Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 19

INTERPERSONAL RELATIONS AND GROUP PROCESSES

The Psychological Bases of Ideology and Prejudice:


Testing a Dual Process Model

John Duckitt Claire Wagner and Ilouize du Plessis


University of Auckland University of Pretoria

Ingrid Birum
University of Kansas

The issue of personality and prejudice has been largely investigated in terms of authoritarianism and
social dominance orientation. However, these seem more appropriately conceptualized as ideological
attitudes than as personality dimensions. The authors describe a causal model linking dual dimensions of
personality, social world view, ideological attitudes, and intergroup attitudes. Structural equation mod-
eling with data from American and White Afrikaner students supported the model, suggesting that social
conformity and belief in a dangerous world influence authoritarian attitudes, whereas toughmindedness
and belief in a competitive jungle world influence social dominance attitudes, and these two ideological
attitude dimensions influence intergroup attitudes. The model implies that dual motivational and
cognitive processes, which may be activated by different kinds of situational and intergroup dynamics,
may underlie 2 distinct dimensions of prejudice.

The study of prejudice, ethnocentrism, and intergroup hostility effect is typically powerful, with effect sizes averaging around .50
has been an important topic of social scientific inquiry for much of (Adorno et al., 1950; Bierly, 1985; Duckitt, 1992), and pervasive,
the past century. During this time, two general approaches have holding across very different out-groups and even for totally
dominated research. One has viewed prejudice and ethnocentrism fictitious out-groups (Fink, 1971; Hartley, 1946).
as a group phenomenon to be explained in terms of intergroup The generality of prejudice suggests that some stable attribute of
dynamics and processes (Pettigrew, 1958; Sherif, 1967; Tajfel & individuals, such as personality or enduring beliefs, may cause a
Turner, 1979). A second, which has been relatively neglected for generalized disposition to adopt prejudiced and ethnocentric atti-
the past half century, has viewed prejudice as an individual phe- tudes. Two such individual-differences dimensions have empirical
nomenon and set out to explain individual differences in people’s support. First, an authoritarian personality dimension was origi-
propensity to hold ethnocentric intergroup attitudes (Adorno, nally described by Adorno et al. (1950) and later refined by
Frenkel-Brunswick, Levinson, & Sanford, 1950; Altemeyer, 1981; Altemeyer (1981, 1988, 1998), who used his Right Wing Authori-
Pratto, 1999; Sidanius & Pratto, 1999). tarianism (RWA) Scale to measure the three covarying attributes
The importance of the individual-differences approach to prej- of conventionalism, authoritarian aggression, and authoritarian
udice has been empirically demonstrated by the generality of submission. Second, Sidanius and Pratto (1993, 1999) conceptu-
prejudice. Persons who are unfavorably disposed to one particular alized and measured social dominance orientation (SDO) as a
out-group or minority tend to be less favorable to others. This “general attitudinal orientation toward intergroup relations, reflect-
ing whether one generally prefers such relations to be equal, versus
hierarchical” and the “extent to which one desires that one’s
John Duckitt, Department of Psychology, University of Auckland, ingroup dominate and be superior to outgroups” (Pratto, Sidanius,
Auckland, New Zealand; Claire Wagner and Ilouize du Plessis, Depart- Stallworth, & Malle, 1994, p. 742).
ment of Psychology, University of Pretoria, Pretoria, South Africa; Ingrid Both the RWA and the SDO Scales have been shown to pow-
Birum, Department of Psychology and Research in Education, University erfully predict a wide range of political, ideological, and inter-
of Kansas. group phenomena and to be particularly powerful predictors of
Portions of this article were included in a paper presented at the Annual
chauvinistic ethnocentrism and generalized prejudice (Altemeyer,
Scientific Meeting of the International Society of Political Psychology,
Seattle, Washington, July 2000, and some of the findings from Study 2
1988, 1998; Sidanius & Pratto, 1999). Despite this, the two scales
were also reported in Duckitt (2001). seem relatively independent, often being nonsignificantly or only
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to John weakly correlated with each other (Altemeyer, 1988, 1998; Mc-
Duckitt, Department of Psychology, University of Auckland, Private Bag Farland, 1998; McFarland & Adelson, 1996; Sidanius & Pratto,
92019, Auckland, New Zealand. E-mail: j.duckitt@auckland.ac.nz 1999). Altemeyer (1998) therefore recently suggested that the
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2002, Vol. 83, No. 1, 75–93
Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0022-3514/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0022-3514.83.1.75

75
76 DUCKITT, WAGNER, DU PLESSIS, AND BIRUM

RWA and SDO Scales measure two different kinds of authoritar- personality and social world views might together determine ideo-
ian personality, with both determining a basic susceptibility to logical attitudes and generalized prejudice.
generalized prejudice and ethnocentrism. D’Andrade (1992) and Strauss (1992) have suggested that indi-
However, a number of commentators have pointed out that the viduals’ social values and attitudes tend to express or represent
items of the RWA Scale and those of its predecessor, the F Scale what they have termed motivational goal schemas. These motiva-
(Adorno et al., 1950), do not pertain to personality traits and tional goal schemas consist of a motivational goal that has been
behavior but to social attitudes and beliefs of a broadly ideological made salient for the individual by the activation of particular social
nature (Feldman & Stenner, 1997; Goertzel, 1987; Saucier, 2000; schemas, or ways of perceiving social reality. Motivational goals
Stone, Lederer, & Christie, 1993, p. 232; Verkuyten & Hagen- that are important for individuals derive from social schemas that
doorn, 1998). The items of the SDO Scale also consist of state- are highly accessible for them. Such highly accessible social
ments of social attitude and belief, and Pratto et al. (1994; see also schemas can be seen more broadly as forming coherent social
Sidanius & Pratto, 1999) described the SDO Scale as a measure of world views, or relatively stable interpretations or beliefs about
enduring beliefs. Moreover, the RWA and F Scales have been others and the social world (Ross, 1993), so making particular
shown to be highly reactive to situational threat manipulations. For motivational goals generally salient for individuals. Individuals’
example, Altemeyer (1988) showed that scenarios that depicted world views reflect their social realities but should also be influ-
threatening social changes markedly increased RWA scores (cf. enced by their personalities, or generalized behavioral disposi-
also Doty, Peterson, & Winter, 1991; Meloen, 1983; Sales, 1973; tions, as personality influences the nature, outcome, and interpre-
Sales & Friend, 1973). tations of individuals’ interactions with their social realities. In
Both the RWA and the SDO Scales therefore seem more ap- addition to influencing individuals’ motivational goals indirectly
propriately viewed as measuring social attitude or ideological by influencing their world views, individuals’ personalities also
belief dimensions rather than personality. This is supported by a seem likely to directly influence the kind of motivational goals that
review of research showing that investigations of the structure of might be generally salient for them. The motivational goals that are
sociopolitical attitudes and sociocultural values have typically important or chronically salient for individuals are then directly
revealed two roughly orthogonal dimensions (Duckitt, 2001, see expressed in their social attitudes and values. For example,
Table 3 and pp. 46 – 49), with one corresponding broadly to RWA Schwartz (1992) has conceptualized sociocultural values as direct
and the other to SDO. The RWA-like dimension has typically been expressions of the importance of motivational goals for individuals
labeled social conservatism, traditionalism, or collectivism at one and cultural groups.
pole and personal freedom, openness, or individualism at the other This conceptual framework can therefore be readily applied to
pole. The SDO-like dimension has typically been labeled eco- the social attitude dimensions of RWA and SDO, with each di-
nomic conservatism, belief in inequality, power, or power distance mension representing a pair of opposing motivational goal sche-
at one pole and social welfare, social concern, egalitarianism, mas. Thus, high RWA expresses the motivational goal of social
universalism, or humanitarianism at the other pole (cf. Saucier, control and security, activated by a view of the world as dangerous
2000; Schwartz, 1992). and threatening. Low RWA expresses the opposing motivational
Although studies of the structure of sociopolitical attitudes and goal of personal freedom and autonomy, activated by a view of the
of social values have used a wide variety of measures of these two social world as safe, secure, and stable. The predisposing person-
broad value-attitude dimensions, the RWA and SDO Scales have ality dimension is that of social conformity versus autonomy.
typically emerged as the most powerful and consistent predictors Being higher in dispositional social conformity should create a
of ideological and intergroup phenomena (McFarland, 1998; Mc- greater readiness to perceive threats to the existing social order and
Farland & Adelson, 1996), probably because both are psychomet- see the social world as dangerous and threatening. High social
rically highly reliable and unidimensional measures. In contrast, conformity should also have a direct impact on authoritarian
most other measures of these two social attitude and social value attitudes by making the motivational goal of social control, secu-
dimensions have not been particularly reliable and have often been rity, and stability salient for the individual. There is empirical
contaminated by response biases such as direction of wording support for the core proposition here from numerous studies that
effects. However, these other measures have typically correlated have found an association between social threat and authoritarian-
highly with either RWA or SDO. For example, in his recent, ism (e.g., Altemeyer, 1988; Doty et al., 1991; Peterson, Doty, &
large-scale investigation, Saucier (2000) obtained a correlation of Winter, 1993; Sales, 1973; Sales & Friend, 1973). Altemeyer
.77 between the RWA Scale and a more general attitudinal mea- (1988) has also reported powerful correlations between his RWA
sure of social conservatism. Scale and a perception of the social world as dangerous and
If RWA and SDO are more appropriately conceptualized as threatening (measured by his Belief in a Dangerous World Scale;
measuring social or ideological attitudes and values, this suggests Altemeyer, 1988).
that the question of what, if any, personality dimensions may On the other hand, SDO has been empirically linked with the
underlie prejudice has not yet been answered. Indeed, it suggests personality dimension of toughmindedness versus tenderminded-
that the question could be profitably broadened to what underlying ness, a lack of empathy, and power motivation (Eysenck, 1954;
psychological dynamics in individuals might underlie both gener- Goertzel, 1987; McFarland, 1998; Pratto et al., 1994; Sidanius &
alized prejudice and the two ideological attitude dimensions of Pratto, 1999). Powerful correlations have also been found between
authoritarianism and social dominance. Drawing on the develop- the SDO Scale and Altemeyer’s (1998) recently constructed Per-
ment of the theoretical construct of social world view by psycho- sonal Power, Meanness and Dominance (PP-MAD) and Exploitive
logical anthropologists (D’Andrade, 1992; Ross, 1993; Strauss, Manipulative Amoral Dishonesty (E-MAD) Scales, which contain
1992), Duckitt (2000, 2001) has recently proposed a model of how items that were drawn largely from the older Machiavellianism
IDEOLOGY AND PREJUDICE 77

