Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 18

Behavior Analysis in Practice

https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-020-00490-3

RESEARCH ARTICLE

An Evaluation of “Balance”: a Home-Based, Parent-Implemented


Program Addressing Emerging Problem Behavior
Kelsey W. Ruppel 1,2 & Gregory P. Hanley 1,2 & Robin K. Landa 1,3 & Adithyan Rajaraman 1,4

# Association for Behavior Analysis International 2020

Abstract
Programs that prevent the development of severe problem behavior in young children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) are
critically needed. We describe a program designed to do this, and we report on a preliminary evaluation of its effects with four 3-
and 4-year-old children with ASD. Parents served as the primary implementers, with twice-weekly coaching from a Board
Certified Behavior Analyst. Direct measures and Aberrant Behavior Checklist scores reflected decreases in emerging problem
behavior. Direct measures also reflected increases in child communication, social, and cooperation skills, and parents rated the
process as highly acceptable. A randomized controlled trial will be required to evaluate the extent to which the program prevents
the development of problem behavior in young children with ASD.

Keywords autism . early intervention . parent training . prevention . problem behavior

The average age at an autism spectrum disorder (ASD) diagnosis these services would seem to be preventing the development of
in the United States is 4 years 4 months (Centers for Disease severe problem behavior, given that it is more common in chil-
Control and Prevention, 2019), and research focuses on ever- dren with ASD than in typically developing children and children
earlier identification (e.g., English, Tenenbaum, Levine, Lester, with other disabilities (American Psychiatric Association, 2013;
& Sheinkopf, 2019; Franchini et al., 2018). The American Bodfish, Symons, Parker, & Lewis, 2000; Farmer & Aman,
Academy of Pediatrics recommends early intervention services 2011; Mayes et al., 2012). However, most early intensive behav-
as soon as ASD is suspected (American Academy of Pediatrics, ioral intervention outcome studies report little or no data on prob-
2014; Myers & Plauché Johnson, 2007). A critical function of lem behavior (Smith & Iadarola, 2015). To our knowledge, no
programs from any discipline have empirically demonstrated the
Research Highlights prevention of severe problem behavior in children with ASD.
• Parents served as the primary implementers of a skill-building package, However, there is research suggesting the potential utility of
resulting in child cooperation with multistep tasks following denial of a
functional communication response. certain procedures. The Preschool Life Skills Program (Hanley,
• Rates of problem behavior decreased among all participants. Heal, Tiger, & Ingvarsson, 2007) focuses on systematically and
• Behavior analyst support occurred for 2 hr per week, making this package explicitly teaching children how to respond to challenging situa-
potentially useful where service availability is limited. tions they are likely to encounter in daily life. Target skills in-
• Evaluation of the preventive effects of this package on the development of
severe problem behavior in children with autism seems warranted. clude responding to name calls, functional communication,
responding calmly to delays and denials, following instructions,
* Kelsey W. Ruppel and interacting with peers. It has been demonstrated to prevent
kruppel@ftfbc.com the development of problem behavior in typically developing
children (Luczynski & Hanley, 2013). The Preschool Life
1
Psychology Department, Western New England University, Skills Program also reduces existing problem behavior and
Springfield, MA, USA strengthens social skills in typically developing preschoolers
2
Present address: FTF Behavioral Consulting, 40 Southbridge St., and children with developmental disabilities (Fahmie &
Suite 202, Worcester, MA 01608, USA Luczynski, 2018; Gunning, Holloway, & Healy, 2019;
3
Present address: May Institute, Worcester, MA, USA Gunning, Holloway, & Grealish, 2020; Robison, Mann, &
4 Ingvarsson, 2020). No published studies have evaluated its pre-
Present address: Psychology Department, University of Maryland,
Baltimore County, Baltimore, MD, USA ventive effects for children with developmental disabilities.
Behav Analysis Practice

However, given the program’s preventive effects for typically a balance between the two might improve the quality of both and
developing children and its intervention effects for children with ensure that children access important learning opportunities.
developmental disabilities, this seems a logical next step for This general framework of child-led time and adult-led time is
researchers. used in other programs, such as parent–child interaction therapy
Recent research outside of that investigating the Preschool (Masse, McNeil, Wagner, & Chorney, 2007). Parent–child inter-
Life Skills Program suggests some additional components that action therapy is a treatment program typically used with children
might enhance its effects for children with ASD. Comparative with significant, existing behavior problems. Although re-
research suggests that synthesized reinforcers (e.g., escape from searchers have begun to experimentally evaluate its use with
work to tangibles, attention, and sensory reinforcement) produce children with ASD (e.g., Ginn, Clionsky, Eyberg, Warner-
stronger treatment effects than isolated reinforcers (e.g., escape Metzger, & Abner, 2017; Scudder et al., 2019; Solomon, Ono,
only or praise only; Slaton & Hanley, 2018; Slaton, Hanley, & Timmer, & Goodlin-Jones, 2008), no prevention studies have
Raftery, 2017).1 Indeed, research has shown that synthesized been published. In fact, we suspect that there may be components
reinforcers delivered continuously at first, then intermittently important to preventing the development of problem behavior in
and unpredictably, establish social skills to the exclusion of prob- children with ASD that are not included in parent–child interac-
lem behavior in individuals with ASD (e.g., Beaulieu, Van tion therapy, such as the explicit teaching of functional commu-
Nostrand, Williams, & Herscovitch, 2018; Falcomata, Roane, nication and tolerance for request denials (Ala’i-Rosales et al.,
Muething, Stephenson, & Ing, 2012; Falcomata, Muething, 2019; Fahmie, Iwata, & Mead, 2016). We also suspect that some
Gainey, Hoffman, & Fragale, 2013; Greer, Fisher, Saini, components that are included may not be optimal for children
Owen, & Jones, 2016; Hanley, Jin, Vanselow, & Hanratty, with ASD, such as the recommendation that parents model, ex-
2014; Herman, Healy, & Lydon, 2018; Jessel, Ingvarsson, pand, describe, and imitate during child-led time (Ginn et al.,
Metras, Kirk, & Whipple, 2018; Rose & Beaulieu, 2019; 2017; Masse et al., 2007; Scudder et al., 2019). These could
Santiago, Hanley, Moore, & Jin, 2016; Strand & Eldevik, present as demands to children with ASD, decreasing the quality
2017; Taylor, Phillips, & Gertzog, 2018). Therefore, the effects of the child-led time and potentially evoking problem behavior.
of the Preschool Life Skills Program might be enhanced by in- Therefore, we developed a program that is the subject of the
corporating synthesized, personalized reinforcers to shape skills. current study—a home-based, parent-implemented program spe-
This synthesized, personalized reinforcement interval could cifically designed to prevent the development of severe problem
be thought of as a child-led playtime (see also Luckett, Bundy, behavior in children with ASD. It is based on the Preschool Life
& Roberts, 2007). A prevention program might begin by Skills Program, with modifications based on recent research (i.e.,
targeting adult–child interaction during child-led play to create synthesized reinforcement, a focus on balance between child-
the most reinforcing context possible for the child. Access to this and adult-led activity). Given this emphasis, we call it the
context could then be delivered contingent on the demonstration “Balance Program.” It also contains modifications to make it
of target skills during the remainder of the program. accessible to children not receiving intensive early intervention
The Preschool Life Skills Program might also be enhanced by services: The primary interventionists are parents, with 2 hr of
the addition of an emphasis on developing a balance between Board Certified Behavior Analyst (BCBA) support per week,
child- and adult-led activities. The National Association for the and teaching occurs in the home.
Education of Young Children (NAEYC) recommends both In this preliminary investigation, we evaluated the effects of
child- and adult-led experiences (NAEYC, 2009), and yet access the Balance Program using a single-subject experimental design,
to both types of experiences might be limited for children with and we report on direct and indirect measures of emerging prob-
ASD. Research suggests that adults avoid adult-led activities with lem behavior and direct measures of communication, social, and
children with a history of uncooperative behavior (Carr, Taylor, cooperation skills. We also report on procedural integrity, the
& Robinson, 1991). At the same time, the quality of child-led amount and distribution of teaching and support required to
experiences might be routinely compromised due to common achieve the observed outcomes, and the social validity of the
treatment recommendations. For example, caregivers are often goals, procedures, and outcomes. Evaluating the preventive ef-
encouraged to embed teaching into play activities for children fects of the Balance Program will necessitate a randomized con-
with ASD, but research suggests that children may not prefer this, trolled trial, and the present study is a precursor to such an effort.
and that embedded teaching can even punish some children’s
play (Heal & Hanley, 2011; Heal, Hanley, & Layer, 2009).
Therefore, separating child- and adult-led activities and striking Method
1
Studies calling synthesized reinforcers into question have not included compar- Participants
ative treatment phases (Fisher, Greer, Romani, Zangrillo, & Owen, 2016; Greer,
Mitteer, Briggs, Fisher, & Sodawasser, 2019), permitting the conclusion that iso-
lated and synthesized functional analyses may produce different results but pre- Participants were four young children and their primary care-
cluding conclusions about the comparative treatment utility of those results. givers (see Tables 1 and 2). Child inclusion criteria were are
Behav Analysis Practice

