Narcissism

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

THIS ARTICLE HAS BEEN CORRECTED.

SEE LAST PAGE

Journal of Applied Psychology © 2015 American Psychological Association


2015, Vol. 100, No. 4, 1203–1213 0021-9010/15/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0038698

RESEARCH REPORT

Leader Narcissism and Follower Outcomes: The Counterbalancing Effect


of Leader Humility

Bradley P. Owens Angela S. Wallace


Brigham Young University University at Buffalo, State University of New York
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

David A. Waldman
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Arizona State University

In response to recent calls to theorize and examine how multiple leader characteristics may work together in
their effects, the current research examines how leader narcissism and humility interact to predict perceived
leader effectiveness and follower (i.e., direct-report) job engagement and performance. Although an exami-
nation of leaders who are narcissistic yet humble may seem oxymoronic and even paradoxical, researchers
have suggested that seemingly contradictory personal attributes may exist simultaneously and may actually
work together to produce positive outcomes. Results from survey data from followers and leaders working for
a large health insurance organization showed that the interaction of leader narcissism and leader humility is
associated with perceptions of leader effectiveness, follower job engagement, and subjective and objective
follower job performance. Together, these results suggest that narcissistic leaders can have positive effects on
followers when their narcissism is tempered by humility.

Keywords: leader traits, narcissism, humility, job engagement, job performance

Steve Jobs was commonly seen as having a high level of led Apple to be the most valuable company in the world during
narcissism (Isaacson, 2011). However, Jobs’ second try as the his second attempt as company president.
head of Apple was marked with a tempered management style Narcissism is defined as a “complex of personality traits and
(Surowiecki, 2011). For example, he was described as more processes that involve[s] a grandiose yet fragile sense of self as
open to others’ ideas (Beetz, 2005) and more willing to ac- well as a preoccupation with success and demands for admira-
knowledge past mistakes, even referring to his earlier firing tion” (Ames, Rose, & Anderson, 2006, p. 441) and has been
from Apple as “awful tasting medicine . . . [that] the patient described as comprising an excessively self-centered perspec-
needed” (Jobs, 2005). He expressed appreciation for the tal- tive, self-absorption, extreme confidence or superiority, ex-
ented executives around him and was able to retain them ploitiveness/entitlement, and a strong drive to lead (Emmons,
(Isaacson, 2011). Although Jobs was still seen as narcissistic, 1987; Galvin, Waldman, & Balthazard, 2010; Raskin & Hall,
his narcissism appeared to be counterbalanced or tempered with 1979; Rosenthal & Pittinsky, 2006). Despite the fact that nar-
a measure of humility, and it was this tempered narcissist who cissism might seem intuitively anathema to leader success, it
actually has been shown to have mixed effects on outcomes. For
example, Judge, LePine, and Rich (2006) found that narcissism
was positively related to other-report assessments of leadership
This article was published Online First January 26, 2015.
Bradley P. Owens, Romney Institute of Public Management, Marriott effectiveness in one study and negatively related in another
School of Management, Brigham Young University; Angela S. Wallace, study. The findings of Galvin et al. (2010) revealed that on the
Department of Organization and Human Resources, School of Manage- one hand, narcissists are more likely to espouse the type of bold
ment, University at Buffalo, State University of New York; David A. vision that is necessary for organizations to achieve significant
Waldman, Department of Management, W. P. Carey School of Business, change. However, on the other hand, narcissists may be negli-
Arizona State University. gent in pursuing the type of socialized vision (i.e., stakeholder
This article was supported in part by a grant from the John Templeton oriented) that others may find to be inspiring (Brown &
Foundation [Grant 29630] entitled “The Development, Validation, and Treviño, 2006; Sully de Luque, Washburn, Waldman, & House,
Dissemination of Measures of Intellectual Humility and Humility.” The
2008). In short, whether narcissism is good or bad for leader-
authors thank Deidra Schleicher and two anonymous reviewers for their
helpful feedback on this project.
ship effectiveness has been “one of the longest running issues”
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Bradley in the leader narcissism literature (Campbell, Hoffman, Camp-
P. Owens, Department of Romney Institute of Public Management, Mar- bell, & Marchisio, 2011, p. 272).
riott School of Management, Brigham Young University; 760 TNRB Over the past decade, scholars have proposed that narcissism
Provo, UT 84062. E-mail: bpo@byu.edu can be “productive” in some cases because although narcissists
1203
1204 OWENS, WALLACE, AND WALDMAN

are often viewed as arrogant, haughty, and self-centered, they (Maccoby, 2000; Rosenthal, 2006; Vogel, 2006). Although
also tend to be very confident in their abilities, willing to take narcissism has been a strong predictor of leader emergence
bold risks, and persistent in their pursuit of goals despite (Campbell & Campbell, 2009), it has also been an inconsistent
adversity (Maccoby, 2000; Paulhus & Williams, 2002; predictor of leader effectiveness. For example, one recent meta-
Rosenthal & Pittinsky, 2006). In line with recent advances in analysis reported that correlations between leader narcissism
theory on workplace paradoxes (Smith & Lewis, 2011; Zhang, and other-report measures of leader effectiveness ranged from
Waldman, Han, & Li, 2014), we examine how humility inter- r ⫽ ⫺.31 to .48 in studies using the Hogan Development
acts with narcissism to help temper the potential negative Survey-Bold (HDS-Bold) (Hogan & Hogan, 1997) and from
effects of narcissism and magnify the potential positive effects. r ⫽ ⫺.27 to .25 in studies using the Narcissistic Personality
Leader humility is conceptualized as an interpersonal charac- Inventory (NPI) (Grijalva, Harms, Newman, Gaddis, & Fraley,
teristic enacted in a social context (Owens, 2009b; Owens, in press; Raskin & Terry, 1988). In trying to make sense of this
Johnson, & Mitchell, 2013) and is manifested by admitting range of findings and explain how leader narcissism could yield
mistakes and limitations, spotlighting the strengths and contri- positive effects in some instances, leadership scholars have
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

butions of others, and modeling teachability (Owens & Hek- emphasized that since narcissism is a complex of traits and
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