Scale (Christie and Geis, 1970) and that seem to express a view of Third, pressures for cognitive consistency should result in RWA
the social world as a competitive jungle characterized by a ruth- and SDO having positive, reciprocal, causal impacts on each other,
lessly amoral, Darwinian struggle for resources and power (e.g., at least in more ideologized societies in which politics is organized
“It’s a dog-eat-dog world where you have to be ruthless at times”). along a single left (low RWA and low SDO) versus right (high
This suggests that the personality dimension ultimately underlying RWA and high SDO) dimension. Thus, the correlation between
SDO might be toughmindedness versus tendermindedness, char- RWA and SDO over a number of samples was shown to be
acterized by the contrasting traits of being hard, tough, ruthless, significantly higher in Western European countries and New Zea-
and unfeeling to others, as opposed to compassionate, generous, land, possibly because powerful labor movements produced a
caring, and altruistic. Toughminded personalities tend to adopt a greater ideological organization of politics in those countries than
view of the world as a ruthlessly competitive jungle in which the in the United States (Duckitt, 2001). The correlation between
strong win and the weak lose, and this view tends to activate the RWA and SDO was also stronger for older than for younger
motivational goals of power, dominance, and superiority over persons, seemingly reflecting the effects of individuals being po-
others, which in turn are expressed in high SDO. Tenderminded litically socialized in late adolescence and early adulthood in
personalities, on the other hand, should tend to adopt the directly societies in which politics tends to be ideologically organized
opposing view of the social world as a place of cooperative and (Duckitt, 2001).
altruistic harmony in which people care for, help, and share with The strength of the effects of RWA and SDO on each other also
each other. This should make salient the motivational goal of seem likely to depend on which ideology is more dominant in a
altruistic social concern, which is expressed in low SDO. particular sociopolitical context. For example, in societies in which
The causal model of personality, world view, ideological atti- authoritarian– conservatism is the dominant ideology into which
tudes, and ethnocentrism that is suggested by this reasoning is people are socialized earlier and more intensively, pressures for
summarized in Figure 1 (see also Duckitt, 2000, 2001). The model ideological consistency between RWA and SDO should produce a
sees the two ideological attitude dimensions, RWA and SDO, as stronger effect of RWA on SDO than the reverse. Thus, the
causally determined by their corresponding personality and social direction and existence of effects between RWA and SDO should
world view dimensions. In addition, causal links are also proposed be context dependent, being weak or nonexistent in societies in
among the two personality dimensions, the two world view dimen- which politics is not ideologically organized, stronger in societies
sions, and the two ideological attitude dimensions. First, because in which politics is ideologically organized, and with directional
toughmindedness is socially deviant and undesirable, more so- differences possible where socialization into one ideological di-
cially conforming persons are less likely to report being high on mension is clearly dominant relative to the other.
toughmindedness. Modeling a causal path from social conformity Finally, on the basis of a good deal of previous research (Alte-
to toughmindedness would therefore statistically control the im- meyer, 1998; McFarland, 1998; McFarland & Adelson, 1996;
pacts of more socially desirable responding by persons higher in Pratto et al., 1994; Sidanius & Pratto, 1999), the model predicts
social conformity on toughmindedness as well as on a competitive- that both RWA and SDO will directly increase both pro-in-group
jungle world view, both of which tend to be seen as socially and anti-out-group attitudes. Personality and world view, however,
undesirable and unconventional in most sociocultural contexts. are not seen as having general, direct impacts on prejudiced or
Second, if people see the social world as a ruthlessly competi- ethnocentric attitudes, only indirect effects mediated through these
tive jungle, this should inevitably cause them to see it as more ideological attitudes.
dangerous and threatening. On the other hand, if people see the The model has been supported by recent research. Three studies
social world as more dangerous and threatening, this does not showed that the personality dimensions of social conformity and
necessarily also cause them to see it as a competitive jungle. This toughmindedness can be reliably measured and related as expected
suggests a causal path from a competitive world view to a dan- to ideological attitudes (Duckitt, 2001, Study 1). Thus, social
gerous one but not the reverse. conformity was strongly correlated with RWA but not with SDO,
whereas toughmindedness was correlated with SDO but not with
RWA. Structural equation modeling (SEM) with latent variables
showed good fit for the model in two large, European-origin
(Pakeha), New Zealand student samples (Duckitt, 2001, Study 2
and 3), with in-group and out-group attitudes being pro-European
and antiminority attitudes. All the predicted causal paths were
confirmed, except an originally suggested direct path from tough-
mindedness to SDO, which was then dropped from the model and
is therefore not shown in Figure 1 here. However, these two
analyses did reveal two relatively weaker but significant unpre-
dicted effects on prejudice. One was a direct effect of dangerous
world beliefs in increasing antiminority prejudice, and the second
was an inconsistent, direct effect of social conformity in reducing
Figure 1. A causal model of the impact of personality and social world antiminority prejudice.
view on the two ideological attitude dimensions of right-wing authoritari- Both these effects were interpreted as probably context depen-
anism (RWA) and social dominance orientation (SDO) and their impact on dent. Thus, dangerous world beliefs seemed likely to have a direct
intergroup attitudes. The dashed lines indicate context-dependent causal effect, increasing prejudice only when out-groups were in a di-
paths. rectly threatening relationship with the in-group. This seemed
78 DUCKITT, WAGNER, DU PLESSIS, AND BIRUM

plausible in terms of the tensions bedeviling contemporary Measures


majority–minority relations in New Zealand, with the Pakeha
RWA and SDO. Twenty items were randomly sampled from Alte-
(European origin) majority confronted by radical Maori activism
meyer’s (1996) RWA Scale, and 12 items were sampled from Pratto et al.’s
and a major influx of culturally different Asian and Polynesian (1994) SDO Scale so as to give equal numbers of pro- and contrait items
immigrants. This interpretation is also consistent with a second in each of the two shortened scales. The alpha coefficients for this sample
finding from these two studies. In both samples, promajority were .88 and .78, respectively.
attitudes had a powerful and highly significant impact on antimi- Social conformity and toughmindedness. We measured both of these
nority attitudes, a pattern of covariation that several prior studies personality constructs by asking participants to rate the degree to which
have suggested seems characteristic only for in-group– out-group personality trait adjectives were characteristic or uncharacteristic of their
relations that are directly competitive and threatening (Brown et “personality and behavior.” The 14 social conformity items (with 7 protrait
al., 1992; Duckitt & Mphuthing, 1998; Struch & Schwartz, 1989). and 7 contrait) were drawn from a 20-item personality trait adjective rating
The nonpredicted but inconsistent effect of social conformity in scale— originally developed by Saucier (1994)—after judges’ ratings had
excluded any items that might pertain to attitudes or beliefs. Item and factor
reducing prejudice was interpreted as possibly reflecting social
analyses showed these 14 items provided a unidimensional and reliable
norms against antiminority prejudice among traditionally liberal
scale (Duckitt, 2001). The alpha coefficient in this sample was .80, and the
New Zealand students, with the inconsistency due to fluctuations highest loading items were “obedient” and “predictable” (protrait) and
in the strength and salience of these norms with changing social “nonconforming” and “unconventional” (contrait).
and political circumstances. The Toughmindedness Versus Tendermindedness Scale had been devel-
The current research set out to test the generality of the theo- oped earlier (see Duckitt, 2001) from a core set of trait adjectives used by
retical model relating personality and world view to ideology and Goertzel (1987) to measure that construct. Item and factor analyses re-
prejudice by using SEM with latent variables in two very different vealed 24 items that were unambiguously personality or behavior trait
samples: American college students and White Afrikaans South descriptors of the construct and provided a highly reliable and unidimen-
African students. Second, it also set out to investigate several sional scale. The alpha coefficient in this sample was .91, and the two
hypotheses suggested by the nonpredicted effects obtained in the highest loading items were “harsh” and “uncaring” for protrait and “kind”
and “gentle” for contrait. All the items of these two scales are shown in
two earlier studies. Thus, it attempted to investigate whether belief
Appendix A.
in a dangerous world and pro-in-group attitudes would directly
Social world view: Belief in a dangerous world and belief in a
impact on anti-out-group attitudes only when out-groups seemed competitive-jungle world. The concept of social world view is defined as
to have a directly competitive and threatening relationship with the a coherent set of beliefs about the nature of the social world, specifically
in-group. It also set out to assess whether social conformity would about what people are like, how they are likely to behave to oneself, and
have direct effects, reducing negative out-group attitudes that how they should be responded to and treated. The world view of a
should be nonnormative in the social contexts studied. dangerous and threatening social world is conceptualized as having two
opposing poles. At one pole is the belief that the social world is a
dangerous and threatening place in which good, decent people’s values and
Study 1 way of life are threatened by bad people, whereas at the other pole is the
belief that the social world is a safe, secure, and stable place in which
Study 1 used a sample of American college students. For in- almost all people are fundamentally good. A balanced 10-item scale was
group attitudes, we used a measure of American nationalism, and used to measure belief in a dangerous world (see Duckitt, 2001). Eight of
for out-group attitudes, we used a measure of generalized prejudice these items were drawn from Altemeyer’s (1988) Belief in a Dangerous
toward a variety of ethnic and national out-groups (e.g., Arabs, World Scale, with 2 new, specially written items added. The alpha coef-
Africans, Asians, Japanese, Indians, Blacks, and Hispanics). Be- ficient for this 10-item scale was .80.
cause these groups collectively did not appear to be in a directly We measured belief in a competitive-jungle world using a balanced
20-item scale (see Duckitt, 2001). The construct was defined as the belief
competitive and threatening relationship to the United States at the
that the social world is a competitive jungle characterized by a ruthless,
time of the research during 1999, it seemed likely that neither
amoral struggle for resources and power in which might is right and
Americans’ belief in a dangerous world nor their nationalism winning everything (protrait pole) versus a view of the social world as one
would have direct impacts on these out-group attitudes. However, of cooperative harmony in which people care for, help, and share with each
because prejudice toward these groups should be generally viewed other (contrait pole). We selected items that were judged to conform to this
as nonnormative among American college students, we hypothe- definition from closely related existing measures such as Altemeyer’s
sized that social conformity would have a direct effect in reducing (1998) PP-MAD and E-MAD Scales, both of which drew on items from the
generalized prejudice to these out-groups. original Machiavellianism Scale, and some specially written items. The
resulting 20-item balanced scale was found to be unidimensional and
highly reliable (see Duckitt, 2001). The alpha coefficient in this sample
Method was .84. The highest loading items were “It’s a dog-eat-dog world where
you have to be ruthless at times” (protrait) and “The best way to lead a
Participants and Procedure group under one’s supervision is to show them kindness, consideration, and
treat them as fellow workers, not as inferiors” (contrait). All the items of
The research participants were 146 undergraduate students at a Mid- both the social world view scales are shown in Appendix B.
western American university who completed a survey questionnaire during American nationalism and generalized out-group prejudice. We mea-
1999. Fifty-seven were male, and 89 were female, and they had a mean age sured American nationalism using 11 items previously used by Pratto et al.
of 19.60 years (SD ⫽ 2.62). Expectation maximization (Schafer, 1997) was (1994) and Kosterman and Feshbach (1989) to measure nationalism. This
used to replace isolated missing values so that a covariance matrix based on item set was not balanced against acquiescence, as there were 10 protrait
the entire sample could be generated for the SEM analysis. and only 1 contrait item. The alpha coefficient was .83.
IDEOLOGY AND PREJUDICE 79