Table 1 Child characteristics

Name Age Sex Diagnosis Race/ethnicity Language EPB Tasks


(yr:mo) levela

Walt 4:3 M ASD White 3 Whining, yelling, hitting Cleaning up, putting on outerwear, sharing with
brother, listening to story
Jaden 4:7 M ASD White 4 Yelling, throwing,b kicking air, Cleaning up, handwriting, putting on outerwear,
stomping, squeezing parent’s face making bed, brushing teeth
Max 4:1 M ASD White 4 Whining, yelling, throwing,b Cleaning up, putting on particular clothes,
flopping, verbal attacks playing parent-selected game, playing alone
Kelly 3:3 F ASD Black/African 1 Whining, kicking air, pushing Cleaning up, listening to story, early VB tasks,
American items/people away, flopping playing alone

Tasks include those used in baseline and Steps 6 and thereafter; yr, years; mo, months; ASD, autism spectrum disorder; EPB, emerging problem behavior;
VB, verbal behavior
a
1 = nonverbal; 2 = one-word utterances; 3 = short disfluent sentences; 4 = full fluency
b
This included small/lightweight items, such as socks or game pieces, not in the direction of another person

follows: (a) between 3 and 6 years old, (b) diagnosed with Three master’s-level BCBAs participated, all of whom
ASD by an outside evaluator within the last year, (c) living were enrolled in a behavior-analytic doctoral program at the
at home, and (d) reported by parents to engage in frequent time of the study and were supervised by the second author, a
uncooperative behavior or nondangerous problem behavior doctoral-level behavior analyst. One BCBA completed all
that appeared to be socially mediated. visits for a family.
We recruited participants by distributing flyers to local ser-
vice providers, with the request that they offer them to families
of young children recently diagnosed with ASD. The first au- Overview
thor screened respondents using the inclusion criteria. For the
fourth criterion, the researcher asked the parent whether the The Balance Program was designed to fit within existing ser-
child exhibited any uncooperative or problematic behaviors, vice systems for children with ASD. Practice guidelines for
and if so, to describe the topographies, frequency, intensity, the behavior-analytic treatment of ASD include at least 2 hr
and situations in which they tended to occur. Children were per week of case supervision by a BCBA (The Council of
included when parent report suggested “emerging” socially me- Autism Service Providers, 2020). Therefore, the Balance
diated behavior problems. An “emerging” behavior problem Program involves two 1-hr visits by a BCBA per week, with
was defined as the parent-reported daily occurrence of uncoop- the recommendation that parents practice at least five trials
erative behavior or nondangerous problem behavior. If parents with their child twice daily between visits. To promote child
reported the occurrence of severe problem behavior (i.e., self- skill acquisition and provide parents with immediate rein-
injury, pica, elopement, aggression resulting in injury to others, forcement for their teaching efforts, we asked parents to teach
or behavior that resulted in observable damage to property),2 or in short, circumscribed practice sessions with short intertrial
if parent report suggested automatic reinforcement, we referred intervals (Francisco & Hanley, 2012) early in the program.
the child to other programs within our clinic. We specifically suggested that they not attempt to teach all
Although our participants were not referred for dangerous day or embed the trials in typical routines until Step 10. To
problem behavior, their parents nevertheless reported that the minimize demands on parents, we did not ask them to collect
emerging problem behavior had adverse effects on the family. data on practice sessions. We anticipated that it would be
Parents had difficulty running errands, completing household apparent from the child’s responding at the start of the next
tasks, and caring for siblings, and some reported persistent visit whether high-quality practice had occurred between
family conflict and/or mental health concerns related to par- visits, and we planned to continue practicing with the parent
enting issues. and child at the current level until performance criteria were
met (see Appendix D).
2 We conducted assessments during the first and second
Severe problem behavior is sometimes observed in children under 5. For
example, Kurtz et al. (2003) reported on 30 children’s self-injury. The mean visits. During subsequent visits, the BCBA taught the parent
age was 2 years 9 months, and all produced tissue damage; 83% engaged in to teach the child a skill (see Tables 3 and 4). Following
head banging (see also Kurtz, Chin, Huete, & Cataldo, 2012). Children who mastery of all skills, there was a break of at least 1 month,
engaged in such behaviors were not eligible for the current study, given the
preventive focus of the program. after which follow-up was conducted.
Behav Analysis Practice

Table 2 Parent characteristics

Child Participating Race/ethnicity Primary Highest education Employment Other Training/professional


name caregivers language children experience—early childhood

Walt Mother, mother White English College degree, Full time outside 1 No
postgraduate degree home
Jaden Mother, father White English College degree, high Full time outside 0, 1 Yes (general), no
school degree home
Max Mother White English Some college Not employed 1 No
outside home
Kelly Mother Black/African English College degree Full time outside 2 Yes (general)
American home

For Walt and Jaden, when a column contains one response, it applies to both parents. When a column contains two responses separated by a comma, one
response pertains to each parent

Setting and Materials program-related written materials, and empty bins, which
the BCBA provided. Items related to child- and adult-led
BCBA visits were conducted in one room in the partic- activities were identified via interview and sorted into
ipants’ homes until Step 10, at which point skills were two bins of the same size (approximately 30 cm3 or
practiced throughout the home. All materials originated 60 cm × 40 cm × 30 cm). Parents were not asked to
in the participants’ homes, except for video equipment, obtain any new items for the program.

Table 3 Objectives by teaching steps

Parent objective Child objective Selected references

Teaching 1 Present no EOs for Play in the absence of EPB. Ghaemmaghami, Hanley, Jin, & Vanselow, 2016; Hanley et al., 2014 &
process EPB. repl.; Heal et al., 2009; Heal & Hanley, 2011; McLaughlin & Carr, 2005;
NAEYC, 2009
2 Shape responding to Stop, look, say “yes” to Beaulieu, Hanley, & Roberson, 2012, 2013; Beaulieu & Hanley, 2014;
name. name call. Everett et al., 2005; Hamlet, Axelrod, & Kuerschner, 1984; Hanley et al.,
2007 & repl.; Kraus, Hanley, Cesana, Eisenberg, & Jarvie, 2012; Miller
et al., 2017
3 Shape functional Emit part of request. Carr & Durand, 1985; Ghaemmaghami, Hanley, Jin, & Vanselow, 2016;
communication. Hanley et al., 2007 & repl.; Hanley et al., 2014 & repl.; Horner & Day,
1991; Reeve & Carr, 2000
4 Teach complete Emit complete request. Ghaemmaghami, Hanley, Jessel, & Landa, 2018; Hanley et al., 2007 & repl.;
request. Hanley et al., 2014 & repl.; Hernandez, Hanley, Ingvarsson, & Tiger, 2007
5 Shape tolerance. Respond to denials calmly. Carr & Carlson, 1993; Fagen & Hill, 1987; Hanley et al., 2007 & repl.;
Hanley et al., 2014 & repl.; Skinner, 1953
6 Shape cooperation. Follow 1–3 instructions. Ghaemmaghami, Hanley, & Jessel, 2016; Hanley et al., 2007 & repl.; Hanley
et al., 2014 & repl.; Lalli & Casey, 1996; Piazza et al., 1996;
Tarbox et al., 2007
Post 7 Shape persistence. Complete all steps of an Hanley et al., 2007 & repl.; Hanley et al., 2014 & repl.
activity.
Generality 8 Practice with new Complete new activities. Stokes & Baer, 1977
instructions.
9 Practice without bins. Demonstrate all skills without
bins.
10 Practice during typical Demonstrate all skills during
routines around typical routines around
house. house.

