number of academic articles that focus on the muscular ideal (Alexander, 2003). For example, Holmlund (1997) found that
between 2000 and 2007 compared with the previous seven years images of muscular women increase male anxiety because they
(Thompson & Cafri, 2007). Investigation into male physique is threaten to abolish the visible distinction. Muscular women can
important, for it has become a means of gender differentiation and threaten male dominance in sexuality, sex, gender, the workplace,
can define how gender roles and gender identity are portrayed and social power (Schulze, 1990). Therefore, men are searching for
(Mishkind, Rodin, Silberstein, & Striegel-Moore, 1986). With an a feature that distinguishes them from the opposite sex (Randall,
increasing number of men dissatisfied with their appearance (e.g., Hall, & Rogers, 1992). As Klein (1993) puts it:
Berscheid, Walster, & Bohrnstedt, 1973; Cash, Winstead, & Janda,
1986; Garner, 1997) it has been suggested that a cultural shift from The path taken by those wishing to accrue flesh has much more
occupational to physical identity has encouraged many males to to do with gender insecurities. To be male has, in our culture,
seek a lean, muscular, and mesomorphic body type in order to been linked with dangerous and demanding occupations and
distinguish themselves from women (Klein, 1993; Peoples, 2001; roles, such as frontiersman, soldier, policeman and doctor. But
Petrie et al., 1996). the golden era when ‘men were men’ has passed, and the
Because males are naturally muscular and women are not, this powerful roles traditionally the exclusive province of men have
difference in physique can symbolize a difference between the vanished, weakened or are no longer gender-specific. Accord-
sexes (Goldberg, 1997). A muscular body signifies masculine ingly, many bodybuilding men have clutched to themselves the
only trait that gives them hegemony over women; their size
(p. 36).
* Corresponding author at: Department of Health Science, Kinesiology, Recrea-
tion, and Dance, College of Education and Health Professions 308 HPER Building,
University of Arkansas, Fayetteville, AR 72701, United States. Tel.: +1 479 575 2858;
Roach and Eigher (1973) suggest that the ideal human form is a
fax: +1 479 575 3119. representative consensus dictated by society, but that these ideals
E-mail address: baghurst@sbcglobal.net (T. Baghurst). can change over time and vary across cultures (Creekmore &

1740-1445/$ – see front matter ß 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.2009.03.002
222 T. Baghurst, C. Lirgg / Body Image 6 (2009) 221–227