We measured generalized out-group prejudice using McFarland’s (1998) logical attitude scales are shown in Table 1. As expected, social
12-item balanced scale. The items express negative or positive sentiments conformity was highly significantly associated with higher RWA
toward a variety of out-groups relevant to Americans, such as Asians, but not SDO, whereas toughmindedness was associated with
Africans, Arabs, Bosnians, Hispanics, Blacks, Indians, Japanese, Russians, higher SDO but not RWA. Both RWA and SDO had significant
“other races,” “minorities,” and “foreign religions like Buddhism, Hindu-
positive correlations with both nationalism and prejudice, which in
ism, and Islam.” The alpha coefficient was .82.
turn were positively correlated with each other. There was a weak,
though significant, positive correlation between RWA and SDO.
Latent Variable Analyses Figure 2 shows the standardized path coefficients for the struc-
For the SEM analysis, the items of each scale were randomly assigned tural equation model. To simplify the diagram, we do not show the
to create three manifest indicators, with equal numbers of pro- and contrait manifest variables and paths from latent to manifest indicators.
items for each manifest indicator wherever possible. Item parcels were However, the paths from each of the latent variables to its manifest
used as manifest indicators rather than individual items to keep the model’s indicators were all highly significant and powerful, with the weak-
degrees of freedom to a reasonable level. The use of at least three indicators est coefficient being .66. The overall fit indices indicated good fit
has been recommended because models with fewer indicators for a latent for a model of this size, ␹2(236, N ⫽ 146) ⫽ 319.8, ␹2/df
variable may, in some cases, be underidentified (Bentler & Chou, 1987).
ratio ⫽ 1.35, RMSEA ⫽ .049 (90% confidence limits [CL] ⫽
Item parcels also had the considerable advantage over individual items of
providing more reliable manifest variables and so reducing random error as
.035, .063), SRMR ⫽ .066, population GFI ⫽ .95, CFI ⫽ .96. Fit
well as reducing systematic error due to method effects such as direction of for this model was clearly better than that for the null model, for
wording (pro- vs. contrait item formulation), which has been shown to which ␹2(276) ⫽ 2,636.4. Overall, the model accounted for sub-
substantially affect factor analyses of measures such as the RWA Scale stantial proportions of the variance in RWA (51%), SDO (46%),
(Altemeyer, 1981, 1988). In the analysis, the three manifest indicators for generalized out-group prejudice (67%), and nationalism (31%).
each scale were allowed to relate only to their one specific latent variable. The basic paths predicted by the model were all clearly signif-
Maximum likelihood estimation was used for these analyses and the icant. Thus, social conformity had a significant path to dangerous
following SEM analyses. We assessed overall model fit using Hu and world beliefs, and both social conformity and dangerous world
Bentler’s (1999) recent recommendations. Their investigation of the opti- beliefs had relatively strong paths to RWA. Toughmindedness had
mal cutoff values of standard maximum likelihood fit indices suggests that
a powerful, significant path to competitive world beliefs, and
good fit is best indicated by values close to or better than .06 for root-
mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA), .08 for standardized root-
competitive world beliefs had an equally powerful path to SDO.
mean-square residual (SRMR), and .95 for the comparative fit index (CFI) Just as in the earlier New Zealand research, toughmindedness did
and goodness-of-fit index (GFI). In the case of GFI, Steiger (1989) and not have a significant, direct path to SDO (when modeled, ␤ ⫽
others (Maiti & Mukerjee, 1990) have noted that Jöreskog and Sörbom’s ⫺.10, z ⫽ ⫺.94, p ⫽ .35). The links expected among these
(1996) widely used index of sample GFI is biased downward when degrees personality, world view, and ideological attitude variables were
of freedom are large relative to sample size and proposed a correction to also supported. Social conformity had a significant negative path
GFI that provides a more robust population estimate. Because the SEM to toughmindedness, and competitive world beliefs had a margin-
analyses in this and the next study tested very large models relative to ally significant positive path to dangerous world beliefs. RWA and
sample size, we used Steiger’s (1989) corrected population GFI (also SDO did have reciprocal positive paths to each other, but both
referred to as population gamma index) rather than Jöreskog and Sörbom’s
were weak, with a nonsignificant path from RWA to SDO and a
sample GFI. Finally, the widely used rule of thumb for very large models,
marginally significant path from SDO to RWA.
in which chi-square values tend to be large, of viewing a ␹2/df ratio of less
than 2 as a criterion of good fit was also used (Shumacker & Lomax, 1996). The effects of personality, world view, and ideology on in-group
and out-group attitudes were also as hypothesized. Both RWA and
SDO had significant, direct, positive paths to both nationalistic
Results
attitudes and out-group prejudice. Moreover, as predicted for this
The correlations among prejudice, nationalism, the two person- sample, neither dangerous world beliefs nor pro-in-group nation-
ality scales, the two social world view scales, and the two ideo- alistic attitudes had significant effects on anti-out-group prejudice.

Table 1
Correlations Among the Variables for Study 1 (N ⫽ 142–146)

Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8

1. RWA —
2. SDO .21* —
3. Social conf .44*** ⫺.08 —
4. Tough mind .00 .29*** ⫺.39*** —
5. Dang world .43*** .08 .15* ⫺.01 —
6. Comp world .00 .55*** ⫺.42*** .59*** .03 —
7. Nationalism .41*** .32*** .17* .19* .11 .29*** —
8. Prejudice .43*** .60*** ⫺.07 .28*** .18* .47*** .39*** —

Note. RWA ⫽ right-wing authoritarianism; SDO ⫽ social dominance orientation; Social conf ⫽ social
conformity; Tough mind ⫽ toughmindedness; Dang world ⫽ dangerous world; Comp world ⫽ competitive-
jungle world.
* p ⬍ .05. *** p ⬍ .001.
80 DUCKITT, WAGNER, DU PLESSIS, AND BIRUM

Figure 2. Standardized maximum likelihood coefficients from Study 1 (N ⫽ 146) for the structural equation
model with the following latent variables: two personality, two world view, two ideological attitude, nationalism,
and generalized out-group prejudice. To simplify, we do not show manifest variables or the paths from latent to
manifest variables. Dashed lines indicate nonsignificant paths. RWA ⫽ right-wing authoritarianism; SDO ⫽
social dominance orientation. ⫹ p ⬍ .10. * p ⬍ .05. ** p ⬍ .01.

As expected, the path from nationalism to out-group prejudice was not SDO and of toughmindedness with SDO but not RWA repli-
also nonsignificant. Finally, as predicted, social conformity had a cates the findings reported for samples in Canada, England, and
significant, direct effect, reducing out-group prejudice. New Zealand (Duckitt, 2001). A number of previous studies have
Modification indices (Lagrange multiplier tests) were used to reported positive correlations for RWA and SDO with both na-
test for additional nonpredicted effects that might be significant tionalism and prejudice and significant correlations between na-
and improve overall fit, in particular to check for evidence of any tionalism and prejudice in America (e.g., Altemeyer, 1998; Duc-
further unexpected direct effects of personality and world view on kitt, 2001; McFarland, 1998; McFarland & Adelson, 1996; Pratto
prejudice or nationalism. However, no other effects were found et al., 1994; Sidanius & Pratto, 1999). Finally, the correlation
that improved fit or would have been statistically significant if between RWA and SDO was positive and, although significant,
included in the model. relatively weak, as has been the case for previous findings with
Thus, the only direct effect of personality on the two outcome student samples in the United States (Altemeyer, 1998; Duckitt,
variables, prejudice and nationalism, was for social conformity, 2001, see Table 1 for a review of findings; McFarland, 1998,
which had its predicted direct effect of reducing prejudice (␤ ⫽ McFarland & Adelson, 1996; Pratto et al., 1994).
⫺.25, z ⫽ ⫺2.58, p ⬍ .005). In addition to this, social conformity The findings for the SEM analysis clearly support the model.
also had its expected indirect effect (primarily through RWA) of The overall fit was good, the paths predicted were significant and
increasing prejudice (␤ ⫽ .19, z ⫽ 2.19, p ⫽ .03). Thus, the two broadly of the magnitude expected, and the model accounted for
opposite effects for social conformity on prejudice almost exactly
substantial proportions of the variance in ideological and inter-
cancelled each other out (total effect from the SEM analysis was
group attitudes. These findings closely replicate the findings ob-
␤ ⫽ ⫺.06, z ⫽ ⫺.71, p ⫽ .48). In all other instances, the two
tained previously from SEM analyses in two large New Zealand
personality variables had their expected indirect effects through
samples (Duckitt, 2000, 2001), indicating that the model seems to
RWA or SDO of increasing prejudice and nationalism. The total
have cross-national validity. One minor difference from these prior
effects from the SEM analysis were therefore positive and signif-
findings is that RWA and SDO had significant positive impacts of
icant for social conformity on nationalism (␤ ⫽ .18, z ⫽ 2.79), for
moderate magnitude on each other in both the New Zealand
toughmindedness on nationalism (␤ ⫽ .17, z ⫽ 3.52), and for
toughmindedness on prejudice (␤ ⫽ .30, z⫽ 4.35). samples. In this American sample, there also appear to have been
positive reciprocal effects, but, as expected, they were weaker, so
that the effect for SDO on RWA was marginally significant and
Discussion
that of RWA on SDO was nonsignificant. However, the two
The correlational findings are entirely consistent with previous effects were reasonably similar in magnitude. Thus, these findings
research. Thus, the association of social conformity with RWA but are not inconsistent with the expectation that RWA and SDO
IDEOLOGY AND PREJUDICE 81

should have weak reciprocal effects on each other, and both of (W. Johnson, 1994). The second group was the numerically small,
these effects might have proved significant with greater statistical socially marginal, and politically conservative Indian minority,
power. This finding is consistent with the generally weak associ- who, after majority rule, tended to give electoral support to the
ation between RWA and SDO in North American student samples, White Afrikaner-dominated National party opposition (Guelke,
as opposed to the stronger associations for societies in which 1996). Thus, if in-group attitudes and dangerous world beliefs only
politics tends to be more highly ideologized, such as New Zealand, increase prejudice toward groups seen as directly competitive and
and for older, nonstudent American samples (cf. Duckitt, 2001, see threatening, then this should be the case for White Afrikaners’
Table 1 for a summary of prior North American studies). attitudes to Africans but not to Indians.
As predicted, dangerous world beliefs and pro-in-group nation- Tentative predictions could also be formulated for two other
alistic attitudes had no direct effects on out-group prejudice in this context-dependent effects proposed by the original model. First, in
American sample, in contrast to their positive significant effects in the new, post-Apartheid South Africa, anti-Black and particularly
the earlier New Zealand research. This is consistent with the anti-African prejudice is counternormative. Thus, even among
hypothesis that dangerous world beliefs and pro-in-group attitudes White Afrikaners, being higher in social conformity should have
only increase prejudice against out-groups that are in a directly direct effects in reducing anti-Black and particularly Anti-African
competitive and threatening relationship with the in-group. The prejudice. Second, the relationship between RWA and SDO should
SEM analysis also showed that the significant correlation obtained depend on how ideologized politics is in a particular society. In
between nationalism and prejudice in this sample and in previous South Africa, politics has always been ethnically rather than ideo-
American research (Altemeyer, 1998; McFarland, 1998; McFar- logically organized (Guelke, 1996; R. Johnson & Schlemmer,
land & Adelson, 1996; Pratto et al., 1994) appears to be noncausal, 1994). Thus, the correlation between RWA and SDO should be
reflecting the impact of RWA and SDO on both. relatively weak, as in the United States, and not powerful, as in
Social conformity also had its predicted direct effect of reducing New Zealand or Western European societies. Moreover, it has
prejudice, in contrast to its indirect effect, mediated through RWA, been argued that White Afrikaner nationalism in South Africa has
of increasing prejudice. The direct effect of reducing prejudice been primarily characterized by threat and authoritarianism arising
replicates the effect obtained in prior New Zealand research. This out of a historical experience of extreme insecurity (de Kiewiet,
seems plausibly explained by the counternormative nature of the 1957; MacCrone, 1937; see also the discussion in Duckitt, 1992,
negative attitudes being studied for both American and New Zea- pp. 98 –100). This suggests that ideological beliefs in group in-
land college students. Thus, in liberal social settings in which equality and the right to dominate other groups might be secondary
prejudice is nonnormative, social conformity seems to have two to a threat-driven, ideological emphasis on group control, cohe-
directly opposing effects on prejudice: an indirect effect of increas- sion, and security in this culture. This suggests that RWA should
ing prejudice, mediated through more authoritarian attitudes, as affect SDO more powerfully than the reverse.
well as a significant, direct effect of reducing prejudice.
Finally, one caveat that might be noted derives from the use of
Method
item packages for the SEM analyses. Although the use of item
packages for the SEM analyses was necessary to keep the model to Participants and Procedure
a reasonable size, this did mean that the SEM measurement model
did not provide as strong a test of the factorial differentiation of the A survey questionnaire was administered in late 1999 to introductory
variables as would be provided by item-level analyses. The issue psychology students at the University of Pretoria, a predominantly White
of factorial differentiation of the constructs seems particularly Afrikaans-language university in South Africa. The questionnaire was
relevant in the case of the two sets of relatively closely associated translated into Afrikaans and then back-translated to check equivalence. It
was completed by 233 White Afrikaans students, of whom 145 were
personality, world view, and ideological attitude variables, the first
female and 88 male, with an average age of 19.90 years (SD ⫽ 1.87).
being RWA, dangerous world beliefs, and social conformity and
the second being SDO, competitive-jungle world beliefs, and
toughmindedness (Duckitt, 2001). We therefore used item-level Measures
factor analyses in Study 2 to check the factorial differentiation of
RWA. We measured RWA using a balanced 14-item short form of
these two sets of variables. Altemeyer’s (1981) RWA Scale, which had previously been developed
from an item analysis of responses from a White South African student
Study 2 sample (Duckitt, 1993). The reliability and validity of this shortened RWA
scale was supported in a series of studies using South African samples
The second study tests the theoretical model using a very (Duckitt, 1993; Duckitt & Farre, 1994). In the present sample, however, 4
different sample and intergroup setting; that is, White Afrikaans of these items had nonsignificant item–total correlations and had to be
college students in South Africa. In-group attitudes were pro- discarded. These 4 items were all contrait and all involved expressions of
Afrikaner attitudes, and we assessed out-group attitudes toward support for antigovernment activism and radical dissidence. Thus, agree-
two different Black groups that seemed to vary considerably in the ment with these items could have reflected not just antiauthoritarianism but
also support for the right-wing conservative, radical Afrikaner activists
degree to which they stood in a directly competitive and threaten-
opposing the new left-wing Black government. The remaining 10-item
ing relationship to White Afrikaners. The first of these groups was RWA Scale was therefore only partially balanced, with 7 protrait and 3
the politically and numerically dominant Africans, who are Afri- contrait items. The alpha coefficient in this sample was .67.
kaners’ historical rival for power (e.g., Thompson, 1995) and who SDO. Ten items from the SDO Scale (Pratto et al., 1994) were ran-
now, after majority rule, pose an ongoing threat to the social and domly sampled to give equal numbers of pro- and contrait items. The alpha
economic advantages still enjoyed by Afrikaners in South Africa was .79.
82 DUCKITT, WAGNER, DU PLESSIS, AND BIRUM

