Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

Article

The Family Journal: Counseling and


Therapy for Couples and Families
Who Can Give Me Satisfaction? Partner 2015, Vol. 23(3) 247-253
ª The Author(s) 2015
Reprints and permission:
Matching in Fear of Intimacy sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/1066480715573709
and Relationship Satisfaction tfj.sagepub.com

Maria Pedro Sobral1, Cátia P. Teixeira2, and Maria Emı́lia Costa1

Abstract
This study aimed to better understand couples’ satisfaction. We hypothesized that similar levels on the two dimensions of fear of
intimacy (fear of losing the self [FLS] and fear of losing the other [FLO]) would buffer the negative association between fear of
intimacy and relationship satisfaction. Findings revealed a positive influence in relationship satisfaction from partner similarity in FLS.
No effects were found from partner similarity in FLO. Relationship satisfaction decreased when one partner’s higher fear of losing
the self was combined with lower fear of losing the other from the other partner. Repercussions for couple therapy are discussed.

Keywords
fear of intimacy, relationship satisfaction, partner matching, similarity

Research has shown that fear of intimacy has a negative influ- similarity hypothesis postulates that individuals search for
ence on relationship quality (Brunell, Pilkington, & Webster, partners who are similar in personality, behaviors, goals, or
2007; Descutner & Thelen, 1991; Thelen, Vander Wal, Thomas, attitudes. This hypothesis rises from the assumption that
& Harmon, 2000). We argue that this link may not be that individual preference for similar partners would have the
straightforward and that fear of intimacy is not always negative psychological advantage of providing self-enhancement,
for a relationship. Previous research examined the impact of fear since partners who are similar provide each other with
of intimacy on relationship quality of the individual without con- favorable feedback concerning representations of the self and
sidering the two members of the romantic dyad. We know, how- the world (Baumeister, 1982). Research provided support for
ever, that partner matching effects are crucial for the similarity as a positive influence in romantic relationships.
understanding of intimate relationship dynamics (Robins, Caspi, Couples with similar personalities, attachment styles, and
& Moffitt, 2000; Strauss, Morry, & Kito, 2012). For instance, values were found to experience more intimacy, greater
partner similarity on personality traits or values seems to increase satisfaction with life, and longer lasting relationships (Gaunt,
relationship satisfaction (Gaunt, 2006; Robins et al., 2000). 2006; Robins et al., 2000; Strauss et al., 2012).
This approach allows for a better understanding of the Nevertheless, literature on the link between partner similar-
(apparently) negative association between fear of intimacy and ity and relationship outcomes is not consensual. The opposite
relationship satisfaction at the individual level (by controlling the idea, that is, the complementary hypothesis, has also received
potential effect of the romantic partner). More importantly, it some empirical support (see Holmes & Johnson, 2009, for a
allows to analyze to what extent fear of intimacy of one member review). The complementary hypothesis postulates that
of the romantic dyad influences the impact of fear of intimacy on complementary preferences provide self-consistency, by
relationship satisfaction of the other one. We hypothesize that the allowing individuals to confirm expectations about themselves
association between fear of intimacy and relationship satisfaction and others (Swann & Read, 1981). As a matter of fact, research
is negative when partners are dissimilar in fear of intimacy. In suggests that similarity and complementarity may both be
contrast, similarity between partners will buffer this link.

1
Similarity as a Protective Factor for Center for Psychology, Faculty of Psychology and Education Sciences, Uni-
versity of Porto, Porto, Portugal
Romantic Relationships 2
Université catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Nueve, Belgium
Similarity in romantic relations has been studied mainly in an
Corresponding Author:
attempt to explain (a) why individuals choose their romantic Maria Pedro Sobral, Faculdade de Psicologia e Ciências da Educação,
partners instead of somebody else and (b) how two partners Universidade do Porto, Rua Alfredo Allen, 4200-135 Porto, Portugal.
maintain high levels of relationship satisfaction. The Email: mpsobral@fpce.up.pt
248 The Family Journal: Counseling and Therapy for Couples and Families 23(3)