man, 2012).1 Although it may appear paradoxical that a leader processes, some of narcissism’s properties could yield produc-
can be both narcissistic and humble at the same time, possess- tive or positive results in a leadership role, such as confidence,
ing seemingly opposing characteristics is not in conflict with vision boldness, a fierce drive to succeed, and a strong desire to
competing values theory (Cameron & Quinn, 2011), trait affec- lead (Deluga, 1997; Galvin et al., 2010; Khoo & Burch, 2008).
tivity theory (Watson & Clark, 1984), and behavioral motives We theorize that one important factor relevant to the effects of
research (Grant & Mayer, 2009; Konrath, Bushman, & Grove, leader narcissism is humility, which captures specific interper-
2009; Meglino & Korsgaard, 2006). For example, it is possible sonal patterns that are interrelated and could create a foil or
to be both self- and others-focused, as well as agentic and buffer to the more toxic aspects of narcissism.
communal (Konrath, Bushman, & Grove, 2009). In the current
study, we advance theory suggesting that when a narcissistic Leader Humility
leader is also viewed as having humility, it helps to temper the
Since the beginning of the new millennium, scholars have
potential ill effects of narcissism, allowing potentially positive
increasingly argued that leaders will need “more humility and
or “constructive” (Kets de Vries & Miller, 1985) effects to
less hubris” to successfully handle the unique and fast-changing
manifest. Because narcissism and humility both have clear
demands of the 21st century (Weick, 2000, p. 106; see also
relational implications (see Morf & Rhodewalt, 2001; Owens,
Doty & Gerdes, 2000; Hughes, 2010; Ruggero, 2009). Indeed,
Rowatt, & Wilkins, 2011), we focus on the impact of this
recent research has shown humility’s positive effects in orga-
interaction on leader effectiveness from the perspective of
nizational contexts (Ou, Tsui, Kinicki, Waldman, Xiao, &
followers and followers’ responses to the leader in terms of
Song, 2014; Owens & Hekman, 2012; Owens et al., 2013;
follower job engagement and performance.
Owens & Hekman, in press). By defining humility as an inter-
This research makes three contributions to the literature.
personal quality that plays out in a social context (in line with
First, this effort is the first to examine how leader narcissism
Owens et al., 2013), we take a behavioral view of humility as a
and humility jointly influence indicators of leader effectiveness,
personality characteristic, which view suggests that consistent
providing novel insight into the long-standing debate over
“behavioral acts are the building blocks [and core indicators] of
whether and when narcissism is good for effective leadership
traits, and the stronger an individual’s propensity toward a trait,
(Campbell et al., 2011). Second, in response to calls to more
the more frequently and intensely the individual enacts a cor-
fully integrate leader characteristics, this research furthers the-
responding set of behaviors” (Grant, Gino, & Hofmann, 2011,
ory and empirical evidence about how leader characteristics can
p. 530; see also Buss & Craik, 1983; Fleeson, 2001). As
interact to explain the leader-influence process by theorizing
humility has qualitatively been shown to be a malleable attri-
how combinations of characteristics work together to foster
bute that is subject to development or deterioration and that can
leader effectiveness rather than by studying characteristics in
fluctuate according to life experience (Owens, 2009a; Vera &
isolation (for a review, see Zaccaro, 2007; Zaccaro, Kemp, &
Rodriguez-Lopez, 2004), humility is akin to what personality
Bader, 2004). Third, we further theory on leader humility, as the
psychologists call a “modifiable trait” that people can “increase
current study represents the first empirical test of the humility-
dramatically by practic[e]” (Dunning, 1995, p. 1302; see also
temperance hypothesis (Park & Peterson, 2003), and the first
Duval & Silvia, 2002). This is in line with Dweck, Hong, and
attempt, to our knowledge, to empirically examine how humil-
Chiu’s (1993) conception of trait incrementalism, or the belief
ity interacts with other leadership characteristics in the predic-
that personal attributes are malleable. Individuals may proactively
tion of leader effectiveness and follower outcomes.
choose to develop the trait of humility, and this malleable view of

Theoretical Background
1
These behavioral dimensions of humility have been found to co-occur
and foster each other (Owens & Hekman, 2012). This operationalization of
Leader Narcissism humility is empirically distinct from measures of the Big Five, modesty,
narcissism, honesty-humility, self-efficacy, learning goal-orientation, and
Narcissism plays an influential role in organizations (Lasch, core self-evaluation (Owens et al., 2013) and from transformational, au-
1979), as those who seek and ascend to leadership positions are thentic, servant, and charismatic leadership (Owens, Rowatt, & Wilkins,
often found to have a moderate to high degree of narcissism 2011).
LEADER NARCISSISM AND HUMILITY 1205

humility leaves open the possibility that narcissistic individuals may attention to others’ unique expertise so they can be accorded
self-regulate (Baumeister, 2014) their narcissism by practicing authority and influence in those areas of expertise.
humility, eventually inculcating this attribute into their character.2 We propose that the tempering effect of humility on narcissism
Humility has been viewed as a metavirtue that undergirds other described above will allow the leader to integrate a range of
virtues (Grenberg, 2005), in part because it helps to temper other seemingly diverse characteristics and role expectations associated
characteristics from going to extremes (Park & Peterson, 2003). In with implicit theories of leadership. Specifically, implicit leader-
past reviews, scholars have documented that the reasons why ship research (Lord, de Vader, & Alliger, 1986; Offermann, Ken-
leaders fail often entail an untempered or excessive expression of nedy, & Wirtz, 1994) would suggest that prototypical leaders are
otherwise important leadership characteristics (for review see strong, aggressive, confident, determined, directing, competitive,
Burke, 2006). These include leader confidence swelling to arro- and decisive. However, at the same time, they are also open-
gance, leader self-esteem inflating to hubris, leader charismatic or
minded, caring, understanding, compassionate, and cooperative.
innovative behavior ballooning to eccentricity, leader competitive-
Indeed, such an androgynous leader-characteristic balance has
ness leading to unethical behavior, and leader optimism turning
been shown to be associated with leader effectiveness (Cann &
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

into dysfunctional strategic persistence. As power has been shown


Siegfried, 1990; Kark, Waismel-Manor, & Shamir, 2012). Thus,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

to sometimes change individuals in negative ways (see Kipnis,


1972), when leaders are given power, the traits that helped them to we expect the following:
emerge as leaders in the first place may be the same traits that
Hypothesis 1: Narcissistic leaders with high humility will be
cause them to derail if these traits go unchecked.
perceived by followers to be more effective than narcissistic
leaders with low humility.
Leader Narcissism Combined With Humility
In addition to perceived leader effectiveness, we propose the
Paradox theory provides a theoretical lens for understanding combination of leader narcissism and leader humility will also
how narcissism and humility may interact to predict leader effec-
engender actual leader influence in terms of follower attitudinal
tiveness. As defined by Smith and Lewis (2011, p. 386), a paradox
motivation (i.e., job engagement; Schaufeli & Bakker, 2004) and
entails seemingly “contradictory . . . elements that exist simulta-
behavioral motivation (i.e., job performance). Horowitz and Ar-
neously and persist over time.” The management of paradoxes is
thur (1988) theorized that unbridled narcissism would be associ-
being increasingly recognized in theories pertaining to workplace
effectiveness (Lewis, 2000). An important element of the theory ated with followers becoming less motivated and engaged. Fol-
put forth by Smith and Lewis is that although the two poles of a lower motivation is hampered when narcissistic leaders do not
paradox may seem incongruent or even negatively related, the heed follower ideas or share due credit for success. However, we
effective management of the paradox requires their harmonization propose that humility will help to buffer the effects of the most
and integration. Such thinking follows from the yin-yang philos- toxic, demotivating dimensions of narcissism, while allowing the
ophy, which purports that although the seemingly opposite poles of potentially constructive or motivating aspects of narcissism to
a paradox may appear to oppose or even negate each other, they arouse employees, create perceptions of leader effectiveness (as
can actually be interdependent and potentially complementary argued above), and foster a sense of supportiveness, all of which
(Chen, 2002; Chen, Xie, & Chang, 2011; Fang, 2012; Kaiser & have been shown to be important antecedents to followers’ will-
Overfield, 2010; Vroom & Yetton, 1973). We argue that although ingness to invest themselves fully in their work tasks (Dvir, Eden,
the combination of narcissism and humility may seem like an Avolio, & Shamir, 2002; Kahn, 1990). We argue that the com-
irreconcilable paradox, in reality, as shown by Ou et al. (2014), the bined effect of leader narcissism and humility will enhance fol-
traits can occur simultaneously. Thus, we suggest that it is possible lowers’ attitudinal and behavioral motivation to work hard at their
for narcissism and humility to work together harmoniously to jobs. Thus, we expect the following:
predict leader effectiveness (see Kets de Vries & Miller, 1985;
Maccoby, 2000). Hypothesis 2: Narcissistic leaders with high humility will
We propose that humility can enable narcissistic leaders to be have more engaged followers than narcissistic leaders with
effective because the most self-focused, toxic dimensions of nar- low humility.
cissism are counteracted and mollified by the dimensions of hu-
mility. Thus, narcissistic tendencies toward exploiting others, be-
ing self-absorbed, and demanding admiration are offset by overtly 2
According to self-regulation theory, people will manage their behavior
drawing attention to and promoting others’ strengths and contri- through the use of specific self-controlled and integrated mechanisms
butions. A sense of superiority is neutralized by the leader’s (Karoly, 1993) to maintain a positive view among others (a primary goal
admissions of limits and mistakes, and self-centered perspectives of the self). Successful self-regulation requires resisting or overriding
are transcended or expanded by the leader’s being receptive to natural impulses that might detract from achieving goals or lead to a
negative image (Baumeister & Vohs, 2012). Although we propose that this
others’ ideas and feedback. In addition, the effect of potentially is one reason why narcissists may display humility, we are not proposing
constructive aspects of narcissism may be enhanced by humility that all expressions of humility are necessarily instrumental. A narcissist
by, for example, extreme confidence being tempered by the lead- may choose to practice, develop, and in time, truly possess the character-
er’s acknowledgment of his or her faults and limitations, a preoc- istic of humility out of an intrinsic desire for self-change (Dweck, 2001).
For our purposes to understand follower outcomes, the followers’ consen-
cupation with personal success being balanced with recognizing sual perceptions of humility are more important than the degree to which
others for success as well, and a strong desire to lead and have the leader truly possesses the characteristic of humility or the leader’s
authority over others being countered by a willingness to draw reasons for displaying humility.
1206 OWENS, WALLACE, AND WALDMAN

