Podlog (2020)

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/342069573

A Cognitive Behavioral Intervention for College Athletes With Injuries

Article in Sport Psychologist · June 2020


DOI: 10.1123/tsp.2019-0112

CITATIONS READS
3 1,597

7 authors, including:

Leslie W. Podlog John Heil


Université de Montréal Psychological Health Roanoke
188 PUBLICATIONS 3,052 CITATIONS 38 PUBLICATIONS 507 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Ryan Donald Burns Brad Fawver


University of Utah Walter Reed Army Institute of Research
199 PUBLICATIONS 1,464 CITATIONS 44 PUBLICATIONS 310 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

The effect of context on anticipation, decision making, and perception of deceptive intent View project

Sport Advocacy, Hazing & Interpersonal Violence View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Leslie W. Podlog on 18 November 2021.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


The Sport Psychologist, (Ahead of Print)
https://doi.org/10.1123/tsp.2019-0112
© 2020 Human Kinetics, Inc. APPLIED RESEARCH

A Cognitive Behavioral Intervention


for College Athletes With Injuries
Leslie W. Podlog John Heil
University of Utah Psychological Health Roanoke

Ryan D. Burns, Sean Bergeson, Tom Iriye, Brad Fawver, and A. Mark Williams
University of Utah

The authors used a quasi-experimental design to examine the efficacy of a cognitive-behavioral-therapy (CBT) intervention for
enhancing psychological well-being (positive and negative affect, vitality, self-esteem), rehabilitation adherence, and clinical
rehabilitation outcomes (pain, physical function) in 16 NCAA (National Collegiate Athletics Association) Division I athletes
experiencing a range of severe injuries. ANCOVAs, with adjusted baseline scores, revealed significant differences between the
experimental and control groups for positive affect at rehabilitation midpoint (T2; adjusted mean difference (AMD) = 0.41,
p = .04, η2 = .34) and return to play (T3; AMD = 0.67, p < .001, η2 = .70), negative affect at T3 (AMD = −0.81, p = .01, η2 = .47),
and vitality at T2 (AMD = 0.99, p = .01, η2 = .48) and T3 (AMD = 1.08, p = .02, η2 = .33). Given decrements in emotional
functioning after injury, the data support the use of CBT-based interventions for facilitating the emotional well-being of athletes
with severe injuries.

Keywords: athlete well-being, injury education, quasi-experimental design, vitality

The negative influence of sport injury on an athlete’s future Given the psychological challenges associated with injury
performance, as well as mental health and psychosocial well-being, rehabilitation and return to sport, researchers and practitioners
is well documented (Clement, Arvinen-Barrow, & Fetty, 2015; have sought to enhance athletes’ coping skills through implementa-
Leddy, Lambert, & Ogles, 1994; Podlog & Eklund, 2006; tion of various psychological interventions. In particular, psycho-
Putukian, 2016; Tracey, 2003). An examination of the literature logical strategies such as goal setting, imagery, relaxation, and
on the psychology of sport injury suggests that athletes with injuries expressing gratitude have all been shown to positively affect the
experience an array of concerns in the immediate-postinjury after- rehabilitation of athletes with injuries (Cupal & Brewer; 2001; Evans
math, during rehabilitation, and on their return to sport after injury & Hardy, 2002; Naoi & Ostrow, 2008; Salim & Wadey, 2019;
recovery. Prominent problems after injury occurrence include chal- Theodorakis, Beneca, Malliou, & Goudas, 1997; Theodorakis,
lenges with pain management, heightened negative affect, and Malliou, Papaioannou, Beneca, & Filactakidou, 1996). However,
difficulties accepting one’s new incapacitations (Tracey, 2003). there remains a dearth of rigorous methodological designs in this
During the rehabilitation phase other psychological challenges area of research (Cupal, 1998; Schwab Reese, Pittsinger, & Yang,
commonly emerge, such as a loss of confidence in one’s physical 2012). In highlighting this paucity, Schwab Reese et al. (2012) found
capabilities, feelings of social isolation, loss of athletic identity, only six published studies examining the efficacy of psychological
and inadequate social support (Clement & Shannon, 2011; interventions on sport-injury-rehabilitation outcomes. Two studies
Ruddock-Hudson, O’Halloran, & Murphy, 2012; Tracey, 2003). used randomized control trials (Cupal & Brewer, 2001; Evans &
Finally, as an athlete’s return to sport approaches, excitement over Hardy, 2002), two used “before and after study designs” (Johnson,
resumption of competitive activities may be dampened by reinjury 2000; Mankad & Gordon, 2010), and two employed case-study
approaches (Mahoney & Hanrahan, 2011; Rock & Jones, 2002).
anxieties, worries about regaining previous competitive levels, and
Since the publication of Schwab Reese et al.’s review, few additional
trepidation about the impact of injury on skill execution (Podlog &
well-designed intervention studies have been conducted (e.g.,
Dionigi, 2010; Podlog & Eklund, 2006). Moreover, returning
Maddison et al., 2012; Salim & Wadey, 2019). Furthermore, re-
athletes might also worry about meeting internal and external searchers have typically ignored the relationships between interven-
performance expectations, self-presentational concerns about ap- tion skills and adherence behaviors or clinical rehabilitation outcomes,
pearing athletically incompetent, and doubts over their level of namely, pain levels and perceived functional ability (Walker &
physical fitness (Evans, Hardy, & Fleming, 2000; Podlog & Arvinen-Barrow, 2013), shortcomings we sought to address in the
Eklund, 2006; Walker, Thatcher, & Lavallee, 2010). current investigation. Finally, to our knowledge, no intervention
studies to date have specifically focused on college athlete popula-
Podlog, Burns, Bergeson, and Williams are with the Dept. of Health, Kinesiology, tions, a cohort facing unique challenges given the need to maintain
and Recreation; Iriye, the Athletics Dept.; and Fawver, the Div. of Physical academic standing in the face of competitive and scholarship demands
Medicine and Rehabilitation, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, USA. Heil (Wilson & Pritchard, 2005).
is with Psychological Health Roanoke, Roanoke, VA, USA. Podlog (les.podlog@ Despite calls for well-designed psychological interventions in
utah.edu) is corresponding author. the area of injury rehabilitation (Cupal, 1998), few researchers have,
1
2 Podlog et al.