Pedersen, 1979). Over the last 50 years, the ideal male physique has body image in male bodybuilders, weight lifters, and a non weight
become more muscular (Goldberg, 1997) and has been observed in lifting/bodybuilding sample of psychology university students; the
several countries including South Africa (Hitzeroth, Wessels, latter was classed as a control group. Bodybuilders were
Zungu-Dirwayi, Oosthuizen, & Stein, 2001), the United States categorized as such if they had trained for twice a week for at
(Pope, Gruber, Choi, Olivardia, & Phillips, 1997), and China (Ung, least a year. No effort was made to determine prior competition
Fones, & Ang, 2000). experience or the purpose for exercising in a gym setting. Weight
Current expectations of cultural health, fitness, and beauty are lifters were required to be members of a weight lifting association.
clearly promoted within American culture, which emphasizes a Although acting as a control group, it is possible that the university
lean, muscular male to be the ideal (Lantz, Rhea, & Mayhew, 2001; students did not represent a true control, for approximately 66%
Pope, Phillips, & Olivardia, 2000). Unfortunately, if perceptions of participated in sports that included soccer, football, running,
self do not equate to expectations as defined by culture, a distorted martial arts, and swimming. No data were ascertained to
view of self body image can develop. This can lead to dangerous determine the level of participation.
behaviors and attitudes associated with physique, specifically Kanayama, Barry, Hudson, and Pope (2006) investigated steroid
through an emphasis on increased muscular development and use among weight lifters. The selection of their sample was based
leanness (Gonzalez, McLachlan, & Keaney, 2001). on whether participants could bench press 275 lbs. Although the
Muscle dysmorphia has been described as a desire or drive to authors recognize that a lack of a non-weight lifting sample or
increase muscularity yet simultaneously reduce body fat percen- control group limited the findings, it is unclear whether the weight
tage (Morgan, 2000). It is predominantly evident in males (Pope lifters were bodybuilders or lifted weights primarily for other
et al., 2000) when there is a pathological preoccupation with a lack purposes such as health or sports.
of muscular size and leanness (Olivardia, 2001). Although Goldfield et al. (2006) made the distinction between
The etiology of these body image concerns, specifically the competitive and recreational bodybuilders in an investigation of
desire of men to be lean and muscular, is relatively unknown body image, binge eating, and bulimia nervosa, their definition of
although the media is considered to be one possible contributing recreational bodybuilding has similarities to definitions of weight
factor (Pope et al., 2000). For example, Leit, Gray, and Pope (2002) lifting. No data were obtained to determine whether participants
found that media images can alter ideal self-perceived and desired were in fact bodybuilders or whether they were weight training for
muscularity by creating a discrepancy between an individual’s an alternative purpose such as sporting performance or for health
perceived muscularity and ideally desired muscularity. Parental reasons. In addition, no data concerning the duration of time spent
influences and peer experiences can also encourage muscularity in at the gym were ascertained. Using a battery of tests to assess body
adolescent males (Stanford & McCabe, 2005). Specifically, fathers image, Pickett et al. (2005) investigated a group of 40 male
and male peers appear to encourage muscle development and competitive bodybuilders, 40 non-competitive weight trainers,
messages about muscularity whereas mothers emphasize weight and 40 athletic non-weight trainers. In addition, a Fat Free Mass
loss and body shape. Index was obtained for all participants using measurements of
Those individuals that invest heavily in their physiques through height and percent body fat obtained by calipers. It was found that
weight lifting or bodybuilding are the most prominently although competitive bodybuilders had a greater fat-free body
researched (Lantz, Rhea, & Cornelius, 2002). Bodybuilders or mass and reported higher social self-esteem, they also wished to be
individuals focused on muscular symmetry are often used in heavier than the other groups and had a greater eating disturbance.
empirical research because muscle dysmorphia is more prevalent The groups did not differ in their situational body image discomfort
within this population (Esco, Olson, & Williford, 2005). For and interestingly did not differ significantly in percent body fat.
example, Lantz et al. found muscle dysmorphia to be significantly However, the bodybuilders and weight trainers were more
more common in bodybuilders than power lifters. psychologically invested in their appearance and had a more
Unfortunately, although research suggests that bodybuilders positive global appearance evaluation. The authors concluded that,
are more likely to exhibit characteristics associated with muscle ‘‘somewhat surprisingly. . .men who compete in public competi-
dysmorphia, there are several methodological issues with these tions were no more invested in their appearance than non-
findings that should be first considered. Because the criteria (see competitive weight trainers’’ (p. 220).
Pope et al., 1997) for muscle dysmorphia were developed primarily Although the findings of Pickett et al. (2005) initially appear
through research involving either bodybuilders or individuals who intriguing, their veracity has been questioned (see Smith, Wright,
used steroids and were heavily invested in their physiques (e.g., Bruce-Low, & Hale, 2005). Although the competitive bodybuilding
Blouin & Goldfield, 1995; Pope et al.), it is unclear whether these group was clearly defined, the rationale for weight training was not
traits are specific to these individuals. Thus, because the majority ascertained in the weight training group. Thus, there remains the
of research has employed bodybuilders as participants, there exists possibility that participants within this group could have classed
a possibility that other individuals such as athletes may exhibit themselves as non-competitive bodybuilders, power lifters, or
traits associated with muscle dysmorphia (Lantz et al., 2001). For motivated fitness lifters. Consequently, Smith et al. suggest that a
example, no significant differences have been found between group of non-competitive bodybuilders and a group of non-
competitive male bodybuilders and weight trainers in respect to a bodybuilding weight trainers should have been included in the
drive for bulk and a drive for muscle tone (Goldfield, Blouin, & study to allow for further comparisons.
Woodside, 2006). Furthermore, Pickett, Lewis, and Cash (2005) An additional concern of past research involving body-
found no differences in the reported investment in appearance building participants is a failure to delineate between those who
between male competitive bodybuilders and non-competitive were natural bodybuilders and those who were not. Natural
weight trainers suggesting that dissatisfaction in physique may bodybuilders must undergo drug testing for banned substances
exist in individuals who are not considered to be competitive and submit to a polygraph prior to competition (International
bodybuilders. Natural Bodybuilding Association, n.d.). Non-natural body-
Although this research highlights some important findings, it is builders are not required to follow such guidelines and such
important for future research to clearly delineate between differences could potentially alter findings associated with body
participants that are bodybuilders, weight lifters, or lift weights image.
for other purposes such as health or sporting performance. For Because muscle dysmorphia is relatively unknown and
example, Hallsworth, Wade, and Tiggemann (2005) investigated uncharted, Lantz et al. (2001) suggest several areas for investiga-
T. Baghurst, C. Lirgg / Body Image 6 (2009) 221–227 223