Hypothesis 3: Narcissistic leaders with high humility will feedback (“My leader shows a willingness to learn from others”).
have higher performing followers than narcissistic leaders Two additional humility items were developed and tested, based
with low humility. on qualitative research suggesting that in a leadership context
humble individuals admit their mistakes (“My leader admits it
Method when he or she makes mistakes”) and are aware of both their
strengths and their weaknesses (“My leader shows awareness of
personal strengths and weaknesses”) for a total of 11 humility
Participants
items.3 Items were anchored on a 5-point agreement scale (5 ⫽
Participants consisted of 876 employees (76% female, 57% strongly agree; ␣ ⫽ .98).
White, average age 37, average tenure under current leader was Perceived leader effectiveness. Participants were asked to
21.26 months) rating 138 leaders (61% female, 57% White) work- complete a 4-item leadership effectiveness scale from Colbert,
ing in a Fortune 100 health insurance organization headquartered Kristof-Brown, Bradley, and Barrick (2008), designed to capture a
in North America, with employees in the United States, Puerto broad range of leadership effectiveness behaviors: communicating,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Rico, and Western Europe. Of the leaders, 57% returned surveys setting goals, leading change, and coaching. Sample items include
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

evaluating follower performance; thus, our list-wise sample for our “My leader overcomes natural resistance to organizational change;
leader-rated performance criteria was 230 followers rated by 78 strives to behave in ways that are consistent with the change goals
leaders. In addition, only 116 followers of 16 leaders had objective and corporate strategy” and “My leader influences the perfor-
archival performance data. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) tests mance of others in achieving the goals of the organization; includ-
revealed no significant differences between these three groups on ing communicating goals to others, modeling appropriate behav-
any demographic variable. The data presented in this article were iors, coaching others to help them attain goals, and providing
part of a broader data collection effort. reinforcement upon the attainment of the goal.” Items were an-
chored on a 5-point agreement scale (5 ⫽ strongly agree; ␣ ⫽ .95).
Procedures Follower job engagement. Participants were asked to com-
plete a 9-item job engagement scale (Schaufeli, Bakker, &
Study scales were included as part of a voluntary annual orga- Salanova, 2006). Sample items include “I am immersed in my
nizational assessment. The follower and leader forms of the survey work,” “At my job, I feel strong and vigorous,” and “I am enthu-
were administered online in two parts. Part 1 contained the pre- siastic about my job.” Items were anchored on a 7-point agreement
dictor variables (i.e., follower ratings of leader humility, leader scale (7 ⫽ strongly agree; ␣ ⫽ .93).
self-ratings of narcissism). Approximately 1 month later, Part 2 of Follower subjective performance. Leader evaluation of fol-
the survey was administered with the follower form containing the lower performance was measured using a 4-item individual per-
follower job engagement scale and the leader effectiveness scale formance scale developed by Walumbwa, Avolio, and Zhu (2008).
and with the leader form containing the leader-rated follower job The anchors on this scale range from 1 ⫽ consistently performs
performance scale. The response rates for the follower survey for way below expectations to 5 ⫽ consistently performs way above
Times 1 and 2 were 74% and 65%, respectively. One month after expectations. Sample items include “How would you judge the
the Time 2 survey was administered, we requested and received overall quality of this employee’s work?” and “All in all, how
archival performance data from the organization. Time 1 and Time competently does this employee perform?” (␣ ⫽ .94).
2 surveys were matched by follower email addresses. Leader Follower objective performance. As an additional measure
assessments of performance were matched to followers by super- of follower performance, objective productivity data were re-
visor numbers provided by the organization.
quested and received for the month after the survey administration.
These productivity data reflect how much of the followers’ time
Measures was spent performing the core task of responding to customer-
Leader narcissism. Leader narcissism was measured using service calls, as opposed to taking breaks, chatting with coworkers,
the self-report NPI-16-item scale (Ames, Rose, & Anderson, and using the Internet for nonwork-related purposes during work
2006). This forced-choice scale captures subclinical or social nar- hours. The company’s phone bank system produces a monthly
cissism and has respondents choose which of two statements best rating between 0 and 1000, with the higher number representing
describes themselves. Sample items from this scale include the more productive use of company time. According to the organi-
following (the first item in each pairing reflects narcissism): “I am zation’s performance management team, these metrics signifi-
an extraordinary person” or “I am much like everybody else” and cantly influence decisions about follower merit pay increases,
“People always seem to recognize my authority” or “Being an bonuses, and promotions, and it is a core performance measure
authority doesn’t mean that much to me” (␣ ⫽ .71). within this context.
Leader humility. Leader humility was measured with a con- Controls. As we argue above that narcissism and humility
sensus other-report scale that was developed and validated by may capture a form of leader androgyny, we controlled for leader
Owens et al. (2013), and adapted to assess a leader. This scale and follower gender, because gender role stereotypes influence
captures the dimensions of willingness to view oneself accurately expectations about leader behavior and styles (e.g., female leaders
(“My leader admits it when he or she doesn’t know how to do
something”), appreciation of others strengths and contributions 3
Though we report results using the 11 item scale, the correlation
(“My leader acknowledges when others have more knowledge and between the original 9 item and 11 item scale was r ⫽ .997; thus, results
skills than him- or herself”), and openness to others ideas and were virtually identical when using either scale.
LEADER NARCISSISM AND HUMILITY 1207

are expected to be more communal and male leaders more agentic) Table 2
and followers’ own gender may influence their receptiveness to Regression Analyses Testing the Interaction of Leader
those different styles (Eagly & Karau, 2002). In addition, we Narcissism and Leader Humility on Perceived
controlled for team size because leaders tend to behave differently Leader Effectiveness
toward large versus small teams (Ancona & Caldwell, 1992; Bass,
1990). Perceived leader effectiveness
Dependent variable Model 1 Model 2
Analysis Predictors
Leader narcissism ⫺.00 (.29) ⫺.05 (.28)
Other-report ratings of leader humility and leader effectiveness
Leader humility .59 (.08)ⴱⴱⴱ .56 (.08)ⴱⴱⴱ
were aggregated to the team level after computing within-group Interaction
interrater agreement (rwg; James, Demaree, & Wolf, 1993) and Leader narcissism ⫻ Leader humility 1.21 (.47)ⴱⴱ
ICC values to justify this aggregation. Leader humility had an F 26.00ⴱⴱⴱ 20.18
R2 .26 .29
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

average rwg value of .78 with ICC [1, 2] values of .13 and .49,
⌬R2 .03ⴱⴱ
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

respectively. Leader effectiveness had an average rwg value of .84


with ICC [1, 2] values of .15 and .68, respectively. To ensure Note. N ⫽ 138 teams. All regression values reflect unstandardized ␤
construct differentiation between follower ratings of leader humil- coefficients with SEs given in parentheses.

