Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 6

Marine Pollution Bulletin 88 (2014) 249–254

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Marine Pollution Bulletin


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/marpolbul

The effect of fine-scale sampling frequency on estimates of beach litter


accumulation
Peter G. Ryan a,⇑, Annerie Lamprecht b, Debbie Swanepoel a, Coleen L. Moloney b
a
Percy FitzPatrick Institute of African Ornithology, DST/NRF Centre of Excellence, University of Cape Town, Rondebosch 7701, South Africa
b
Marine Research Institute and Department of Biological Sciences, University of Cape Town, Rondebosch 7701, South Africa

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: The effect of sampling frequency on estimates of the rate of litter accumulation was determined for two
Available online 16 September 2014 South African sandy beaches. After initial cleaning, all manufactured items >10 mm diameter were col-
lected in alternating bouts of daily or weekly cleanups. Daily sampling collected 2.5 (range 2.1–3.4) times
Keywords: more litter items than weekly samples and 1.7 (1.3–2.3) times more litter by mass. Low density items
Stranded debris such as foamed polystyrene showed a greater differential (4–5 times more items from daily sampling),
Plastics presumably due to faster turnover of lightweight litter items. Variation in weekly samples was not con-
Sample rate
sistently less than daily estimates, suggesting that less frequent samples only partly integrate short-term
Accumulation
Meta-analyses
fluctuations in litter dynamics. Researchers using beach accumulation data to infer trends in nearshore
South Africa marine litter, or to assess the efficacy of litter mitigation measures, need to ensure consistency in sam-
pling frequency.
Ó 2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction such as the number of beach visitors and urban run-off); and for-
mal or informal cleanup efforts (Merrell, 1980; Bowman et al.,
Plastic and other drifting artefacts are major marine pollutants, 1998; Williams and Tudor, 2001). Also, increases in the standing
and considerable efforts have been made to prevent the release of stock of beach litter may reflect long-term accumulation rather
plastic articles into the environment (e.g. Coe and Rogers, 1997). than a change in the amount of debris at sea (Ryan et al., 2009).
Assessing the efficacy of measures to reduce marine debris at sea As a result, estimates of litter accumulation rates made by repeat-
is complicated by its great spatial heterogeneity and the high cost edly cleaning the same stretch of beach currently are best practice
of at-sea surveys (Ryan et al., 2009). As a result, much of what we for using beach surveys to assess marine litter trends (e.g. Sheavly,
know about marine litter is inferred from surveys of litter strand- 2007; OSPAR Commission, 2007; Ryan et al., 2009; Ribic et al.,
ing on beaches (e.g. Derraik, 2002; Barnes et al., 2009), and major 2010, 2012).
regional initiatives to assess trends in marine debris rely heavily on One problem with litter accumulation estimates is that they
beach monitoring (Sheavly, 2007; OSPAR Commission, 2007). might be sensitive to the frequency with which litter is sampled
Initial studies of beach litter reported standing stocks of accu- because of differences in litter turn-over rates (Ryan et al., 2009).
mulated litter, which can show gross changes in the abundance Dixon and Cooke (1977) showed that the weekly retention rate
and distribution of litter (e.g. Ryan and Moloney, 1990), but are of marked bottles on beaches in Kent, UK, varied among beaches
not necessarily a good indicator of the abundance of litter in adja- and was influenced by the type of bottle, because plastic bottles
cent coastal waters (Escardó-Boomsma et al., 1995; Ryan et al., remained ashore longer than glass bottles. Subsequent studies
2009). Other factors that affect the amount of litter on beaches have confirmed different retention rates depending on beach struc-
include the physical environment (beach structure, wave action ture and the type of litter (Merrell, 1980; Garrity and Levings,
and local currents, which influence both stranding and removal 1993; Bowman et al., 1998; Kataoka et al., 2013). Escardó-
rates); beach dynamics (burial or exposure of litter); proximity to Boomsma et al. (1995) differentiated the amount of litter arriving
urban centres (which influences exposure to land-based sources on a beach (the loading rate) from the amount of litter that accu-
mulates per unit of time (net accumulation rate). However, this
distinction has been largely ignored by subsequent studies, which
⇑ Corresponding author. Tel.: +27 21 6502966; fax: +27 21 6503295. have reported accumulation rates at sampling frequencies ranging
E-mail address: pryan31@gmail.com (P.G. Ryan).
from daily (Eriksson et al., 2013), every two weeks (Williams and