tion. In particular, they suggest that not only should the prevalence Demographic Questionnaire (DQ)
of the disorder be investigated in the overall population, but also All participants completed a questionnaire asking for demo-
within target subgroups such as gender and sport type. Further- graphic data concerning each participant, which included age,
more, involvement in sports directing focus on muscular size and weight, height, and ethnicity. Participants were also asked to
strength such as bodybuilding, power lifting, and football may indicate whether they had ever met with a health professional.
facilitate muscle dysmorphic tendencies. Consequently, the
purpose of this study was to identify differences in traits associated Exercise Specific Questionnaire (ESQ)
with muscle dysmorphia between samples of male collegiate The ESQ determined previous and current exercise status.
football players, weight trainers for physique, and competitive Generic questions for all groups included the number of years
natural and non-natural bodybuilders. Based on current research, lifting weights, the frequency of training, the type of training, and
it was hypothesized that, (1) the non-natural bodybuilding group the desired outcome of the training. Additional questions
would score significantly higher than the natural bodybuilding, answered by the NBB and NNBB groups included the number of
weight training for physique, and football groups on all subscales years bodybuilding, and the number of competitions completed. In
of the Muscle Dysmorphia Inventory (MDI; Rhea, Lantz, & order to confirm participants’ inclusion within these two groups,
Cornelius, 2004), (2) the natural bodybuilding group would score each participant was required to indicate the date and location of
significantly higher than the weight training for physique and their most recent competition or the competition for which they
football groups on all subscales of the MDI, and (3) the weight were currently training.
training for physique group would score significantly higher than
the football group on all subscales of the MDI. Muscle Dysmorphia Inventory (MDI)
Participants in all four groups completed the Muscle Dysmor-
Method phia Inventory (Rhea et al., 2004). The MDI aims to identify
characteristics associated with muscle dysmorphia based on the
Participants and design criteria proposed by Lantz et al. (2001). The questionnaire uses 27
questions on a six-point Likert scale 1 = never to 6 = always to
Participants consisted of four groups of males: collegiate NCAA assess characteristics associated with muscle dysmorphia. These
Division One football players (n = 66), weight trainers for physique characteristics are broken down into six subscales: Size/Symmetry
(n = 115), competitive natural bodybuilders (n = 65), and compe- (e.g., I am concerned with losing muscle mass), Physique
titive non-natural bodybuilders (n = 47). All participants were Protection (e.g., I wear bulky clothing to hide my muscular
required to be training with weights at least three days a week for a physique from others), Exercise Dependence (e.g., It bothers me to
minimum average of 45 min per training session. miss a scheduled workout), Supplement Use (e.g., I use supple-
Collegiate football players (ATH) were recruited from a Mid- ments to help me recuperate from strenuous workouts), Dietary
South university. At the time of data collection, participants were Behavior (e.g., I monitor my diet closely to limit my fat intake), and
registered to play on the football team at the university. Pharmacological Use (e.g., I use steroids). Subscales are measured
Competitive natural bodybuilding (NBB) participants and compe- separately, as each is designed to measure conceptually indepen-
titive non-natural bodybuilding (NNBB) participants were dent traits. A high score on a subscale indicates a higher propensity
recruited through contact made on established bodybuilding to exhibit characteristics associated with that subscale. Thus, an
websites and forums. Natural bodybuilding refers to those individual who scores highly on the Supplement Use subscale
competitions in which participants are drug tested and must pass would use supplements more frequently than an individual that
a polygraph regarding their lack of prohibited drug use. Non- does not score highly.
natural bodybuilding refers to those competitions where partici- The MDI subscales has reported Cronbach’s a reliability
pants are not required to pass a polygraph, nor are they drug tested estimates ranging between .72 and .94 (Rhea et al., 2004). Rhea
prior to competition. To be included in the study, bodybuilders had et al. assessed construct validity using item-to-total correlations,
either competed in a bodybuilding competition matching their exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses, and structure
natural/non-natural status within the previous 12 months or were equation modeling. Convergent validity was determined by
actively training for a competition to be held within the next 12 calculating the correlations between the subscales of the MDI,
months. the Body Building Dependence Scale, and the Drive for Thinness
Weight training for physique (WTP) participants were recruited subscale of the Eating Disorder Inventory, all of which were
through contact made on recognized weight training websites and significantly and positively correlated to the MDI.
forums. To be included in this group, the primary purpose of In the present study, internal consistency reliability of the MDI’s
participants’ weight training was to improve physique (i.e., six subscales was assessed using Cronbach’s a. Physique Protection
participants did not weight train primarily for health reasons, had the highest internal consistency value (.82) followed by
for athletic performance, bodybuilding competitions, or for any Dietary Behavior (.77), Size/Symmetry (.76), Pharmacological Use
other reason). Participants had not competed as bodybuilders (.76), Supplement Use (.71), and Exercise Dependence (.57)
within the previous 12 months, nor had any intentions to do so respectively. Acceptance of an internally reliable scale is typically
within the next 12 months. set at .70 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001), although a value of .90 could
suggest that the subscale may be exhibiting multicollinearity
Instruments (Streiner, 2003). Thus, the exercise dependence subscale of the
MDI was eliminated from further analysis.
Surveys were presented in two formats. A paper-based format
was handed out to the ATH group. The NBB, NNBB, and WTP groups Procedure
were presented with the survey through an online service. Each
survey included the Exercise Specific Questionnaire (ESQ), Demo- Institutional Review Board permission was obtained prior to
graphic Questionnaire (DQ), both self-produced, and the MDI. data collection. Confidentiality was ensured by numerically coding
Survey sets were tailored to the group under investigation. questionnaires. Responses of paper-based questionnaires were
Questions pertaining to bodybuilding were added to the ESQ for anonymous and at no point were participants required to provide
the NBB and NNBB groups. their names. However, Internet responses retrieved through e-
224 T. Baghurst, C. Lirgg / Body Image 6 (2009) 221–227