EO, establishing operation; EPB, emerging problem behavior; repl., replications


Behav Analysis Practice

Table 4 Teaching procedures

Content Process Dosage

BCBA teaches How to: Written, vocal description and rationale Two 1-hr visits/week
parent Initiate trials Modeling
Prompt desired child responses & fade prompts Prompting, feedback as parent
Differentially reinforce child responding practices with child
Parent teaches child How to: Adult-initiated trials Two 1-hr visits/week with BCBA
Respond to name calls Prompting: and
Emit FCR Three step or wait out Two 5-trial sessions/day
Emit TR Differential reinforcement: independently
Cooperate with adult-led activities of Synthesized reinforcers
increasing length, diversity Intermittent, unpredictable schedule

BCBA, Board Certified Behavior Analyst; FCR, functional communication response; TR, tolerance response. See Appendix A for BCBA and parent
implementation integrity checklists

Data Collection and Response Definitions Response Definitions

Data Collection The definition of emerging problem behavior was individual-


ized per child (see Table 1). Hitting was rarely observed and of
The primary dependent variables were emerging problem low intensity—that is, distinct from “severe problem
behavior, child-led play, responding to name, functional behavior.”
communication, tolerance responding, and cooperation. Child-led play was defined as the child manipulating toy(s)
Responses per minute were used to analyze emerging prob- and/or engaging in activity-related conversation with the care-
lem behavior; percentage of opportunities correct was used giver in the absence of problem behavior. Responding to name
for responding to name, functional communication, and definitions were individualized per participant but generally
tolerance responding; and percentage of 20-s whole inter- involved stopping, looking toward the parent, and/or saying
vals was used to analyze play and cooperation. Data were “yes.” Functional communication was defined as any instance
also collected on parent integrity of the teaching proce- of a request for access to items or to play the child’s way, to
dures (see Appendix A), which are reported as a percentage escape or avoid items or parental instruction, or any combina-
of 20-s intervals in which no errors were made. A checklist tion thereof, emitted in the absence of emerging problem be-
was used to measure each BCBA’s fidelity to the support havior. The topography of the functional communication re-
procedures (see Appendix A). In baseline, all direct mea- sponse (FCR) taught was individualized for each child based
sures were collected in 5-min sessions. In the teaching pro- on the child’s language level. A tolerance response was defined
cess, data were collected from one 5-min or 5-trial session as any instance of an explicit and calm response to a delay or
at the start of each visit. In follow-up, data were collected denial imposed by the parent (e.g., “OK”). Cooperation was
from one or two 5-trial sessions at the start of the visit. All defined as behaving in accordance with the adult instruction or
sessions were video-recorded and scored later. expectation in place at the time. For example, if the adult asked
Indirect measures included a semistructured parent inter- the child to color and left the area, the expectation that the child
view called the Individualized Features of Reinforcement color would be considered in place until the adult returned. See
Meeting (InFORM; see Appendix B); the Aberrant Table 1 for participant-specific tasks.
Behavior Checklist, Second Edition (ABC-2; Aman &
Singh, 2017); and social validity assessments (see Interobserver Agreement
Appendix C). The purpose of the InFORM, administered
during the first visit, was to identify the materials and ac- Interobserver agreement was evaluated by having a sec-
tivities to be used during teaching. The ABC-2 is a 58-item ond independent observer review the video recordings
rating scale completed by parents/caregivers and often used and collect data on all target behaviors at a separate
in evaluations of interventions for children with ASD out- time from the primary data collector. Each session was
side of behavior analysis (Kaat, Lecavalier, & Aman, divided into 20-s intervals. Interobserver agreement for
2014). The ABC-2 was completed by each participant’s emerging problem behavior was calculated by dividing
primary caregiver at baseline and again after Step 10. the smaller total number of occurrences by the larger
Parents completed versions of the social validity assess- total number of occurrences in each interval, adding
ment at baseline, after Step 10, and at follow-up. up the proportions, dividing by the number of intervals,
Behav Analysis Practice

and multiplying by 100. Interobserver agreement for all Teaching Procedures The procedures by which BCBAs
other measures was calculated by dividing the number taught parents and parents taught children are outlined in
of agreements (intervals marked with the same symbol Table 4. Before teaching began, the BCBA described two
by both data collectors) by the number of agreements prompting approaches—three-step prompting (verbal,
plus disagreements, and then multiplying by 100. model, physical prompt; e.g., Tarbox, Wallace, Penrod,
Interobserver agreement data were collected for at least & Tarbox, 2007) and a “wait-out” (parent delivers a
20% of sessions across all conditions and participants verbal instruction, removes tangibles unrelated to the
and averaged at least 86% (range 67%–100%) for all instruction, and waits for the child to follow the
participants and all measures. instruction; Piazza, Moes, & Fisher, 1996). Parents were
asked to select the approach they preferred. Jaden’s,
Design Max’s, and Kelly’s parents opted to use three-step
prompting. Walt’s parents opted to use a wait-out.
Functional control by the independent variable over child Each teaching step built on the previous ones such that the
emerging problem behavior and over parent teaching behavior child continued to practice and, intermittently, contact imme-
was demonstrated using a nonconcurrent multiple-baseline diate reinforcement for all previously learned skills. The order
across-participants design. Functional control over child skills of trial types varied across sessions such that the response that
(responding to name, FCR, tolerance responding, and cooper- would result in immediate reinforcement on each trial was
ation) was demonstrated via repeated measures and replication unpredictable (see Appendix D).
of the effects of the independent variable on multiple depen-
dent variables across points in time. The effects of the package Posttest The data from Step 7 were considered the posttest. At
on the balance between child- and adult-led time were inferred this time, all the skills had been taught and the same contextual
rather than demonstrated. features as in the baseline were present.

Generality Steps 8, 9, and 10 were considered generality steps,


Procedures
as they extended the process beyond that which was assessed
in baseline. Step 8 involved introducing new activities, Step 9
Natural Baseline
involved items kept in their regular locations (rather than
bins), and Step 10 involved practice in new areas of the home
The BCBA distributed the materials identified in the parent
and during natural routines.
interview and directed the parent to engage with the child as
they normally would. Two or three 5-min sessions were con-
Performance Criteria The teaching process was considered
ducted. These data were used to assess the balance between
complete following two consecutive sessions at Step 10 in
time spent on child-led and adult-led activities, prior to any
which the child displayed (a) near-zero rates of emerging
instruction to the parent in this regard.
problem behavior, (b) independent skills (responding to
name, functional communication, and tolerance
Prompted Baseline responding) in most opportunities, (c) cooperation and
play in most applicable intervals, and when parent- and
The BCBA instructed the parent to allow the child to play for child-led times each composed roughly half of the
about half of the time and try to have the child complete tasks session.
for the other half of the time. No further prompts were given.
Three to five 5-min sessions were conducted. These data were
used to evaluate the parent’s ability to create balance between Booster and Follow-Up
child-led and parent-led activities when specifically asked to
do so, as well as to evaluate child behavior under these The BCBA conducted a booster session between 4 and 10
conditions. weeks after the last Step 10 visit, depending on the family’s
availability. The BCBA observed the parent and child practice
Teaching (Steps 1–10) Step 10 and provided feedback as needed. The BCBA then
returned later the same week to record a five-trial session of
Following baseline, participants moved into the teaching pro- Step 10. If the child’s performance met the criteria noted pre-
cess. Beginning with Step 1, the BCBA taught the parent to viously, a second session was recorded during that visit. If not,
teach each skill outlined in Table 3. At least one visit was the BCBA provided feedback, asked the parent to practice
dedicated to each step, with progression to the next step based with the child, and returned to record another session when
on parent and child performance (see Appendices for details). the family’s schedule allowed.
Behav Analysis Practice