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.marpolbul.2014.08.036
0025-326X/Ó 2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
250 P.G. Ryan et al. / Marine Pollution Bulletin 88 (2014) 249–254

Tudor, 2001), monthly (Corbin and Singh, 1993; Thornton and increase in the density of litter prevented both beaches being sam-
Jackson, 1998; Sheavly, 2007), quarterly (OSPAR Commission, pled in full on the same day. Prior to each experiment, a team of
2007), every six months (Slip and Burton, 1991) to annually volunteers removed all accumulated macro litter (articles
(Merrell, 1984; Edyvane et al., 2004). Some studies have sampled >10 mm diameter) between the water line and the dune vegetation
at different frequencies in different seasons (e.g. Walker et al., (including any litter visible in the vegetation) from each study area,
1997; Ivar do Sul et al., 2011). and from adjacent buffer zones approximately 25 m wide at each
Sheavly (2007) reported no difference in accumulation rate for end of the study sections. The following day (24 h, or two tidal
samples collected at different intervals, but Ribic et al. (2010, cycles later), sampling of ‘new’ litter commenced at both sites.
2012) found that frequent sampling (614 days) and very infre- Teams of three to six observers worked systematically along each
quent sampling (>180 days) affected debris counts relative to their beach, collecting all litter items in each of the ten subsections. Each
28-day standard sampling interval. Ribic et al. (2010, 2012) failed subsection was searched until no items had been found for several
to report how these changes in sampling frequency affected litter minutes, with searches of each section taking roughly 5–15 min,
counts, but the inference was that too frequent sampling reduced depending on the amount of litter present.
litter counts, and very infrequent samples increased litter counts. In 1994, daily sampling continued for 15 days (18 October–1
However, Ryan et al. (2009) included results from an unpublished November), and was followed by one sample after 4 days, one after
MSc dissertation (Swanepoel, 1995) to suggest that summing daily 6 days and four weekly samples, and then another 14 daily sam-
accumulation samples to obtain weekly data yielded up to six- ples (9–23 December). In 1995, the initial cleaning took place on
times more litter items than weekly sampling at the same beaches. 26 June, with a weekly sample on 3 July, 14 daily samples to 17
More recently, Eriksson et al. (2013) showed that daily sampling at July, and then a final weekly sample on 24 July. In 2012, daily sam-
sub-Antarctic Macquarie Island increased litter accumulation rates pling continued for 10 days (2–11 October), followed by three
almost ten-fold compared to monthly sampling, although surveys weekly samples (18 and 25 October and 1 November), 10-days of
took place in different years. These findings suggest that sampling daily sampling (2–11 November), four weekly samples (18 and
frequency, at least at fine temporal scales, has a major impact on 25 November and 2 and 9 December), and then a final 10 daily
estimates of litter accumulation rates. samples (10–19 December). There was no consistent difference
In this paper we report the findings from Swanepoel’s (1995) in tidal phase (neap or spring) between daily and weekly sample
work in more detail, and supplement them with additional sam- periods.
pling. We compare the amount (number and mass) of litter col- All litter samples were returned to the University of Cape Town to
lected daily with weekly accumulations collected at the same be sorted and counted. Each debris item was identified as far as pos-
sites to evaluate the extent to which more frequent sampling sible and categorised by type of material (plastic, metal, glass, ciga-
increases estimates of the amount of litter arriving on beaches. rette butts, etc.). Wood items were included if they were ‘worked’
We also assess whether less frequent sampling reduces the vari- rather than natural wood. Plastic items were further subdivided by