mailed requests included each participant’s e-mail address thereby Demographic Questionnaire (DQ) responses
eliminating complete anonymity. All participants who completed
the Internet survey were informed that responses would not be With respect to mean age by years, the ATH group were
anonymous, but remain confidential. significantly younger (20.5, SD = 4.41; F(1, 13) = 8.52, p = .01) than
Participants in all four groups were informed prior to the WTP group (29.78, SD = 10.22), NNB group (32.22, SD = 11.12),
participation that the purpose of the study was to investigate and NNBB group. A total of 78.64% of participants identified
male body image. It was explained that participation in the study themselves as Caucasian, 11.65% African-American, 4.03% Hispa-
implied consent. Following this explanation, participants were nic, 3.28% Asian, and 4.2% as an ethnicity not listed.
provided questions appropriate to their group – either by the With respect to marital status, 52.3% of the NBB group was
primary researcher or through the survey link – and asked to married. Of the remaining three groups, 66.96% of the participants
complete them as carefully and honestly as possible. were single. This was most evident in the ATH group where 99.5%
Data from the ATH group were obtained through the use of of the participants were single. A total of 69.37% of the NBB, NNBB,
paper-based questionnaires distributed by the primary researcher. and WTP participants were not students whereas 100% of the ATH
However, questionnaires for the NBB, NNBB, and WTP groups were group participants were. Participants were asked whether they had
collected through two methods. First, a web-based duplicate of the ever visited a mental health professional. The majority of
questionnaires was posted on a well-established survey website. participants responded that they had not (90.63%) with a small
An explanation of the study and a link to the survey website proportion responding in the affirmative (6.04%) or declining to
inviting participation was posted on specific bodybuilding and answer (3.33%).
weight training websites. Second, prospective participants whose
e-mails were readily available on the Internet were sent the Exercise Specific Questionnaire (ESQ) responses
information personally. An e-mail inviting future participation was
sent to 431 prospective participants. After three days, an additional Participants’ age, weight, height, body fat percentage (self-
e-mail which contained a website link to the study was sent out to reported), number of years weight training, number of work outs
the same group of individuals. A follow-up e-mail was sent one per week, and length of work outs are provided in Table 1. Several
week later to those participants that did not participate the first additional questions were asked of those who were competitive
time reminding them of the opportunity. bodybuilders. The NBB participants reported competing on an
average of 11.47 (SD = 14.16) occasions and had competed as
Treatment of data bodybuilders for 6.28 (SD = 5.74) years. Participants within this
group consisted of 19.7% who classed themselves as amateur and
Face validity for the ESQ and DQ was obtained through a pilot 80.3% as professional. The NNBB participants reported competing
study using three participants from each group. The purpose of the on 10 (SD = 10.61) occasions and had been competing as body-
pilot study was to provide feedback concerning the functionality of builders for 7.24 (SD = 6.22) years. All participants within this
the questionnaires and their appropriateness to the sample for group classed themselves as professional bodybuilders.
which they were intended. It also allowed participants to provide
input on creating additional questions based on their respective Muscle Dysmorphia Inventory (MDI) responses
group’s identity. For this purpose participants were known to the
researcher so that feedback could be provided. Prior to data analysis of the MDI, items responses were checked
Internal consistency reliability of the MDI was assessed using for completion. For the 16 items that were left blank, the mean
Cronbach’s a. Construct validity was assessed using a principal scores of the remaining item responses within that item’s
component analysis (PCA). With low to moderate correlations respective subscale were entered as the item response to ensure
between MDI subscales, a one-way MANOVA was used to a complete dataset.
determine differences within each subscale by group. Follow-up
tests were conducted using Tukey’s Honestly Significant Differ- Factor analysis
ence. Significance was set at an alpha level of .05. Although the MDI has been previously shown to be valid, a
principal component analysis (PCA) was conducted to establish
Results whether the factors and loadings of the five subscales with these
samples conformed to previous findings. Because only five
Participants and response rate subscales were internally consistent, a five-factor solution was
used in the factor analysis with an oblique, varimax rotation. All
Of the e-mail request to prospective participants in the NBB, subscale items loaded with their designated subscale except for
NNBB, and WTP groups, 143 participants responded accounting for item 23 which did not load highest on its designed factor of
33.2% of the invitations sent. Remaining participants were Physique Protection or highly on any factor; thus, analysis was
obtained from survey links posted on Internet forums. Participants conducted with the item removed. Once removed, five eigenvalues
that weight trained primarily for health or a sport other than exceeded the value of one with all five eigenvalues accounting for
bodybuilding were excluded from the study. at least 5% of the variance with a cumulative proportion of 61.02%.

Table 1
Participants’ physical and weight training data by mean and standard deviation.

Group Weight (lbs) Height (in.) Body fat (%) Years weight training Work outs per week Length of work outs (min)

Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD

NBB 192.63 24.92 68.45 8.17 9.43 3.11 12.90 7.76 5.31 1.57 72.69 21.18
NNBB 211.49 29.56 65.91 13.92 10.28 2.36 16.02 10.26 5.21 1.32 72.28 21.49
WTP 195.15 34.27 69.93 5.09 12.83 7.04 8.51 8.16 4.42 1.23 69.21 27.80
ATH 226.93 40.86 73.05 3.27 10.31 4.68 5.68 2.44 3.89 0.90 79.58 26.65
T. Baghurst, C. Lirgg / Body Image 6 (2009) 221–227 225