Table 5 Modifications for Max and Kelly

Child Modifications Rationale Timing

Max (1) Moved to clinic-based practice: - Low child skill emission suggests need for more practice After first follow-up
(i) BCBA practiced Steps 8, 9 with child - Low parent-reported confidence suggests need for: phase
Parent collected treatment integrity data More modeling by BCBA
Parent & BCBA discussed scores Practice under more controlled conditions
(ii) Parent practiced Step 9 with child & BCBA,
then child alone
(2) Returned to home-based practice:
(i) BCBA practiced Steps 9, 10 with child before
parent did
(3) BCBA conducted parent education modules -Parent questions suggest need for additional education
Kelly (1) Added BCBA practice with child in home - Low child skill emission suggests need for more practice After Step 9 visits
(2) Eliminated response to name and tolerance - Decrease in parent time for practice for family/personal
response trials reasons

Note. BCBA, Board Certified Behavior Analyst. All BCBA-run sessions were conducted using the same procedures parents had used. Parent education
modules consisted of didactic instruction and discussion only, without the child present, and are detailed in Appendix E

Modifications were required (third and fourth panels on the left). All
participants played in the absence of emerging problem
Based on Max’s and Kelly’s performance and on feedback behavior for most child-led intervals in all conditions (sec-
from their parents, we made modifications for these partici- ond panel on the left), although an increase in play was
pants. These are detailed in Table 5. The reasons for the mod- observed with Jaden after teaching. Parental integrity to
ifications were different for the two children, leading us to the teaching procedures improved from the prompted base-
make different changes for each. line to postprogram for all participants, with more substan-
For Kelly, we attempted a set of modifications (marked as tial improvements observed for Max’s mother following
“Modif.” phase on Figure 5) that were unsuccessful, prior to modifications to the program (bottom-right panel).
the successful modifications detailed in Table 5 and marked as
“Express” on the figure. The unsuccessful modifications Emerging Problem Behavior
consisted of adding BCBA practice with Kelly in the home,
returning to Step 2, and reinforcing only independent re- Individual participant data are displayed in Figures 2, 3, 4 and
sponses. When these modifications did not yield the desired 5. Figure 6 displays emerging problem behavior data from the
rates of independent FCRs over six visits, we consulted with prompted baseline and the first three steps of teaching for all
Kelly’s mother to determine which skills she most wanted participants, to highlight the multiple-baseline across-partici-
Kelly to learn. She indicated the FCR and cooperation. pants design.
Therefore, we developed a shortened version of the program During the natural baseline, all participants except Walt
called “Balance Express,” which omitted responding to a emitted some emerging problem behavior. Jaden and Max
name call and tolerance responding. emitted it following certain parent actions during play (e.g.,
when parents did not follow their instructions), and Kelly
emitted it during her mother’s attempts to implement adult-
Results led time. During the prompted baseline, all parents imposed
some adult-led activities, and all participants engaged in in-
Summary creasing (Walt, Jaden, Max) or stable (Kelly) rates of emerg-
ing problem behavior. With the introduction of Teaching Step
Figure 1 summarizes data for all participants. Black sym- 1, rates of emerging problem behavior decreased to zero or
bols represent performance given the standard program, near zero and remained below the baseline average throughout
and gray symbols represent Max’s and Kelly’s perfor- the teaching process and follow-up for all participants, except
mance with their parents, in the home, following modifi- for a few isolated spikes for Max and Kelly.
cations. For all participants, emerging problem behavior
(top-left panel) decreased from the prompted baseline to Play
postprogram (posttest, generality, and follow-up), even
given balance between child- and adult-led times (top- Walt and Kelly engaged in play in the absence of emerging
right panel) and given that social skills and cooperation problem behavior for 100% of the child-led time in baseline
Behav Analysis Practice

Figure 1 Summary Data for All Participants. Note. The baseline mean 8, 9, and 10. The follow-up mean was calculated from all sessions of
was calculated from all sessions of the prompted baseline for all measures follow-up. Gray symbols (Max, Kelly) represent performance in the
except for “Balance,” which was calculated from all sessions of the home, with the parent as implementer, following modifications to the
natural baseline. The posttest mean was calculated from all sessions of package
Step 7. The generality test mean was calculated from all sessions of Steps

(Figures 2 and 5, second panels). Jaden and Max engaged in the time in which play was permitted, but the child either did
play for the entire child-led time, but their play was sometimes not play or played while emitting emerging problem
accompanied by emerging problem behavior (Figures 3 and 4, behavior). In Step 1, Jaden’s and Max’s play in the absence
second panels; black portions of the bars reflect play in the of emerging problem behavior increased to 100%. All partic-
absence of emerging problem behavior; gray portions reflect ipants engaged in play for all or most of the allotted time
Behav Analysis Practice

Figure 2 Individual Data for


Walt. Note. Numbers in brackets
refer to teaching steps. Nat. =
natural baseline; Prompt. =
prompted baseline; Teach. Integ.
= teaching integrity

across all teaching sessions and follow-up. Because there was 2. Across subsequent teaching steps and follow-up,
little change in the quality of play from baseline to teaching, responding generally remained high for Walt, Jaden, and
we characterize our participants as having had initially strong Max. Kelly’s responding was more variable. Following the
child-led play skills. first set of modifications to Kelly’s program, responding im-
mediately increased and became more stable; however, it be-
Responding to Name came variable again following the reintroduction of Step 3.

Walt’s and Max’s parents did not call their names during Functional Communication
baseline, and Jaden’s and Kelly’s did so infrequently.
Neither Jaden nor Kelly responded to these name calls. All caregivers attempted to restrict access to tangibles and/or
Responding emerged only following the introduction of Step place demands during the prompted baseline, creating
Behav Analysis Practice

Figure 3 Individual Data for


Jaden. Note. Numbers in brackets
refer to teaching steps. Nat. =
natural baseline; Prompt. =
prompted baseline; Teach. Integ.
= teaching integrity

opportunities for children to emit requests to retain the tangi- Tolerance Responses
bles and escape the demands. However, only Max did so in
the absence of problem behavior, and he only did so on one Because participants emitted few or no requests using
out of four opportunities. Responding emerged for all partic- words in baseline, there were few explicit request denials
ipants in Steps 3 and 4. Walt and Jaden emitted FCRs in most by parents and therefore few opportunities to emit a tol-
or all opportunities across subsequent steps. Max’s and erance response. Tolerance responding emerged follow-
Kelly’s responding was variable until modifications were ing the introduction of Step 5 for Walt, Jaden, and Max
made, at which time responding for both became high and and in the third visit at Step 5 for Kelly. Responding
stable and remained so across subsequent phases. maintained in most opportunities across subsequent steps
Behav Analysis Practice

Figure 4 Individual Data for


Max. Note. Numbers in brackets
refer to teaching steps. At parent
request, Step 4 (gain attention)
was terminated prior to mastery.
Nat. = natural baseline; Prompt. =
prompted baseline; Teach. Integ.
= teaching integrity; BCBA =
Board Certified Behavior Analyst

for Walt and Jaden. Max’s responding was variable until requested intervals with his father but only in 29% of intervals
modifications were made, at which time it became high with his mother.
and stable and remained so across subsequent phases.
Kelly’s responding decreased following Step 7, and the Balance
tolerance response was not included in her modified
program. During the natural baseline, most of the time was spent in
child-led activity for all participants. During most prompted
Cooperation baseline sessions, time was either allocated disproportionately
to child- or adult-led activity. During Step 1, time was again
All participants engaged in some cooperation during baseline; allocated exclusively to child-led activity by design, but this
however, it averaged less than 50% of requested intervals for decreased gradually across teaching steps for all participants
all participants. Cooperation was targeted beginning in Step 6, until follow-up, at which point child- and adult-led time were
and it increased for Walt, Jaden, and Kelly. Max’s cooperation allocated more equally.
was variable but increased to consistently high levels follow-
ing modifications. Parent Integrity to Teaching Procedures
At follow-up, Walt and Max emitted cooperation in all
requested intervals, and Kelly emitted cooperation in 95% of Parent integrity to the teaching procedures increased up-
requested intervals. Jaden emitted cooperation in 100% of on the introduction of the teaching process for all
Behav Analysis Practice