ability in beach litter accumulation data by integrating fine-scale function: packaging and other single-use items, user items including
variations (given that processes such as weather, wave action fishing gear, and plastic fragments of uncertain provenance. Foamed
and tidal patterns might be expected to affect litter arrival and polystyrene items (including cups and fast-food trays as well as
exhumation rates at fine temporal scales). Our findings have rele- packing chips and moulded packaging) were placed in a separate
vance for the design of beach litter monitoring programmes, and category because of their lower density than most other litter items.
are especially important for meta-analyses of beach litter accumu- Articles were dried and cleaned of sand before weighing. In 1994, the
lation data collected at different sampling frequencies. entire sample was weighed to the nearest 25 g with a Pesola spring
balance, but large pieces of timber and other very large litter items
were not weighed. No mass data were recorded in 1995, but in
2. Materials and methods 2012 each litter category was weighed separately to the nearest
0.1 g on an electronic top-pan balance.
The study took place at two beaches in Table Bay, South Africa Litter samples were kept separate for each 50- or 25-m subsec-
(Fig. 1), in the Cape Town metropolitan area. The beaches were tion of beach and data compared among subsections to check for
chosen to have similar physical structures but to be different dis- consistent differences in fine-scale accumulation of litter. Such dif-
tances from the urban centre and to have contrasting ease of access ferences were detected, presumably linked to local currents, wave
and user profiles. Milnerton Beach is a popular recreational beach action and resultant beach structure (Bowman et al., 1998), but
10 km from the city centre, used for walking, surfing, swimming there was no tendency for the end subsections at each beach to
and other recreational activities, and attracts large numbers of vis- have the highest accumulation rates, suggesting that lateral drift
itors daily, especially in summer (November–March). Koeberg Nat- from adjacent areas outside the sampling area was minimal (data
ure Reserve is 25 km farther north (35 km from the city centre) and not shown). Thereafter, samples were pooled for each beach and
the study beach is closed to visitors. Both are exposed, west-facing, accumulation data were summarised as the number or mass of lit-
fine-grained sandy beaches with gentle to moderate beach profiles ter items recorded per metre of beach per day, calculated as the lit-
(10–15° slopes), and both are backed by steep, vegetated dunes ter yield divided by the length of beach sampled (m) and by the
which limit shoreward migration of litter. Neither study site was number of days since the last collection (days). For comparative
cleaned by local authorities during the surveys, but adjacent bea- purposes, the rate estimated from the 6-day accumulation sample
ches at both sites were cleaned: Milnerton was cleaned by munici- in 1994 was combined with the weekly samples, but the 4-day
pal workers weekly (1994 and 1995) or daily (2012), whereas accumulation sample was discarded.
Koeberg was cleaned by volunteers every four months. We could The effects of daily versus weekly sampling on the accumula-
not control informal cleaning efforts by members of the public. tion of litter on the beaches was tested with a general linear model
At each beach, a study area was demarcated, divided into ten (GLM), with beach and year as covariates:
equal subsections along the beach (Fig. 1). In 1994 and 1995,
Accumulated litter ðm1 d Þ ¼ eb0 þ bweekly=daily þ bbeach þ byear þ e
1
500 m of beach was sampled (ten 50-m subsections) at each beach,
and the same site was sampled at Koeberg in 2012. However, at
Milnerton only the southern 250 m surveyed in the 1990s was where b0 is a constant; bweekly/daily, bbeach and byear are parameter
sampled in 2012 (ten 25-m subsections) because the marked sets describing the effects of the predictor variables (sampling
P.G. Ryan et al. / Marine Pollution Bulletin 88 (2014) 249–254 251