associated with positive relationship outcomes when consider- components of fear of intimacy, that is, fear of losing the self
ing different variables, samples, or contexts. For instance, Mar- (FLS) and fear of losing the other (FLO). Both high-FLS and
key and Markey (2007) found that romantic couples who high-FLO individuals feel discomfort in intimacy, however,
reported the highest levels of relationship quality were more for different reasons. High-FLS individuals feel discomfort
dissimilar in terms of dominance but more similar in terms with allowing themselves to depend on the romantic partner,
of warmth than romantic couples who reported the lowest lev- whereas high-FLO individuals fear to expose the self so they
els of relationship quality. lose the other’s (their partner’s) approval.
Importantly for the present study, research on fear of inti- Following this conception of fear of intimacy, there is two
macy showed that individuals tend to date partners with similar ways of being (dis)similar. We will talk about within-
levels of fear of intimacy (Thelen et al., 2000). This result is in dimension or cross-dimension similarity. The first type of simi-
accordance with findings revealing that individuals tend to date larity refers to a more quantitative perspective, as it looks at
partners with similar beliefs and feelings about closeness, inti- effects of different levels of fear of intimacy between partners
macy, and dependence (Collins & Read, 1990). Nevertheless, it for the same measure (e.g., high FLS–high FLS or low FLS–
remains to verify whether this similarity preference is associ- high FLS). Cross-dimension similarity concerns not only quan-
ated with positive relationship outcomes, that is, whether titative but also qualitative differences in fear of intimacy (e.g.,
high-fear-of-intimacy individuals are happy in a relationship high FLS–high FLO or high FLS–low FLO).
with high-fear-of-intimacy partners.
Given that individuals with high levels of fear of intimacy
feel discomfort with sharing and depending on their partners
Distinguishing Relationship Satisfaction From
(Sobral & Costa, 2015), it is reasonable to assume that Other Relationship Quality Components
high-fear-of-intimacy individuals seek relationships with low Overall, research showed that fear of intimacy hinders intimate
levels of sharing and interdependence. Therefore, although relationships and individual well-being (Alperin, 2006; Hook,
individual fear of intimacy is associated with low relationship Gerstein, Detterich, & Gridley, 2003). Particularly, fear of inti-
quality and length (Brunell et al., 2007; Descutner & Thelen, macy was shown to be an obstacle to intimacy (Thelen et al.,
1991; Thelen et al., 2000), this should be the case only for 2000) and to relationship quality (Brunell et al., 2007). How-
individuals whose partners ‘‘demand’’ sharing and ever, relationship outcomes of fear of intimacy still need better
dependency. If one’s romantic partner has the same intimacy understanding. Although we assume the importance of other
reluctance as oneself, we can hypothesize that both will be relationship quality indicators such as commitment or trust,
reassured and, therefore, satisfied. In turn, if partners have we need to distinguish the association between fear of intimacy
different attitudes toward sharing and dependence (i.e., if and relationship quality from its association with relationship
partners mismatch in fear of intimacy), we can expect that satisfaction. Relationship satisfaction is considered a compo-
both will be threatened and therefore unsatisfied. We can thus nent of relationship quality and one can conceive other compo-
presume that partner similarity on fear of intimacy may nents as leading to or covarying with satisfaction (Fletcher,
prevent high-fear-of-intimacy individuals from relationship Simpson, & Thomas, 2000). Although relationship satisfaction
dissatisfaction. In turn, dissimilarity on fear of intimacy may highly correlates with other components of relationship quality
aggravate the negative link between fear of intimacy and (e.g., commitment, intimacy, trust), it is possible that people
relationship satisfaction. are satisfied with a relationship in which the other quality com-
ponents are low—provided that their desires and needs are sat-
isfied. This is particularly important when considering fear of
The Two Dimensions of Fear of Intimacy intimacy. That is, if high-fear-of-intimacy individuals feel dis-
Literature on fear of intimacy has traditionally focused on a comfort with intimacy (Descutner & Thelen, 1991; Sobral &
one-dimension conception. Indeed, since fear of intimacy was Costa, 2015), one may expect that these individuals can be sat-
proposed as ‘‘the inhibited capacity of an individual, because of isfied within relationships where the levels of other compo-
anxiety, to exchange thoughts and feelings of personal signifi- nents of relationship quality, such as commitment, are low.
cance with another individual who is highly valued’’ (Descutner For example, commitment may be a weakest predictor of rela-
& Thelen, 1991, p. 219), research has been mainly based on this tionship satisfaction for individuals with high levels of FLS
definition (e.g. Cash, Thériault, & Annis, 2004; Phillips et al., than for individuals with low levels of FLS, given that the for-
2013). However, an intimate relationship requires not only the mer are more concerned with not losing their individuality in
capacity to exchange thoughts and feelings but also the capac- the relationship. It may well be that people are ‘‘satisfied’’ in
ity to be autonomous and yet comfortable with dependence their relationship in spite of not scoring high in all variables
(e.g., Alperin, 2006; Arseth, Kroger, Martinussen, & Bakken, measuring relationship ‘‘quality.’’
2009; Beyers & Seiffge-Krenke, 2010). Following this line
of reasoning, Sobral and Costa (2015) proposed a two-
dimension conception of fear of intimacy that encompasses
Male and Female Experiences of Intimacy
both exchanging and dependence as components of intimacy. Previous research on gender differences in fear of intimacy has
The authors found empirical support for the existence of two presented inconsistent results. Research showed support for
Sobral et al. 249