Table 2
Fit Indices for the Confirmatory Factor Analyses With Correlated Trait and Correlated
Direction-of-Wording Method Factors for the Personality, World View,
and Ideological Attitude Item Sets

Analysis ␹2 df ␹2/df RMSEA SRMR GFI CFI

SDO, competitive-jungle world beliefs, and toughmindedness item set


American sample (Study 1)
Three-factor model 2,387.1 1,424 1.67 .063 .076 .83 .81
Best fitting two-factor model 2,523.6 1,426 1.77 .070 .122 .79 .78
One-factor model 2,681.9 1,427 1.88 .079 .073 .76 .75
South African sample (Study 2)
Three-factor model 1,563.9 854 1.83 .061 .067 .87 .80
Best fitting two-factor model 1,718.9 856 2.01 .071 .070 .84 .76
One-factor model 1,790.0 857 2.09 .074 .067 .82 .74

RWA, dangerous world beliefs, and social conformity item set


American sample (Study 1)
Three-factor model 1,383.3 856 1.62 .065 .088 .86 .76
Best fitting two-factor model 1,505.9 858 1.76 .073 .110 .83 .70
One-factor model 1,632.2 859 2.52 .086 .093 .78 .64
South African sample (Study 2)
Three-factor model 965.1 489 1.97 .067 .073 .89 .73
Best fitting two-factor model 1,058.3 491 2.16 .070 .080 .87 .68
One-factor model 1,080.6 492 2.20 .074 .077 .86 .67

Note. Goodness-of-fit index (GFI) is population GFI (Steiger, 1989). Study 1, N ⫽ 146; Study 2, N ⫽ 233.
RMSEA ⫽ root-mean-square error of approximation; SRMR ⫽ standardized root-mean-square residual; CFI ⫽
comparative fit index; SDO ⫽ social dominance orientation; RWA ⫽ right-wing authoritarianism.

Belief in a dangerous world. We measured belief in a dangerous world We therefore used item-level confirmatory factor analyses (CFAs), with
using the same 10 items as in the previous study, with an alpha in this pro- and contrait items loading both on their content factors and on separate
sample of .82. pro- and contrait method factors, to control direction-of-wording effects,
Belief in a competitive-jungle world. We measured belief in a using the data from both Study 1 and this study. For each set of related
competitive-jungle world using a balanced 14-item subset of the items used personality, world view, and ideological attitude items, the analyses com-
in the previous study, with an alpha of .76. pared the fit of the hypothesized three-factor model against the one-factor
Social conformity. We measured social conformity using the same 14 model and the 3 two-factor models, reflecting the three possible combina-
items as in the previous study, with an alpha of .72. tions of the three scales. As Marsh and Grayson (1995) have recom-
Toughmindedness. We measured toughmindedness using 20 of the mended, all three of the CFA models that have been used to analyze
items (with equal numbers of pro- and contrait items) used in the previous multitrait–multimethod data (correlated traits with uncorrelated methods,
study, with an alpha of .88. correlated traits with correlated methods, and correlated traits with corre-
Intergroup attitudes. The same eight generalized intergroup attitude lated uniqueness) were used for each analysis, but these produced essen-
items (four protrait and four contrait), selected to be appropriate to assess tially the same findings.
attitudes toward any social group, were used to assess attitudes toward Table 2 shows the fit indices for the CFAs with correlated trait and
White Afrikaners, Africans, and Indians. Examples of the items are “I have correlated method factors for the three-factor, one-factor, and best fitting
a very positive attitude to Indians/Africans/White Afrikaners” and “I can two-factor models for the two personality, world view, and ideological
understand people having a negative attitude to Indians/Africans/White attitude item sets. The three-factor solutions for both the SDO,
Afrikaners.” The alpha coefficients were .72 for attitudes toward White competitive-jungle world beliefs, and toughmindedness item set and the
Afrikaners, .84 for attitudes toward Africans, and .77 for attitudes toward RWA, dangerous world beliefs, and social conformity item set produced
Indians. reasonably acceptable solutions given the magnitude of these very large
models, with RMSEA close to .06. The one- and two-factor solutions were
Factorial Differentiation of the Measures less satisfactory, with RMSEA clearly exceeding .06. The chi-square
difference test indicated that the superiority of the three-factor solution to
We used item-level factor analyses to check the factorial differentiation all other solutions was highly significant (the smallest chi-square differ-
of the two relatively closely associated sets of personality, world view, and ence was 93.2, with df ⫽ 2, p ⬍ .0001).
ideological attitude measures, the first being RWA, dangerous world
beliefs, and social conformity, and the second being SDO, competitive-
jungle world beliefs, and toughmindedness. However, item-level explor- Results
atory factor analyses of both the RWA and the SDO Scales have typically
shown strong direction-of-wording method effects, with protrait and con- The correlations among all the variables are shown in Table 3.
trait items loading on separate method factors (Altemeyer, 1981, 1988, The correlation between RWA and SDO was .21, which, although
1998). Exploratory factor analyses of the scales used in this and the significant, is relatively weak, as expected (using Cohen’s, 1988,
previous study also revealed strong method factors, which tended to conventions of effect size strength). Social conformity was signif-
preclude clear-cut solutions. icantly associated with RWA but not SDO, and toughmindedness
IDEOLOGY AND PREJUDICE 83

Table 3
Correlations Among the Variables for Study 2 (N varies between 215 and 231)

Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

1. RWA —
2. SDO .21** —
3. Social conformity .44** .00 —
4. Tough minded ⫺.11 38*** ⫺.17* —
5. Dangerous world .45*** 29*** .17* .12 —
6. Competitive world .03 .58*** ⫺.17** .48*** .19** —
7. Pro-Afrikaner .49*** .43*** .27*** .06 .37*** .12 —
8. Anti-African .39*** .49*** .04 .20** .46*** .29*** .50*** —
9. Anti-Indian .33*** .42*** .05 .14* .29*** .21** .27*** .61*** —

Note. RWA ⫽ right-wing authoritarianism; SDO ⫽ social dominance orientation.


* p ⬍ .05. ** p ⬍ .01. *** p ⬍ .001.

was significantly associated with SDO but not RWA. RWA and ted to load only on its manifest indicators. A partial correlation
SDO were both significantly associated with pro-Afrikaner, anti- (correlated error) was modeled between anti-African and anti-
African, and anti-Indian attitudes. Pro-Afrikaner attitudes were Indian attitudes.
significantly correlated with attitudes toward both the out-groups, Figure 3 shows the standardized path coefficients from the
Africans and Indians, which were strongly associated with each analysis. To simplify the diagram, we do not show the manifest
other. variables and paths to them. However, all loadings of latent vari-
The covariance matrix for the SEM analysis was based on a ables on manifest indicators were substantial, with the weakest
sample size of 233, with isolated missing values estimated by standardized loading being .68. The overall fit indices for the
expectation maximization (Schafer, 1997). As in Study 1, we model indicated good fit for a model of this size, with ␹2(302, N ⫽
constructed three manifest item package indicators for each hy- 233) ⫽ 436.1, ␹2/df ratio ⫽ 1.44, RMSEA ⫽ .044 (90% CL ⫽
pothesized latent variable by randomly assigning the items of each .033, .052), SRMR ⫽ .053, population GFI ⫽ .96, CFI ⫽ .95. Fit
scale to three subsets, with, where possible, equal numbers of pro- for this model was clearly better than that for the null model, for
and contrait items in each subset. Each latent variable was permit- which ␹2(351, N ⫽ 233) ⫽ 3,187.5. The model accounted for 61%

Figure 3. Standardized maximum likelihood coefficients from Study 2 (N ⫽ 233) for the structural equation
model with the following latent variables: two personality, two world view, two ideological attitude, pro-in-
group attitudes, and negative attitudes toward two out-groups. To simplify, we do not show manifest variables
or the paths from latent to manifest variables. Dashed lines indicate nonsignificant paths. RWA ⫽ right-wing
authoritarianism; SDO ⫽ social dominance orientation. ⫹ p ⬍ .10. * p ⬍ .05. ** p ⬍ .01.
84 DUCKITT, WAGNER, DU PLESSIS, AND BIRUM