to date, heeded such recommendations (for some exceptions see rehabilitation adherence (Schwab Reese et al., 2012), a shortcom-
Cupal & Brewer, 2001; Evans & Hardy, 2002; Maddison et al., ing we sought to rectify in the current investigation. Finally, given
2012; Salim & Wadey, 2019). As highlighted herein, the present empirical evidence suggesting positive links between rehabilitation
study represented an effort to add to the relatively limited body of adherence and enhanced clinical and functional outcomes, we
intervention research, in particular with injured NCAA (National hypothesized that our intervention would not only augment adher-
Collegiate Athletics Association) Division I athletes. We also sought ence behaviors but also hold positive implications for injured
to build on existing research by examining the influence of a athletes’ functional capabilities.
psychological intervention on a critical recovery behavior— Based on previous research demonstrating improvements in
rehabilitation adherence (Evans & Hardy, 2002)—as well as ad- athlete well-being and functional abilities as rehabilitation pro-
dressing key indicators of clinical/functional outcomes. gresses (Evans et al., 2000; Clement et al., 2015; Tracey, 2003), we
The development of our intervention was informed by tenets hypothesized that both control and experimental groups would
of cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT), a widely used and demonstrate changes over time in our dependent outcomes of
exhaustively investigated intervention method that has demon- interest. Furthermore, we hypothesized that athletes in the experi-
strated clinical efficacy in helping individuals manage dysfunc- mental condition would show greater well-being (enhanced affect,
tional emotions, maladaptive behaviors, and disruptive cognitive vitality, self-esteem) and rehabilitation adherence and improved
processes (Zhang et al., 2019). Focusing on how individuals clinical outcomes (reduced pain and increased perceived functional
appraise or interpret events is a central component of CBT-based ability) than athletes in the control group receiving only a program
interventions (McGinn & Sanderson, 2001). In CBT, personal of physical rehabilitation. Based on the central tenets of CBT,
evaluations, meanings, and philosophies about the world are experimental-group athletes were expected to demonstrate enhanced
critical for understanding how people react to events in their self-regulation skills (relative to controls), which would in turn
lives, whether an event be a sport injury, a perceived negative promote adaptive rehabilitation behaviors and outcomes.
interaction with a colleague, or a failure to achieve a personal goal
(McGinn & Sanderson, 2001). From a CBT standpoint, an injured
athlete’s maladaptive thoughts and concerns are likely to lead to Methods
unpleasant emotions, which in turn negatively influence motivation
and engagement in important rehabilitation behaviors such as adher- Power Analysis
ence to the prescribed rehabilitation program. The central premise of A power analysis was conducted using ANOVA procedures with
CBT is that altering maladaptive thoughts leads to a change in an expected large effect (f = 0.40), a factorial design within-
psychological affect (emotions) and in subsequent behavior between interaction for each outcome variable, and a correlation
(McGinn & Sanderson, 2001; Zhang et al., 2019). Adherence between repeated measures of r = .5. The expected large effect
behaviors such as showing up to rehabilitation sessions, complying size was based on previous intervention research showing similar
with activity restrictions, managing pain, and putting appropriate effects (Cupal & Brewer, 2001; Maddison et al., 2012). For
effort into rehabilitation exercises have all been shown to predict example, Cupal and Brewer (2001) found experimental evidence
enhanced clinical and return-to-sport outcomes (Alzate Saez de supporting the effects of relaxation and guided imagery on reinjury
Heredia, Ramirez, & Lazaro; 2004; Brewer et al., 2004; Evans & anxiety and pain. Specifically, variance effect-size calculations
Hardy, 2002; Evans et al., 2000; Pizzari, Taylor, McBurney, & revealed that the treatment accounted for approximately 62% of
Feller, 2005). the variance in reduction of reinjury anxiety (η2 = .62) and approx-
Given the limitations associated with past psychological inter- imately 76% of the variance in the overall reduction of pain
ventions conducted with injured athletes, the purpose of the present (η2 = .76). Using the aforementioned parameters, 14 participants
study was to examine the efficacy of a CBT intervention for would need to be recruited for the current study to achieve 80%
enhancing the well-being, rehabilitation adherence, and clinical statistical power.
outcomes of athletes with injuries. Guided by CBT tenets, we chose
to examine cognitive and affective indicators of well-being shown
Participants
to be disrupted in the aftermath of injury (Clement et al., 2015;
Leddy et al., 1994; Podlog, Lochbaum, & Stevens, 2010) and to Sixteen athletes with injuries were recruited from a large uni-
examine behavioral (adherence) and clinical outcomes (pain and versity in the southwestern United States with the assistance of
functional ability) recognized as fundamental to the success of the associate director of athletic training. To be eligible for
rehabilitation (Alzate Saez de Heredia et al., 2004; Brewer et al., participation, athletes were required to be 18 years of age or
2004; Evans & Hardy, 2002; Pizzari et al., 2005). Given extensive older, have incurred an injury requiring a minimum 4-week
research demonstrating the prevalence of negatively valanced absence from training or competitive performance (i.e., a period
emotions after injury (Smith et al., 1993; Tracey, 2003; Walker; of competitive absence likely to result in well-being decrements;
Thatcher, & Lavallee, 2007), as well as evidence highlighting the Podlog & Eklund, 2006), and not have entered the sports-
debilitating impact of injury on athletes’ energy levels (vitality) and medicine clinic more than 1 week prior to their involvement
self-esteem (Podlog, Lochbaum, & Stevens, 2010; Tracey, 2003; in the study. This final criterion helped ensure that all partici-
Wasley & Lox, 1998), it seemed reasonable to examine the impact pants were at the same point in their rehabilitation schedule and
of our CBT intervention on these salient cognitive and affective that athletes in the experimental group had the opportunity
variables of well-being. Furthermore, as indicated, although to cultivate and use CBT skills throughout their rehabilitation.
numerous studies have demonstrated positive links between adher- Any participants not meeting all three eligibility criteria were
ence and rehabilitation outcomes (Alzate Saez de Heredia et al., excluded. These criteria resulted in a sample of 12 female and
2004; Brewer et al., 2004; Evans & Hardy, 2002; Pizzari et al., 4 male NCAA Division I athletes (6 female and 2 male in
2005), surprisingly few scholars have examined the implications experimental and control groups, respectively) with an age
of psychological interventions on critical behaviors such as of 19.94 ± 1.69 years (M ± SD; mean age = 20.13 in the
(Ahead of Print)
Cognitive-Behavioral Injury Intervention 3

experimental group and 19.75 in the control group). All parti- CBT Intervention
cipants had experienced a serious injury that entailed absence
from sport training/competition for 13.31 ± 12.34 weeks. The The experimental group was trained in four CBT skills over four
average injury time loss for the experimental group was consecutive weeks. Training in the CBT skills involved participa-
15.13 weeks, and 11.5 weeks for the control group, t(14) = tion in four separate 1-hr sessions (i.e., a total of 4 hr), involving
0.42, p = .67, suggesting that athletes in both groups were one-to-one meetings between the athlete with an injury and a
missing sport-specific training and competition for relatively member of the research team. In addition to the four individual
prolonged periods of time (i.e., approximately 3 months), with meetings, athletes partaking in the CBT intervention completed a
no statistically significant differences between groups. Injury number of homework assignments—described in greater detail
types included torn anterior cruciate ligament (n = 6), stress following—that collectively took approximately 2.5–3 hr to com-
fracture/reaction (n = 3), sprain (n = 2), tendonitis (n = 1), torn plete over the 4-week intervention. In total, athletes in the experi-
knee cartilage (n = 1), broken fibula and tibia (n = 1), torn disc mental condition engaged in 6.5–7 hr of training over and above
and arthritis in lumbar spine (n = 1), and rotator cuff overuse their physical rehabilitation. To facilitate standardization of psy-
(n = 1). Participants competed in a range of sports, specifically, chological-skills content and delivery, a CBT intervention work-
track and field (n = 5), football (n = 3), basketball (n = 1), soccer book was developed. The member of the research team who
(n = 3), volleyball (n = 1), diving (n = 1), gymnastics (n = 1), and delivered the intervention had graduate training in sport psychol-
ogy, a detailed knowledge of the workbook content, and experience
tennis (n = 1). Given the serious nature of the injuries across the
working as a student-athlete academic mentor. Intervention skills
experimental and control groups, none of the participants were
included injury education (Skill 1), attentional focus and distraction
engaged in sport-specific training—be it coach-dictated or per-
control (Skill 2), managing emotions (Skill 3), and pain manage-
sonal training. All but three participants had experienced at least
ment (Skill 4). Consistent with CBT principles, Skills 1 and 2 were
one previous severe injury (M = 1.93) requiring a 4-week com-
designed to alter maladaptive thinking (i.e., cognitive appraisals
petitive absence. Ethical approval was obtained from the lead
related to the injury), while Skills 3 and 4 were intended to address
institution before data collection, and all athletes gave their
written consent before participating. the affective and behavioral component of CBT. A copy of
intervention materials is available from the first author on request.
During the initial session, the intervention provider met indi-
Design
vidually with the injured athlete after referral from the treating
Our aim in the current study was to examine the impact of our athletic trainer. The purpose of the initial session was fivefold: to
CBT-grounded intervention on NCAA Division I varsity athletes build initial rapport, to ask the athlete about the nature of the injury,
with a current injury. We adopted a quasi-experimental design, to introduce the four skills included in the CBT intervention, to give
given that participants were rehabilitating in a single facility the athlete an initial self-assessment exercise, and to discuss an
where there was open and regular communication between injury education guideline. The self-assessment asked the athlete to
athletes. As such, we wanted to avoid contamination effects “rate your perceived knowledge or ability on each of the four
whereby athletes in the experimental group discussed contents of skills.” A definition of each skill was provided followed by a rating
the intervention (i.e., skills they learned and/or workbook ma- scale of 0 (very poor) to 10 (excellent). The athlete received a score
terials they received) with those in the control group. Conse- out of a possible 40, with higher scores indicating greater knowl-
quently, we used a quasi-experimental, repeated-measures edge or ability on the CBT intervention skills. The overall and
design in which the first eight athletes meeting the eligibility individual item scores gave the intervention provider information
criteria were assigned to the experimental/intervention (CBT) on the injured athlete and helped initiate a conversation regarding
group and the second cohort of eight athletes was assigned to the why the athlete gave him- or herself a particular score on a specific
control group. Athletes in the control group completed their skill. Finally, in Session 1, the provider and the athlete discussed
regularly scheduled program of physical rehabilitation as set out the injury education guideline. The guideline was designed to
by their treating athletic trainer. The nature and content of the enhance Skill 1, that is, to cultivate the athlete’s knowledge of
rehabilitation sessions varied as a function of the athletes’ injury the rehabilitation process, to develop his or her sense of personal
type. Athletes in the control group did not receive any of the CBT investment in the recovery process, and to facilitate adherence to
intervention materials or training sessions described herein. We treatment tasks. Example content areas included basic anatomy of
employed a single-blind design in which injured participants the injured area, active/passive rehabilitation methods, rationales
were unaware of the specific aims of the study or the group to for limits on physical activity during the acute injury phase, and the
which they were assigned. All participants were informed that notion of rehabilitation as an active collaborative learning process.
they were being asked to partake in a research study examining After reviewing the education guideline, the intervention provider
the well-being of athletes with injury and their rehabilitation asked the athlete to schedule an appointment with his or her athletic
and recovery outcomes. Athletic trainers were not blind to trainer. The guideline served as a heuristic to facilitate a question-
the purposes of the study or which group participants were and-answer session between the athlete and the treating athletic
allocated to, for the following reasons: We required approval trainer. The guideline was intended to identify specific or targeted
from the associate director of athletic training—that is, one of points that might not emerge as part of the natural consultation or
the treating athletic trainers in the study—to conduct the inves- treatment process. During the meeting, the athlete and athletic
tigation; we relied on treating athletic trainers to facilitate trainer worked collaboratively to identify, discuss, and take notes
participant recruitment, so they were aware of participants’ on bullet points listed in the education guidelines that were
group allocation; and athletic trainers were involved in the germane to the athlete’s injury. Given the variability of injury
intervention itself, specifically in terms of the “question and types and severity, athletic trainers necessarily provided idiosyn-
answer” session involving the injury education guideline dis- cratic responses to the injury education items (see example items
cussed following. listed earlier). That said, there was likely some consistency in the