both the absence of gender differences (e.g., Doi & Thelen, have reason to expect different results for our hypotheses
1993; Hook et al., 2003) and the presence of higher levels of across genders. Considering that relationship endurance is
fear of intimacy in men than in women (e.g., Sobral, Matos, affected by female and not male fear of intimacy (Thelen
& Costa, 2014; Thelen et al., 2000). Independent of the ques- et al., 2000), partner relationship satisfaction should also be
tion of differences in levels of intimacy, scholars argue that more influenced by female than by male fear of intimacy. In
males and females experience intimacy in different ways. Tra- other words, (4) we expect the predicted partner effects to be
ditionally, women strive more for intimacy than men do, and stronger for men (revealing the stronger influence of the female
men seek more for autonomy than women do (Hatfield, partner) than for women.
1984; Hook et al., 2003). Women also desire more intimacy
than men do, and women’s fear of intimacy is more determi-
nant of relationship endurance (Thelen et al., 2000). Thelen and Method
colleagues (2000) argue that these gender differences may
explain why women are more likely than men to terminate
Participants and Procedure
romantic relationships: When the female partner fears Data were collected in a website devised for this research in
intimacy, she will not follow her ‘‘traditional’’ role of striving which couples data were programmed to be paired. To couple
for intimacy, which will lead to dissatisfaction with the rela- the responses, each participant provided the e-mail of her or his
tionship and ultimately to the end of it. Given the importance partner. E-mails were automatically encrypted into a code,
of gender for intimacy experience, we analyzed gender mod- allowing the anonymous coupling of responses. To avoid bias
eration effects on the link between partner similarity in fear associated with remarriage, participants who had ever got
of intimacy and relationship satisfaction. divorced and their respective partners were excluded (n ¼ 12
dyads). Our final sample had 276 heterosexual couples, with
ages ranging from 18 to 55 (M ¼ 32.79, SD ¼ 8.91). Average
The Present Study relationship length was 10.74 years (SD ¼ 8.95), 49.4% were
In this study, we explore how matching in fear of intimacy in married, 41.3% were dating, and 10.9% were cohabiting.
couples can influence relationship satisfaction. We do so con-
sidering three important research gaps. First, we distinguish the Measures
two dimensions of fear of intimacy (FLS and FLO; Sobral &
Costa, 2015). Second, we examine the impact of fear of inti- Fear of Intimacy Components questionnaire. The Fear of Intimacy
macy specifically on relationship satisfaction rather than on a Components Questionnaire (Sobral & Costa, 2015) is an
higher order assessment of relationship quality indicators. attachment-based questionnaire that assesses two dimensions
Finally, we collect information about fear of intimacy and rela- of fear of intimacy within the current romantic relationship,
tionship satisfaction from both the individual and his or her namely, FLS and FLO. Each dimension is measured by 5 items.
partner. These methodological options will allow us to have a FLS addresses feelings of discomfort regarding dependence
clearer picture of how fear of intimacy affects relationship toward the romantic partner (e.g., ‘‘I don’t like to justify myself
satisfaction in an actual romantic relationship. to my partner’’). FLO regards fear of exposure, self-disclosure,
People differ in needs and goals within relationships, and and rejection (e.g., ‘‘I try to hide my weaknesses from my part-
partners differ in the ability to satisfy them (Holmes & Johnson, ner’’). Responses were rated on a 5-point Likert-type scale.
2009). The (mis)matching in fear of intimacy can therefore add Confirmatory factor analyses revealed adequate goodness-of-
information to the previously supported negative link between fit for male (comparative fit index [CFI] ¼ .946; root mean
fear of intimacy and relationship satisfaction. Given that part- square error of approximation [RMSEA] ¼ .070; w2/df ¼
ner similarity in attitudes toward intimacy can increase rela- 2.277) and female (CFI ¼ .988; RMSEA ¼ .032; w2/df ¼
tionship satisfaction (Collins & Read, 1990), we assume that 1.255) samples. Both subscales were found to have good com-
the opposite (i.e., partner dissimilarity in attitudes towards inti- posite reliability (CR) for both men (CR ¼ .82 and CR ¼ .87
macy) can hinder relationship satisfaction. Therefore, we for FLS and FLO, respectively) and women (CR ¼ .82 and
expect the link between fear of intimacy and relationship satis- CR ¼ .87 for FLS and FLO, respectively).
faction to be negative only when partners are dissimilar in fear
of intimacy. Specifically, we predict that (1) relationship satis- Satisfaction component of the Perceived Relationship Quality
faction will decrease when there is a mismatching on dimen- Components Inventory. Relationship satisfaction was assessed
sions of fear of intimacy (i.e., when higher levels of FLS are via the satisfaction subscale of the Perceived Relationship
combined with higher levels of FLO), (2) partners’ higher lev- Quality Components Inventory (PRQC; Fletcher et al., 2000).
els of FLS will buffer the negative link between actor-FLS and The PRQC has 18 items equally distributed by six subscales.
relationship satisfaction, and (3) partners’ higher levels of FLO These subscales (Satisfaction, Commitment, Intimacy, Trust,
will buffer the negative link between actor-FLO and relation- Passion, and Love) can form a global index of relationship
ship satisfaction as well. quality, with higher scores indicating greater perceived rela-
Given that men and women experience intimacy differently tionship quality. However, each subscale assesses a specific
(Hook et al., 2003; Sobral et al., 2014; Thelen et al., 2000), we and relatively independent component of perceived
250 The Family Journal: Counseling and Therapy for Couples and Families 23(3)