of the variance in RWA, 59% of SDO, 55% of pro-Afrikaner Discussion


attitudes, 31% of anti-Indian attitudes, and 58% of anti-African
attitudes. The findings again clearly support the theoretical model. Overall
All the paths predicted by the model on ideological attitudes fit for the SEM analysis was good, all critical predicted paths were
were significant and closely replicated the findings in Study 1. significant and broadly of the magnitude expected, and the model
Thus, social conformity had a weak to moderate effect on danger- accounted for substantial proportions of the variance in ideological
ous world beliefs, and both social conformity and dangerous world and intergroup attitudes. These findings closely replicate those
beliefs had strong effects on RWA. Tougmindedness had a pow- obtained for the American sample in Study 1 and those obtained
erful effect on competitive-jungle world beliefs, and competitive- for research using two large samples of New Zealand students
jungle world beliefs a powerful effect on SDO. Again, there was (Duckitt, 2001).
no significant direct effect of toughmindedness on SDO, but there One interesting difference from the previous studies, which had
was a moderate to strong indirect effect mediated through com- been tentatively predicted, was for the effects of RWA and SDO
petitive world beliefs. on each other. As predicted for a society in which politics is
The links between the two personality, world view, and ideo- ethnically rather than ideologically organized, the correlation be-
logical attitude systems were also as expected. Social conformity tween these two ideological attitude dimensions was relatively
had a negative effect on toughmindedness, whereas competitive weak and similar in magnitude to that obtained in the United States
world beliefs had a positive effect on dangerous world beliefs. The in Study 1 and in previous research (see Duckitt, 2001). However,
effects of RWA and SDO on each other differed from those unlike the prior American and New Zealand SEM analyses, which
obtained in the American sample in Study 1, but in the direction had suggested reciprocal positive effects between them, these
that we predicted might be the case for this White Afrikaner findings suggest only a unidirectional impact of RWA on SDO.
sample. Thus, these two ideological attitudes did not have recip- This fits the characterization of Afrikaner society as a threat-
control culture in which the ideological belief in inequality and the
rocal positive effects on each other. RWA had a significant posi-
right to dominate other groups is secondary to and stems from
tive effect on SDO (␤ ⫽ .28, z ⫽ 3.42), but SDO did not have a
threat- and insecurity-driven authoritarianism (de Kiewiet, 1957;
positive effect on RWA (␤ ⫽ ⫺.05, z ⫽ ⫺.56).
Duckitt, 1992, 2000; MacCrone, 1937).
The effects on pro-in-group and anti-out-group attitudes were
This interpretation that threat-driven authoritarianism is the
also as expected. Thus, RWA and SDO both had significantly
dominant ideology in Afrikaner culture also suggests that the
positive paths to pro-Afrikaner, anti-African, and anti-Indian atti-
White Afrikaner students should be much higher than the Amer-
tudes. As predicted, dangerous world beliefs and pro-Afrikaner
ican students in dangerous world beliefs and RWA but not neces-
attitudes did not significantly increase prejudice against Indians,
sarily higher in SDO or competitive world beliefs. To check this,
the nonthreatening out-group, but did have significant direct ef-
we used those items that were used in both studies (6 RWA, 10
fects of increasing prejudice against Africans, the more threatening
dangerous world, 14 competitive world, and 10 SDO items) to
out-group. Social conformity had direct negative effects on anti-
compute item means for these scales in the two samples, which
African and anti-Indian attitudes, reducing prejudice in both cases,
were then statistically compared. These means are shown in Ta-
but the effect, although significant for anti-African attitudes, did ble 4. As expected, the Afrikaner sample was markedly higher than
not reach significance for anti-Indian attitudes (␤ ⫽ ⫺.15, z ⫽ the American sample on dangerous world beliefs and RWA but no
⫺1.28, p ⫽ .21). Just as in the previous study, social conformity different in SDO and significantly lower in competitive world
also had significant indirect effects in the opposite direction of its beliefs.
direct effects, which were mediated mainly through RWA, of The SEM findings confirm the prediction that belief in a dan-
increasing both anti-African prejudice (indirect effect ⫽ .36, gerous world and pro-in-group attitudes would increase negative
z ⫽ 3.50, p ⬍ .001) and anti-Indian prejudice (indirect effect ⫽ attitudes to the more threatening and competitive out-group, Af-
.27, z ⫽ 2.60, p ⫽ .009). As a result of the opposing direct and ricans, but not to the apparently less threatening out-group, Indi-
indirect effects for social conformity on prejudice against both ans. This finding clearly supports the hypothesis that these effects
Africans and Indians, social conformity had nonsignificant total are context dependent. Although a moderating role for intergroup
effects on both anti-African (.09, z ⫽ 1.25) and anti-Indian prej- threat was not directly demonstrated by the research because threat
udice (.13, z ⫽ 1.60). itself was not directly measured, the characterization of Africans as
In all other instances, the personality variables had only indirect a more threatening out-group than Indians is supported by the
effects mediated through RWA or SDO of increasing pro-in-group out-group attitude scale means. The anti-African attitude mean
or anti-out-group attitudes. Thus, toughmindedness had no direct (M ⫽ 5.67, SD ⫽ 12.88) was higher than the anti-Indian attitude
effects on pro-in-group and anti-out-group attitudes, but its indirect mean (M ⫽ 1.44, SD ⫽ 9.91), and the difference was highly
effects through SDO produced significant total effects, increasing significant, F(1, 230) ⫽ 37.73, p ⬍ .0001.
pro-Afrikaner attitudes (.18, z ⫽ 3.96), anti-African prejudice (.23, Social conformity was expected to have direct effects of reduc-
z ⫽ 4.77), and anti-Indian prejudice (.18, z ⫽ 4.16). Social con- ing anti-African and anti-Indian prejudice, as all racist attitudes
formity also had significant indirect effects through RWA of should be generally viewed as socially nonnormative in the new
increasing pro-Afrikaner attitudes (.39, z ⫽ 4.86). nonracial and democratic South Africa. Both effects were as pre-
Modification indices (Lagrange multiplier tests) were again dicted, although the effect for Indians was weaker than that for
used to test for additional nonpredicted effects that might be Africans and was not statistically significant. The weaker effect for
significant and improve overall fit. However, no other effects were Indians could be due to norms for nonprejudice to Indians, a
found. relatively peripheral and marginal minority in South Africa, being
IDEOLOGY AND PREJUDICE 85

Table 4
Social World View and Ideological Attitude Means for Afrikaners (n ⫽ 230 –234)
and Americans (n ⫽ 146)

White
Afrikaners Americans

Measures M SD M SD F df Cohen’s d

Ideological attitudes
RWA 1.73 1.23 ⫺0.60 1.31 306.75**** 1,375 1.37
SDO ⫺0.83 1.44 ⫺0.92 1.23 0.35 1,375 0.07
Social world views
Dangerous world 1.69 1.38 0.37 1.20 89.15**** 1,375 0.90
Competitive world ⫺2.01 0.98 ⫺1.47 1.12 23.49**** 1,374 ⫺0.51

Note. The means are item means with a theoretical range from 4 (highest) to ⫺4 (lowest). RWA ⫽ right-wing
authoritarianism; SDO ⫽ social dominance orientation.
**** p ⬍ .0001.

somewhat weaker and less salient than norms of nonprejudice for a linear causal model, with SDO causally affecting competitive-
the politically and numerically dominant Africans. Again, as in the jungle world beliefs, which in turn affect prejudice, in contrast to
previous study, the significant direct effects of social conformity the current model, which expects competitive-jungle world beliefs
reducing prejudice and its indirect effects of increasing prejudice to affect SDO, which then affects prejudice. However, when this
mediated primarily through RWA largely cancelled each other out alternative, linear social dominance model was tested for general-
overall for both anti-African and anti-Indian prejudice. ized out-group prejudice in Study 1 and then separately for anti-
Indian and anti-African prejudice in Study 2, the fit indices tended
General Discussion to be poor and were clearly inferior to fit for the equivalent linear
models proposed by the current model, for which fit was good in
The results of both the SEM studies indicate good fit of the
all three cases (see Appendix C).
empirical data to the theoretical model of personality, world view,
Overall, therefore, the SEM analyses from both the studies
ideological attitudes, and intergroup attitudes. This finding seems
reported here and two prior, large-sample New Zealand studies
encouraging, as the difficulty of obtaining good fit indices for large
(Duckitt, 2001) provide clear support for the theoretical model of
and complex models has frequently been commented on (Potthast,
how two personality and world view dimensions might influence
1993; Yung & Bentler, 1994), and the model tested here is clearly
the dual ideological attitude dimensions of RWA and SDO and,
a large and complex one. In addition, all the basic causal paths
through them, intergroup attitudes. Three findings seem to merit
proposed by the model were significant in both samples, and the
particular comment: first, the effects of the two personality and
critical paths were strong enough to explain substantial variance in
world view dimensions on the ideological attitude dimensions of
ideological and intergroup attitudes. These findings also closely
replicate those obtained in SEM analyses in two large New Zea- RWA and SDO; second, the effects of personality and world view
land samples (Duckitt, 2001), supporting the cross-national gen- on in-group and out-group attitudes; and, third, findings pertaining
erality of the model. to the hypothesized context dependence of certain effects.
Ideally, the model would have been compared with plausible Thus, in the first instance, the SEM analyses indicated strong
alternative models. In these two studies, the alternative model was effects for the two personality and world view dimensions on the
the null or independence model simply because no clearly plausi- two ideological attitude dimensions of RWA and SDO. The find-
ble alternative models seemed apparent. However, reviewers of ings also confirm an interesting asymmetry noted in the two
this article suggested several possible alternatives. One is a full previous New Zealand studies (Duckitt, 2001) for the effects of the
alternative model in which each of the two sets of personality, personality and world view dimensions on the two ideological
world view, and ideological attitude variables might be caused by attitudes. Although social conformity had a relatively strong, direct
a single, unmeasured causal variable. Thus, social conformity, effect on RWA and a weaker, indirect effect mediated through
dangerous world beliefs, and RWA might be caused by Latent dangerous world beliefs, the effect for toughmindedness on SDO
Variable X, and toughmindedness, competitive-jungle world be- was entirely indirect and mediated through competitive-jungle
liefs, and SDO might be caused by Latent Variable Y, with X and world beliefs. These two effects seem intuitively plausible. Being
Y affecting in-group and out-group attitudes. However, when we toughminded—that is, dispositionally hard, ruthless, and unfeel-
tested this full alternative model using the data from Studies 1 ing—seems highly likely to elicit reactions from others that pro-
and 2, the fit indices, which are shown in Appendix C, indicated vide powerful confirmation that the social world really is a com-
poor fit in both cases. petitive jungle, and this should generate a very strong effect.
A second alternative proposal was derived from social domi- Moreover, being toughminded only seems to lead to beliefs in
nance theory and suggests that the competitive-jungle world view inequality and social dominance when this kind of competitive-
can be viewed as a legitimizing myth through which people who jungle view of the world is adopted. On the other hand, although
are high in SDO justify holding prejudiced attitudes. This suggests highly conforming persons tend to identify with conventional
86 DUCKITT, WAGNER, DU PLESSIS, AND BIRUM