(Ahead of Print)
4 Podlog et al.

types of information conveyed to athletes experiencing similar valanced emotions and to promote muscle-to-mind relaxation. The
injuries and recovery timelines, for instance, among the partici- 15-min script started by having the athletes turn their attention to
pants undertaking rehabilitation for a torn anterior cruciate liga- their breath, simply noticing the way they were breathing (fast/
ment or a stress fracture. To solidify retention of key take-home slow, shallow/deep, smooth and regular). They were encouraged to
points from the meeting with the athletic trainer, athletes in the breath in a manner that is “slow, deep, and regular.” This compo-
experimental group discussed take-home points with the CBT nent was followed by a muscle relaxation, involving a count from 1
intervention provider. to 10, with each number associated with a particular area of the
In Session 2 (1-hr individual meeting), the intervention pro- body. Each athlete in the experimental condition was given the CD
vider asked the athlete to discuss key points from the question-and- and asked to complete the relaxation homework exercise on two
answer meeting with the athletic trainer. This discussion was occasions before the fourth and final session the following week.
followed by engagement in three different exercises designed to Session 4 (1-hr individual meeting) focused on pain manage-
enhance Skill 2, that is, to help the athlete enhance attentional focus ment (Skill 4). The intervention provider took the athlete through a
and manage distracting thoughts (Skill 2). In particular, the ex- scripted visualization session (30–45 min) designed to help meet
ercises helped the athlete problem solve by converting his or her the challenge of severe and persistent pain (Heil, 1993). The
worries into productive action (Exercise 1), manage expectations visualization session invoked images (e.g., eye of storm) and
(Exercise 2), and shift from negative to positive thoughts (thought sensations (heat and cold) that enabled athletes to associate to
stopping; Exercise 3). In Exercise 1, the intervention provider their pain and help them work with calm intensity within their pain
discussed the idea that worry was thinking that tended to be cyclical limits. The idea of associating with one’s pain—as opposed to
in nature, consumed energy and created anxiety, and generally dissociating or blocking out one’s pain—was based on the paradox
accomplished no useful coping purpose. After this discussion, of acceptance and surrender as a way of gaining power and control
athletes were asked to complete a table in which they made a over the pain (Heil, 1993). As with the other skills and exercises,
list of concerns, devised solutions to specific concerns, and articu- the intervention provider delivered the pain-visualization script to
lated pros and cons associated with each solution. The intervention experimental-group athletes on an individual basis. Prior to the
provider suggested to the athlete that he or she work to adopt pain-management visualization exercise, the athlete listened to
solutions with the greatest number of pros and the least number of the mind–body relaxation CD (15 min) described previously. At
cons. Exercise 2 (managing expectations) involved a discussion the completion of Session 4, as a homework assignment, the
about the potentially facilitative and/or debilitative role of expecta- intervention provider asked the athlete to go through the pain-
tions, followed by completion of a table in which athletes listed management visualization on two occasions during the following
injury-related expectations, the source of the expectation (internal week. The athlete was also encouraged to use the visualization, as
vs. external), and whether they felt the expectation “weighed them or when needed, throughout his or her rehabilitation.
down or lifted them up.” Based on the response to the latter
question, athletes were instructed to either “keep the expectation”
Measures
or to “get rid of it.” Finally, Exercise 3 (thought stopping) helped
the athletes redirect negative cognitions to more facilitative, action- We collected relevant demographic information including age,
oriented thoughts that might be instrumental in addressing injury- sport type, gender, injury type, severity (expected time loss
related challenges. In particular, the athletes completed a table in from competition), date of injury, and the number of severe injuries
which they identified specific negative thoughts, invoked a key previously incurred (defined as 4 weeks or more out of training and
word/image/feeling (e.g., “stop,” “enough,” a rubber band snap- competition) from participants at the time of their first question-
ping against one’s wrist), and wrote down a replacement thought or naire completion, that is, within their first week of rehabilitation.
action to help them redirect their attentional focus. Three indicators of well-being relevant to athletes with an injury
In Session 3 (1-hr individual meeting), experimental-group included the 20-item Positive and Negative Affect Schedule
athletes participated in two prescribed intervention tasks. These (PANAS; Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988), the 7-item Subjective
included a guided written emotional-disclosure exercise (Mankad, Vitality Scale (Ryan & Frederick, 1997), and the 10-item Self-
Gordon, & Wallman, 2009) and an audio-guided relaxation CD Esteem Scale (Rosenberg, 1965).
(Heil, 1993), both of which were designed to facilitate the injured
PANAS. The PANAS contained 10 items measuring positive
athletes’ emotional adjustment (Skill 3). With regard to the emo-
affect (e.g., excited, enthusiastic, determined; α = .82) and 10 items
tional-disclosure exercise, each athlete was given a list of questions
measuring negative affect (e.g., distressed, upset, irritable; α = .85;
intended to help him or her become more self-aware of any injury-
Watson et al., 1988). Responses to the statement stem “At present,
related thoughts and emotions. The questions were not intended to
I feel . . .” were recorded on a Likert-type scale from 1 (strongly
be exhaustive but, rather, to help the athletes consider potentially
disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Subscale items were averaged to
relevant emotional aspects of their injury. Example questions
form positive-affect and negative-affect scores. Researchers have
included “What has been most difficult for you about your injury?”
observed the PANAS to demonstrate factorial validity, as well as
“What are the things you miss the most about your sport?”
acceptable Cronbach’s alphas and test–retest reliabilities across
“Describe any impact of your injury on your energy levels.”
different temporal instructions (Crawford & Henry, 2004; Watson
“Of all your emotions about your injury, which have been most
et al., 1988). The scale has been used in previous sport-injury
difficult and why?” and “In what ways has your social life changed
research (Podlog, Lochbaum, & Stevens, 2010; Wadey, Podlog,
or do people treat you differently?” After completion of the written
Galli, & Mellalieu, 2016).
emotional-disclosure exercise, athletes were given the opportunity
to discuss any comments they had written with the intervention Subjective Vitality Scale. Six items from the Subjective Vitality
provider. The second half of Session 3 involved athletes listening to Scale (e.g., “I feel alive and vital,” “I have energy and spirit,” “I
an audio-guided mind–body relaxation CD. The relaxation CD was look forward to each new day”) were used to assess athletes’
designed to help the athletes modulate the intensity of negatively subjective feelings of vitality (α = .85). Responses to the statement
(Ahead of Print)
Cognitive-Behavioral Injury Intervention 5