10 km

Koeberg

Blouberg

Table View
Robben
Table
Island
Bay
Rietvlei

Milnerton

Black River
Cape Town

Fig. 1. Table Bay, South Africa, showing the location of the two study sites at Milnerton and Koeberg, each comprising 10 subsections.

frequency, beach and year) on litter accumulation, and e refers to numerically from both beaches in all three years, comprising
the residuals (there were no significant interaction effects). The 86.5% by number in 1994, 89.3% in 1995 and 93.5% in 2012. The
model was applied to all litter items and also to each major category contribution of plastics to the mass of litter was only recorded in
of litter. Where there were zero values in the data set (for non- 2012, when they comprised 58.9% overall, with wood (21.9% of
plastic items), a small constant (equal to 10% of the mean total mass) and glass (12.8%) dominating the mass of non-plastic
abundance) was added to each data point before a logarithmic items. Plastics were more dominant at Koeberg (95.3% by number
transformation was applied to reduce heteroscedasticity and nor- in all years and 62.0% by mass in 2012), which is farther from the
malise the data (both checked by residual analysis). Coefficients of city centre and has many fewer visitors than Milnerton (91.3% by
variation corrected for sample size [=SD/mean ⁄ (1 + (1/4n))] were number and 57.7% by mass). After plastics, wood contributed most
used to compare relative variability in the number and mass of litter to the mass of litter at both Koeberg (30.4%) and Milnerton (18.4%),
items recorded in daily and weekly samples from the same beach/ but glass items were more important at Milnerton (16.9% of total
year. However, there were too few weekly samples in July 1995 litter mass) than at Koeberg (2.7%).
to compare their variation with daily samples. The GLM indicated that estimates of daily accumulation rates of
all litter items varied with sample site (F = 335, df = 1, 169,
p < 0.00001), year (F = 81, df = 2, 169, p < 0.00001) and sampling
3. Results frequency, with daily sampling providing consistently greater esti-
mates than weekly sampling (F = 32, df = 1, 169, p < 0.00001;
In October–December 1994, the 34 accumulation samples at Table 1). On average, after accounting for the effects of beach
both beaches comprised 38,155 litter items weighing at least and year, daily samples captured 2.5 times more litter overall than
151.1 kg. In July the following year 39,963 items were sampled weekly samples (Fig. 2). This factor was consistent (1.8–2.5) for all
in only 16 collections, and 37 collections in 2012 yielded 199,303 broad litter categories apart from foamed polystyrene (4.2), which
items weighing 257.8 kg. Plastic articles dominated the samples had the largest factor of all litter types in five of six beach-year
252 P.G. Ryan et al. / Marine Pollution Bulletin 88 (2014) 249–254

Table 1
The effect of sampling frequency (daily versus weekly) on estimates of litter accumulation rates (number of items per metre of beach per day) at two South African beaches in
three years. ‘Factor’ is the number of times the rate estimated from daily samples exceeds that from weekly samples (=daily rate/weekly rate).

Site and litter type 1994 1995 2012


Day Week Factor Day Week Factor Day Week Factor
n = 29 n=5 n = 14 n=2 n = 30 n=7
Milnerton
Packaging 0.40 0.22 1.8 1.35 0.60 2.2 4.32 1.63 2.6
Polystyrene 0.71 0.11 6.7 0.37 0.04 10.5 1.38 0.32 4.3
User items 0.15 0.04 3.3 0.47 0.13 3.7 1.54 0.89 1.7
Fragments 0.05 0.01 4.2 0.27 0.10 2.7 6.26 2.27 2.8
All plastics 1.31 0.39 3.4 2.46 0.86 2.8 13.50 5.11 2.6
Cigarettes 0.16 0.05 3.4 0.18 0.02 7.6 0.57 0.21 2.7
Wood 0.04 0.01 4.0 0.06 0.01 8.3 0.13 0.05 2.5
Other non-plastics 0.04 0.01 3.1 0.17 0.03 5.6 0.38 0.09 4.1
All non-plastics 0.24 0.07 3.5 0.41 0.06 6.7 1.08 0.36 3.0
All litter 1.55 0.46 3.4 2.87 0.93 3.1 14.58 5.47 2.7

Koeberg
Packaging 0.11 0.05 2.1 0.38 0.26 1.4 0.75 0.32 2.3
Polystyrene 0.16 0.07 2.1 0.50 0.10 5.1 0.50 0.09 5.7
User items 0.04 0.02 2.3 0.21 0.15 1.5 0.27 0.10 2.7
Fragments 0.02 0.01 2.2 0.12 0.06 2.1 0.48 0.10 4.6
All plastics 0.33 0.15 2.1 1.21 0.56 2.1 1.99 0.61 3.3
Cigarettes 0.01 0.00 3.7 0.01 0.00 2.9 0.01 0.00 3.0
Wood 0.01 0.01 2.0 0.04 0.02 1.9 0.02 0.01 2.5
Other non-plastics 0.01 0.00 4.9 0.04 0.03 1.2 0.02 0.01 3.3
All non-plastics 0.03 0.01 2.8 0.09 0.06 1.5 0.05 0.02 2.8
All litter 0.35 0.16 2.2 1.30 0.62 2.1 2.04 0.63 3.2