Figure 5 Individual Data for


Kelly. Note. Numbers in brackets
refer to teaching steps. At parent
request, Step 4 (gain attention)
was terminated prior to mastery.
Nat. = natural baseline; Pr. =
prompted baseline; Teach. Integ.
= teaching integrity; BCBA =
Board Certified Behavior
Analyst; Modif. = modification

participants. It remained high for Walt’s, Jaden’s, and Time Requirement


Kelly’s parents across teaching and follow-up. For
Max’s mother, it decreased upon the introduction of The entire process, from the InFORM through follow-up, re-
Step 2 and remained variable across teaching, although quired 15 one-hour BCBA visits for Walt, 20 for Jaden, 59 for
it was generally higher than in baseline. Following mod- Max, and 56 for Kelly.
ifications to the program, the level of teaching integrity
increased and variability decreased.
Aberrant Behavior Checklist–2

The results for each subsection of the ABC-2 are summarized


BCBA Integrity to Support Procedures
in Table 6, with raw scores appearing first and percentile
ranks3 in parentheses. According to the ABC-2’s authors, a
BCBA integrity to the support procedures was scored for
score in the 75th percentile or above suggests the need for
an average of 17% of sessions (range 14%–22%) across
intervention (Aman & Singh, 2017). At baseline, both of
all three conditions (baseline, teaching process, and
follow-up) for all participants and averaged at least 3
Based on 1,036 parent ratings of children under the age of 6 with an ASD
93% (range 86%–100%). diagnosis (Aman & Singh, 2017).
Behav Analysis Practice

Figure 6 Emerging problem


behavior multiple baseline across
participants

Walt’s parents rated his behaviors at or above the 75th per- highly acceptable (M = 7), and their comfort implementing
centile for Hyperactivity/Noncompliance and Inappropriate the procedures as high (M = 6.7). At Step 10 and follow-up,
Speech. Max’s mother rated his behavior at the 75th percentile parent ratings of their satisfaction with the effects outside of
for Irritability. All participants’ parents rated behaviors in all BCBA visits varied across participants (M = 5.6, range 3–7).
the other categories below the 75th percentile, supporting our Generally, Walt’s and Jaden’s parents rated themselves as
designation of the participants’ behaviors as “emerging” (rath- more satisfied than Max’s and Kelly’s. Walt’s, Jaden’s, and
er than “severe”) problems. Kelly’s parents provided only positive responses to open-
The ABC-2’s authors state that generally, a reduction of ended questions, and Max’s mother did not respond to any
one standard deviation or more might be considered clinically open-ended questions. (See Appendix C for participant-
significant (Aman & Singh, 2017). Walt’s Parent A’s ratings specific scores and comments.)
met this criterion in the areas of Hyperactivity/Noncompliance
and Inappropriate Speech, and his Parent B’s ratings met this
criterion in all areas except Stereotypic Behavior. Max’s
mother’s ratings met this criterion in the areas of Irritability Discussion
and Stereotypic Behavior.4 By contrast, Jaden and Kelly did
Direct measures suggest that the Balance Program reduced
not meet this criterion in any area. However, their baseline
ratings were lower (at or below the 50th percentile), making emerging problem behavior, improved social and cooperation
skills, and was associated with the development of balance
this outcome unsurprising. All participants’ raw scores im-
between child- and adult-led activities for all participants.
proved by at least three points in at least one category, and
none worsened by the same amount. These outcomes were demonstrated in the home, with parents
providing all or most of the teaching. Parents socially validat-
ed the goals and procedures, and an indirect, standardized
Social Validity measure of problem behavior detected general improvements.
In other words, the Balance Program produced important child
All parents rated the goals of the program as very important behavior outcomes relevant to children with an ASD
(M = 6.9 on a 7-point Likert-type scale), the procedures as diagnosis.
Our evaluation also revealed some areas for improvement
4
All participants displayed some restricted/repetitive behavior or interests, and important directions for future research. First, two out of
consistent with the diagnostic criteria for ASD. The Stereotypic Behavior
section of the ABC-2 focuses on motor stereotypy specifically. For our par- four children acquired all the targeted skills given the “stan-
ticipants, vocal stereotypy and restricted interests were more common. dard” package. Modifications to the program were associated
Behav Analysis Practice

Table 6 Aberrant behavior checklist (ABC-2) scores (with percentiles)

Name Irritability Social Withdrawal Stereotypic Behavior Hyperactivity/ Noncompliance Inappropriate Speech

Max. 45 Max. 48 Max. 21 Max. 48 Max. 12


Clin. Sig. 20 Clin. Sig. 15 Clin. Sig. 8 Clin. Sig. 29 Clin. Sig. 6

Pre Post Pre Post Pre Post Pre Post Pre Post

Walt
Parent A 9 (37th) 1 (<16th) 4 (25th) 0 (<16th) 0 (16th) 1 (25th) 35 (84th) 9 (16th) 6 (75th) 0 (16th)
Parent B 18 (70th) 3 (<16th) 13 (70th) 4 (25th) 1 (25th) 1 (25th) 32 (75th) 13 (32nd) 6 (75th) 1 (32nd)
Jaden 6 (25th) 6 (25th) 9 (50th) 6 (37th) 1 (25th) 1 (25th) 19 (50th) 21.4 (50th) 2 (37th) 2 (37th)
Max 21 (75th) 12 (50th) 1 (<16th) 0 (<16th) 4 (50th) 0 (16th) 28 (70th) 19 (50th) 3 (50th) 4 (63rd)
Kelly 7 (32nd) 2 (<16th) 7 (37th) 3 (16th) 1 (25th) 2 (25th) 12 (32nd) 12 (32nd) 1 (45th) 1 (45th)

Clin. Sig. = clinically significant score per ABC-2 authors. Boldface denotes a clinically significant reduction according to criteria provided in the ABC-2
manual. Post measures reported for Max were collected following Session 58. Mean scores for a sample of children with autism spectrum disorder, as
rated by their parents, are as follows: Irritability = 12.8 (50th percentile); Social Withdrawal = 10 (50th percentile); Stereotypic Behavior = 5 (50th
percentile); Hyperactivity/Noncompliance = 18.7 (50th percentile); and Inappropriate Speech = 3.7 (63rd percentile; Aman & Singh, 2017)