stem “At present, I feel . . .” were recorded on a Likert-type scale Procedure


from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Item scores were After we received institutional-review-board approval for the
averaged to form an overall vitality score. The scale has established study, participants completed the informed-consent procedures.
reliability and construct validity (Ryan & Frederick, 1997) and has They were assigned on a rolling basis to either the control (n = 8) or
been used in past sport-injury research (Podlog, Lochbaum, & experimental group (n = 8) as described previously. Questionnaires
Stevens, 2010). assessing psychological well-being and clinical outcomes (pain
Self-Esteem Scale. The 10-item Self-Esteem Scale was used to and perceived function) for experimental- and control-group par-
assess injured athletes’ feelings about themselves at the time of ticipants were administered by a research assistant during the first
their study participation (e.g., “On the whole I am satisfied with week of rehabilitation (T1), at the rehabilitation midpoint (T2;
myself,” “I have a number of good qualities,” “I take a positive identified by the treating certified athletic trainer), and on medical
attitude toward myself”; Rosenberg, 1965). Responses to the clearance to return to play (T3). Adherence was assessed by the
statement stem “I currently feel that . . .” were recorded on a treating athletic trainer at T2 and T3. While participants’ length of
Likert-type scale of 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). recovery varied as a function of their injury type and severity, all
Item scores were averaged to form an overall self-esteem score participants completed T1–T3 measures at approximately the same
(α = .86). The scale has established reliability and construct validity stage of their rehabilitation and return to play. Moreover, since we
(Rosenberg, 1965) and has been adopted by previous sport-injury assessed our dependent outcomes of interest over the duration of
researchers (Podlog, Lochbaum, & Stevens, 2010; Wasley & Lox, athletes’ full rehabilitation program (i.e., until they were medically
1998). cleared to return to sport), we were able to determine the actual
length of each athlete’s recovery time. Based on this, the treating
Rehabilitation Adherence Measure for Athletic Training. The athletic trainers’ accuracy in their projections of T2 being close to
Rehabilitation Adherence Measure for Athletic Training is a 16- the actual midpoint of rehabilitation was decided by the authors to
item measure of adherence developed in a college athletic-training be methodologically acceptable (within 1 week from participants’
setting (Granquist, Gill, & Appaneal, 2010). The scale contains actual rehabilitation midpoint). Questionnaires were completed
three subscales assessing attendance/participation (example item: during athletes’ regularly scheduled rehabilitation session, and
“attends scheduled rehabilitation sessions,” α = .75), communica- all responses were kept confidential per ethics guidelines.
tion (example item: “communicates with the athletic trainer if there
is a problem with the exercises,” α = .48), and attitude/effort
(example item: “has a positive attitude during rehabilitation ses- Statistical Analysis
sions,” α = .84). Given the low alpha score for the communication Data were screened for outliers using box plots and z scores (using
subscale, we omitted it from further analysis. The measure was a ±3.0-z cut point) and checked for Gaussian distribution using
completed by certified athletic trainers to ascertain their perspec- k-density plots. No data within any dependent variable met the
tives on athlete adherence. Subscales and the total scale scores have criteria for a potential outlier, so all were included in the analyses.
demonstrated internal consistency and the ability to discriminate Baseline differences between experimental and control groups
between adherence levels (Clark, Bassett, & Siegert, 2018; were analyzed using independent t tests. To examine differences
Granquist et al., 2010). between experimental and control groups at the T2 and T3 time
Short-Form McGill Pain Questionnaire. The valid and reliable points, analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) tests were employed,
Short-Form McGill Pain Questionnaire consists of 15 descrip- controlling for scores at baseline. This method was used to address
tors (11 sensory, α = .71; 4 affective, α = .68) rated on an a conditional research question assuming no differences among
intensity scale (0 = none, 1 = mild, 2 = moderate, or 3 = severe; dependent-variable scores at T1 (Fitzmaurice, 2001). Because there
Melzack, 1987). Two pain scores were derived from the sum of were no statistical differences between experimental and control
the intensity rank values of the words chosen for the sensory and groups at T1, the T1 covariate reduced baseline random noise,
affective domains. The Short-Form McGill Pain Questionnaire thereby potentially increasing statistical power and improving
has shown adequate test–retest reliability in individuals with a internal validity. The between-groups assumption of homogeneity
variety of conditions such as osteoarthritis and musculoskeletal of variance was examined using Levene’s test, and the ANCOVA
pain and has demonstrated adequate internal consistency assumption of homogeneity of regression slopes was examined by
testing the statistical significance of a T1 × Group interaction term.
(α > .75; Melzack & Katz, 2001) and content validity (Trudeau
The initial alpha level for all statistical analyses was set at p < .05,
et al., 2012).
and effect sizes were estimated using partial eta-squared (η2).
Oswestry Disability Index. The Oswestry Disability Index is a
10-item, validated disability measure assessing perceived func-
tional ability (example item: “I can lift only very light weights,” Manipulation Check
α = .74; Fairbank, Couper, Davies, & O’Brien, 1980). Participants A manipulation check was conducted by ascertaining athlete
responded to 10 sections (i.e., pain intensity, personal care, lifting, perceptions of how much their treatment during rehabilitation
walking, sitting, standing, sleeping, sex life [if applicable], social focused on features present in the CBT training and presumably
life, traveling) in which they are asked to “tick one box only in each not present among those receiving physical rehabilitation only
section that most closely describes you today.” For each section the (control group). A statement asking participants to “Rate the extent
total possible score was 5: If the first statement was marked, to which your rehabilitation helped you to . . .” was followed by
the section score was 0; if the last statement was marked, it was 5. items including “learn more about the nature of your injury,
The disability scores range from 0 to 100%, with higher scores manage worries and expectations,” “shift your focus from negative
indicating greater disability. The instrument has been widely used to positive thoughts,” “manage emotions,” and “work through
in previous rehabilitation research (Fairbank & Pynsent, 2000; pain.” All responses were recorded on a Likert scale ranging
Podlog et al., 2019). from 1 (very unhelpful) to 5 (very helpful). A grand mean score
(Ahead of Print)
6 Podlog et al.