comparisons (Table 1). The consistent pattern of crude factors 4. Discussion


when comparing daily to weekly estimates for each site and year
(Table 1) provides further evidence for the generality of this effect. Beaches are dynamic systems where litter can be stranded by
Sampling frequency appears to have slightly less of an impact the sea; dumped by beach visitors; buried and exhumed; exported
on estimates of the mass of litter accumulating on beaches. The inland, offshore or along-shore by wind, wave or tidal action; or
crude mass estimates in 1994 indicated that daily masses at both removed by formal or informal beach cleaning efforts (Ryan
beaches were 1.5 times greater than estimates based on weekly et al., 2009). The best way to monitor the persistence of litter items
samples. These values increased to 1.8 (Koeberg) and 2.3 (Milner- (and, by inference, estimate the scale of these movements and the
ton) in 2012, when the average mass of each litter item (1.3 g) was resultant litter turnover rates) is by mark-recapture studies (e.g.
considerably less than in 1994 (4.0 g), despite the fact that large Merrell, 1980; Garrity and Levings, 1993; Bowman et al., 1998;
pieces of wood and other very large litter items were not weighed Williams and Tudor, 2001; Kataoka et al., 2013). However, mark-
in 1994. Data on how the masses of different categories of litter dif- recapture studies are logistically challenging on a large scale at
fered in their response to sampling frequency were restricted to urban beaches characterised by large litter loads; most studies to
2012. Plastic litter items tended to have a slightly greater day:week date have only sampled specific litter types (Dixon and Cooke,
factor (Milnerton 2.5, Koeberg 1.9) than non-plastic items (Milner- 1977; Merrell, 1980; Williams and Tudor, 2001). Our study sam-
ton 2.1, Koeberg 1.7). Foamed polystyrene again had the greatest pled over 280,000 litter items; attempting to mark and follow
factors (Milnerton 3.2, Koeberg 3.3), although these were lower the fate of even a small subsample of this number would be almost
than the factors for numbers of polystyrene items in 2012 impossible. By alternating daily and weekly beach litter collections
(Table 1). we were able to show that more frequent sampling increased the
The numbers of litter items accumulating daily in different estimate of the amount of litter found on a beach.
time periods at each beach varied considerably (Table 2), with Our results were consistent across beaches in three different
an average coefficient of variation of 59%. This was slightly greater years, sampling in both summer and winter, with little variation
than the average weekly coefficients of variation in 1994 and in the magnitude of the effect of more frequent sampling. For most
2012 (49%), but there was no consistent pattern comparing daily litter items, daily sampling resulted in 2–3 times more litter being
and weekly CVs. Daily variation in the mass of litter (64%) also collected than weekly sampling. This presumably results from
was slightly greater than weekly samples (49%), but again there rapid turnover of many litter items which are missed in weekly
was no consistent pattern. For example, in 1994, the daily CV in samples because they are either buried or transported off the
mass was greater than the weekly CV at Koeberg, but weekly var- beach (blown inshore, blown or washed offshore, carried along-
iability was greater at Milnerton (Table 2). Coefficients of varia- shore, or removed by informal beach cleaning efforts by well-
tion tended to be greater at Koeberg than at Milnerton meaning members of the public). Small, lightweight items are
(Table 2), possibly linked to the smaller volumes of litter at Koe- more likely to be blown inland or buried, resulting in rapid turn-
berg. In 1994/95, daily CVs were greatest in winter (July) and over ( Merrell, 1984; Williams and Tudor, 2001), and hence should
decreased through spring (October) to summer (December). How- have a greater differential between daily and weekly estimates of
ever, in 2012 CVs of daily litter counts increased from October to accumulation rates. This was shown by foamed polystyrene, which
December (but there was no corresponding pattern in CVs of litter is particularly prone to wind dispersal and had more than a four-
masses; Table 2). fold increase in litter accumulation rates based on daily samples.
P.G. Ryan et al. / Marine Pollution Bulletin 88 (2014) 249–254 253