p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⱕ .01. ⴱⴱⴱ p ⬍ .001.
ity and leader effectiveness, we conducted a confirmatory factor
analysis using structural equation modeling (AMOS 21; Arbuckle,
2012). The two-factor solution (␹2[79] ⫽ 358.22; RMSEA ⫽ .06;
CFI ⫽ .99; TLI ⫽ .98) fit the data much better than the one-factor narcissism and leader humility predicted perceived leader effec-
solution (␹2[80] ⫽ 1293.34; RMSEA ⫽ .12; CFI ⫽ .94; TLI ⫽ tiveness beyond the main effects and study controls (b ⫽ 1.21,
.92), suggesting these leadership constructs are distinct. SE ⫽ .47, p ⬍ .01; ⌬R2 ⫽ .03), providing support for Hypothesis
As all variables in Hypothesis 1 were on Level 2, we tested 1. As displayed in Figure 1, leader narcissism positively predicted
Hypothesis 1 using regression (SPSS, 2012). Because Hypotheses perceived leader effectiveness, but only when the leader also had
2 and 3 entail Level 2 predictors with a Level 1 outcome, we tested higher humility (simple slope t ⫽ 3.20, p ⬍ .001). When the leader
these hypotheses using hierarchical linear modeling with robust was low in humility, leader narcissism did not significantly predict
SEs (HLM 7; Raudenbush, Bryk, Cheong, Congdon, & du Toit, leader effectiveness (simple slope: t ⫽ ⫺1.17, ns).
2011). For all outcomes, we conducted line-slope analyses for For Hypotheses 2 and 3, we proposed that higher leader narcis-
interaction effects (Aiken & West, 1991). As results were signif- sism and humility would be positively associated with follower job
icant whether or not we included controls, we report the models engagement and job performance. As shown in Models 2, 4, and 6
without controls. For our HLM analyses, a test of variance ex- in Table 3, the interaction term was positively associated with
plained was calculated based on the following formula from Singer follower job engagement (b ⫽ .98, SE ⫽ .43, p ⬍ .05; pseudo
(1998): pseudo R2 ⫽ (Level-2 unrestricted error—Level-2 re- ⌬R2 ⫽ .09), follower subjective (leader-rated) job performance
stricted error)/Level-2 unrestricted error. (b ⫽ 1.38, SE ⫽ .52, p ⬍ .01; pseudo ⌬R2 ⫽ .17), and follower
objective (archival) job performance (b ⫽ 3.82, SE ⫽ .98, p ⬍ .01;
pseudo ⌬R2 ⫽ .27). In Figure 2, high leader narcissism was
Results
positively associated with higher follower job engagement when
Table 1 displays descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations leader humility was also high (simple slope t ⫽ 2.00, p ⬍ .05),
for all study variables. The results of the regression analysis are lending support for Hypothesis 2. In Figures 3 and 4, high leader
summarized in Table 2. In Model 2, the interaction of leader narcissism was positively associated with both higher follower

Table 1
Descriptive Statistics and Correlations

M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
ⴱⴱ ⴱⴱ ⴱⴱ
1. Leader gender .39 .49 .29 .04 .20 .12 .03 .04 .05 .29ⴱⴱ
2. Follower gender 1.24 .19 .16 ⫺.05 ⫺.03 .04 .02 .03 ⫺.06 ⫺.07
3. Team size 2.99 1.05 ⫺.01 ⫺.12 ⫺.23ⴱⴱ ⫺.05 ⫺.10 ⫺.10 .01 .08
4. Leader narcissism .39 .17 .25ⴱ ⫺.14 ⫺.20 .00 .03 .04 .05 .41ⴱⴱ
5. Leader humility 3.95 .60 .27ⴱⴱ .09 .03 ⫺.06 .66ⴱⴱ .29ⴱⴱ .37ⴱⴱ .12
6. Perceived leader effectiveness 4.10 .71 .07 ⫺.07 .04 ⫺.09 .52ⴱⴱ .40ⴱⴱ .26ⴱⴱ .14
7. Follower job engagement 5.02 .95 .03 .01 ⫺.31 .02 .33ⴱⴱ .36ⴱⴱ .21ⴱⴱ .40ⴱⴱ
8. Follower subjective performancea 3.71 .82 .04 ⫺.06 .01 .03 .37ⴱⴱ .25ⴱ .20ⴱ .31
9. Follower objective performanceb 137.66 129.46 .32 ⫺.24 .33 .36 .15 .02 .43 .33
Note. Values above the diagonal represent individual-level correlations and values below the diagonal, team-level correlations. N ⫽ 876 followers from
k ⫽ 138 teams. Gender coded as 1 ⫽ male, 0 ⫽ female. Race coded as 1 ⫽ White, 0 ⫽ non-White.
a
Denotes leader-rated follower job performance (n ⫽ 230 from k ⫽ 78 teams). b Denotes an archival performance measure of follower productivity (n ⫽
116 from k ⫽ 16 teams).

p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01.
1208 OWENS, WALLACE, AND WALDMAN
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Figure 2. The interactive effect of leader narcissism and leader humility


Figure 1. The interactive effect of leader narcissism and leader humility
on follower job engagement.
on perceived leader effectiveness.

tially harmful effects of narcissism. We found that when leaders


subjective (simple slope t ⫽ 2.90, p ⬍ .01) and higher objective show humility, narcissism is associated with positive effects in
(simple slope t ⫽ 4.29, p ⬍ .001) job performance when leader terms of leader effectiveness and follower outcomes. The impli-
humility was also high, lending support for Hypothesis 3. In each cation of this study is that it is important to remember that
case, the slopes for low humility were all nonsignificant (see narcissistic leaders are not one-dimensional individuals; rather,
Figures 2, 3, and 4). other characteristics may interact with the leaders’ narcissism to
determine its effects on followers. The results of this study suggest
Discussion that narcissists who practice humility may avoid derailment and be
effective as leaders because expressions of humility may mollify
In their review of narcissistic leader research, Rosenthal and the effects of the most toxic aspects of narcissism and temper the
Pittinsky (2006, p. 618) suggested that counterproductive, harmful potentially constructive aspects so their benefits can be realized.
effects of narcissism occur “when one is unable to integrate the
idealized beliefs one has about oneself with the realities of one’s
Leader Trait Theory
inadequacies.” As humility entails acknowledging inadequacies
and limits, the current study is the first of which we are aware to The current research also contributes to evolving debates about
empirically test the idea that humility can counteract the poten- leader trait research. Since the resurgence of this theory, scholars