of the average of responses on all five items was calculated, with size, and higher adjusted vitality means at T2 (adjusted mean
responses showing high internal consistency (α = .866). A t test difference = 0.99, p = .01 η2 = .48) and T3 (adjusted mean differ-
revealed that the experimental group scored significantly higher ence = 1.08, p = .02, η2 = .33; Figure 3), represented by large effect
than the control group on this measure (intervention M = 4.28, sizes. There were no statistically significant Group × Time inter-
SD = 0.89; control M = 3.47, SD = 0.51), t(14) = −2.21, p = .04, actions for disability (p = .49, η2 = .21), sensory pain (p = .57,
suggesting the effectiveness of the CBT intervention in promoting η2 = .10), affective pain (p = .36, η2 = .15), self-esteem (p = .99,
the intended psychological skills. We also assessed the extent to η2 < .001), or Rehabilitation Adherence Measure for Athletic
which experimental-group members (n = 8) completed the home- Training (p = .82, η2 < .001).
work assigned on the five requested occasions. On average,
participants reported completing 4.63 of the 5 homework assign-
ments (SD = 0.48), indicating fairly high self-reported adherence to
the intervention.
Discussion
We examined the efficacy of a CBT-based intervention for
Results enhancing the well-being, rehabilitation adherence, and clinical
outcomes of athletes with injuries using a quasi-experimental
Descriptive statistics across all time points for each dependent design. The current study was an attempt to add to the relatively
variable are provided in Table 1. There were no statistically limited body of psychological interventions with NCAA Divi-
significant differences between groups at the T1 on any dependent sion I athletes with severe injuries. As noted by Simpson
variables. Table 2 presents the results from the ANCOVA tests for and Post (2014), challenges associated with recruitment of
each dependent variable. ANCOVAs revealed significant group busy student-athletes for participation in intervention research
differences, with the intervention group displaying higher adjusted (engagement in treatment sessions and completion of repeated-
positive affect means at T2 (adjusted mean difference = 0.41, measure designs) are a likely reason why there are so few
p = .04, η2p = .34) and T3 (adjusted mean difference = 0.67, psychological interventions with college athlete cohorts. As
p < .001, η2 = .70; Figure 1). Additional group differences were such, the present study adds to the literature by providing
observed at T3, with the intervention group displaying lower evidence for the value of an ecologically valid CBT-based
adjusted negative affect means (adjusted mean difference = intervention that addresses scheduling constraints and allows
− 0.81, p = .01, η2 = .47; Figure 2), represented by a large effect for flexible service delivery.

Table 1 Descriptive Statistics Across All Time Points, M (SD)


Time Total sample, Control group, Intervention group, Baseline group
point N = 16 n=8 n=8 difference, p
Positive affect T1 3.12 (0.57) 3.04 (0.60) 3.21 (0.57) .60
T2 3.45 (0.49) 3.21 (0.32) 3.70 (0.53)
T3 3.91 (0.46) 3.54 (0.29) 4.28 (0.23)
Negative affect T1 3.03 (0.73) 3.21 (0.63) 2.85 (0.82) .38
T2 2.57 (0.81) 2.91 (0.63) 2.24 (0.87)
T3 2.31 (0.61) 2.72 (0.52) 1.90 (0.37)
Vitality T1 3.63 (0.95) 3.42 (0.90) 3.83 (1.03) .41
T2 4.24 (0.73) 3.75 (0.50) 4.73 (0.56)
T3 4.86 (0.94) 4.34 (0.72) 5.39 (0.87)
Disability T1 2.12 (0.51) 2.30 (0.35) 1.40 (1.40) .92
T2 1.58 (0.56) 1.63 (0.64) 1.40 (1.40)
T3 1.28 (0.36) 1.35 (0.37) 1.00 (1.00)
Sensory pain T1 0.87 (0.52) 0.95 (0.59) 0.77 (0.44) .34
T2 0.43 (0.30) 0.38 (0.27) 0.51 (0.34)
T3 0.16 (0.14) 0.14 (0.15) 0.20 (0.15)
Affective pain T1 0.42 (0.53) 0.31 (0.42) 0.53 (0.65) .44
T2 0.22 (0.20) 0.19 (0.18) 0.25 (0.23)
T3 0.14 (0.26) 0.03 (0.08) 0.25 (0.33)
Self-esteem T1 2.02 (0.34) 2.21 (0.34) 1.78 (0.64) .13
T2 1.90 (0.52) 2.09 (0.40) 1.65 (0.60)
T3 1.83 (0.46) 2.01 (0.39) 1.60 (0.45)
RAdMAT total T2 44.56 (4.37) 42.62 (4.34) 46.50 (3.66) .07
T3 41.81 (4.85) 39.62 (3.33) 44.00 (5.31)
Note. T1 = baseline; T2 = rehabilitation midpoint; T3 = medical clearance to return to play; RAdMAT = Rehabilitation Adherence Measure for Athletic Training.
Adherence (RAdMAT) was assessed at T2 and T3. As such, there are no T1 adherence scores.

(Ahead of Print)
Cognitive-Behavioral Injury Intervention 7

Table 2 Experimental-Group Main Effects


From the Analysis-of-Covariance Tests Using
a Baseline-Score Covariate
Dependent variable Time point F p η2p
Positive affect T2 5.78 .04* .34
T3 26.06 <.001* .70
Negative affect T2 1.65 .23 .13
T3 9.92 .01* .47
Vitality T2 12.19 .01* .48
T3 6.50 .02* .33
Disability T2 0.04 .85 .01
T3 0.29 .68 .10
Sensory pain T2 1.60 .23 .12
T3 0.19 .67 .02
Affective pain T2 0.05 .83 .00
T3 2.38 .15 .16
Self-esteem T2 0.23 .64 .02 Figure 2 — Mean negative affect (Likert scale from 1 [very slightly or
not at all] to 5 [extremely]) across intervention time points by group.
T3 0.74 .41 .06
T1 = baseline; T2 = intervention midpoint; T3 = intervention end point.
RAdMAT total T3 1.09 .32 .08
Note. η2p = effect-size metric partial eta-squared; T2 = rehabilitation midpoint;
T3 = medical clearance to return to play; RAdMAT = Rehabilitation Adherence
Measure for Athletic Training. As there were no baseline scores for adherence
(RAdMAT), T2 RAdMAT scores were used as the ANCOVA covariate; hence
only T3 scores are reported.
*Significant at p < .05.

Figure 3 — Vitality across intervention time points by group.


CBT = cognitive-behavioral therapy; T1 = baseline; T2 = intervention
midpoint; T3 = intervention end point.

Figure 1 — Mean positive affect (Likert scale from 1 [very slightly or


well-being, rehabilitation adherence, and clinical outcomes
not at all] to 5 [extremely]) across intervention time points by group.
T1 = baseline; T2 = intervention midpoint; T3 = intervention end point. compared with athletes in the control group receiving a program
of physical rehabilitation only. Moderate support was found
for this hypothesis, with athletes in the experimental condition
showing greater well-being (enhanced affect, vitality, self-
We hypothesized that both control and experimental groups esteem) relative to control-group members. In particular,
would demonstrate improvements over time in relation to our increased positive affect and vitality at the T2 and T3 and
outcome variables. As shown in Table 1, findings supported this diminished negative affect at T3 were documented, with large
hypothesis, with improvements in psychological well-being and effect sizes. These findings support the value of multimodal
clinical function (i.e., decreases in disability) documented psychological interventions such as CBT for enhancing pleas-
across time for both groups. Furthermore, we anticipated that ant/adaptive, and decreasing unpleasant/maladaptive, emotions
athletes in the experimental condition would show improved during the rehabilitation period.
(Ahead of Print)
8 Podlog et al.