Table 1. Means, Standard Deviations, and Correlations Among All Study Variables for Male and Female Partners.

Male Female

M (SD) 2 3 1 2 3

Male 1 ¼ FLS 2.24 (0.68) .572** .350** .228** .241** .278**


2 ¼ FLO 2.31 (0.78) — .400** .226** .294** .368**
3 ¼ Relationship satisfaction 6.26 (0.90) — — .236** .237** .661**
Female 1 ¼ FLS 2.08 (0.69) — — — .501** .358**
2 ¼ FLO 2.10 (0.74) — — — — .395**
3 ¼ Relationship satisfaction 6.32 (0.88) — — — — —

Note. FLS ¼ fear of losing the self; FLO ¼ fear of losing the other.
**p < .01. *p < .05.

Table 2. Male and Female Models for Interactive Effects, Individual Fear of Intimacy, and Partner Fear of Intimacy on Relationship Satisfaction.

Male Relationship Satisfaction Female Relationship Satisfaction

Predictor B SE B b B SE B b

M-Satisfaction — — — .529 .052 .540***


F-Satisfaction .581 .057 .569*** — — —
M-FLS .201 .076 .153*** .062 .074 .048
M-FLO .131 .068 .115*** .086 .065 .077
F-FLS .006 .072 .005 .151 .068 .120*
F-FLO .070 .068 .057 .214 .063 .180***
M-FLS  F-FLS .358 .123 .228** .137 .119 .089
M-FLO  F-FLO .098 .119 .063 .078 .113 .051
M-FLS  F-FLO .151 .130 .1000 .155 .124 .106
M-FLO  F-FLS .345 .106 .220*** .014 .104 .009
M-FLS  M-FLO .022 .078 .016 .040 .075 .029
F_FLS  F_FLO .029 .082 .019 .056 .078 .038
Note. M ¼ Male; F ¼ Female; FLS ¼ fear of losing the self; FLO ¼ fear of losing the other.
*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