with more robust performance for the other two children, and to teaching, she demonstrated improved teaching integrity.
some of these modifications might be incorporated from the Although the current data do not allow for conclusions about
beginning for future participants. In addition, on the social which changes were responsible for either Max’s or his
validity assessment, some parents provided equivocal ratings mother’s improvements, his mother may have benefited from
for the outcomes of the program outside of BCBA visits; the higher dose of modeling from the BCBA and from the
therefore, steps might be taken to improve outcome generality. experience of scoring teaching integrity until she did so with
Once these areas have been addressed, future researchers no errors. In future in-home applications, the BCBA could
might consider a randomized controlled trial to evaluate the model implementation or show video models and teach the
program’s preventive effects on problem behavior in children parent to accurately evaluate teaching integrity.
with ASD. The in-home, parent-implemented Balance Program could
All children in the present study acquired at least a simple also be considered a Tier 1 intervention in a response-to-
FCR, and three out of four acquired responding to their name intervention model. That is, most or all children admitted to
and tolerance to denial responses. It is likely that initial lan- early intervention services could receive the Balance Program,
guage skills are a moderator of outcomes in this program. The implemented by parents in the home. If the Balance Program
three participants who acquired responding to their name and does not produce the desired outcomes for a child, a Tier 2
tolerance response communicated in short, disfluent sentences intervention would be indicated and considered. A Tier 2 in-
or with full vocal-verbal fluency at the start of the program, tervention might consist of professionally implemented ses-
whereas the participant who did not acquire these responses sions a few times per week, either in the home or in a clinic. A
(Kelly) was nonverbal at the start of the program. Kelly’s Tier 3 intervention, should it be required, might consist of
individual data suggest that these responses were attainable intensive outpatient or inpatient services. Therefore, under this
for her in isolation; in several sessions, she emitted high levels conceptualization, given that the Tier 1 intervention did not
of responding to her name, but she never emitted criterion yield a general-enough change in Max’s behavior, Max expe-
levels of responding to her name and the FCR in the same rienced a Tier 2 version of the program.
session, suggesting that the desired conditional discrimination Walt’s, Jaden’s, and Max’s performances suggest a possi-
was not established. Future researchers should evaluate the ble relation between teaching integrity averaging at least 80%
program with additional participants who have little verbal and robust child outcomes—child performance reached
behavior in baseline. If similar outcomes are observed, the criteria in phases in which teaching integrity averaged 80%
program might prescribe a modified approach (e.g., Balance or higher and did not otherwise. Future implementers might
Express) for children with little initial language and/or parents consider adding a role-play component to the program and
who foresee having limited time to dedicate to practice. coaching parents until they demonstrate at least 80% correct
Max’s modifications also suggest revisions to the program. implementation before beginning to teach their children a giv-
Max’s performance improved substantially in sessions imple- en step. In other words, in future iterations, the parent-training
mented by the BCBA in the clinic with higher teaching integ- procedures might include all the steps of behavioral skills
rity than his mother had exhibited. When his mother returned training (BST; e.g., Drifke, Tiger, & Wierzba, 2017;
Behav Analysis Practice

Sawyer, Crosland, Miltenberger, & Rone, 2015). We did not We inferred, rather than demonstrated, control by the pro-
initially incorporate role-play because of expected time con- gram over the balance between adult- and child-led activities.
straints in practice and because we hoped the program might In early teaching steps, child-led play was delivered contin-
be useful in telehealth settings, in which role-play is more gent on relatively short responses, and a great deal of the
difficult. However, future researchers might consider adding session was spent in child-led play by design; balance was
opportunities for parents to demonstrate robust teaching skills achieved gradually, emerging around Step 6 or later. We
prior to practicing with their children. deemed it undesirable to extend participants’ baselines for
Parents’ ratings of the goals and procedures of the program the time that would be required to demonstrate stability until
were overwhelmingly positive, and parents reported that the after balance emerged for earlier participants. However, given
BCBA support was enough to make them feel comfortable the demographic and geographic differences among the fam-
teaching the skills. However, parents’ ratings of the outcomes ilies, and the varying amounts of time between baseline and
of the program outside of BCBA visits were mixed, with demonstration of balance, it seems implausible that balance
Walt’s and Jaden’s parents rating the outcomes more highly would have emerged as a function of some factor other than
than Max’s and Kelly’s. Walt and Jaden progressed through the program.
the program more quickly than Max and Kelly, with no mod- There is precedent for teaching each of the skills in the
ifications. Some variables that might have influenced both the Balance Program (e.g., Durand & Moskowitz, 2015; Everett,
child’s progress and parents’ satisfaction with outcomes out- Olmi, Edwards, & Tingstrom, 2005; Hanley et al., 2014;
side of visits include the amount and/or procedural integrity of Luczynski & Hanley, 2013); nevertheless, whittling down
practice outside of visits throughout the program. In the future, the program to the behaviors that are essential for preventing
researchers might collect data on between-visit practice and the development of severe problem behavior is an important
evaluate the effects of procedural modifications on the fre- task for researchers. Such an evaluation might be most pro-
quency and quality of practice, as well as the generality of ductive after a demonstration of the preventive effects of the
outcomes. Interestingly, Kelly’s mother’s open-ended com- program via a randomized controlled trial; the question will
ments on the effects of the program seem to contrast with then be which components of the program are necessary to
her rankings, suggesting that future researchers might find produce those preventive effects. It is possible, for example,
multiple types of social validity measures informative. that differentially reinforcing various durations of cooperation
Future researchers implementing the program using a with personalized and synthesized reinforcers may be enough.
single-subject design might also consider design modifica- Future researchers, before or after a randomized controlled
tions to enhance the demonstration of functional control over trial, might also investigate the moderating role of initial play
all child skills by the program. In the present study, there were skills. All children engaged with the materials their parents
no baseline opportunities for children to demonstrate some of had nominated as preferred for most of the child-led time in
the skills. Future researchers could ask parents to provide op- baseline, and no parents prompted their children to do so.
portunities for specific skills during the prompted baseline or Despite these robust child performances in baseline, all
afterward, as its own baseline condition, if no opportunities BCBAs conducted instruction in Step 1 to address parent
are observed in the prompted baseline. behavior—such as interfering with child toy manipulation or
However, even when parents did not present opportunities attempting to teach—that might have made access to the play
in baseline, we have several reasons to infer that children materials a less potent reinforcing context. It is unclear what
lacked these skills and acquired them as a function of the the effects of the program would have been had the partici-
program. First, it is unlikely that a child has had pants not demonstrated robust child-led play in baseline. It
opportunities to acquire a skill if, over repeated seems probable that baseline play behavior moderates the ef-
observations, the parent does not provide opportunities for fects of the program; whether this is the case and how to
the child to emit that skill. Skill acquisition requires practice; address it are important research questions.
Luczynski and Hanley (2013) demonstrated that, in the ab- Despite the many remaining questions, the promising ef-
sence of explicit teaching, typically developing preschoolers fects of the Balance Program shown in the current study—and
did not develop similar skills. The fact that the parents did not the studies showing positive effects of similar skill-based
provide baseline opportunities for commonplace parent–child teaching programs in different service models (e.g., Hanley
interactions suggests that parental attempts had been et al., 2007; Hanley et al., 2014; Jessel et al., 2018; Robison
extinguished by persistent child noncooperation or punished et al., 2020)—suggest that Balance may be a useful program
by emerging problem behavior, as suggested by Carr et al. for professionals supporting parents of young children with
(1991). Finally, all parents rated the goals of the program as ASD in the home.
very important. It seems unlikely they would have done so if
children had a history of reliably emitting these skills in rele- Supplementary Information The online version contains supplementary
material available at https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-020-00490-3.
vant opportunities.
Behav Analysis Practice