Researchers have previously demonstrated a predominance of from this investigation suggest that relatively brief interventions
unpleasant emotions (e.g., anger, anxiety, depression, isolation) after (4 weeks long) may have sustained impact for the affective states
initial injury occurrence and rehabilitation setbacks (Ruddock- and vitality of injured athletes. These findings bolster those of
Hudson et al., 2012; Tracey, 2003), as well as the deleterious impact Johnson (2000), who also found that a short-term multimodal
of negative emotions on the healing processes (Gouin & Kiecolt- intervention (stress management and cognitive control, goal
Glaser, 2011) and rehabilitation adherence (Brewer, Cornelius, & setting, and relaxation/guided imagery) helped elevate positive
Van Raalte, 2013). Findings regarding improved psychological mood in competitive athletes with long-term injuries. Further
well-being outcomes in the CBT intervention group are valuable research is needed to better ascertain dose-response effects (how
in three respects. First, although psychological interventions have much and for how long) regarding psychological interventions for
demonstrated efficacy in past research (Evans & Hardy, 2002; athletes with long-term injuries.
Johnson, 2000; Maddison et al., 2012), relatively few well- No statistical support was found for between-groups differences
controlled studies have been implemented (Brewer & Redmond, in rehabilitation adherence or clinical function, perhaps because
2016; Schwab Reese et al., 2012). Although we adopted a quasi- rehabilitation protocols are already well addressed in the university’s
experimental versus true-experimental design, our investigation adds established athlete injury-intervention programs. Specifically, ath-
to the relatively modest body of psychological-intervention research letes in both conditions were involved in structured rehabilitation
by demonstrating the value of a CBT-based intervention in facilitat- where extensive support systems were available (e.g., sport and
ing emotional adjustment after sport injury. clinical psychologists, nutritionists, strength and conditioning ex-
Second, our findings provide further support for the value of perts), and athletes had regular contact with experienced athletic
multimodal interventions (Evans & Hardy, 2002; Evans et al., 2000; trainers who held them accountable for their rehabilitation efforts.
Johnson, 2000) for enhancing the well-being of injured athletes This suggestion is supported by Brewer (2010), who asserted that
during injury rehabilitation. An important advantage of multimodal there is a strong tendency for athletes to attend their scheduled
interventions is that they provide injured athletes a variety of rehabilitation appointments. In the current study, we suspect that
cognitive-behavioral strategies and the ability to use a particular functional capabilities after injury recovery did not differ as a result
method if other approaches in the intervention armory are not of group membership given the nonsignificant differences between
personally appealing (Brewer & Redmond, 2016). Ultimately, the groups in rehabilitation adherence. For example, in previous work,
intervention strategies employed should address the specific stres- rehabilitation adherence has been associated with enhanced func-
sors experienced by athletes with particular injury types (Petrie, tional ability after severe injury (Alzate Saez de Heredia et al., 2004;
Tomalski, & Clevinger, 2020). For instance, Petrie et al. (2020) Brewer et al., 2004; Pizzari et al., 2005).
suggest two broad categories of psychosocial intervention— Several limitations are evident in the current investigation.
instrumental and relational interventions—that serve potentially First, although our power analysis revealed that our sample size of
different functions for athletes with injuries. Instrumental interven- 16 participants was adequate for achieving 80% statistical power, it
tions refer to tangible strategies (e.g., goal setting, imagery, self-talk) is possible that the relatively small sample size could nonetheless
designed to facilitate physical recovery and help athletes take account for the lack of between-groups differences in adherence
ownership of their recovery. Relational interventions such as social and clinical function. Researchers are encouraged to obtain larger
support and formal counseling can be used to help improve psy- sample sizes in future intervention research.
chological recovery by helping athletes feel cared for and connected Second, as indicated, our control group performed physical
to significant others. While multimodal interventions may help rehabilitation only. To better control for the nonspecific effects of
address a multitude of stressors and serve a variety of functions, the experimental treatment (e.g., the fact that the experimental
one limitation of such intervention programs is that it is unclear group received social support in the form of interaction with a
which particular strategy or technique accounts for resulting effects. treatment provider), it would have been valuable to recruit an
For example, one of the strategies employed in the current investi- attention-control group. For instance, it is possible that the “act” of
gation that has previously shown promise is written emotional receiving support or time spent with the treatment provider—rather
disclosure (Mankad et al., 2009). While it seems likely that the than the actual CBT-based skills—may have accounted for some of
writing exercise conducted in this study had positive implications for the observed well-being effects. Recruitment of an attention-
athletes’ emotional status, the multimodal nature of our intervention control group (e.g., providing injured athletes with reading materi-
prevents firm conclusions on this account. als on recovery, videos, various forms of social support) is
Third, previous work has demonstrated the value of psycho- therefore advocated for future work in this area.
logical interventions for a variety of sport-injury outcomes such Third, the quasi-experimental design and heterogeneity of
as athlete mood states, pain, self-efficacy, reinjury anxiety, and injuries are also potential issues that could have influenced the
knee laxity (Cupal & Brewer, 2001; Evans et al., 2000; Johnson, findings. As indicated, given our interest in minimizing potential
2000; Maddison et al., 2012). However, researchers have yet to interactions and sharing of information between experimental-
demonstrate the value of multimodal intervention strategies with and treatment-group athletes, we intentionally assigned partici-
regard to the specific emotions examined in this study, namely, pants to the experimental group and on completion of their
increases in positive (pleasant) affect and vitality and reductions rehabilitation began enrolling athletes into the control group.
in negative affect. In regard to vitality, evidence suggests that it If, or when, circumstances permit (e.g., situations where con-
has protective effects in augmenting personal health and well- tamination effects are unlikely), researchers are encouraged to
being (Penninx et al., 2000; Strijk, Proper, Beek, & Mechelen, adopt true experimental designs. Along these lines, to whatever
2012) and may be particularly salient for athletes who have extent pragmatic considerations allow, it would be valuable to
sustained long-term injuries, whose motivation levels may work with participants with similar injury types and severities to
decrease over a prolonged rehabilitation (Levy, Polman, reduce the potential confounding influence of such variables on
Nicholls, & Marchant, 2009). Given the lengthy period of reha- outcomes of interest (e.g., well-being, adherence, clinical/func-
bilitation for participants in this study (M = 13.31 weeks), findings tional ability).
(Ahead of Print)
Cognitive-Behavioral Injury Intervention 9

A fourth potential limitation relates to the fact that different Conclusions


athletes were treated by different athletic trainers throughout their
rehabilitation, an issue that may have influenced the types of Our data provide support for use of a CBT-based intervention in
interactions between athletes and their treatment providers, thereby facilitating emotional well-being in injured athletes. Given the
influencing treatment perceptions and rehabilitation experiences/ acknowledged stressors of injury rehabilitation, as well as the link
outcomes. This issue, however, may be an inherent feature of between ongoing negative affect and maladaptive rehabilitation
research with NCAA Division I athletes, many of whom likely outcomes (Brewer et al., 2013; Tracey, 2003), improving positive
rehabilitate in environments where different athletic trainers may emotional adjustment is of clear clinical importance for injured
be assigned to different sports. athletes. Findings from the current study suggest that rehabilita-
Fifth, our use of a single- versus double-blind design may be a tion practitioners may increase athlete well-being through the use
limitation insofar as athletic trainers’ knowledge of the purposes of of easily implemented strategies such as injury-education heur-
the study and participant allocation to experimental/control groups istics, written emotional-disclosure exercises, scripted relaxation,
may have influenced the former’s treatment of, or interactions with, imagery, and cognitive reframing strategies. Sample materials are
athletes. available from the corresponding author on request. In addition,
Sixth, it is possible that previous exposure to mental-skills by increasing athletes’ positive affect and vitality over the course
training differed between experimental conditions at baseline, a of rehabilitation, practitioners may facilitate adherence, motiva-
possibility we did not assess in the current study, and one that might tion, and persistence in the face of rehabilitation challenges and
have influenced our findings. setbacks. Finally, from a practical standpoint, multimodal CBT
A seventh limitation pertains to the fact that it would have interventions such as those implemented in the current study may
been informative to report athletes’ self-assessment scores on their be valuable in providing athletes options and choices that increase
knowledge and ability of the CBT intervention skills. Although the adoption and use of psychological skills during sport-injury
scores were given to the athletes in the experimental group and rehabilitation. Despite important findings from the current study,
used for discussion purposes, we did not retain these scores for further randomized controlled trials are needed to examine the
reporting in the manuscript. value of psychological interventions in addressing important
A final limitation of our study relates to the fact that the rehabilitation outcomes.
intervention period of 4 weeks was relatively brief. Had experi-
mental-group athletes had more prolonged engagement with the
skills, they might have experienced greater benefit and hence Acknowledgments
change in behavioral (adherence) or clinical outcomes of interest
(pain, functional ability). Along these lines, it is possible that some We would like to acknowledge the Association for Applied Sport Psy-
athletes in the experimental group made more extensive use of the chology (AASP) for funding the current study. Special thanks also go to
CBT skills after completion of the individual 4-week intervention. the athletes participating in this investigation.
As such, it is hard to say whether differences in the amount of
engagement with the skills after completion of the intervention time
period affected athlete responses to outcomes of interest over the References
course of their respective rehabilitations (i.e., at T2 and T3 follow-
up assessments). Interventions of potentially longer durations Alzate Saez de Heredia, R., Ramirez, A., & Lazaro, I. (2004). The effect
might be warranted to further examine the impact of CBT-skills of psychological response on recovery of sport injury. Research
training on injured athletes’ well-being, adherence, and functional in Sports Medicine, 12(1), 15–31. doi:10.1080/15438620490
outcomes. 280567
The limitations of this study notwithstanding, findings from Brewer, B.W. (2010). The role of psychological factors in sport injury
the study can advance research and applied practice in several rehabilitation outcomes. International Review of Sport and Exercise
respects. While our adoption of a quasi-experimental design Psychology, 3(1), 40–61. doi:10.1080/17509840903301207
appeared more rigorous than some studies—but less rigorous Brewer, B.W., Cornelius, A.E., & Van Raalte, J.L. (2013). Predictors of
than true experimental studies—we nonetheless provided further adherence to home rehabilitation exercises following anterior cruciate
support for the value of a CBT-based psychological intervention ligament reconstruction. Rehabilitation Psychology, 58(1), 64–72.
relative to the well-being outcomes. These data suggest that PubMed ID: 23438001 doi: 10.1037/a0031297
changes in affect should be a priority for rehabilitation programs, Brewer, B.W., Cornelius, A.E., Van Raalte, J.L., Brickner, J.C., Tennen,
because other downstream behavioral outcomes likely depend on H., Sklar, J.H., : : : Pohlman, M.H. (2004). Comparison of concurrent
athletes’ maintaining adaptive emotional profiles needed to perse- and retrospective pain ratings during rehabilitation following anterior
vere through the stressors inherent in the rehabilitation process. On cruciate ligament reconstruction. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psy-
a practical level, the program that we used to implement the CBT chology, 26(4), 610–615. doi:10.1123/jsep.26.4.610
skills in the current study lends itself to instruction and implemen- Brewer, B.W., & Redmond, C. (2016). Psychology of sport injury.
tation with sport psychology and sports-medicine professionals, Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
who can, in turn, deliver skills to injured athletes in the context of a Clark, H., Bassett, S., & Siegert, R. (2018). Validation of a comprehensive
rehabilitation session. Finally, adoption of a multimodal interven- measure of clinic-based adherence for physiotherapy patients. Phys-
tion provides athletes options and choices that may increase their iotherapy, 104(1), 136–141. PubMed ID: 28778609 doi:10.1016/j.
receptivity and use of CBT skills during sport-injury rehabilitation. physio.2017.07.003
Future work should seek to examine how to create the right Clement, D., Arvinen-Barrow, M., & Fetty, T. (2015). Psychosocial
“medley” of psychological-skills training to develop adaptive responses during different phases of sport-injury rehabilitation: A
emotional profiles, enhance physical and psychological recovery, qualitative study. Journal of Athletic Training, 50(1), 95–104.
and improve return-to-play outcomes in elite-level athletes. PubMed ID: 25322346 doi:10.4085/1062-6050-49.3.52