relationship quality (Fletcher et al., 2000). Since our purpose (mean centered) and all the respective two-way interactions
was to analyze partners’ subjective satisfaction, we selected the were entered as predictors in the regression. Because actor and
satisfaction component. Responses to each item were made on partner relationship satisfaction are correlated (see Table 1),
a 7-point Likert-type scale (e.g., How content are you with your these variables were included as covariates in the regression
relationship?’’). CR was good for male partner (.97) and female models. Findings were different for male and female relation-
partner (.98) subsamples. ship satisfaction (see Table 2), F(11,235) ¼ 21.972, p < .001,
and F(11,235) ¼ 24.241, p < .001, respectively. For males,
results revealed moderating effects of partner-FLS on the link
Results between actor-FLO and relationship satisfaction and on the link
Men showed higher levels of FLS and FLO than women, t(246) between actor-FLS and relationship satisfaction. No modera-
¼ 2.883, p < .005; t(246) ¼ 3.715, p < .001, respectively. No tion effects from partner-FLO were found. For females, no sig-
gender differences were found for relationship satisfaction, nificant interaction effects emerged (see Table 2).
t(246) ¼ 1.161, p > .05. In line with previous research, associa- Simple slope analyses designed to decompose the statisti-
tions between individual fear of intimacy (FLS and FLO) and cally significant interactions (Aiken & West, 1991) revealed
relationship satisfaction were negative and significant, for both that for males our first and second hypotheses were supported.
genders (see Table 1). The first hypothesis was supported by the significant interac-
To test for moderating effects from partners’ fear of inti- tion of actor-FLS and partner-FLO on relationship satisfaction
macy on relationship satisfaction, we performed two interac- (Figure 1). Relationship satisfaction decreased significantly
tion models, namely, one model for the male partner when higher levels of FLS of one partner were combined with
relationship satisfaction in which female partner’s fear of inti- higher levels of FLO of the other partner. In turn, the links
macy was the moderator and one model for the female partner between FLS or FLO and relationship satisfaction were non-
relationship satisfaction in which male partner’s fear of inti- significant when lower levels of FLS were combined with
macy was the moderator. Actors’ and partners’ FLS and FLO lower levels of FLO.
Sobral et al. 251

Low partner-FLS High partner-FLS Low partner-FLS High partner-FLS


6 6

5 5
Relationship satisfaction

Relationship satisfaction
4 4

β = .036 ns β = .094 ns
3 3

2 2
β = -.342*** β = .323***

1 1
Low actor-FLO High actor-FLO Low actor-FLS High actor-FLS

Figure 1. Interactive effects between partners’ fear of intimacy on male relationship satisfaction. ***p < .001.

The second hypothesis was also supported. The association buffered by partner similarity and aggravated by partner dis-
between actor-FLS and relationship satisfaction was signifi- similarity in the dimensions of fear of intimacy (i.e., when
cantly negative when partner-FLS was lower, whereas the asso- partner-FLS was lower, the link between actor-FLO and rela-
ciation between actor-FLS and actor-relationship satisfaction tionship satisfaction was non-significant, but when partner-
was nonsignificant when the partner presented higher levels FLS was higher, the link between actor-FLO and relationship