Table 3
HLM Analyses Testing the Interaction of Leader Narcissism and Leader Humility on Follower Job Engagement, Subjective
Performance, and Objective Performance

Follower subjective Follower objective


Follower job engagementa performanceb performancec
Dependent variable Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Model 5 Model 6
ⴱⴱⴱ ⴱⴱⴱ ⴱⴱⴱ ⴱⴱⴱ
Intercept, ␥00 4.94 (.04) 4.94 (.04) 3.65 (.04) 3.65 (.05) .04 (.21) .00 (.15)
Predictors
Leader narcissism, ␥a0 .11 (.27) .05 (.27) .52 (.29)† .50 (.27)† 2.76 (1.10)ⴱ 3.65 (.88)ⴱⴱ
Leader humility, ␥b0 .05 (.08) .05 (.27) .36 (.07)ⴱⴱⴱ .29 (.08)ⴱⴱⴱ .28 (.38) .13 (.23)
Interaction
Leader narcissism ⫻ Leader humility, ␥c0 .98 (.43)ⴱ 1.38 (.52)ⴱⴱ 3.84 (1.16)ⴱⴱ
Log likelihood 2624.36 2617.96 561.33 551.93 298.99 294.09
⫺2 [L␤reduced – ␤full)] ⫺12.80 ⫺18.80 ⫺9.08
⌬ Pseudo R2 .09 .17 .27
Note. Predictor and interaction variables were modeled at Level 2. All regression values reflect unstandardized ␤ coefficients with SEs given in
parentheses.
a
N ⫽ 876 followers from k ⫽ 138 teams. b N ⫽ 230 followers from k ⫽ 78 teams. c N ⫽ 116 followers from k ⫽ 16 teams.

p ⬍ .10. ⴱ p ⱕ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01. ⴱⴱⴱ p ⬍ .001.
LEADER NARCISSISM AND HUMILITY 1209

measure that can circumvent issues related to social desirability


(e.g., disguised-intent or forced-choice format) to examine whether
the level of agreement in self- and other-report humility assess-
ment is predictive of positive leadership outcomes.
Paradoxes in organizations. Our study could provide new
directions in the study of paradoxes facing leaders and organiza-
tions. Since the 1980s, paradoxes have been largely considered in
terms of macrolevel challenges that organizations often face. For
example, Smith and Lewis (2011) considered the organizational
learning paradox inherent in exploitation versus exploration, and
the performing paradox that can occur from the competing de-
mands of stakeholder groups. While Smith and Lewis considered
leader qualities (e.g., behavioral complexity) to best deal with the
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

demands of organizational paradoxes, they did not consider how


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

such qualities themselves might be paradoxical in nature. Accord-


ingly, our contribution is to demonstrate that while the combina-
tion of leader narcissism and humility may seem like an irrecon-
cilable paradox, in reality, these traits can coexist in such a way as
to produce favorable outcomes. We see our research as advancing
thinking beyond a consideration of the paradoxes faced by orga-
Figure 3. The interactive effect of leader narcissism and leader humility nizations by revealing how leaders may need to harmonize para-
on follower subjective performance. doxes within themselves, such as integrating narcissism and hu-
mility.
Our study complements emerging research (e.g., Zhang et al.,
have suggested that there has been too much focus on examining 2014) that shows that paradoxes are also relevant to leader traits
a limited set of traits operating in isolation and too little focus on and behaviors per se as well as resulting follower outcomes.
incorporating patterns of traits into models of leader effectiveness, However, our research extends beyond work of Zhang et al. (2014)
as reflected by Zaccaro (2007, p. 12): “although many recent in three ways. First, their work was directed toward five dimen-
studies have taken a multivariate approach to maximize explained sions of what they referred to as paradoxical leadership in people
variance in leadership, few studies have taken an integrated ap- management, only one of which is along the lines of the current
proach to describe how multiple traits are combined in optimal research. Specifically, they conceived of self-centeredness com-
ways to jointly influence leadership.” As early as 1980, leadership bined with other-centeredness, which is narrower than our broader
scholars have proposed that leadership emergence would be best treatment of narcissism combined with humility. Second, Zhang et
captured by examining patterns of multiple leader traits (Holmes, al. relied on what they termed double-barreled items from a single
Sholley, & Walker, 1980). This idea was confirmed in research source (i.e., ratings by followers). The current study used measures
showing how individuals who are categorized in groups based on
patterns of traits are more likely to emerge as leaders (Smith &
Foti, 1998). However, the prediction of leader effectiveness, rather
than emergence, is a more controversial issue in leader trait re-
search (Zacarro, 2007). The current study provides evidence that
theory-based couplings of leader traits may enhance the prediction
of perceptions of leader effectiveness and actual indicators of
effectiveness—follower job engagement and performance.
Leader humility. Our research also contributes to leader hu-
mility theory as the first empirical test of the temperance-virtue
idea. As a virtue that is proposed to guard against excess (Park &
Peterson, 2003) and keep other characteristics from going to ex-
tremes (Owens & Hekman, 2012), humility has been argued to be
a complementary construct that facilitates greater effectiveness for
other traditional leadership approaches (such as transformational
leadership, Morris, Brotheridge, & Urbanski, 2005; and charis-
matic leadership, Nielsen, Marrone, & Slay, 2010). However, we
are aware of no prior research that has empirically examined how
humility interacts with other characteristics associated with lead-
ership. Thus, this study makes a theoretical contribution by testing
the temperance-virtue concept (Colquitt & Zapata-Phelan, 2007).
As we relied on a consensus other-report measure to capture our
behavioral consistency view of humility as a trait, we recommend Figure 4. The interactive effect of leader narcissism and leader humility
that future research develop and use a self-reported, humility on follower objective performance.
1210 OWENS, WALLACE, AND WALDMAN

from two sources (i.e., self-ratings and follower ratings) to tap explore whether and when trait homogeneity between leaders and
narcissism and humility, respectively. Third, Zhang et al. focused followers is more predictive of leader effectiveness versus when
more narrowly on performance ratings by supervisors for the trait heterogeneity is more predictive of leader success. Lastly, we
dependent variable. Outcome assessment in the current study came suggested above that one reason why narcissists show humility is
from an additional source (i.e., followers) and involved an objec- out of a self-regulation motive. The self-regulation literature sug-
tive indicator of follower performance. In total, our study com- gests that depletion, stress, or fatigue may predict lapses into
bined with the work of Zhang et al. suggests that more research is unbridled narcissistic behavior for leaders who are trying to temper
warranted to investigate the effects of paradoxical combinations of these inclinations (Baumeister, 2002; Baumeister, Bratslavsky,
leader characteristics. Muraven, & Tice, 1998). We recommend future research to ex-
amine this potential self-regulation mechanism by exploring
whether workplace stress, fatigue, and other forms of ego deple-
Strengths, Limitations, and Future Research
tion impact the degree to which narcissistic leaders display humil-
Our study has several methodological strengths that enhance ity, which may ultimately impede the effectiveness of a narcissistic
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

confidence in our findings. First, compared with a cross- leader.