(Ahead of Print)
10 Podlog et al.

Clement, D., & Shannon, V.R. (2011). Injured athletes’ perceptions about Mankad, A., & Gordon, S. (2010). Psycholinguistic changes in athletes’
social support. Journal of Sport Rehabilitation, 20(4), 457–470. grief response to injury after written emotional disclosure. Journal of
PubMed ID: 22012499 doi:10.1123/jsr.20.4.457 Sport Rehabilitation, 19(3), 328–342. doi:10.1123/jsr.19.3.328
Crawford, J.R., & Henry, J.D. (2004). The Positive and Negative Affect Mankad, A., Gordon, S., & Wallman, K. (2009). Psycho-immunological
Schedule (PANAS): Construct validity, measurement properties, and effects of written emotional disclosure during long-term injury
normative data in a large non-clinical sample. British Journal of rehabilitation. Journal of Clinical Sport Psychology, 3(3), 205–
Clinical Psychology, 43(3), 245–265. PubMed ID: 15333231 doi:10. 217. doi:10.1123/jcsp.3.3.205
1348/0144665031752934 McGinn, L.K., & Sanderson, W.C. (2001). What allows cognitive behav-
Cupal, D.D. (1998). Psychological interventions in sport injury prevention ioral therapy to be brief: Overview, efficacy, and crucial factors
and rehabilitation. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 10(1), 103– facilitating brief treatment. Clinical Psychology: Science and Prac-
123. doi:10.1080/10413209808406380 tice, 8(1), 23–37. doi:10.1093/clipsy.8.1.23
Cupal, D.D., & Brewer, B.W. (2001). Effects of relaxation and guided Melzack, R. (1987). The short-form McGill Pain Questionnaire. Pain,
imagery on knee strength, reinjury anxiety, and pain following 30(2), 191–197. PubMed ID: 3670870 doi:10.1016/0304-3959(87)
anterior cruciate ligament reconstruction. Rehabilitation Psychology, 91074-8
46(1), 28–43. doi:10.1037/0090-5550.46.1.28 Melzack, R., & Katz, J. (2001). The McGill Pain Questionnaire: Appraisal
Evans, L., & Hardy, L. (2002). Injury rehabilitation: A goal-setting inter- and current status. In D.C. Turk& R. Melzack (Eds.), Handbook of
vention study. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 73(3), 310– pain assessment (2nd ed., pp. 35–52). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
319. PubMed ID: 12230338 doi:10.1080/02701367.2002.10609025 Naoi, A., & Ostrow, A. (2008). The effects of cognitive and relaxation
Evans, L., Hardy, L., & Fleming, S. (2000). Intervention strategies with interventions on injured athletes’ mood and pain during rehabilita-
injured athletes: An action research study. The Sport Psychologist, tion. Athletic Insight: The Online Journal of Sport Psychology, 10(1).
14(2), 188–206. doi:10.1123/tsp.14.2.188 1–25.
Fairbank, J., Couper, J., Davies, J., & O’Brien, J.P. (1980). The Oswestry Penninx, B.W., Guralnik, J.M., Bandeen-Roche, K., Kasper, J.D.,
low back pain questionnaire. Physiotherapy, 18, 271–273. Retrieved Simonsick, E.M., Ferrucci, L., & Fried, L.P. (2000). The protective
from https://journals.lww.com/spinejournal/Fulltext/2000/11150/ effect of emotional vitality on adverse health outcomes in disabled
The_Oswestry_Disability_Index.17.aspx#pdf-link older women. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 48(11),
Fairbank, J.C., & Pynsent, P.B. (2000). The Oswestry Disability Index. 1359–1366. PubMed ID: 11083309 doi:10.1111/j.1532-5415.2000.
Spine, 25(22), 2940–2953. PubMed ID: 11074683 doi:10.1097/ tb02622.x
00007632-200011150-00017 Petrie, T.A., Tomalski, J., & Clevinger, K. (2020). Groups and the
Fitzmaurice, G. (2001). A conundrum in the analysis of change. Nutrition, rehabilitation of athletic injury: From psychoeducation to counseling.
17(4), 360–361. PubMed ID: 11369183 doi:10.1016/S0899- In A. Ivarsson& U. Johnson (Eds.), Psychological bases of sport
9007(00)00593-1 injuries (4th ed., pp. 187–204). Morgantown, WV: Fit Publishing.
Gouin, J.P., & Kiecolt-Glaser, J.K. (2011). The impact of psychological Pizzari, T., Taylor, N.F., McBurney, H., & Feller, J.A. (2005). Adherence
stress on wound healing: Methods and mechanisms. Immunology and to rehabilitation after anterior cruciate ligament reconstructive sur-
Allergy Clinics of North America, 31(1), 81–93. PubMed ID: gery: Implications for outcome. Journal of Sport Rehabilitation,
21094925 doi:10.1016/j.iac.2010.09.010 14(3), 202–214. doi:10.1123/jsr.14.3.202
Granquist, M.D., Gill, D.L., & Appaneal, R.N. (2010). Development of a Podlog, L., Burns, R., Dimmock, J., Jackson, B., Hall, M., & Fritz, J.
measure of rehabilitation adherence for athletic training. Journal of (2019). Does motivation mediate the relationship between compe-
Sport Rehabilitation, 19(3), 249–267. PubMed ID: 20811076 doi:10. tence perceptions and patient outcomes among individuals with
1123/jsr.19.3.249 chronic low back pain? A multiple mediation analysis. Disability and
Heil, J. (1993). Psychology of sport injury. Champaign, IL: Human Rehabilitation. Advance online publication. doi:10.1080/09638288.
Kinetics. 2019.1643421
Johnson, U. (2000). Short-term psychological intervention: A study of Podlog, L., & Dionigi, R. (2010). Coach strategies for addressing psycho-
long-term-injured competitive athletes. Journal of Sport Rehabilita- social challenges during the return to sport from injury. Journal of
tion, 9(3), 207–218. doi:10.1123/jsr.9.3.207 Sports Sciences, 28(11), 1197–1208. PubMed ID: 20700853 doi:10.
Leddy, M.H., Lambert, M.J., & Ogles, B.M. (1994). Psychological 1080/02640414.2010.487873
consequences of athletic injury among high-level competitors. Podlog, L., & Eklund, R.C. (2006). A longitudinal investigation of
Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 65(4), 347–354. PubMed competitive athletes’ return to sport following serious injury. Jour-
ID: 7886284 doi:10.1080/02701367.1994.10607639 nal of Applied Sport Psychology, 18(1), 44–68. doi:10.1080/
Levy, A.R., Polman, R.C.J., Nicholls, A.R., & Marchant, D.C. (2009). 10413200500471319
Sport injury rehabilitation adherence: Perspectives of recreational Podlog, L., Lochbaum, M., & Stevens, T. (2010). Need satisfaction, well-
athletes. International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, being and perceived return-to-sport outcomes among injured athletes.
7(2), 212–229. doi:10.1080/1612197X.2009.9671901 Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 22(2), 167–182. doi:10.1080/
Maddison, R., Prapavessis, H., Clatworthy, M., Hall, C., Foley, L., Harper, 10413201003664665
T., : : : Brewer, B. (2012). Guided imagery to improve functional Putukian, M. (2016). The psychological response to injury in student
outcomes post-anterior cruciate ligament repair: Randomized- athletes: A narrative review with a focus on mental health. British
controlled pilot trial. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine & Science Journal of Sports Medicine, 50(3), 145–148. PubMed ID: 26719498
in Sports, 22(6), 816–821. PubMed ID: 21564307 doi:10.1111/j. doi:10.1136/bjsports-2015-095586
1600-0838.2011.01325.x Rock, J., & Jones, M.A. (2002). Preliminary investigation into the use of
Mahoney, J., & Hanrahan, S. (2011). A brief educational intervention counseling skills in support of rehabilitation from sport injury. Journal
using acceptance and commitment therapy: Four injured athletes’ of Sport Rehabilitation, 11(4), 284–304. doi:10.1123/jsr.11.4.284
experiences. Journal of Clinical Sport Psychology, 5(3), 252–273. Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton,
doi:10.1123/jcsp.5.3.252 NJ: Princeton University Press.