With a five-factor loading, all subscale items loaded with their the Physique Protection subscale, where the ATH group scored
designated subscale. Item factor loadings ranged from .55 to .89 on significantly higher.
designated factors and .00 to .36 on other factors. The second hypothesis stated that the NBB group would score
higher than the WTP and ATH groups on all of the MDI subscales.
Subscale correlations The NBB group scored significantly higher than the WTP group on
According to Huberty and Morris (1989), when addressing the Dietary Behavior and Supplement Use subscales. No significant
potentially underlying constructs a significant multivariate main differences were found between the two groups on the Physique
effect should be followed by a report detailing the inter- Protection, Size/Symmetry, and Pharmacological Use subscales.
correlations between the variables. However, an assessment of With respect to the ATH group, the NBB group scored significantly
correlations was carried out beforehand in order to determine higher on all subscales except the subscales Physique Protection
whether a multivariate assessment was warranted. Correlations and Pharmacological Use, where the ATH group scored signifi-
ranged between .01 and .50. cantly higher than the NBB group.
The third hypothesis stated that that the WTP group would score
MANOVA normality and outliers higher on all MDI subscales than the ATH group. The WTP group was
Because of the moderate correlations between subscales, a one- found to score significantly higher on the Dietary Behavior and Size/
way MANOVA was used to determine differences within each of Symmetry subscales. No differences were found on the Supplement
the five internally consistent subscales of the MDI by group. Prior Use subscale while the ATH group scored significantly higher on the
to analysis, item outliers were corrected or eliminated from Physique Protection and Pharmacological Use subscales.
analysis. Multivariate normality and outliers were calculated using
the maximum Mahalanobis distance. With a critical value of 22.46, Discussion
the data were reassessed for outliers following a maximum
Mahalanobis distance score of 36.16. As a consequence, two The purpose of this study was to identify differences in traits
participants from the NBB group and two participants from the associated with muscle dysmorphia between collegiate football
ATH group were removed from further calculations. Further review players, weight trainers for physique, competitive non-natural
of these participants’ scores indicated an almost random selection, bodybuilders, and competitive natural bodybuilders. Findings are
suggesting that they had either become mixed up by the questions discussed by group in relation to current research.
or did not take the questions seriously. Their removal resulted in a
maximum Mahalanobis distance score of 19.57 which falls under Non-natural bodybuilding (NNBB) and Natural bodybuilding (NBB)
the critical value. groups

MANOVA findings Although it was hypothesized that the NNBB group would score
The MANOVA revealed a significant main effect for group significantly higher than the other groups on all subscales of the
(Wilks’ lambda = .52, F(3, 289) = 13.68, p = .01, hp2 = .19) indicating MDI, when compared with the NBB group, the NNBB did not score
that one or more groups significantly differed in one or more significantly higher on any subscale except for the Pharmacological
subscale. Univariate results are shown in Table 2 with all five Use. This subscale difference is not surprising, as natural body-
subscales significant. builders are drug tested whereas non-natural bodybuilders are not.
However, it is important to recognize that on all other subscales,
Follow-up tests those competitive bodybuilders that are natural are equally
Tukey’s Honestly Significant Difference post hoc tests were concerned about those traits associated with muscle dysmorphia
performed to determine the differences between groups on each as are competitive non-natural bodybuilders. This is a clear
subscale of the MDI and address the preset hypotheses. The first strength of the current study, for a distinction between natural and
hypothesis stated that the NNBB group would score significantly non-natural bodybuilders has not always been considered in
higher than the NBB, WTP, and ATH groups on all subscales of the methodology of some research (e.g., Pickett et al., 2005).
MDI. When compared with the NBB group, the NNBB group did not Steroid use has been widely accepted as being a common
score significantly higher on any subscale other than the indicator of a drive for muscularity and muscle dysmorphia (Cafri
Pharmacological Use subscale. The NNBB score was significantly et al., 2005; Kanayama et al., 2006; McCreary & Sasse, 2000; Pope
higher than the WTP group on all subscales except for the Physique et al., 2000). However, based on the current findings, the NBB group
Protection and Size/Symmetry subscales. The NNBB group scored did not score significantly less on any other subscale of the MDI. This
significantly higher than the ATH group on all subscales except for is particularly important given that both the NNBB and NBB groups

Table 2
Means, standard deviations, and significance of MDI subscale scores by groupa.

MDI subscale NBB NNBB WTP ATH F (3, 289) Prob. Effect sizeb

Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD

Dietary Behavior 23.35 4.73 23.04 3.37 20.17c,d 4.89 16.56c,d,e 4.85 28.27 p = .01 hp2 = .23
Supplement Use 16.63 3.99 17.85 3.83 13.82c,d 4.96 12.3c,d 4.60 18.99 p = .01 hp2 = .17
Physique Protection 13.46 4.82 14.38 5.53 13.08 5.79 17.38c,d,e 5.62 9.02 p = .01 hp2 = .09
Size/Symmetry 20.02 5.14 21.15 4.92 19.52 5.67 16.83c,d,e 4.80 7.12 p = .01 hp2 = .07
Pharm. Use 3.65 1.38 6.29c 2.57 3.79d 1.47 5.62c,e 4.03 18.76 p = .01 hp2 = .17
Note. Higher subscale score indicates a greater likelihood of exhibiting the traits associated with that subscale. Possible scores for each subscale range between 0 and 30
except for Supplement Use and Pharmacology Use which have a maximum possible score of 24 and 12, respectively.
a
Significance was set at p < .05.
b
Partial eta squared.
c
Significance between NNB and other groups.
d
Significance between NNBB and other groups.
e
Significance between WTP and ATH groups.
226 T. Baghurst, C. Lirgg / Body Image 6 (2009) 221–227