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

organization study, our cross-unit study within the same organi-


zation better addresses causality issues by keeping particular Practical Implications and Conclusion
threats to internal validity constant (Shadish, Cook, & Campbell,
A key practical implication of this study is that narcissistic
2001) because industry, organizational culture, and reward struc-
leaders who behave humbly may help to prevent their narcissism
tures were consistent across units. Second, our study involved both
from damaging their leadership effectiveness. Narcissistic leaders
multisource measurement with interaction terms, which reduces
may not be “ultimately doomed to fail” (Rosenthal & Pittinsky,
concerns for common method variance (Siemsen, Roth, & Ol-
2006, p. 618) and may not necessarily need to abandon their strong
iveira, 2010), and multistage measurement, which enhances the
or agentic tendencies if they can instead allow these tendencies to
proposed causal order of our theory.
be tempered by counterbalancing characteristics. As new leaders
Our study also has limitations. First, although our cross-unit
engage in their in-role identity construction, they may hear advice
design has some strengths, it is limited in that the study sample
that reflects opposite approaches to leadership: “show no fear,”
came from the same industry (i.e., health services); thus, bringing
“assert yourself,” and “keep a strong front”; or conversely, “be
into question the study’s generalizability to other industry sectors.
humble,” “learn from your people,” and “give due credit to oth-
Industries as well as cultures have been shown to vary in norms or
ers.” Such advice might be confusing to leaders who view these
values that could potentially influence followers’ receptiveness to
approaches as incongruous. However, our study suggests that
narcissism and humility—values such as masculinity or feminin-
effective leaders are more likely to be those individuals who learn
ity, power centralization, collectivism, and power distance (Burns
to harmonize what may initially seem to be disparate leadership
& Stalker, 1961; Cameron & Quinn, 2011; Hofstede, 1984). We
qualities.
recommend that future research be done to explore whether these
In conclusion, scholars have generally viewed narcissism as a
industrial and national cultural differences influence our proposed
volatile characteristic that is likely to be detrimental to leader
interaction effects.
effectiveness. However, more-recent work also suggests the pos-
In addition, our study was limited in that it focused on whether
sibility of narcissism being constructive or productive (Galvin et
a leader displays humility but not why (or the underlying motive
al., 2010; Maccoby, 2000; Paulhus & Williams, 2002). We theo-
for displaying humility). It is conceivable that leaders have varying
rized and tested the idea that leader narcissism can have a positive
motives for displaying humility, upon which followers may dis-
effect on follower perception and motivation when it is tempered
cern. Although our results show clear support for the positive
by humility. As suggested by Zaccaro et al. (2004, p. 123),
benefits of leaders displaying humility even without accounting for
“Understanding leader traits and attributes will require a deeper
potential motive variability, exploring the motive question directly
conceptualization of how such traits . . . operate jointly to influence
in future research could yield additional theoretical insight about
different leadership outcomes.” Our examination of the interaction
these relationships. As assessing leader humility and perceived
of leader narcissism and humility furthers theory on paradoxes in
leader effectiveness from the same source is a limitation of our
organizations and on the role of humility in leadership. We hope
study, future research should also seek to replicate these findings
that this research spurs future work regarding novel examinations
using other indicators of leader effectiveness, such as ratings from
of how leader characteristics interact to predict leader success.
the leader’s boss or department performance metrics. Relatedly,
our Level 2 sample size for our objective job performance metric References
was relatively low. Although we sought to triangulate this effect on
performance with our leader-rated follower job performance out- Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and
come, future research should replicate the effect of our interaction interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
on objective follower performance. Ames, D. R., Rose, P., & Anderson, C. P. (2006). The NPI-16 as a short
measure of narcissism. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 440 –
Future research should also explore ways to help leaders en-
450. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2005.03.002
hance self-awareness of core values, personal characteristics, and Ancona, D. G., & Caldwell, D. F. (1992). Bridging the boundary: External
assumptions that foster counterproductive behaviors and to help activity and performance in organizational teams. Administrative Sci-
narcissistic leaders identify practical methods for minimizing ence Quarterly, 37, 634 – 665. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2393475
harmful behaviors commonly associated with this trait. It may also Arbuckle, J. L. (2012). IBM® SPSS® AmosTM 21 user’s guide. Chicago, IL:
be meaningful to include follower traits in future research and SPSS.
LEADER NARCISSISM AND HUMILITY 1211

Bass, B. M. (1990). From transactional to transformational leadership: Social Psychology, 21, 1297–1306. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
Learning to share the vision. Organizational Dynamics, 18, 19 –31. 01461672952112007
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0090-2616(90)90061-S Duval, T. S., & Silvia, P. J. (2002). Self-awareness, probability of im-
Baumeister, R. F. (2002). Ego depletion and self-control failure: An energy provement, and the self-serving bias. Journal of Personality and Social
model of the self’s executive function. Self and Identity, 1, 129 –136. Psychology, 82, 49 – 61. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.82.1.49
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/152988602317319302 Dvir, T., Eden, D., Avolio, B. J., & Shamir, B. (2002). Impact of trans-
Baumeister, R. F. (2014). Self-regulation, ego depletion, and inhibition. formational leadership on follower development and performance: A
Neuropsychologia, 65, 313–319. field experiment. Academy of Management Journal, 45, 735–744. http://
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). dx.doi.org/10.2307/3069307
Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Person- Dweck, C. S. (2001). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality,
ality and Social Psychology, 74, 1252–1265. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ and development (1st ed.). Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press.
0022-3514.74.5.1252 Dweck, C. S., Hong, Y. Y., & Chiu, C. Y. (1993). Implicit theories:
Baumeister, R. F. & Vohs, K. D. (2012). Self-Regulation and the executive Individual differences in the likelihood and meaning of dispositional
function of the self. In M. R. Leary & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), Handbook inference. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 644 – 656.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