(Ahead of Print)
Cognitive-Behavioral Injury Intervention 11

Ruddock-Hudson, M., O’Halloran, P., & Murphy, G. (2012). Exploring Trudeau, J., Turk, D., Dworkin, R., Benson, C., Biondi, D., Kim, M., : : :
psychological reactions to injury in the Australian Football League Katz, N. (2012). Validation of the Revised Short Form McGill Pain
(AFL). Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 24(4), 375–390. doi:10. Questionnaire (SF-MPQ-2) for self-report of pain qualities in patients
1080/10413200.2011.654172 with acute low back pain. Journal of Pain, 13(4), S4. doi:10.1016/j.
Ryan, R.M., & Frederick, C. (1997). On energy, personality, and health: jpain.2012.01.022
Subjective vitality as a dynamic reflection of well-being. Journal of Wadey, R., Podlog, L., Galli, N., & Mallellieu, S.D. (2016). Stress-related
Personality, 65(3), 529–565. PubMed ID: 9327588 doi:10.1111/j. growth following sport injury: Examining the applicability of the
1467-6494.1997.tb00326.x organismic valuing theory. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine &
Salim, J., & Wadey, R. (2019). Using gratitude to promote sport injury- Science in Sports, 26(10), 1132–1139. PubMed ID: 26589377
related growth. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology. Advance online doi:10.1111/sms.12579
publication. doi:10.1080/10413200.2019.1626515 Walker, N., & Arvinen-Barrow, M. (2013). Conclusions and future direc-
Schwab Reese, L., Pittsinger, R., & Yang, J. (2012). Effectiveness tions. In M. Arvinen-Barrow & N. Walker (Eds.), The psychology of
of psychological intervention following sport injury. Journal of sport injury and rehabilitation (pp. 199–202). Abingdon, United
Sport and Health Science, 1(2), 71–79. doi:10.1016/j.jshs.2012.06. Kingdom: Routledge.
003 Walker, N., Thatcher, J., & Lavallee, D. (2007). Psychological responses
Simpson, N., & Post, L. (2014). Injured athlete support group: Evaluation of to injury in competitive sport: A critical review. Journal of the Royal
a pilot program. 2014 NCAA Innovations Grant. http://www.ncaa.org/ Society for the Promotion of Health, 127(4), 174–180. PubMed ID:
sites/default/files/RES_NCAAfinalreport_Simpson_PostAug2015_ 17711063 doi:10.1177/1466424007079494
20160606.pdf Walker, N., Thatcher, J., & Lavallee, D. (2010). A preliminary development
Smith, A.S., Stuart, M.J., Wiese-Bjornstal, D.M., Milliner, E.K., O'Fallon, of the Re-Injury Anxiety Inventory (RIAI). Physical Therapy in Sport,
W.M., & Crowson, C.S. (1993). Competitive athletes: Preinjury and 11(1), 23–29. PubMed ID: 20129120 doi:10.1016/j.ptsp.2009.09.003
postinjury mood state and self-esteem. Mayo Clinic Proceedings, 68(10), Wasley, D., & Lox, C.L. (1998). Self-esteem and coping responses of
939–947. doi:10.1016/S0025-6196(12)62265-4 athletes with acute versus chronic injuries. Perceptual and Motor
Strijk, J.E., Proper, K.I., Beek, A.J., & Mechelen, W.V. (2012). A worksite Skills, 86(Suppl. 3), 1402–1402. PubMed ID: 9700819 doi:10.2466/
vitality intervention to improve older workers’ lifestyle and vitality- pms.1998.86.3c.1402
related outcomes: Results of a randomised controlled trial. Journal of Watson, D., Clark, L.A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and valida-
Epidemiology and Community Health, 66(11), 1071–1078. PubMed tion of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS
ID: 22268128 doi:10.1136/jech-2011-200626 scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063–
Theodorakis, Y., Beneca, A., Malliou, P., & Goudas, M. (1997). Examining 1070. PubMed ID: 3397865 doi:10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063
psychological factors during injury rehabilitation. Journal of Sport Wilson, G., & Pritchard, M. (2005). Comparing sources of stress in college
Rehabilitation, 6(4), 355–363. doi:10.1123/jsr.6.4.355 student athletes and non-athletes. Athletic Insight: The Online Journal
Theodorakis, Y., Malliou, P., Papaioannou, A., Beneca, A., & Filactakidou, of Sport Psychology, 7(1), 1–8.
A. (1996). The effect of personal goals, self-efficacy, and self- Zhang, A., Borheimer, L.A., Weaver, A, Franklin, C, Hai, A.H., Guz, S., &
satisfaction on injury rehabilitation. Journal of Sport Rehabilitation, Shen, L. (2019). Cognitive behavioral therapy for primary care
5(3), 214–223. doi:10.1123/jsr.5.3.214 depression and anxiety: A secondary meta-analytic review using
Tracey, J. (2003). The emotional response to the injury and rehabilitation robust variance estimation in meta-regression. Journal of Behavioral
process. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 15(4), 279–293. Medicine, 42(6), 1117–1141. PubMed ID: 31004323 doi:10.1007/
doi:10.1080/714044197 s10865-019-00046-z

(Ahead of Print)

View publication stats

You might also like