society and therefore to be more sensitive to threats to it, it seems It also seems theoretically plausible that out-group threat should
plausible that this effect is no more than weak to moderate in influence the impact of dangerous world beliefs and pro-in-group
magnitude. The relatively strong, direct effects of social confor- attitudes on out-group attitudes. Dangerous world beliefs should
mity of personality on RWA should therefore reflect a strong index individuals’ reactivity to threat and therefore should deter-
tendency for people who are dispositionally inclined to behave in mine attitudes to threatening groups and not to nonthreatening
a socially conforming manner to also adopt conventional, tradi- groups. Similarly, pro-in-group attitudes should generate negative
tional, and proauthority social attitudes and values congruent with attitudes toward out-groups that threaten the in-group but have
their personality. little effect on out-groups that are not threatening to or directly
In the second instance, the SEM analyses confirmed the expec- competitive with the in-group (cf. also Brewer, 1979, 1999).
tation that the effects of personality and world view on pro-in- A cautionary note is in order here, however. Although the
group and anti-out-group attitudes would be largely indirect and results are consistent with a moderating role for out-group threat
mediated through ideological attitudes. Thus, the SEM analyses on the effects of dangerous world beliefs and pro-in-group atti-
indicated that both the two personality and the two world view tudes on out-group attitudes, they do not demonstrate this moder-
dimensions had statistically significant indirect effects of increas- ating effect directly. This would have required us to directly
ing pro-in-group and anti-out-group attitudes in every case, with measure degree of threat from the different out-groups, and we did
the magnitude of these effects varying between a low of .17 for not do this in this research. In similar fashion, direct measures of
toughmindedness on nationalism in Study 1 and a high of .48 for the degree to which particular prejudiced attitudes are nonnorma-
competitive-jungle world beliefs on prejudice in Study 1. In addi- tive in particular social contexts would be necessary for us to
tion to these indirect effects, however, the analyses indicated two demonstrate more definitively that this moderates the direct effects
direct effects of personality (social conformity) or world view of social conformity in reducing negative attitudes toward partic-
(dangerous world beliefs) on anti-out-group attitudes, but both ular out-groups.
seemed to be context or out-group dependent rather than consistent The model had also expected reciprocal but context-dependent
effects, as had been predicted from the model. causal effects between RWA and SDO. The current findings are
Thus, it had been hypothesized that social conformity would also broadly consistent with this. First, the correlation between
reduce prejudices that were nonnormative in the samples stud-
RWA and SDO, though significant, was relatively weak in both
ied—an effect directly opposite to social conformity’s expected
the American and the South African samples. In the latter case, this
general effect of increasing anti-out-group prejudice indirectly by
is to be expected, because politics in South Africa tends to be
raising RWA. The present research clearly demonstrates both these
ethnically rather than ideologically organized. The expected recip-
opposing effects, with significant indirect effects increasing prej-
rocal effects of RWA and SDO in the American sample, though
udice against all three the target groups in the two studies and
both positive, were weak, with the path from SDO to RWA being
direct effects reducing prejudice against all the three target groups,
marginally significant and that from RWA to SDO nonsignificant.
although the effect in one case, for anti-Indian prejudice, was not
The finding is therefore not entirely clear but is not inconsistent
significant. Because these prejudiced attitudes were all nonnorma-
with weak reciprocal effects, which would be expected in a society
tive, these direct effects of social conformity in reducing prejudice
in which politics is not strongly organized ideologically and in
are consistent with expectation, except for the nonsignificance of
the direct effect reducing anti-Indian prejudice. However, even the which neither of the two ideological dimensions seemed particu-
weaker effect here seems reasonably easily explained, because larly dominant. The finding for South Africa is clearer and seems
norms against anti-Indian prejudice in South Africa should be to illustrate the context dependency in the relationships between
much less salient than norms against anti-African prejudice, be- RWA and SDO. Here, the effects were clearly different, with the
cause Indians are a marginal and relatively peripheral minority, effect of RWA on SDO positive and significant and the effect of
whereas Africans are the politically dominant majority. SDO on RWA nonsignificant and close to zero. This is consistent
Context-dependent effects were also clearly supported for pro- with the expectation that in a culture in which authoritarian–
in-group attitudes on anti-out-group attitudes and for dangerous conservative ideological beliefs are dominant and therefore
world beliefs on out-group attitudes. In both cases, these effects primary in socialization, cognitive pressures for ideological con-
varied as hypothesized according to the degree that out-groups sistency should flow from the dominant ideological belief di-
seemed to be in directly threatening and competitive relationships mension to the less dominant one. Thus, for White Afrikaners,
with the in-group. Thus, as predicted, dangerous world beliefs and authoritarian– conservatism ideological beliefs should influence
pro-in-group attitudes significantly increased anti-African preju- the adoption of beliefs in social dominance and group inequality,
dice, as Africans are an out-group that is clearly threatening to and not the reverse. The relative weakness of the effect therefore
White Afrikaners, but did not significantly affect attitudes to the reflects the relatively weak degree of ideological organization of
nonthreatening Indian out-group. Moreover, neither dangerous politics in a society where politics is primarily organized in terms
world beliefs nor pro-in-group attitudes significantly affected prej- of ethnicity.
udice in Study 1, in which the target out-groups would generally In evaluating the findings from these two studies, we find that
not be seen as directly competitive or threatening to Americans at several important methodological issues merit comment. One issue
the time of the study. Several previous findings have also found pertains to the use of self-report psychometric measures of the
that the effect of pro-in-group attitudes on anti-out-group attitudes constructs proposed, specifically to their factorial independence
seems to be context or out-group dependent and moderated by from each other and their construct validity. A risk here is that
perceived threat from the out-group (Brown et al., 1992; Duckitt & content overlap between subsets of items in different scales might
Mphuthing, 1998; Struch & Schwartz, 1989). build in spurious relationships between the scales. However, a
IDEOLOGY AND PREJUDICE 87

number of steps were taken to control for this in the development level that they adopt ideological beliefs in inequality and social
of the scales used here. dominance.
Thus, in pilot testing, items that did not clearly correlate more The construct in the current research that has had the least prior
powerfully with their own scales than with other scales were attention is that of social conformity. The items for this construct
deleted or rewritten. Independent judges carefully checked all the were developed by Saucier (1994), who used factor analysis to
measures used for item-content overlap between scales and dis- derive nonevaluative personality scales using trait adjective de-
carded any dubious items. For example, Saucier (1994) had in- scriptors and who had named this scale Norm Orientation because
cluded items such as “liberal,” “conservative,” “traditional,” “re- its items (the highest loading in Saucier’s research being “rebel-
ligious,” and “nonreligious” in his original Norm Orientation lious” and “conforming”) seemed to express a dispositional ten-
personality scale, from which the social conformity scale was dency to behave in conformity with social and societal norms and
derived. These were excluded from the social conformity scale rules. However, several of Saucier’s original items could pertain to
because they could be used to describe attitudes as well as behav- attitudes and beliefs (e.g., “conservative,” “liberal”) as well as
ior and might have spuriously inflated the relationship between personality, and we therefore excluded them to avoid possible
conformity and RWA. In the development of the scales, we used content overlap with the attitudinal items of the RWA Scale. We
factor analyses to assess scale unidimensionality and the factorial also renamed the scale Social Conformity because this seemed a
independence of the scales. In addition, as Altemeyer (1981) has more appropriate conceptualization of its item content. A reviewer
recommended, we checked the correlations between individual of this article, however, suggested that other items that had re-
items and other scales to confirm that scale intercorrelations de- mained in the scale, such as “unorthodox,” “free-living,” and
rived from all the items of the scales and were not just created by “old-fashioned,” did not seem to be classic personality trait de-
particular subsets of items. scriptors and might also reflect social attitudes. If so, this might
Finally, the good fit obtained in the SEM analyses for the two have built in spurious content overlap between this scale and the
samples reported here also supports the factorial independence of RWA Scale. However, we feel this is not likely for several reasons.
the latent variables. Although item packages rather than items were First, the instructions for the scale explicitly and prominently
used in these analyses, the items of each scale were randomly asked participants “to rate the extent to which you feel each of the
assigned to their packages so that each package would represen- following descriptive adjectives is characteristic or uncharacteris-
tatively sample the items of the scale. Item-level CFAs, however, tic of your PERSONALITY AND BEHAVIOR”. Second, the item-
also provided more rigorous support for the factorial differentia- level CFAs of the social conformity, RWA, and Dangerous World
tion of the two sets of related personality, world view, and ideo- Scales supported the hypothesized three-factor solution and
logical attitude measures. showed it to be significantly superior to a solution in which RWA
An issue closely related to that of the factorial differentiation of and social conformity items loaded together on one factor. Third,
the scales is that of their construct validity, or the degree to which to eliminate any possibility of content overlap, we asked three
the item sets used measured the constructs specified. Considerable independent judges to select only those items of the social con-
evidence has been reported for the construct validity of the RWA, formity scale that unambiguously pertained only to behavioral
SDO, and Belief in a Dangerous World Scales (Altemeyer, 1981, dispositions and personality and not to social attitudes. Five items
1988, 1996; Pratto et al., 1994). The competitive-jungle world were unanimously rated as such: “obedient,” “predictable,” “re-
view items were largely drawn from the older Machiavellianism spectful,” “erratic,” and “unpredictable.” We recomputed both the
Scale, but here the items were content validated to a much correlational and the SEM analyses for both studies using only
more precisely defined and more specific construct: that of a these five items as the social conformity scale (with alphas in the
competitive-jungle world view. two studies of .59 and .51). There were no changes in the signif-
A good deal of prior research had validated Machiavellianism as icance of effects, and the correlations and path coefficients be-
a predictively useful individual-differences construct (cf. Wilson, tween this purged social conformity scale and RWA were only
Near, & Miller, 1996), but the scales to measure Machiavellianism slightly weaker. For example, the correlation between this short-
appeared to be complexly multidimensional (e.g., Allsopp, Ey- ened five-item social conformity scale and RWA, corrected for the
senck, & Eysenck, 1991; Hunter, Gerbing, & Boston, 1982). lower reliability of the shortened social conformity scale, was .38
However, the most comprehensive investigation of these Machia- (as opposed to .43 for the full scale) in Study 1 and .35 (as opposed
vellianism measures concluded that the central core of their items to .44 for the full scale) in Study 2.
and of the construct is a cynical and competitive view of and Fourth, an important finding from these two studies seems to
orientation toward others (Hunter et al., 1982). This conclusion is provide compelling evidence for divergent validity between the
entirely congruent with the current conceptualization of a social conformity and RWA Scales. In both these studies and in the
competitive-jungle world view, which drew on these core items for two prior New Zealand studies, this social conformity scale had
its measure. The highest loading item for this competitive-jungle nonsignificant correlations with all the indices of prejudice used
world view scale expresses this clearly: “It’s a dog-eat-dog world (correlations varied between ⫺.07 and .10, with the averaged
where you have to be ruthless at times.” The SEM analyses in both correlations over five analyses being .05, with n ⫽ 1,169), whereas
the samples and in prior New Zealand studies suggest that this RWA correlated highly significantly with prejudice in each case
construct plays the critical role in mediating the effect of tough- (correlations varied between .31 and .49, with the averaged cor-
mindedness as a personality construct on the ideological attitude relations being .39, with n ⫽ 1,176). These different effects
dimension of SDO, with no direct effects of toughmindedness on therefore clearly support the conceptual distinction between the
SDO. This suggests that it is only when toughminded personalities measures of social conformity and RWA. The significant direct
see the social world as a competitive jungle at an interpersonal effects of social conformity in reducing prejudice that emerged in
88 DUCKITT, WAGNER, DU PLESSIS, AND BIRUM