Plastics Non-plastics In addition to differences among litter types, the effect of sam-
pling frequency on estimates of litter arrival rates varies with the
0.10
0.8 magnitude of the difference in sampling frequency. Thus Eriksson
et al. (2013) found almost an order of magnitude more litter by
0.08
sampling daily rather than monthly at beaches on two sub-Antarc-
0.6 tic islands. The size of the effect probably also varies between bea-
0.06 ches, as a result of differences in turnover rates linked to beach
0.4 structure (e.g. slope, orientation, exposure) and local conditions
0.04 (e.g. wind strength and direction). In general, the faster the turn-
over rate, the greater the disparity between arrival rates estimated
0.2 0.02 at different sampling frequencies (cf. Kataoka et al., 2013). The
effect of daily sampling was greater in 2012 than in the 1990s, pre-
0.0 0.00 sumably because the mean mass of litter items in 2012 was lower,
increasing turnover rates.
Litter accumulation rate (items per metre per day)

Packaging Polystyrene Our findings are particularly important for meta-analyses com-
paring beach litter accumulation data collected at different fre-
0.25
quencies. For example, Barnes and Milner (2005) presented
0.3 broad latitudinal patterns in beach litter density, ostensibly repre-
0.20 senting annual accumulation rates, but scrutiny of their sources
indicate a diversity of sampling approaches from standing stock
0.2 0.15 surveys to seasonal and monthly sampling. Even if analyses control
for sampling frequencies, the intrinsic differences in litter turnover
0.10 rates among beaches (see above) are likely to confound compari-
0.1 sons unless samples are collected at a high frequency (e.g. daily).
0.05 Unfortunately, daily sampling is costly to implement because it is
time and labour intensive. Less frequent, regular sampling (e.g.
0.0 0.00 monthly) of the same site or set of sites is adequate to track
long-term changes in litter loads. Such programmes would benefit
Fragments User items from short bouts of daily sampling to assess the extent to which
infrequent sampling underestimates the loading rates of specific
0.05 litter items (i.e. to evaluate the proportions of various litter items
0.10 arriving on a beach that actually accumulate there). This informa-
0.04 tion could be used to develop correction factors to improve the
0.08 accuracy of monitoring programmes, although turnover rates
0.03 probably depend on the size as well as type of litter item. Ideally,
0.06
litter comparisons among beaches should be based on estimates
0.02 of loading rate rather than accumulation rate.
0.04

0.01 0.02
Acknowledgements
0.00 0.00
Day Week We thank the numerous volunteers who assisted with litter
Day Week
collections, especially P. Hardcastle, M. Lamprecht, A. Legodi,
Fig. 2. The effect of sampling frequency (daily versus weekly) on estimates of litter T. Oosthuizen and J. Oosthuizen. G. Greef, J. Le Roux and
accumulation rates for different categories of beach litter determined by a GLM H. Westman kindly gave permission to work in the Koeberg Private
including data from two Cape Town beaches over three years. Error bars show 95% Nature Reserve. G. Frost (Milnerton Municipality) and J. Kieser
confidence intervals.
(Plastics SA) supplied rubbish bags and assisted with initial
cleanups. The National Research Foundation – South Africa and
the University of Cape Town – South Africa provided funds for field
Table 2
expenses and bursary support for A.L. and D.S.
Coefficients of variation (CV) in daily and weekly litter collections at two South
African beaches in three years. Too few weekly samples were collected in July 1995 to
estimate the weekly CV, and there were no mass data for 1995. References
Year and month Milnerton Koeberg
Barnes, D.K.A., Milner, P., 2005. Drifting plastic and its consequences for sessile
Number Mass Number Mass organism dispersal in the Atlantic Ocean. Mar. Biol. 146, 815–825.
Barnes, D.K.A., Galgani, F., Thompson, R.C., Barlaz, M., 2009. Accumulation and
1994
fragmentation of plastic debris in global environments. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 364,
October daily (n = 15) 48% 59% 47% 87% 1985–1998.
December daily (n = 14) 24% 44% 42% 71% Bowman, D., Manor-Samsonov, N., Golit, A., 1998. Dynamics of litter pollution on
Weekly (n = 5) 27% 71% 65% 57% Israeli Mediterranean beaches: a budgetary, litter flux approach. J. Coastal Res.
1995 14, 2588–2597.
Coe, J.M., Rogers, D.B., 1997. Marine Debris: Sources, Impacts and Solutions.
July daily (n = 14) 89% – 130% –
Springer-Verlag, New York.
2012 Corbin, C.J., Singh, J.G., 1993. Marine debris contamination of beaches in St. Lucia
October daily (n = 10) 38% 67% 36% 94% and Dominica. Mar. Pollut. Bull. 26, 325–328.
November daily (n = 10) 49% 27% 40% 77% Derraik, J.G.B., 2002. The pollution of the marine environment by plastic debris: a
December daily (n = 10) 61% 23% 102% 88% review. Mar. Pollut. Bull. 44, 842–852.
Weekly (n = 7) 44% 23% 59% 54% Dixon, T.R., Cooke, A.J., 1977. Discarded containers on a Kent Beach. Mar. Pollut.
Bull. 8, 105–109.
254 P.G. Ryan et al. / Marine Pollution Bulletin 88 (2014) 249–254