of FLS (see Figure 1). In contrast, our third hypothesis did not satisfaction was significantly negative); (2) the negative link
receive empirical support. Indeed, matching in FLO did not between FLS and relationship satisfaction was buffered by
affect satisfaction as evident by the absence of interactions partner similarity and aggravated by partner dissimilarity in
between actor-FLO and partner-FLO (see Table 2). levels of FLS (i.e., when partner-FLS was higher, the link
between actor-FLS and relationship satisfaction was nonsigni-
ficant, but when partner-FLS was lower, the link between
Discussion actor-FLS and relationship satisfaction was significantly nega-
This study aimed to verify the effect of fear of intimacy match- tive). In summary, relationship satisfaction of higher-FLO
ing between partners on relationship satisfaction. Building on a males was aggravated when the couple mismatched in dimen-
two-dimension conception of fear of intimacy (FLS and FLO), sions of fear of intimacy (i.e., when their female partners had
we analyzed the interactive effects between partners’ levels of higher levels of FLS). Relationship satisfaction of higher-
fear of intimacy on relationship satisfaction. Our major ques- FLS males was aggravated when the couple mismatched in lev-
tion was whether similarity in fear of intimacy (either in its lev- els of FLS (i.e., males who were high in FLS were with low
els or in its components) would moderate the negative link FLS female partners).
between individual fear of intimacy and relationship satisfac- The negative influence that dissimilarity presented on rela-
tion. This question emerged from two different arguments tionship satisfaction supported our first hypothesis for males.
found in previous research. On one hand, individual fear of inti- Since high-FLS individuals fear dependence and high-FLO
macy has a negative influence on the quality of the relationship individuals fear disapproval (Sobral & Costa, 2015), a high-
(Brunell et al., 2007; Thelen et al., 2000). On the other hand, FLS individual will likely become uncomfortable with a
partner matching on personality or values may change it high-FLO partner who searches for proximity and approval.
(Gaunt, 2006; Robins et al., 2000). The fact that similarity on In turn, high-FLO individuals will likely feel anxious with
either positive or negative dispositions toward intimacy high-FLS partners whose distant behavior probably confirms
enhances relationship quality (Collins & Read, 1990) is partic- their fear of rejection.
ularly interesting when focusing on the effects of couples’ fear The fact that male relationship dissatisfaction was aggravated
of intimacy. We argued therefore that the negative effects of by partner dissimilarity and buffered by partner similarity in lev-
fear of intimacy on relationship satisfaction could be attenuated els of FLS supported our second hypothesis for men. This find-
by the levels of fear of intimacy of the romantic partner. ing can be understood in light of the characteristics related to this
Going beyond the traditional approach that considers the dimension of fear of intimacy. On one hand, high-FLS individ-
individual as the primary unit of analysis, we analyzed fear uals experience discomfort with allowing themselves to depend
of intimacy of both partners. For males, we verified that (1) the on the romantic partner (Sobral & Costa, 2015). On the other
negative link between FLO and relationship satisfaction was hand, fear of intimacy is associated with desired intimacy and
252 The Family Journal: Counseling and Therapy for Couples and Families 23(3)