Author Note This manuscript was prepared in partial fulfillment of the Carr, E. G., & Durand, V. M. (1985). Reducing behavior problems
doctoral program requirements of the Psychology Department at Western through functional communication training. Journal of Applied
New England University. We thank Amy J. Henley, Rachel H. Behavior Analysis, 18(2), 111–126. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.
Thompson, and Jonathan W. Pinkston for their comments on earlier ver- 1993.26-157.
sions of this manuscript. Carr, E. G., Taylor, J. C., & Robinson, S. (1991). The effects of severe
behavior problems in children on the teaching behavior of adults.
Compliance with Ethical Standards Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,24(3), 523–535. https://doi.
org/10.1901/jaba.1991.24-523.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2019). Spotlight on: Delay
Conflict of interest During the peer review and publication process for between first concern to accessing services. https://www.cdc.gov/
this manuscript, Kelsey Ruppel and Gregory Hanley received salaries
ncbddd/autism/addm-community-report/delay-to-accessing-
from FTF Behavioral Consulting, an organization that offers training on
services.html
the Balance Program among other services. Neither were employed by
Drifke, M. A., Tiger, J. H., & Wierzba, B. C. (2017). Using behavioral
FTF Behavioral Consulting before or during the conduct of the study.
skills training to teach parents to implement three-step prompting: A
Robin Landa and Adithyan Rajaraman declare no conflict of interest.
component analysis and generalization assessment. Learning and
Motivation, 57, 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lmot.2016.12.001.
Ethical approval All procedures performed in studies involving human Durand, V. M., & Moskowitz, L. (2015). Functional communication
participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institu- training: Thirty years of treating challenging behavior. Topics in
tional review board of the university and with the 1964 Helsinki declara- Early Childhood Special Education, 35(2), 1–11. https://doi.org/
tion and its later amendments. 10.1177/0271121415569509.
English, M. S., Tenenbaum, E. J., Levine, T. P., Lester, B. M., &
Informed consent Informed consent was obtained from all individual Sheinkopf, S. J. (2019). Perception of cry characteristics in 1-
participants included in the study. month-old infants later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 49, 834–844.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-3788-2.
Everett, G. E., Olmi, D. J., Edwards, R. P., & Tingstrom, D. H. (2005).
References The contributions of eye contact and contingent praise to effective
instruction delivery in compliance training. Education and
Ala’i-Rosales, S., Cihon, J. H., Currier, T. D. R., Ferguson, J. L., Leaf, J. Treatment of Children, 28(1), 48–62.
B., Leaf, R., McEachin, J., & Weinkauf, S. M. (2019). The big four: Fagen, S. A., & Hill, J. M. (1987). Teaching acceptance of frustration.
Functional assessment research informs preventative behavior anal- Teaching Exceptional Children, 19, 49–51.
ysis. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 12, 222–234. https://doi.org/10. Fahmie, T. A., Iwata, B. A., & Mead, S. C. (2016). Within-subject anal-
1007/s40617-018-00291-9. ysis of a prevention strategy for problem behavior. Journal of
Aman, M. G., & Singh, N. N. (2017). The Aberrant Behavior Checklist Applied Behavior Analysis, 49(4), 915–926. https://doi.org/10.
(2nd ed.). Slosson Education Publications. 1002/jaba.343.
American Academy of Pediatrics. (2014). AAP publications reaffirmed Fahmie, T. A., & Luczynski, K. C. (2018). Preschool life skills: Recent
or retired. Pediatrics, 134, e1520. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds. advancements and future directions. Journal of Applied Behavior
2014-2679. Analysis, 51(1), 183–188. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.434.
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Neurodevelopmental disor- Falcomata, T. S., Muething, C. S., Gainey, S., Hoffman, K., & Fragale, C.
ders. In Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th (2013). Further evaluations of functional communication training
ed.). https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596.dsm.01 and chained schedules of reinforcement to treat multiple functions
Beaulieu, L., & Hanley, G. P. (2014). Effects of a classwide teacher- of challenging behavior. Behavior Modification, 37(6), 723–746.
implemented program to promote preschooler compliance. Journal https://doi.org/10.1177/0145445513500785.
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 47(3), 594–599. https://doi.org/10. Falcomata, T. S., Roane, H. S., Muething, C. S., Stephenson, K. M., &
1002/jaba.138. Ing, A. D. (2012). Functional communication training and chained
Beaulieu, L., Hanley, G. P., & Roberson, A. A. (2012). Effects of schedules of reinforcement to treat challenging behavior maintained
responding to a name call and group call on preschoolers’ compli- by terminations of activity interruptions. Behavior Modification, 36,
ance. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 45(4), 685–707. https:// 630–649. https://doi.org/10.1177/0145445511433821.
doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2012.45-685. Farmer, C. A., & Aman, M. G. (2011). Aggressive behavior in a sample
Beaulieu, L., Hanley, G. P., & Roberson, A. A. (2013). Effects of peer of children with autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism
mediation on preschoolers’ compliance and compliance precursors. Spectrum Disorders, 5(1), 317–323. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 46(3), 555–567. https://doi. 2010.04.014.
org/10.1002/jaba.66. Fisher, W. W., Greer, B. D., Romani, P. W., Zangrillo, A. N., & Owen, T.
Beaulieu, L., Van Nostrand, M. E., Williams, A. L., & Herscovitch, B. M. (2016). Comparisons of synthesized and individual reinforce-
(2018). Incorporating interview-informed functional analyses into ment contingencies during functional analysis. Journal of Applied
practice. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 11(4), 385–289. https:// Behavior Analysis, 49(3), 596–616. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.
doi.org/10.1007/s40617-018-0247-7. 314.
Bodfish, J. W., Symons, F. J., Parker, D. E., & Lewis, M. H. (2000). Franchini, M., Duku, E., Armstrong, V., Brian, J., Bryson, S. E., Garon,
Varieties of repetitive behavior in autism: Comparisons to mental N., Roberts, W., Roncadin, C., Zwaigenbaum, L., & Smith, I. M.
retardation. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), (2018). Variability in verbal and nonverbal communication in in-
237–243. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1005596502855. fants at risk for autism spectrum disorder: Predictors and outcomes.
Carr, E. G., & Carlson, J. I. (1993). Reduction of severe behavior prob- Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48(10), 3417–
lems in the community using a multicomponent treatment approach. 3431. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-3607-9.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 26(2), 157–172. https://doi. Francisco, M. T., & Hanley, G. P. (2012). An evaluation of progressively
org/10.1901/jaba.1993.26-157. increasing intertrial intervals on the acquisition and generalization of
Behav Analysis Practice