of self and identity (2nd ed., pp. 180 –197). New York: The Guilford http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167293195015
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Press. Eagly, A. H., & Karau, S. J. (2002). Role congruity theory of prejudice
Beetz, K. (2005). Steven Paul Jobs. In International dictionary of business toward female leaders. Psychological Review, 109, 573–598. http://dx
biographies. Retrieved from http://www.encyclopedia.com/topic/ .doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.109.3.573
Steven_Paul_Jobs.aspx#1 Emmons, R. (1987). Narcissism: Theory and measurement. Journal of
Brown, M. E., & Treviño, L. K. (2006). Ethical leadership: A review and Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 11–17.
future directions. The Leadership Quarterly, 17, 595– 616. http://dx.doi Fang, T. (2012). Yin Yang: A new perspective on culture. Management
.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2006.10.004 and Organization Review, 8, 25–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1740-
Burke, R. J. (2006). Why leaders fail: Exploring the dark side. Interna- 8784.2011.00221.x
tional Journal of Manpower, 271, 91–100. Fleeson, W. (2001). Toward a structure- and process-integrated view of
Burns, T., & Stalker, G. M. (1961). The management of innovation. personality: Traits as density distribution of states. Journal of Person-
London, United Kingdom: Tavistock. ality and Social Psychology, 80, 1011–1027. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
Buss, D. M., & Craik, K. H. (1983). The act frequency approach to 0022-3514.80.6.1011
personality. Psychological Review, 90, 105–126. http://dx.doi.org/ Galvin, B. M., Waldman, D. A., & Balthazard, P. (2010). Visionary
10.1037/0033-295X.90.2.105 communication qualities as mediators of the relationship between nar-
Cameron, K. S., & Quinn, R. E. (2011). Diagnosing and changing orga- cissism and attributions of leader charisma. Personnel Psychology, 63,
nizational culture: Based on the competing values framework. San 509 –537. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1744-6570.2010.01179.x
Francisco, CA: Wiley. Grant, A. M., Gino, F., & Hofmann, D. A. (2011). Reversing the extra-
Campbell, W. K., & Campbell, S. M. (2009). On the self-regulatory verted leadership advantage: The role of employee proactivity. Academy
dynamics created by the peculiar benefits and costs of narcissism: A of Management Journal, 54, 528 –550. http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/AMJ
contextual reinforcement model and examination of leadership. Self and .2011.61968043
Identity, 8, 214 –232. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15298860802505129 Grant, A. M., & Mayer, D. M. (2009). Good soldiers and good actors:
Campbell, W. K., Hoffman, B. J., Campbell, S. M., & Marchisio, G. Prosocial and impression management motives as interactive predictors
(2011). The role of personality in human resource management. Human of affiliative citizenship behaviors. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94,
Resource Management Review, 21, 268 –284. 900 –912. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0013770
Cann, A., & Siegfried, W. D. (1990). Gender stereotypes and dimensions Grenberg, J. M. (2005). Kant and the ethics of humility: A story of
of effective leader behavior. Sex Roles, 23, 413– 419. http://dx.doi.org/ dependence, corruption and virtue. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge Uni-
10.1007/BF00289229 versity Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511627859
Chen, M. J. (2002). Transcending paradox: The Chinese middle way Grijalva, E., Harms, P. D., Newman, D. A., Gaddis, B. H., & Fraley, R. C.
perspective. Asia Pacific Journal of Management, 19, 179 –199. http:// (in press). Narcissism and leadership: A Meta-analytic review of linear
dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1016235517735 and nonlinear relationships. Personnel Psychology.
Chen, X. P., Xie, X. F., & Chang, S. Q. (2011). Cooperative and compet- Hofstede, G. (1984). Culture’s consequences: International differences in
itive orientation among Chinese people: Scale development and valida- work-related values (Vol. 5). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
tion. Management and Organization Review, 7, 353–379. http://dx.doi Hogan, R., & Hogan, J. (1997). Hogan development survey manual. Tulsa,
.org/10.1111/j.1740-8784.2011.00215.x OK: Hogan Assessment Systems.
Colbert, A. E., Kristof-Brown, A. L., Bradley, B. H., & Barrick, M. R. Holmes, C. M., Sholley, B. K., & Walker, W. E. (1980). Leader, follower,
(2008). CEO transformational leadership: The role of goal importance and isolate personality patterns in black and white emergent leadership
congruence in top management teams. Academy of Management Jour- groups. The Journal of Psychology, 105, 41– 46. http://dx.doi.org/
nal, 51, 81–96. http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/AMJ.2008.30717744 10.1080/00223980.1980.9915130
Colquitt, J. A., & Zapata-Phelan, C. P. (2007). Trends in theory building Horowitz, M. J., & Arthur, R. J. (1988). Narcissistic rage in leaders: The
and theory testing: A five-decade study of the Academy of Management intersection of individual dynamics and group process. International
Journal. Academy of Management Journal, 50, 1281–1303. http://dx.doi Journal of Social Psychiatry, 34, 135–141. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
.org/10.5465/AMJ.2007.28165855 002076408803400208
Deluga, R. J. (1997). Relationship among American presidential charis- Hughes, L. W. (2010). Leadership under pressure: Tactics from the front
matic leadership, narcissism, and rated performance. The Leadership line. Leadership and Organization Development Journal, 31, 187–188.
Quarterly, 8, 49 – 65. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1048-9843(97)90030-8 http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/01437731011024439
Doty, J., & Gerdes, D. (2000). Humility as a leadership attribute. Military Isaacson, W. (2011). Steve Jobs. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster.
Review, 80, 89 –90. James, L. R., Demaree, R. G., & Wolf, G. (1993). rwg: An assessment of
Dunning, D. (1995). Trait importance and modifiability as factors influ- within-group interrater agreement. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78,
encing self-assessment and self-enhancement motives. Personality and 306 –309. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.78.2.306
1212 OWENS, WALLACE, AND WALDMAN

Jobs, S. (2005, June 14). You’ve got to find what you love. Stanford Ou, A. Y., Tsui, A. S., Kinicki, A. J., Waldman, D. A., Xiao, Z., & Song,
Report. Retrieved from http://news.stanford.edu/news/2005/june15/ L. J. (2014). Humble chief executive officers’ connections to top man-
jobs-061505.html agement team integration and middle managers’ responses. Administra-
Judge, T. A., LePine, J. A., & Rich, B. L. (2006). Loving yourself tive Science Quarterly, 59, 34 –72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
abundantly: Relationship of the narcissistic personality to self- and other 0001839213520131
perceptions of workplace deviance, leadership, and task and contextual Owens, B. (2009a). Humility in organizational leadership. (Doctoral dis-
performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 762–776. http://dx.doi sertation). Available from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Database.
.org/10.1037/0021-9010.91.4.762 (AAT 3370531).
Kahn, W. A. (1990). Psychological conditions of personal engagement and Owens, B. (2009b, August). Humility in organizations: Establishing con-
disengagement at work. Academy of Management Journal, 33, 692–724. struct, nomological, and predictive validity. Paper selected for best paper
http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/256287 proceedings at the annual meeting of the Academy of Management,
Kaiser, R. B., & Overfield, D. V. (2010). Assessing flexible leadership as Anaheim, CA.
a mastery of opposites. Consulting Psychology Journal: Practice and Owens, B., & Hekman, D. (2012). Modeling how to grow: An inductive
Research, 62, 105–118. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0019987 examination of humble leader behaviors, contingencies, and outcomes.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Kark, R., Waismel-Manor, R., & Shamir, B. (2012). Does valuing androg- Academy of Management Journal, 55, 787– 818. http://dx.doi.org/
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