the SEM analyses were also consistent with its conceptualization and prejudice. Social world views should be influenced not only by
as measuring a personality disposition to conform to social and individuals’ personalities but also by social reality. Consequently,
societal norms and rules, as the prejudiced attitudes measured in social environments that really are dangerous and threatening or
these studies seem to be clearly nonnormative in their societies. competitive jungles should both increase prejudice, the former by
Finally, constructs very similar to social conformity, as concep- generating authoritarian attitudes and the latter through generating
tualized and measured here, have emerged in broader investiga- social dominance beliefs. The theoretical model therefore proposes
tions of personality trait dimensions, usually with somewhat dif- hypotheses about the impacts of both personal influences such as
ferent labels. An example is the Rule-Free Versus Rule Bound personality and social environmental influences such as threat on
scale from Lorr’s (1986) Interpersonal Style Inventory, which is ideological beliefs and prejudiced attitudes.
defined as “To follow one’s own way of doing things and resist A second implication arises from the core proposition of the
conventional rules and norms” versus “To respect and comply with model that two basic motivational goals, threat-driven control and
authorities, to follow conventions, rules, and norms.” In Big Five security motivation and competitively driven dominance or supe-
terms, social conformity seems to subsume three highly specific riority motivation, which are expressed in two different kinds of
(narrow bandwidth) and closely related trait subdimensions within social attitudes or ideological beliefs, underlie prejudice. This
Conscientiousness (dutifulness and orderliness) and Openness suggests that there should be two distinct dimensions or kinds of
(creativity) from Goldberg’s (1999) International Personality Item prejudice. In the case of threat-control-driven prejudice, out-
Pool’s 45 AB5C scales. This would locate social conformity as a groups are disliked and feared because they are seen as threaten-
medium-bandwidth trait construct straddling the boundary of C⫹ ing, dangerous, and disruptive to social or group order and secu-
and O⫺. The toughmindedness versus tendermindedness con- rity. This seems likely to generate a particular kind of prejudiced
struct, as conceptualized and measured in the current research, social categorization, with the social world categorized into
seems to subsume three specific component traits of the Big Five “them,” who are bad, dangerous, immoral, and deviant and who
Agreeableness higher order dimension, such as Goldberg’s (1999) threaten “us,” who are normal, morally good, decent people.
closely related three AB5C subdimensions of empathy, sympathy, In the case of competitive-dominance-driven prejudice, out-
and tenderness, locating it as a medium-bandwidth construct groups are despised and derogated because they are believed to be
within A⫺. inferior, worthless, and inadequate. Here the social world is cate-
A second methodological issue that merits comment is that of gorized into “us,” who are superior, strong, competent, and dom-
causality. SEM can test the fit of hypothesized causal models to inant (or should be) and “them,” who are inferior, incompetent,
empirical data but cannot demonstrate causality as experimental or and worthless. This motivationally based distinction between two
longitudinal research might (Hoyle & Smith, 1994). Thus, the dimensions of prejudice is consistent with prior accounts that have
evidence from the SEM studies reported here cannot validate the differentiated two kinds of racism—that is, aversive versus dom-
causal hypotheses proposed by this model. However, there are inative (Kovel, 1970), hot versus cold (Fiske, 1998), and symbolic
experimental or longitudinal findings that do clearly support sev- versus traditional (Kinder & Sears, 1981; McConahay & Hough,
eral critical causal paths proposed by the model. For example, a 1976). It is also consistent with Fiske, Xu, Cuddy, and Glick’s
study by Katz and Hass (1988) bears on the causal directionality (1999) recent distinction between two intergroup attitude dimen-
between ideological attitudes and out-group attitudes. Their exper- sions of disliking versus disrespecting and with the well-
iment showed that priming two ideological attitude measures established existence of two distinct dimensions of evaluative
essentially similar to RWA (a Protestant ethic beliefs scale, stereotypes—that is, beneficence or morality stereotypes (good vs.
which is an expression of social conservatism) and SDO (a bad) and competence stereotypes (superior or competent vs. infe-
humanitarianism– egalitarianism scale) causally influenced racial rior or incompetent; Fiske et al. 1999; Giles & Ryan, 1982; Poppe
attitudes but priming racial attitudes did not causally influence & Linssen, 1999).
ideological attitudes. Other evidence shows that social environ- A third implication is that these two kinds of motivational goals
mental changes that are directly reflected in social world view are activated by characteristics of intergroup relationships. Thus,
seem to causally impact on ideological attitudes and prejudice. For intergroup relationships characterized by out-group threat to in-
example, Altemeyer (1988) showed that scenarios that depicted group values and security should activate threat-driven social
threatening and dangerous social changes markedly increased control and security motivational goals and authoritarian ideolog-
RWA. A more recent experimental investigation also used future ical attitudes generating negative attitudes toward that particular
scenarios to manipulate social threat and found that this produced out-group. When the intergroup relationship is one of inequality in
significant increases in RWA that were entirely mediated through status and power or competition over relative dominance and
elevated dangerous world beliefs (Duckitt & Fisher, 2001). A power, this activates competitively driven dominance and power as
number of other longitudinal or experimental studies have shown motivational goals to generate prejudice toward the lower status or
that social threat seems to increase authoritarianism and prejudice competing out-group. The model therefore seems to have potential
(Doty et al., 1991; Downing & Monaco, 1986; Meloen, 1983; for integrating individual-level theories of prejudice, such as au-
Sales, 1973; Sales & Friend, 1973). thoritarianism and social dominance, and group-level perspectives
Finally, although the model tested in this research provides a focusing on intergroup threat (Pettigrew, 1998; Stangor & Cran-
theory of the psychological bases of individual differences in dall, 2000; Stephan, Ybarra, Martinez, Schwarzwald, & Tur-kaspa,
ideological attitudes and prejudice, it also has implications beyond 1998), inequality in status and power (Fiske et al., 1999), and
the research reported here that suggest potentially fruitful future social competition over relative status (Tajfel & Turner, 1979).
avenues for investigation. One implication suggested by the model Thus, both intergroup and personal processes may influence indi-
pertains to social environmental influences on ideological beliefs viduals’ prejudiced attitudes through the cognitive activation of
IDEOLOGY AND PREJUDICE 89

two different sets of motivational goals and their expression in the United States, 1978 –1987. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
ideological beliefs. chology, 61, 629 – 640.
In conclusion, therefore, this research has proposed a model to Downing, L., & Monaco, N. (1986). In-group/out-group bias as a function
explain how personality and social world views might influence of differential contact and authoritarian personality. Journal of Social
individuals’ ideological attitudes and prejudiced intergroup atti- Psychology, 126, 445– 452.
tudes. The model’s constructs are clearly defined and differenti- Duckitt, J. (1992). The social psychology of prejudice. New York: Praeger.
Duckitt, J. (1993). Right-wing authoritarianism among white South African
ated and seem to have some heuristic value. Preliminary testing of
students: Its measurement and correlates. Journal of Social Psychology,
the model using SEM analyses across a range of samples from
133, 553–563.
different countries has produced highly consistent findings that Duckitt, J. (2000). Culture, personality, and prejudice. In S. Renshon & J.
seem supportive. However, testing thus far has been limited to Duckitt (Eds.), Political psychology: Cultural and crosscultural foun-
cross-sectional survey research using a particular set of self-report dations (pp. 89 –107). New York: New York University Press.
psychometric instruments, several of which can be viewed as Duckitt, J. (2001). A dual process cognitive-motivational theory of ideol-
preliminary and requiring further construct validation. New re- ogy and prejudice. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental
search should test the model using alternative, conceptually equiv- social psychology (Vol. 33, pp. 41–113). San Diego, CA: Academic
alent measures of its constructs and, where possible, actual behav- Press.
ioral indicators of the constructs rather than psychometric self- Duckitt, J., & Farre, B. (1994). Right wing authoritarianism and political
report scales, which are open to response biases. Longitudinal or intolerance among Whites in the future majority rule South Africa.
experimental research would help to test the actual causalities Journal of Social Psychology, 134, 735–742.
proposed by the model. One such direction that is being currently Duckitt, J., & Fisher, K. (2001). The impact of social threat on worldview
developed with promising initial findings uses scenarios and prim- and ideological attitudes. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Duckitt, J., & Mphuthing, T. (1998). Group identification and intergroup
ing procedures to manipulate experience of the social environment
attitudes: A longitudinal analysis in South Africa. Journal of Personality
to observe effects on social world view, motivational goal salience,
and Social Psychology, 74, 80 – 85.
and ideological beliefs (Duckitt & Fisher, 2001).
Eysenck, H. (1954). The psychology of politics. London: Routledge and
Keagan Paul.
References Feldman, S., & Stenner, K. (1997). Perceived threat and authoritarianism.
Political Psychology, 18, 741–770.
Adorno, T., Frenkel-Brunswick, E., Levinson, D., & Sanford, N. (1950). Fink, H. (1971). Fictitious groups and the generality of prejudice: An
The authoritarian personality. New York: Harper. artifact of scales without neutral categories. Psychological Reports, 29,
Allsopp, J., Eysenck, H., & Eysenck, S. (1991). Machiavellianism as a 359 –365.
component in psychoticism and extraversion. Personality and Individual Fiske, S. (1998). Stereotyping, prejudice, and discrimination. In D. Gilbert,
Differences, 12, 29 – 41. S. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (4th
Altemeyer, B. (1981). Right-wing authoritarianism. Winnipeg, Canada: ed., Vol. 2, pp. 357– 411). New York: McGraw-Hill.
University of Manitoba Press. Fiske, S., Xu, J., Cuddy, A., & Glick, P. (1999). (Dis)respecting versus
Altemeyer, B. (1988). Enemies of freedom: Understanding right-wing (dis)liking: Status and interdependence predict ambivalent stereotypes of
authoritarianism. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. competence and warmth. Journal of Social Issues, 55, 473– 489.
Altemeyer, B. (1996). The authoritarian specter. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Giles, H., & Ryan, E. (1982). Prolegomena for developing a social psy-
University Press. chological theory of language attitudes. In E. Ryan & H. Giles (Eds.),
Altemeyer, B. (1998). The other “authoritarian personality.” In M. P. Attitudes toward language variation (pp. 208 –223). London: Arnold.
Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 30, pp. Goertzel, T. (1987). Authoritarianism of personality and political attitudes.
47–92). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Journal of Social Psychology, 127, 7–18.
Bentler, P., & Chou, C. (1987). Practical issues in structural modeling.
Goldberg, L. (1999). A broad-bandwidth, public-domain, personality in-
Sociological Methods and Research, 16, 78 –117.
ventory measuring the lower-level facets of several five-factor models.
Bierly, M. (1985). Prejudice toward contemporary outgroups as a gener-
In I. Mervielde, I. Deary, F. de Fruyt, & F. Ostendorf (Eds.), Personality
alized attitude. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 15, 189 –199.
psychology in Europe (Vol. 7, pp. 7–29). Tillburg, the Netherlands:
Brewer, M. (1979). In-group bias in the minimal intergroup situation: A
Tillburg University Press.
cognitive–motivational analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 307–324.
Guelke, A. (1996). Dissecting the South African miracle. Nationalism and
Brewer, M. (1999). The psychology of prejudice: Ingroup love or outgroup
Ethnic Politics, 2, 141–154.
hate. Journal of Social Issues, 55, 429 – 444.
Brown, R., Hinkle, S., Ely, P., Fox-Cardamone, L., Maras, P., & Taylor, L. Hartley, E. (1946). Problems in prejudice. New York: King’s Crown Press.
(1992). Recognizing group diversity: Individualist-collectivist and Hoyle, R., & Smith, G. (1994). Formulating clinical research hypotheses as
autonomous-relational social orientations and their implications for in- structural equation models: A conceptual overview. Journal of Consult-
tergroup processes. British Journal of Social Psychology, 31, 327–342. ing and Clinical Psychology, 62, 429 – 440.
Christie, R., & Geis, F. (1970). Studies in Machiavellianism. New York: Hu, L., & Bentler, P. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance
Academic Press. structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struc-
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences tural Equation Modeling, 6, 1–55.
(2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Hunter, J., Gerbing, D., & Boston, F. (1982). Machiavellian beliefs and
D’Andrade, R. (1992). Schemas and motivation. In R. D’Andrade & C. personality: Construct invalidity of the Machiavellianism dimension.
Strauss (Eds.), Human motives and cultural models (pp. 23– 44). Cam- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 1293–1305.
bridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Johnson, R., & Schlemmer, L. (1994). Launching democracy in South
de Kiewiet, C. (1957). A history of South Africa: Social and economic. Africa. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
London: Oxford University Press. Johnson, W. (1994). Dismantling apartheid. Ithaca, NY: Cornell Univer-
Doty, R., Peterson, B., & Winter, D. (1991). Threat and authoritarianism in sity Press.
90 DUCKITT, WAGNER, DU PLESSIS, AND BIRUM