were competitive bodybuilders and therefore likely to be heavily of gaming magazines, Harrison and Bond (2007) found that White
invested in their physiques. Although steroid use can often be preadolescent boys’ exposure to these magazines increased their
underreported (Pope et al.) due to a fear of exposure (Johnson, Jay, drive for muscularity. Similar findings were not evident in Black
Shoup, & Rickert, 1989), the data collected for this study was preadolescent boys which the authors suggest could be associated
confidentially collected online for both groups. Therefore, it is with the use of White characters portrayed in these magazines.
assumed that participants were more likely to report their Therefore, further research is needed to determine whether ethnic
pharmacological use than if data were collected in a one-to-one differences exist in male body image.
format. Second, participants in the ATH group may be adhering to
expected norms within this particular athletic group. The high
Weight training for physique (WTP) group Physique Protection scores may not be directly associated with
physique protection but rather team identity and traditions. For
A further strength of this study included the distinction example, the use of team clothing or uniforms is often a means for
between those that weight trained for competition and those increasing team cohesion and identity (Carron & Spink, 1993).
that weight trained for the primary purpose of enhancing their Third, Physique Protection may be higher in this group due to the
physique. Unlike studies such as Goldfield et al. (2006) where type of clothing worn during competition. Football players are
participants’ purpose for weight training was not clearly ascer- expected to wear tight fitting uniforms while potentially competing
tained, participants that weight trained primarily for health or for in a stadium of thousands of fans and/or millions of fans on television.
purposes not associated with their physique were removed from Thus, athletes may be exhibiting a form of social physique anxiety.
analysis. A similar weakness is evident in a study by Pickett et al. Social physique anxiety is defined as the anxiety an individual
(2005) in which competitive bodybuilders were not separated by experiences when evaluated on their physique by another individual
competition format (i.e. natural versus non-natural). Conse- (Hart, Leary, & Rejeski, 1989). Thus, future research should consider
quently, the current study’s groups were clearly defined allowing administering an evaluative tool such as the Social Physique Anxiety
for comparisons across groups. Scale (SPAS-7; Motl & Conroy, 2001) in conjunction with Physique
Although Pickett et al. (2005) reported that non-competitive Protection measures associated with muscularity.
weight trainers were equally invested in their appearance as Also of interest within the ATH group were the high scores of
compared with competitive bodybuilders, the findings of the the Pharmacological Use subscale when compared with the WTP
present study only partially support this finding. With respect to and NBB groups. Although the NBB group was expected to score
the MDI subscales, the WTP group scored significantly lower than low on this subscale since participants must be drug tested prior to
the NNBB and NBB groups on Dietary Behavior and Supplement competition, the ATH was also expected to score low given the
Use. No differences were found on the Physique Protection and illegality of steroids and many forms of diuretics and laxatives in
Size/Symmetry subscales, while WTP scored significantly lower the National Collegiate Athletic Association. However, a lack of
than the NNBB group on the Pharmacological Use subscale but not education regarding the terms steroids, laxatives, and diuretics
the NBB group where no significant differences were found. may have influenced this finding. For example, steroid users may
It is interesting to note that no differences were found between obtain their information from other users, books, and magazines
these three groups with respect to the Size/Symmetry subscale and (Perry, Lund, Deninger, Kutscher, & Schneider, 2005); thus,
might indicate that each group viewed this subscale as equally participants may not have clearly understood the terminology.
important. The key subscales that differentiate WTP from the NNBB
and NBB groups are the Dietary Behavior and Supplement Use Limitations and future research
subscales. Those within the WTP group may either lack dietary
knowledge or fail to value its importance. Also noteworthy is that the Several limitations and areas for future research not previously
WTP group had trained with weights for an average of four years less mentioned should be noted. First, not all data were collected in the
than the NBB group and six years less than the NNBB group. same manner. Participants within the ATH group completed their
Competitive bodybuilding requires careful and meticulous questionnaires as a group and close proximity to others may have
attention to nutrition. For example, in their review of literature influenced responses differently to those that completed their
associated with the macronutrient intake of competitive body- responses privately online. Second, although the ATH group was
builders, Lambert, Frank, and Evans (2004) highlight the crucial comprised entirely of competitive football players, no distinction
adjustments between macronutrients based on the phase of was made between positions due to insufficient group numbers. This
training or stage of the competitive bodybuilding season. Thus, is important, as certain positions typically require specific degrees of
although the WTP group scored significantly lower in areas of muscularity, body fat, and weight compared with others. Their
dietary behavior, this may have been due to a lack of experience in significantly younger age may also have impacted these findings.
the weight training environment, failure to value nutrition as Third, although participants’ ethnicity was ascertained, country
important, or a lack of knowledge concerning nutrition; knowledge of residence or country of origin was not. Though the idealization
of nutrition is a key component within this subscale. of a lean and muscular physique has been reported in various
countries (Hitzeroth et al., 2001; Pope et al., 1997; Ung et al., 2000),
Athlete (ATH) group some participants may have responded differently based on their
current location or country of origin. Future research should
Although Lantz et al. (2001) suggest that characteristics of consider comparing MDI scores based on participants’ origins, for
muscle dysmorphia may be evident in athletic populations, the ATH different cultures may view muscularity differently.
group scored lowest on all subscales of the MDI except for Physique Fourth, although the MDI has been reported to have strong
Protection where they scored highest. Several possible reasons may reliability and validity estimates, the current study suggests that
account for this finding. First, demographically the group was the internal consistency reliability of the Exercise Dependence
heavier, taller, younger, and had a larger percentage of African- subscale was low. In addition, one item was removed from the
American participants than the other groups. Although some Physique Protection subscale due to its poor loading. Thus, future
research has investigated body image among African-Americans, research needs to further address the reliability and validity of this
the vast majority of this research is among females (e.g., Hesse-Biber, Inventory. Because the MDI has not previously been tested on
Howling, Leavy, & Lovejoy, 2004; Snooks & Hall, 2002). In their study natural bodybuilders and football players, future research needs to
T. Baghurst, C. Lirgg / Body Image 6 (2009) 221–227 227