with actual intimacy (Thelen et al., 2000), therefore high-FLS et al., 2009; Hook et al., 2003), the present study shows that,
individuals are expected to both desire and experience lower among males, high levels of FLS coupled with similarly high
levels of intimacy. If both feel discomfort with dependence, both levels of FLS of the partner seem to be some way to prevent
will demand lower intimacy, so the individual will be more sat- them from being dissatisfied in their relationships. Although
isfied with the characteristics of their relationship. it is less likely that a satisfied couple seeks clinical help, we
Contrary to predictions, FLO similarity did not seem to may wonder whether, in clinical interventions with high FLS
affect relationship satisfaction neither for males nor for couples, it is or not wise to develop intimacy-based interven-
females. Indeed, individual FLO had a negative influence on tions since such interventions might endanger couples’ home-
relationship satisfaction, regardless of the partner’s level of ostasis and satisfaction. Clinical studies with high-FLS
FLO. Although high-FLS partners can meet each other’s pre- couples should be conducted in order to verify whether or not
ferences for space and distance and buffer dissatisfaction with to intervene in their fear of intimacy is a good strategy. What-
such relationship functioning, high-FLO partners seem not to ever the case may be, our findings suggest that such interven-
be able to do it. Apparently, and contrary to FLS, the link tions must take great care not to unbalance the members of
between individual FLO and individual relationship satisfac- the couple, by for example, decreasing only the female part-
tion does not seem to be shaped by partner’s fear of intimacy. ner’s FLS. Indeed at least for male satisfaction two high-FLS
Perhaps more independent partners are able to enhance comfort partners seem to be more satisfied than one high-FLS partner
in high-FLS individuals (who fear dependence), but closer part- combined with one low-FLS one.
ners are not able to minimize fears of rejection in high-FLO Although our goal was to examine the effects of partner
individuals. matching—and not to analyze the ability of partners to influ-
The presence of significant effects for males and not for ence one’s fear of intimacy schemas—the potential change in
females is in line with previous research which found that men these schemas may be important to understand the fear-of-
are more affected than women by partner’s intimacy-related intimacy mechanisms. Scholars have already speculated that
variables. Webster, Brunell, and Pilkington (2009) showed that a securely attached romantic partner may buffer his or her part-
female warmth affected their male partners’ experience of dis- ner’s fear of intimacy, that is, individual insecure patterns of
closure reciprocity but not the opposite. Brunell, Pilkington, feelings and behaviors within the relationship may be
and Webster (2007) found that actor perceptions of risk in inti- decreased when the romantic partner is secure and strongly
macy affected partner relationship quality in men, but female committed to her or his partner (Tran & Simpson, 2012). The
relationship quality was not influenced by male perceptions fact that people are likely to have partners with similar beliefs
of risk in intimacy. These findings are thus congruent with the and feelings about intimacy (Collins & Read, 1990; Emmons &
idea that when women fear intimacy they are unable to fulfill Colby, 1995) led us to wonder why this happens. Is this simi-
their traditional role of striving for intimacy which affects both larity there from the beginning, that is, do individuals choose
their and their partners’ relationship satisfaction. their partners based on convergence concerning these disposi-
Because relationship intimacy and relationship satisfaction tions? Or, do couples influence each other, creating with time a
are associated (Fletcher et al., 2000), one could expect that the rapprochement on patterns of fear of intimacy? The present
pattern of associations between fear of intimacy and intimacy cross-sectional study cannot provide such data. Longitudinal
was similar to the pattern of associations between fear of inti- studies will be valuable to analyze whether couples’ interactive
macy and relationship satisfaction. However, previous research effects of fear of intimacy on relationship satisfaction may also
has shown no effects of individual fear of intimacy on partners’ be the product of a long-term adaptation of the two members of
perceptions of relationship intimacy (Thelen et al., 2000). Nev- the romantic dyad to each other. Moreover, further research
ertheless, our findings for males may not necessarily diverge shall verify how these findings may be extended to gay and les-
from these ones. First, Thelen and colleagues did not distin- bian couples, since our sample was limited to participants in a
guish the two components of fear of intimacy (Sobral & Costa, heterosexual relationship.
2015). As a matter of fact, the importance of this distinction Another question that our research triggers is whether or not
was empirically demonstrated in our study by the differential the levels of relationship satisfaction of these high-fear-of-
impact of the two dimensions of fear of intimacy on relation- intimacy individuals can persist in time. We may wonder
ship satisfaction. Second, we find that the present results and whether high-fear-of-intimacy couples, even if satisfied, are
Thelen’s results are somehow complementary. Indeed, even as able to deal with eventual relationship crises as low-fear-
if partners’ fear of intimacy has no role in relationship intimacy of-intimacy ones and which is the role of fear–of-intimacy
(Thelen et al., 2000), it may have a role in relationship satisfac- similarity in these processes.
tion (present study). If avoiding intimacy serves the purpose In a nutshell, our findings demonstrate that in certain cases
of maintaining a comfortably safe interpersonal distance men with higher levels of fear of intimacy can after all be sat-
(Bartholomew, 1990), finding a partner with such an avoidant isfied within the romantic relationship. The answer to who can
disposition toward intimacy may give high-fear-of-intimacy give them satisfaction seems to reside on a combination
individuals what they are looking for in a relationship. between the individual’s and the partner’s fear of intimacy:
These findings give rise to some issues for couple therapy. female partners who have high FLS for men with high FLS,
Even if the benefits of intimacy are widely shown (e.g, Arseth or low FLS levels for men with high FLO. Contrarily,
Sobral et al. 253