Jöreskog, K., & Sörbom, D. (1996). LISREL 8: User’s reference guide. Sales, S., & Friend, K. (1973). Success and failure as determinants of level
Chicago: Scientific Software International. of authoritarianism. Behavioral Science, 18, 163–172.
Katz, I., & Hass, R. (1988). Racial ambivalence and American value Saucier, G. (1994). Separating description and evaluation in the structure of
conflict: Correlational and priming studies of dual cognitive structures. personality attributes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66,
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 893–905. 141–154.
Kinder, D., & Sears, D. (1981). Prejudice and politics: Symbolic racism Saucier, G. (2000). Isms and the structure of social attitudes. Journal of
versus racial threats to the good life. Journal of Personality and Social Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 366 –385.
Psychology, 40, 414 – 431. Schafer, J. (1997). Analysis of incomplete multivariate data. London:
Kosterman, R., & Feshbach, S. (1989). Toward a measure of patriotic and Chapman & Hall.
nationalistic attitudes. Political Psychology, 10, 257–274. Schwartz, S. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values:
Kovel, J. (1970). White racism: A psychological history. New York: Theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. In M. Zanna
Pantheon.
(Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 25, pp. 1– 65).
Lorr, M. (1986). Manual of the Interpersonal Style Inventory. Los Angeles:
San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Western Psychological Services.
Sherif, M. (1967). Group conflict and cooperation. London: Routledge and
MacCrone, I. (1937). Race attitudes in South Africa. London: Oxford
Keagan Paul.
University Press.
Shumacker, R., & Lomax, R. (1996). A beginner’s guide to structural
Maiti, S., & Mukerjee, B. (1990). A note on the distributional properties of
equation modeling. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
the Joreskög-Sörbom fit indices. Psychometrika, 55, 721–726.
Marsh, H., & Grayson, D. (1995). Latent variable models of multitrait- Sidanius, J., & Pratto, F. (1993). The dynamics of social dominance and the
multimethod data. In R. Hoyle (Ed.), Structural equation modeling: inevitability of oppression. In P. Sniderman & P. Tetlock (Eds), Preju-
Concepts, issues, and applications (pp. 177–198). Thousand Oaks, CA: dice, politics, and race in America today (pp. 173–211). Stanford, CA:
Sage. Stanford University Press.
McConahay, J., & Hough, J. (1976). Symbolic racism. Journal of Social Sidanius, J., & Pratto, F. (1999). Social dominance: An intergroup theory
Issues, 32, 23– 45. of social hierarchy and oppression. Cambridge, England: Cambridge
McFarland, S. (1998, July). Toward a typology of prejudiced persons. University Press.
Paper presented at the annual meeting of the International Society of Stangor, C., & Crandall, C. (2000). Threat and the social construction of
Political Psychology, Montreal, Canada. stigma. In T. Heatherton, R. Kleck, M. Helb, & J. Hull (Eds.), The social
McFarland, S., & Adelson, S. (1996, July). An omnibus study of person- psychology of stigma (pp. 62– 87). New York: Guilford Press.
ality, values, and prejudice. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Steiger, J. (1989). EzPATH: A supplementary module for SYSTAT and
International Society of Political Psychology, Vancouver, Canada. SYGRAPH. Evanston, IL: SYSTAT, Inc.
Meloen, J. (1983). De autoritaire reaktie in tijden van welvaart en krisis Stephan, W., Ybarra, O., Martinez, C., Schwarzwald, J., & Tur-kaspa, M.
[The authoritarian response in times of prosperity and crisis]. Unpub- (1998). Prejudice toward immigrants to Spain and Israel: An integrated
lished doctoral dissertation, University of Amsterdam. threat theory analysis. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 29, 559 –
Peterson, B., Doty, R., & Winter, D. (1993). Authoritarianism and attitudes 576.
toward contemporary social issues. Personality and Social Psychology Stone, W., Lederer, G., & Christie, R. (1993). The status of authoritarian-
Bulletin, 19, 174 –184. ism. In W. Stone, G. Lederer, & R. Christie (Eds.), Strength and
Pettigrew, T. (1958). Personality and socio-cultural factors in intergroup weakness: The authoritarian personality today (pp. 229 –245). New
attitudes: A cross-national comparison. Journal of Conflict Resolu- York: Springer.
tion, 2, 29 – 42. Strauss, C. (1992). Models and motives. In R. D’Andrade & C. Strauss
Pettigrew, T. (1998). Reactions toward the new minorities of western (Eds.), Human motives and cultural models (pp. 1–20). Cambridge,
Europe. Annual Review of Sociology, 24, 77–103.
England: Cambridge University Press.
Poppe, E., & Linssen, H. (1999). Ingroup favouritism and the reflection of
Struch, N., & Schwartz, S. (1989). Intergroup aggression: Its predictors and
realistic dimensions of difference between national states in Central and
distinctness from in-group bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
Eastern European nationality stereotypes. British Journal of Social Psy-
chology, 62, 564 –576.
chology, 38, 85–102.
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict.
Potthast, M. (1993). Confirmatory factor analysis of ordered categorical
In W. Austin & S. Worchel (Eds.), The social psychology of intergroup
variables with large models. British Journal of Mathematical & Statis-
tical Psychology, 46, 273–286. relations (pp. 33– 47). Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole.
Pratto, F. (1999). The puzzle of continuing group inequality: Piecing Thompson, L. (1995). A history of South Africa. New Haven, CT: Yale
together psychological, social, and cultural forces in social dominance University Press.
theory. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychol- Wilson, D., Near, D., & Miller, R. (1996). Machiavellianism: A synthesis
ogy (Vol. 31, pp. 191–263). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. of the evolutionary and psychological literatures. Psychological Bulletin,
Pratto, F., Sidanius, J., Stallworth, L., & Malle, B. (1994). Social domi- 119, 285–299.
nance orientation: A personality variable predicting social and political Verkuyten, M., & Hagendoorn, L. (1998). Prejudice and self-categoriza-
attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 741–763. tion: The variable role of authoritarianism and in-group stereotypes.
Ross, M. (1993). The culture of conflict. New Haven, CT: Yale University Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 99 –110.
Press. Yung, Y., & Bentler, P. (1994). Bootstrap-corrected ADF test statistics in
Sales, S. (1973). Threat as a factor in authoritarianism. Journal of Person- covariance structure analysis. British Journal of Mathematical & Sta-
ality and Social Psychology, 28, 44 –57. tistical Psychology, 47, 63– 84.
IDEOLOGY AND PREJUDICE 91

Appendix A

Items Measuring the Two Personality Dimensions


Item

Social conformity
Rebellious Moralistic
Unorthodox Obedient
Conforming Unconventional
Conventional Unpredictable
Old fashioned Erratic
Free-living Respectful
Non-conforming Predictable

Toughmindedness
Kind Hard-hearted
Compassionate Unfeeling
Ruthless Affectionate
Unsympathetic Brutal
Tough minded Pitiless
Tender minded Sympathetic
Forgiving Uncaring
Hard Harsh
Caring Soft-hearted
Giving Generous
Merciless Helpful
Gentle Unaffectionate

Note. The instruction read, “Rate the extent to which you feel each of the
following descriptive adjectives is characteristic or uncharacteristic of
YOUR PERSONALITY AND BEHAVIOUR.”

(Appendixes continue)
92 DUCKITT, WAGNER, DU PLESSIS, AND BIRUM

Appendix B

The Construct Definitions and Items of the Two World View Dimensions
Dangerous and threatening social world view:
Belief that the social world is a dangerous and threatening place in which good, decent people’s values and way of
life are threatened by bad people versus belief that the social world is a safe, secure and stable place in which almost
all people are fundamentally good
1. Although it may appear that things are constantly getting more dangerous and chaotic, it really isn’t so. Every
era has its problems, and a person’s chances of living a safe, untroubled life are better today than ever before
2. Any day now chaos and anarchy could erupt around us. All the signs are pointing to it
3. There are many dangerous people in our society who will attack someone out of pure meanness, for no reason at
all
4. Despite what one hears about “crime in the street,” there probably isn’t any more now than there ever has been
5. If a person takes a few sensible precautions, nothing bad is likely to happen to him or her; we do not live in a
dangerous world
6. Every day as society become more lawless and bestial, a person’s chances of being robbed, assaulted, and even
murdered go up and up
7. My knowledge and experience tells me that the social world we live in is basically a safe, stable and secure
place in which most people are fundamentally good
8. It seems that every year there are fewer and fewer truly respectable people, and more and more persons with no
morals at all who threaten everyone else
9. The “end” is not near. People who think that earthquakes, wars, and famines mean God might be about to
destroy the world are being foolish
10. My knowledge and experience tells me that the social world we live in is basically a dangerous and
unpredictable place, in which good, decent and moral people’s values and way of life are threatened and
disrupted by bad people
Competitive jungle social world view:
Belief that the social world is a competitive jungle characterized by a ruthless, amoral struggle for resources and
power in which might is right and winning everything versus belief that the social world is a place of cooperative
harmony in which people care for, help, and share with each other
1. Winning is not the first thing; it’s the only thing
2. The best way to lead a group under one’s supervision is to show them kindness, consideration, and treat them as
fellow workers, not as inferiors
3. If one has power in a situation, one should use it however one has to in order to get one’s way
4. If it’s necessary to be cold blooded and vengeful to reach one’s goals, then one should do it
5. Life is not governed by the “survival of the fittest.” We should let compassion and moral laws be our guide
6. Money, wealth and luxury are what really count in life
7. It is better to he loved than to be feared
8. It is much more important in life to have integrity in your dealings with others than to have money and power
9. It’s a dog-eat-dog world where you have to be ruthless at times
10. Charity (i.e., giving somebody something for nothing) is admirable not stupid
11. You know that most people are out to “screw” you, so you have to get them first when you get the chance
12. All in all it is better to be humble and honest than important and dishonest
13. My knowledge and experience tells me that the social world we live in is basically a competitive “jungle” in
which the fittest survive and succeed, in which power, wealth, and winning are everything, and might is right
14. Honesty is the best policy in all cases
15. There is really no such thing as “right” and “wrong.” It all boils down to what you can get away with
16. Do unto to others as you would have them do unto you, and never do anything unfair to someone else
17. One of the most useful skills a person should develop is how to look someone straight in the eye and lie
convincingly
18. Basically people are objects to be quietly and coolly manipulated for one’s own benefit
19. One should give others the benefit of the doubt. Most people are trustworthy if you have faith in them
20. We can make a society based on unselfish cooperation, sharing and people generously helping each other, and
NOT on competition and acquisitiveness

Note. All items in the dangerous world dimension except Items 7 and 10 are from the Dangerous World Scale from
Enemies of Freedom: Understanding Right-Wing Authoritarianism (pp. 74, 78) by B. Altemeyer, 1988, San Francisco:
Jossey-Bass. Copyright 1988 by Jossey-Bass. Reprinted by permission of John Wiley & Sons, Inc. All competitive-jungle
dimension items except Items 13 and 20 were taken or adapted from the Personal Power, Meanness, and Dominance Scale
and Exploitive Manipulative Amoral Dishonesty Scale from “The Other ‘Authoritarian Personality’ ” by B. Altemeyer in
Advances In Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 30, edited by Mark P. Zanna, copyright 1998 by Elsevier Science (USA),
reproduced by the permission of the publisher.
IDEOLOGY AND PREJUDICE 93

Appendix C

Fit Indices for Alternative Models


Study and analysis ␹2 df ␹2/df RMSEA SRMR GFI CFI

Full alternative model


Study 1 1,081.7 246 4.40 .151 .140 .66 .65
Study 2 1,090.3 314 3.47 .100 .110 .80 .73
Social dominance alternative model
(SDO 3 CW 3 prejudice)
Study 1 (generalized prejudice) 47.3 25 1.90 .078 .100 .97 .97
Study 2 (anti-African prejudice) 62.9 25 2.56 .082 .083 .96 .95
Study 3 (anti-Indian prejudice) 63.8 25 2.52 .081 .086 .97 .96
Current model’s equivalent to
social dominance model
(CW 3 SDO 3 prejudice)
Study 1 (generalized prejudice) 20.0 25 0.80 .000 .038 1.00 1.00
Study 2 (anti-African prejudice) 33.3 25 1.33 .038 .039 .99 .99
Study 3 (anti-Indian prejudice) 44.9 25 1.80 .059 .052 .98 .98

Note. Goodness-of-fit index (GFI) is population GFI (Steiger, 1989). Study 1, N ⫽ 146; Study 2, N ⫽ 233. The full
alternative model is a latent variable model with social conformity, dangerous world beliefs, and RWA caused by Latent
Variable X and toughmindedness, competitive world beliefs, and SDO caused by Latent Variable Y. RMSEA ⫽ root-mean-
square error of approximation; SRMR ⫽ standardized root-mean-square residual; CFI ⫽ comparative fit index; SDO ⫽
social dominance orientation; CW ⫽ competitive world; RWA ⫽ right-wing authoritarianism.

Received February 8, 2001


Revision received October 24, 2001
Accepted October 31, 2001 䡲

You might also like