determine whether the findings of this study are synonymous with Hart, E. H., Leary, M. R., & Rejeski, W. J. (1989). The measurement of social physique
anxiety. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 11, 94–104.
these particular groups, or whether the reliability and validity of Hesse-Biber, S. N., Howling, S. A., Leavy, P., & Lovejoy, M. (2004). Racial identity and the
the MDI needs to be reevaluated. development of body image Issues among African American adolescent girls. The
Finally, no research has delineated between competitive natural Qualitative Report, 9, 49–79.
Hitzeroth, V., Wessels, C., Zungu-Dirwayi, N., Oosthuizen, P., & Stein, D. J. (2001).
and non-natural bodybuilders to determine whether the purpose of Muscle dysmorphia: A South African sample. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences,
steroid use among non-natural bodybuilders is due to reasons other 55, 521–523.
than competition format. This is a particularly interesting finding in Holmlund, C. (1997). Visible difference and flex appeal: The body, sex, sexuality and
race in the pumping iron films. In P. L. Moore (Ed.), Building bodies (pp. 87–102).
the current study, and future research should determine whether London: Rutgers University Press.
rationale for competing and reasons for becoming a competitive Huberty, C. J., & Morris, J. D. (1989). Multivariate analysis versus univariate analysis.
bodybuilder differ between groups. Currently, it is unclear why Psychological Bulletin, 2, 302–308.
International Natural Bodybuilding Association. (n.d.). Banned substances & policies.
bodybuilders choose to compete in natural or non-natural
Retrieved 07/11, 2007, from http://www.naturalbodybuilding.com/.
competitions. In the present study non-natural bodybuilders did Johnson, M. D., Jay, M. S., Shoup, B., & Rickert, V. I. (1989). Anabolic steroid use by male
not score significantly higher than the natural bodybuilders on any adolescents. Pediatrics, 83, 921–924.
subscale of the MDI except for pharmacology use. Therefore, after Kanayama, G., Barry, S., Hudson, J. I., & Pope, H. G., Jr. (2006). Body image and attitudes
toward male roles in anabolic-androgenic steroid users. The American Journal of
separating laxative, diuretic, and steroid use, future research should Psychiatry, 163, 697–703.
consider whether these pharmacological uses refer to a subset or Klein, A. M. (1993). Of muscles and men. The Sciences, 32–37.
subtype of muscle dysmorphia. Indeed, if other characteristics of Lambert, C. P., Frank, L. L., & Evans, W. J. (2004). Macronutrient considerations for the
sport of bodybuilding. Sports Medicine, 34, 317–327.
muscle dysmorphia do not appear to differentiate natural and non- Lantz, C. D., Rhea, D. J., & Cornelius, A. E. (2002). Muscle dysmorphia in elite-level power
natural bodybuilders, research should attempt to identify the lifters and bodybuilders: A test of differences within a conceptual model. Journal of
motivations behind why bodybuilders choose to use or not use Strength and Conditioning Research, 16, 649–655.
Lantz, C. D., Rhea, D. J., & Mayhew, J. L. (2001). The drive for size: A psycho-behavioral
steroids. Case studies of natural and non-natural bodybuilders may model of muscle dysmorphia. International Sports Journal, 5, 71–86.
help to determine root contributors of pharmacological use. Leit, R. A., Gray, J. J., & Pope, H. G., Jr. (2002). The media’s representation of the ideal
male body: A cause for muscle dysmorphia? International Journal of Eating Dis-
orders, 31, 334–338.
Conclusion McCreary, D. R., & Sasse, D. K. (2000). An exploration of the drive for muscularity in
adolescent boys and girls. Journal of American College Health, 48, 297–304.
Muscle dysmorphia and male body image in general are Mishkind, M. E., Rodin, J., Silberstein, L. R., & Striegel-Moore, R. H. (1986). The
embodiment of masculinity: Cultural, psychological, and behavioral dimensions.
receiving greater attention both within the scientific community American Behavioral Scientist, 29, 545–562.
and the popular media. However, a continued need for further Morgan, J. F. (2000). From Charles Atlas to Adonis complex: Fat is more than a feminist
research is necessary. This study furthers current beliefs about issue. Lancet, 356, 1372–1374.
Motl, R. W., & Conroy, D. E. (2001). The Social Physique Anxiety Scale: Cross-validation,
muscle dysmorphia through several avenues. First, it questions the
factorial invariance, and latent mean structure. Measurement in Physical Education
inclusion of pharmacological use as a trait associated with muscle and Exercise Science, 5, 81–95.
dysmorphia. Second, this study highlights the need for clear Olivardia, R. (2001). Mirror, mirror on the wall, who’s the largest of them all? The
sample definitions between natural and non-natural competitive features and phenomenology of muscle dysmorphia. Harvard Review of Psychiatry,
9, 254–259.
bodybuilders. Third, those that weight train for physique exhibit Peoples, J. G. (2001). The cultural construction of gender and manhood. In T. Cohen
some traits associated with muscle dysmorphia, but differ from (Ed.), Men and masculinity (pp. 9–19). Stamford, CT: Thomson Learning.
bodybuilding groups with respect to their nutritional habits or Perry, P. J., Lund, B. C., Deninger, M. J., Kutscher, E. C., & Schneider, J. (2005). Anabolic