yny and femininity lead to a female advantage? The relationship be- 10.5465/amj.2010.0441
tween gender-role, transformational leadership and identification. The Owens, B., & Hekman, D. (in press). How does leader humility influence
Leadership Quarterly, 23, 620 – 640. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua team performance? Exploring the mechanisms of contagion and collec-
.2011.12.012 tive promotion focus. Academy of Management Journal.
Karoly, P. (1993). Mechanisms of self-regulation: A systems view. Annual Owens, B., Johnson, M. D., & Mitchell, T. R. (2013). Humility in orga-
Review of Psychology, 44, 23–52. nizations: Implications for performance, teams, and leadership. Organi-
Kets de Vries, M. F. R., & Miller, D. (1985). Narcissism and leadership: zation Science, 24, 1517–1538. http://dx.doi.org/10.1287/orsc.1120
An object relations perspective. Human Relations, 38, 583– 601. http:// .0795
dx.doi.org/10.1177/001872678503800606 Owens, B., Rowatt, W. C., & Wilkins, A. L. (2011). Exploring the
Khoo, H. S., & Burch, G. S. J. (2008). The “dark side” of leadership relevance and implications of humility in organizations. In K. S. Cam-
personality and transformational leadership: An exploratory study. Per- eron & G. S. Spreitzer (Eds.), Handbook of positive organizational
sonality and Individual Differences, 44, 86 –97. http://dx.doi.org/ scholarship (pp. 260 –272). Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford Univer-
10.1016/j.paid.2007.07.018 sity Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199734610.013.0020
Kipnis, D. (1972). Does power corrupt? Journal of Personality and Social Park, N., & Peterson, C. (2003). Virtues and organizations. In K. S.
Psychology, 24, 33– 41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0033390 Cameron, J. E. Dutton, & R. E. Quinn (Eds.), Positive organizational
Konrath, S., Bushman, B. J., & Grove, T. (2009). Seeing my world in a scholarship: Foundations of a new discipline (pp. 33– 47). San Fran-
million little pieces: Narcissism, self-construal, and cognitive-perceptual cisco, CA: Berrett-Koehler.
style. Journal of Personality, 77, 1197–1228. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/ Paulhus, D. L., & Williams, K. M. (2002). The dark triad of personality:
j.1467-6494.2009.00579.x Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy. Journal of Research in
Lasch, C. (1979). The culture of narcissism: American life in an age of Personality, 36, 556 –563. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0092-6566
diminishing expectations. New York, NY: Norton Ltd. (02)00505-6
Lewis, M. W. (2000). Exploring paradox: Toward a more comprehensive Raskin, R. N., & Hall, C. S. (1979). A narcissistic personality inventory.
guide. The Academy of Management Review, 25, 760 –776. Psychological Reports, 45, 590. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1979.45.2
Lord, R. G., de Vader, C. L., & Alliger, G. M. (1986). A meta-analysis of .590
the relationship between personality traits and leadership perceptions: Raskin, R., & Terry, H. (1988). A principal-components analysis of the
An application of validity generalization procedures. Journal of Applied Narcissistic Personality Inventory and further evidence of its construct
Psychology, 71, 402– 410. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.71.3 validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 890 –902.
.402 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.5.890
Maccoby, M. (2000). Narcissistic leaders: The incredible pros, the inevi- Raudenbush, S. W., Bryk, A. S., Cheong, Y. F., Congdon, R., & du Toit,
table cons. Harvard Business Review, 78, 68 –77. M. (2011). HLM7: Hierarchical linear and nonlinear modeling. Lincol-
Meglino, B. M., & Korsgaard, M. A. (2006). Considering situational and nwood, IL: Scientific Software International.
dispositional approaches to rational self-interest: An extension and re- Rosenthal, S. A. (2006). Narcissism and leadership: A review and research
sponse to de Dreu (2006). Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 1253– agenda. (Center for Public Leadership Working Paper Series No. 06 –
1259. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.91.6.1253 04). Cambridge, MA: Center for Public Leadership. Retrieved from
Morf, C. C., & Rhodewalt, F. (2001). Unraveling the paradoxes of narcis- http://hdl.handle.net/1721.1/55948
sism: A dynamic self-regulatory processing model. Psychological In- Rosenthal, S. A., & Pittinsky, T. L. (2006). Narcissistic leadership. The
quiry, 12, 177–196. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15327965PLI1204_1 Leadership Quarterly, 17, 617– 633. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua
Morris, J. A., Brotheridge, C. M., & Urbanski, J. C. (2005). Bringing .2006.10.005
humility to leadership: Antecedents and consequences of leader humil- Ruggero, E. (2009, November 2). Gary-Cooper style. The Washington
ity. Human Relations, 58, 1323–1350. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ Post. Available from http://views.washingtonpost.com/leadership/
0018726705059929 panelists/2009/11/gary-cooper-style.html
Nielsen, R., Marrone, J. A., & Slay, H. S. (2010). A new look at humility: Schaufeli, W. B., & Bakker, A. B. (2004). Job demands, job resources and
Exploring the humility concept and its role in socialized charismatic their relationship with burnout and engagement: A multi-sample study.
leadership. Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies, 17, 33– 43. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 25, 293–315. http://dx.doi.org/
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1548051809350892 10.1002/job.248
Offermann, L. R., Kennedy, J. K., Jr., & Wirtz, P. W. (1994). Implicit Schaufeli, W. B., Bakker, A. B., & Salanova, M. (2006). The measurement
leadership theories: Content, structure, and generalizability. The of work engagement with a short questionnaire: A cross-national study.
Leadership Quarterly, 5, 43–58. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/1048- Educational and Psychological Measurement, 66, 701–716. http://dx.doi
9843(94)90005-1 .org/10.1177/0013164405282471
LEADER NARCISSISM AND HUMILITY 1213

Shadish, W. R., Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (2001). Experimental and Vogel, C. (2006). A field guide to narcissism. Psychology Today, 39,
quasi-experimental designs for generalized causal inference (2nd ed.). 68 –74.
Boston, MA: Cengage Learning. Vroom, V. H., & Yetton, P. W. (1973). Leadership and decision-making.
Siemsen, E., Roth, A., & Oliveira, P. (2010). Common method bias in Pittsburgh, PA: University of Pittsburgh Press.
regression models with linear, quadratic, and interaction effects. Orga- Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1984). Negative affectivity: The disposition to
nizational Research Methods, 13, 456 – 476. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ experience aversive emotional states. Psychological Bulletin, 3, 465–
1094428109351241 490. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.96.3.465
Singer, J. D. (1998). Using SAS PROC MIXED to fit multilevel models, Walumbwa, F. O., Avolio, B. J., & Zhu, W. (2008). How transformational
hierarchical models, and individual growth models. Journal of Educa- leadership weaves its influence on individual job performance: The role
tional and Behavioral Statistics, 24, 323–355. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/
of identification and efficacy beliefs. Personnel Psychology, 61, 793–
1165280
825. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1744-6570.2008.00131.x
Smith, J. A., & Foti, R. J. (1998). A pattern approach to the study of
Weick, K. E. (2000). Emergent change as a universal in organizations. In
leadership emergence. The Leadership Quarterly, 9, 147–160. http://dx
M. Beer & N. Nohria (Eds.), Breaking the code of change (pp. 223–242).
.doi.org/10.1016/S1048-9843(98)90002-9
Boston, MA: Harvard Business School Press.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Smith, W. K., & Lewis, M. W. (2011). Toward a theory of paradox: A


dynamic equilibrium model of organizing. The Academy of Management Zaccaro, S. J. (2007). Trait-based perspectives of leadership. American
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Review, 36, 381– 403. http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/AMR.2011.59330958 Psychologist, 62, 6 –16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.62.1.6


SPSS. (2012). IBM SPSS statistics version 21. Boston, MA: International Zaccaro, S. J., Kemp, C., & Bader, P. (2004). Leader traits and attributes.
Business Machines Corp. In J. Antonakis, A. T. Cianciolo, & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), The nature of
Sully de Luque, M., Washburn, N. T., Waldman, D. A., & House, R. J. leadership (pp. 101–124). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
(2008). Unrequited profit: How stakeholder and economic values relate Zhang, Y., Waldman, D. A., Han, Y., & Li, X. (2014). Paradoxical leader
to subordinates’ perceptions of leadership and firm performance. Ad- behaviors in people management: Antecedents and consequences. [Ad-
ministrative Science Quarterly, 53, 626 – 654. http://dx.doi.org/10.2189/ vance online publication]. Academy of Management Journal. http://dx
asqu.53.4.626 .doi.org/10.5465/amj.2012.0995
Surowiecki, J. (2011, October 17). How Steve Jobs changed. The New
Yorker. Retrieved from http://www.newyorker.com/talk/financial/2011/
10/17/111017ta_talk_surowiecki
Vera, D., & Rodriguez-Lopez, A. (2004). Humility as a source of com- Received October 16, 2013
petitive advantage. Organizational Dynamics, 334, 393– 408. http://dx Revision received November 25, 2014
.doi.org/10.1016/j.orgdyn.2004.09.006 Accepted December 5, 2014 䡲
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Correction to Owens et al. (2015)


In the article “Leader Narcissism and Follower Outcomes: The Counterbalancing Effect of Leader
Humility,” by Bradley P. Owens, Angela S. Wallace, and David A. Waldman (Journal of Applied
Psychology, Advance online publication. January 26, 2015. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0038698),
the last name of the second author was misspelled in the Online First version of the article.

All versions of this article have been corrected.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0039149

You might also like