three social skills. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 45(1), Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 40(1), 137–156. https://doi.
137–142. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2012.45-137. org/10.1901/jaba.2007.96-05.
Ghaemmaghami, M., Hanley, G. P., & Jessel, J. (2016). Contingencies Horner, R. H., & Day, H. M. (1991). The effects of response efficiency on
promote delay tolerance. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, functionally equivalent competing behaviors. Journal of Applied
49(3), 548–575. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.333. Behavior Analysis, 24(4), 719–732. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.
Ghaemmaghami, M., Hanley, G. P., Jessel, J., & Landa, R. (2018). 1991.24-719.
Shaping functional communication responses. Journal of Applied Jessel, J., Ingvarsson, E. T., Metras, R., Kirk, H., & Whipple, R. (2018).
Behavior Analysis, 51(3), 502–520. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba. Achieving socially significant reductions in problem behavior fol-
468. lowing the interview-informed synthesized contingency analysis: A
Ghaemmaghami, M., Hanley, G. P., Jin, S. C., & Vanselow, N. R. (2016). summary of 25 outpatient applications. Journal of Applied Behavior
Affirming control by multiple reinforcers via progressive treatment Analysis, 51(1), 130–157. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.436.
analysis. Behavioral Interventions, 31(1), 70–86. https://doi.org/10. Kaat, A. J., Lecavalier, L., & Aman, M. G. (2014). Validity of the
1002/bin.1425. Aberrant Behavior Checklist in children with autism spectrum dis-
Ginn, N. C., Clionsky, L. N., Eyberg, S. M., Warner-Metzger, C., & order. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(5),
Abner, J. P. (2017). Child-directed interaction training for young 1103–1116. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-013-1970-0.
children with autism spectrum disorders: Parent and child outcomes. Kraus, A. J., Hanley, G. P., Cesana, L. L., Eisenberg, D., & Jarvie, A. C.
Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 46(1), 101– (2012). An evaluation of strengthening precursors to increase pre-
109. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2015.1015135. schooler compliance. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 45(1),
Greer, B. D., Fisher, W. W., Saini, V., Owen, T. M., & Jones, J. K. 131–136. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2012.45-131.
(2016). Functional communication training during reinforcement Kurtz, P. F., Chin, M. D., Huete, J. M., & Cataldo, M. F. (2012).
schedule thinning: An analysis of 25 applications. Journal of Identification of emerging self-injurious behavior in young children:
Applied Behavior Analysis, 49(1), 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1002/ A preliminary study. Journal of Mental Health Research in
jaba.265. Intellectual Disabilities, 5(3–4), 260–285. https://doi.org/10.1080/
Greer, B. D., Mitteer, D. M., Briggs, A. M., Fisher, W. W., & 19315864.2011.600809.
Sodawasser, A. J. (2019). Comparisons of standardized and Kurtz, P. F., Chin, M. D., Huete, J. M., Tarbox, R. S. F., O’Connor, J. T.,
interview-informed synthesized reinforcement contingencies rela- Paclawskyj, T. R., & Rush, K. S. (2003). Functional analysis and
tive to functional analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, treatment of self-injurious behavior in young children: A summary
53(1), 82–101. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.601. of 30 cases. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36(2), 205–219.
Gunning, C., Holloway, J., & Grealish, L. (2020). An evaluation of par- https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2003.36-205.
ents as behavior change agents in the Preschool Life Skills Program. Lalli, J. S., & Casey, S. D. (1996). Treatment of multiply controlled
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. Advance online publication. problem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29(3),
https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.660 391–395. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1996.29-391.
Gunning, C., Holloway, J., & Healy, O. (2019). Evaluating the Preschool Luckett, T., Bundy, A., & Roberts, J. (2007). Do behavioural approaches
Life Skills Program to teach school readiness skills: An Irish pilot teach children with autism to play or are they pretending? Autism,
study. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 20(1), 48–68. 11(4), 365–388. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361307078135.
https://doi.org/10.1080/15021149.2018.1531962. Luczynski, K. C., & Hanley, G. P. (2013). Prevention of problem behav-
Hamlet, C. C., Axelrod, S., & Kuerschner, S. (1984). Eye contact as an ior by teaching functional communication and self-control skills to
antecedent to compliant behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior preschoolers. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 46(2), 355–
Analysis, 17(4), 553–557. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1984.17- 368. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.44.
553. Masse, J. J., McNeil, C. B., Wagner, S. M., & Chorney, D. B. (2007).
Hanley, G. P., Heal, N. A., Tiger, J. H., & Ingvarsson, E. T. (2007). Parent-child interaction therapy and high functioning autism: A con-
Evaluation of a classwide teaching program for developing pre- ceptual overview. Journal of Early and Intensive Behavior
school life skills. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 40(2), Intervention, 4(4), 714–735. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0100402.
277–300. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2007.57-06. Mayes, S. D., Calhoun, S. L., Aggarwal, R., Baker, C., Mathapati, S.,
Hanley, G. P., Jin, C. S., Vanselow, N. R., & Hanratty, L. A. (2014). Anderson, R., & Peterson, C. (2012). Explosive, oppositional, and
Producing meaningful improvements in problem behavior of chil- aggressive behavior in children with autism compared to other clin-
dren with autism via synthesized analyses and treatments. Journal of ical disorders and typical children. Research in Autism Spectrum
Applied Behavior Analysis, 47(1), 16–36. https://doi.org/10.1002/ Disorders, 6(1), 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2011.08.001.
jaba.106. McLaughlin, D. M., & Carr, E. G. (2005). Quality of rapport as a setting
Heal, N. A., & Hanley, G. P. (2011). Embedded prompting may function event for problem behavior: Assessment and intervention. Journal
as embedded punishment: Detection of unexpected behavioral pro- of Positive Behavior Interventions, 7(2), 68–91. https://doi.org/10.
cesses within a typical preschool teaching strategy. Journal of 1177/10983007050070020401.
Applied Behavior Analysis, 44(1), 127–131. https://doi.org/10. Miller, M., Iosif, A.-M., Hill, M., Young, G. S., Schwichtenberg, A. J., &
1901/jaba.2011.44-127. Ozonoff, S. (2017). Response to name in infants developing autism
Heal, N. A., Hanley, G. P., & Layer, S. A. (2009). An evaluation of the spectrum disorder: A prospective study. Journal of Pediatrics, 183,
relative efficacy of and children’s preferences for teaching strategies 141–146.e1. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2016.12.071.
that differ in amount of teacher directedness. Journal of Applied Myers, S. M., & Plauché Johnson, C. (2007). Management of children
Behavior Analysis, 42(1), 123–143. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba. with autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics, 120(5), 1162–1182.
2009.42-123. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2007-2362.
Herman, C., Healy, O., & Lydon, S. (2018). An interview-informed syn- National Association for the Education of Young Children. (2009). Key
thesized contingency analysis to inform the treatment of challenging messages of the position statement. https://www.naeyc.org/sites/
behavior in a young child with autism. Developmental default/files/globally-shared/downloads/PDFs/resources/position-
Neurorehabilitation, 21(3), 202–207. https://doi.org/10.1080/ statements/KeyMessages.pdf
17518423.2018.1437839. Piazza, C. C., Moes, D. R., & Fisher, W. W. (1996). Differential rein-
Hernandez, E., Hanley, G. P., Ingvarsson, E. T., & Tiger, J. H. (2007). A forcement of alternative behavior and demand fading in the treat-
preliminary evaluation of the emergence of novel request forms. ment of escape-maintained destructive behavior. Journal of Applied
Behav Analysis Practice

Behavior Analysis, 29(4), 569–572. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba. components. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 50(2), 252–
1996.29-569. 277. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.384.
Reeve, C. E., & Carr, E. G. (2000). Prevention of severe behavior prob- Smith, T., & Iadarola, S. (2015). Evidence base update for autism spec-
lems in children with developmental disorders. Journal of Positive trum disorder. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent
Behavior Interventions, 2(3), 144–160. https://doi.org/10.1177/ Psychology, 44(6), 897–922. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.
109830070000200303. 2015.1077448.
Robison, M. A., Mann, T. B., & Ingvarsson, E. T. (2020). Life skills Solomon, M., Ono, M., Timmer, S., & Goodlin-Jones, B. (2008). The
instruction for children with developmental disabilities. Journal of effectiveness of parent-child interaction therapy for families of chil-
Applied Behavior Analysis, 53(1), 431–448. https://doi.org/10.1002/ dren on the autism spectrum. Journal of Autism and Developmental
jaba.602. Disorders, 38, 1767–1776. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-008-
Rose, J. C., & Beaulieu, L. (2019). Assessing the generality and durability 0567-5.
of interview-informed functional analyses and treatment. Journal of Stokes, T. F., & Baer, D. M. (1977). An implicit technology of general-
Applied Behavior Analysis, 52(1), 271–285. https://doi.org/10.1002/ ization. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 10(2), 349–367.
jaba.504. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1977.10-349.
Santiago, J. L., Hanley, G. P., Moore, K., & Jin, C. S. (2016). The Strand, R. C. W., & Eldevik, S. (2017). Improvements in problem behav-
generality of interview informed functional analyses: Systematic ior in a child with autism spectrum diagnosis through synthesized
replications in school and home. Journal of Autism and analysis and treatment: A replication in an EIBI home program.
Developmental Disorders, 46(3), 797–811. https://doi.org/10. Behavioral Interventions, 33(1), 102–111. https://doi.org/10.1002/
1007/s10803-015-2617-0. bin.1505.
Sawyer, M. R., Crosland, K. A., Miltenberger, R. G., & Rone, A. B.
Tarbox, R. S. F., Wallace, M. D., Penrod, B., & Tarbox, J. (2007). Effects
(2015). Using behavioral skills training to promote the generaliza-
of three-step prompting on compliance with caregiver requests.
tion of parenting skills to problematic routines. Child and Family
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 40(4), 703–706. https://doi.
Behavior Therapy, 37(4), 261–284. https://doi.org/10.1080/
org/10.1901/jaba.2007.703-706.
07317107.2015.1071971.
Scudder, A., Wong, C., Ober, N., Hoffman, M., Toscolani, J., & Handen, Taylor, S. A., Phillips, K. J., & Gertzog, M. G. (2018). Use of synthesized
B. L. (2019). Parent-child interaction therapy (PCIT) in young chil- analysis and informed treatment to promote school reintegration.
dren with autism spectrum disorder. Child & Family Behavior Behavioral Interventions, 33(4), 364–379. https://doi.org/10.1002/
Therapy, 41(4), 201–220. https://doi.org/10.1080/07317107.2019. bin.1640.
1659542. The Council of Autism Service Providers. (2020). Applied behavior anal-
Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. The B. F. Skinner ysis treatment of autism spectrum disorder: Practice guidelines for
Foundation. healthcare funders and managers (2nd ed.). https://casproviders.
Slaton, J. D., & Hanley, G. P. (2018). Nature and scope of synthesis in org/wp-content/uploads/2020/03/ABA-ASD-Practice-Guidelines.
functional analysis and treatment of problem behavior. Journal of pdf
Applied Behavior Analysis, 51(4), 943–973. https://doi.org/10.1002/
jaba.498. Publisher’s Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdic-
Slaton, J. D., Hanley, G. P., & Raftery, K. J. (2017). Interview-informed tional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
functional analyses: A comparison of synthesized and isolated

You might also like