relationship satisfaction of high-FLS men may be harmed by observer reports of social support. Journal of Personality and
dissimilar low-FLS female partners, whereas relationship satis- Social Psychology, 68, 947–959.
faction of higher FLO men may be harmed by higher FLS ones. Fletcher, G., Simpson, J. A., & Thomas, G. (2000). The measurement
At a more general level, our findings put forward the need to of perceived relationship quality components: A confirmatory fac-
consider both members of the dyad when attempting to under- tor analytic approach. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
stand the link between individual dispositions and relationship 26, 340–354.
outcomes. These findings should make us relativize the dysfunc- Gaunt, R. (2006). Couple similarity and marital satisfaction: Are sim-
tional label that we might readily put on certain dispositions by ilar spouses happier? Journal of Personality, 74, 1401–1420.
considering the context in which they might manifest Hatfield, E. (1984). The dangers of intimacy. In V. Derlaga (Ed.),
themselves. Communication, intimacy and close relationships (pp. 207–220).
New York, NY: Praeger.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests Holmes, B. M., & Johnson, K. R. (2009). Adult attachment and
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to romantic partner preference: A review. Journal of Social and Per-
the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. sonal Relationships, 26, 833–852.
Hook, M. K., Gerstein, L. H., Detterich, L., & Gridley, B. (2003). How
Funding close are we? Measuring intimacy and examining gender differ-
The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for ences. Journal of Counseling & Development, 81, 462–472.
the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This study Markey, P. M., & Markey, C. N. (2007). Romantic ideals, romantic
was supported by a grant from the Portuguese Foundation for Science obtainment, and relationship experiences: The complementarity
and Technology, SFRH/BD/60287/2009. of interpersonal traits among romantic partners. Journal of Social
and Personal Relationships, 24, 517–533.
References Phillips, T. M., Wilmoth, J. D., Wall, S. K., Peterson, D. J., Buckley,
Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and R., & Phillips, L. E. (2013). Recollected parental care and fear of
interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. intimacy in emerging adults. The Family Journal: Counseling and
Alperin, R. M. (2006). Impediments to intimacy. Clinical Social Work Therapy for Couples and Families, 21, 335–341.
Journal, 34, 559–572. Robins, R. W., Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (2000). Two personalities,
Arseth, A. K., Kroger, J., Martinussen, M., & Bakken, G. (2009). Inti- one relationship: Both partners’ personality traits shape the quality
macy status, attachment, separation-individuation patterns, and of their relationship. Personality Processes and Individual Differ-
identity status in female university students. Journal of Social and ences, 79, 251–259.
Personal Relationships, 26, 697–712. Sobral, M. P., & Costa, M. E. (2015). Development of Fear of Inti-
Bartholomew, K. (1990). Avoidance of intimacy: An attachment per- macy Components Questionnaire (FICQ): Embracing a new
spective. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 7, dependence component. European Journal of Psychological
147–178. Assessment. DOI: 10.1027/1015-5759/a000238
Baumeister, R. F. (1982). A self-presentational view of social phe- Sobral, M. P., Matos, P. M., & Costa, M. E. (2014, March). The role of
nomena. Psychological Bulletin, 91, 3–26. attachment to parental and romantic figures on couples’ fear of
Beyers, W., & Seiffge-Krenke, I. (2010). Does identity precede inti- intimacy. Communication presented at the IFTA 2014 World Fam-
macy? Testing Erikson’s theory on romantic development in emer- ily Therapy Congress. Panama City, Panama.
ging adults of the 21st century. Journal of Adolescent Research, Strauss, C., Morry, M. M., & Kito, M. (2012). Attachment styles and
25, 387–415. relationship quality: Actual, perceived, and ideal partner matching.
Brunell, A. B., Pilkington, C. J., & Webster, G. D. (2007). Perceptions Personal Relationships, 19, 14–36.
of risk in intimacy in dating couples: Conversation and relationship Swann, W. B. Jr., & Read, S. J. (1981). Self-verification processes:
quality. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 26, 92–119. How we sustain our self-conceptions. Journal of Experimental
Cash, T. F., Thériault, J., & Annis, N. M. (2004). Body image in an Social Psychology, 17, 351–372.
interpersonal context: Adult attachment, fear of intimacy, and social Thelen, M. H., Vander Wal, J. S., Thomas, A. M., & Harmon, R.
anxiety. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 23, 89–103. (2000). Fear of intimacy among dating couples. Behavior Modifi-
Collins, N. L., & Read, S. J. (1990). Adult attachment, working mod- cation, 24, 233–240.
els, and relationship quality in dating couples. Journal of Consult- Tran, S., & Simpson, J. A. (2012). Attachment, commitment, and rela-
ing Psychology, 24, 349–354. tionship maintenance: When partners really matter. In L. Camp-
Descutner, C. S., & Thelen, M. H. (1991). Development and validation bell, J. G. La Guardia, J. M. Olson & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), The
of a fear-of-intimacy scale. Journal of Consulting and Clinical science of the couple: The Ontario symposium, ( Vol. 12, pp.
Psychology, 3, 218–225. 95–117). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
Doi, S. C., & Thelen, M. H. (1993). The fear of intimacy scale: Repli- Webster, G., Brunell, A., & Pilkington, C. (2009). Individual differ-
cation and extension. Psychological Assessment, 5, 377–383. ences in men’s and women’s warmth and disclosure differentially
Emmons, R. A., & Colby, P. M. (1995). Emotional conflict and well- moderate couples’ reciprocity in conversational disclosure. Per-
being: Relation to perceived availability, daily utilization, and sonality and Individual Differences, 46, 292–297.
Copyright of Family Journal is the property of Sage Publications Inc. and its content may not
be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's
express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for
individual use.

You might also like