steroid use in weightlifters and bodybuilders. Clinical Journal of Sports Medicine, 15,
knowledge. Finally, contrary to expectations, collegiate football 326–330.
players do not appear to exhibit traits associated with muscle Petrie, T., Austin, L., Crowley, B., Helmcamp, A., Johnson, C., Lester, R., et al. (1996).
dysmorphia to the same degree as other weight training groups. Sociocultural expectations of attractiveness for males. Sex Roles, 35, 581–602.
Pickett, T. C., Lewis, R. J., & Cash, T. F. (2005). Men, muscles, and body image:
Comparisons of competitive bodybuilders, weight trainers, and athletically active
References controls. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 39, 217–222.
Pope, H. G., Jr., Gruber, A. J., Choi, P., Olivardia, R., & Phillips, K. A. (1997). Muscle
Alexander, S. M. (2003). Stylish hard bodies: Branded masculinity in Men’s Health dysmorphia. An underrecognized form of body dysmorphic disorder. Psychoso-
magazine. Sociological Perspectives, 46, 535–554. matics, 38, 548–557.
Berscheid, E., Walster, E., & Bohrnstedt, G. (1973). The happy American body: A survey Pope, H. G., Jr., Phillips, K. A., & Olivardia, R. (2000). The Adonis Complex: The secret crisis
report. Psychology Today, 7, 119–131. of male body obsession. New York: The Free Press.
Blouin, A. G., & Goldfield, G. S. (1995). Body image and steroid use in male bodybuilders. Randall, A. J., Hall, S. F., & Rogers, M. F. (1992). Masculinity on stage: Competitive male
International Journal of Eating Disorders, 18, 159–165. bodybuilders. Studies in Popular Culture, 14, 57–69.
Cafri, G., Thompson, J. K., Ricciardelli, L., McCabe, M., Smolak, L., & Yesalis, C. (2005). Rhea, D. J., Lantz, C. D., & Cornelius, A. E. (2004). Development of the Muscle Dysmor-
Pursuit of the muscular ideal: Physical and psychological consequences and phia Inventory (MDI). The Journal of Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness, 44, 428–
putative risk factors. Clinical Psychology Review, 25, 215–239. 435.
Carron, A. V., & Spink, K. S. (1993). Team-building in the exercise setting. The Sport Roach, M. E., & Eigher, J. B. (1973). The visible self. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Psychologist, 7, 8–18. Schulze, L. (1990). On the muscle. In J. Gaines & C. Herzog (Eds.), Fabrications: Costume
Cash, T. F., Winstead, B. A., & Janda, L. H. (1986). The great American shape-up: Body and the female body (pp. 59–78). New York: Routledge.
image survey report. Psychology Today, 20, 30–37. Smith, D., Wright, C., Bruce-Low, S., & Hale, B. (2005). ‘‘Read with caution’’: A reply to
Creekmore, A. M., & Pedersen, E. (1979). Body proportions of fashion illustrations, Pickett et al. [Letter to the Editor]. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 39, 874–876.
1840–1940, compared with the Greek ideal of female beauty. Home Economics Snooks, M. K., & Hall, S. K. (2002). Relationship of body size, body image, and self-
Research Journal, 7, 379–388. esteem in African American, European American, and Mexican American middle-
Esco, M. R., Olson, M. S., & Williford, H. N. (2005). Muscle dysmorphia: An emerging class women. Health Care for Women International, 23, 460–466.
body image concern in men. Strength and Conditioning Journal, 27, 76–79. Stanford, J. N., & McCabe, M. P. (2005). Sociocultural influences on adolescent boys’
Garner, D. M. (1997). The 1997 body image survey results. Psychology Today, 30, 30–84. body image and body change strategies. Body Image, 2, 105–113.
Goldberg, J. (1997). Recalling totalities: The mirrored stages of Arnold Schwarzenegger. Streiner, D. L. (2003). Starting at the beginning: An introduction to coefficient alpha and
In
. P. L. Moore (Ed.), Building bodies (pp. 217–249). London: Rutgers University Press internal consistency. Journal of Personality Assessment, 80, 99–103.
Goldfield, G. S., Blouin, A. G., & Woodside, D. B. (2006). Body image, binge eating, and Tabachnick, B., & Fidell, G. L. S. (2001). Using multivariate statistics (4th ed.). New York:
bulimia nervosa in male bodybuilders. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 51, 160–168. Harper & Row.
Gonzalez, A., McLachlan, S., & Keaney, F. (2001). Anabolic steroid misuse: How much Thompson, J. K., & Cafri, G. (2007). The muscular ideal: An introduction. In J. K.
should we know? International Journal of Psychiatry in Clinical Practice, 5, 159–167. Thompson & G. Cafri (Eds.), The muscular ideal: Psychosocial, social, and medical
Hallsworth, L., Wade, T., & Tiggemann, M. (2005). Individual differences in male body- perspectives (pp. 3–13). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
image: An examination of self-objectification in recreational body builders. British Ung, E. K., Fones, C. S., & Ang, A. W. (2000). Muscle dysmorphia in a young Chinese male.
Journal of Health Psychology, 10, 453–465. Annals Academy of Medicine Singapore, 29, 135–137.
Harrison, K., & Bond, B. J. (2007). Gaming magazines and the drive for muscularity in Wesely, J. K. (2001). Negotiating gender: Bodybuilding and the natural/unnatural
preadolescent boys: A longitudinal examination. Body Image, 4, 269–277. continuum. Sociology of Sport Journal, 18, 162–180.

You might also like