Edyvane, K.S., Dalgetty, A., Hone, P.W., Higham, J.S., Wace, N.M., 2004. Long-term Ribic, C.A., Sheavly, S.B., Rugg, D.J., Erdmann, E.S., 2012. Trends in marine debris
marine litter monitoring in the remote Great Australian Bight, South Australia. along the U.S. Pacific Coast and Hawai’i 1998–2007. Mar. Pollut. Bull. 64, 994–
Mar. Pollut. Bull. 48, 1060–1075. 1004.
Eriksson, C., Burton, H., Fitch, S., Schulz, M., 2013. Daily accumulation rates of Ryan, P.G., Moloney, C.L., 1990. Plastic and other artefacts on South African beaches:
marine debris on sub-Antarctic island beaches. Mar. Pollut. Bull. 66, 199–208. temporal trends in abundance and composition. S. Afr. J. Sci. 86, 450–452.
Escardó-Boomsma, J., O’Hara, K., Ribic, C.A., 1995. National Marine Debris Ryan, P.G., Moore, C.J., van Franeker, J.A., Moloney, C.L., 2009. Monitoring the
Monitoring Program, vols. 1–2. US EPA Office of Water, Washington, DC. abundance of plastic debris in the marine environment. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B
Garrity, S.D., Levings, S.C., 1993. Marine debris along the Caribbean coast of Panama. 364, 1999–2012.
Mar. Pollut. Bull. 26, 317–324. Sheavly, S.B., 2007. National Marine Debris Monitoring Program: Final Program
Ivar do Sul, J.A., Barnes, D.K.A., Costa, M.F., Convey, P., Costa, E.S., Campos, L., 2011. Report, Data Analysis and Summary. Ocean Conservancy, Washington, DC.
Plastics in the Antarctic environment: are we looking only at the tip of the Slip, D.J., Burton, H.R., 1991. Accumulation of fishing debris, plastic litter, and other
iceberg? Oecol. Aust. 15, 150–170. artefacts, on Heard and Macquarie Islands in the Southern Ocean. Environ.
Kataoka, T., Hinata, H., Kato, S., 2013. Analysis of a beach as a time-invariant linear Conserv. 18, 249–254.
input/output system of marine litter. Mar. Pollut. Bull. 77, 266–273. Swanepoel, D., 1995. An analysis of beach debris accumulation in Table Bay, Cape
Merrell Jr., T.R., 1980. Accumulation of plastic litter on beaches of Amchitka Island, Town, South Africa. MSc Thesis, University of Cape Town, Cape Town.
Alaska. Mar. Environ. Res. 3, 171–184. Thornton, L., Jackson, N.L., 1998. Spatial and temporal variations in debris
Merrell Jr., T.R., 1984. A decade of change in nets and plastic litter from fisheries in accumulation and composition on an estuarine shoreline, Cliffwood Beach,
Alaska. Mar. Pollut. Bull. 15, 378–384. New Jersey, USA. Mar. Pollut. Bull. 36, 705–711.
OSPAR Commission, 2007. OSPAR Pilot Project on Monitoring Marine Beach Litter: Walker, T.R., Reid, K., Arnould, J.P.Y., Croxall, J.P., 1997. Marine debris surveys at Bird
Monitoring of Marine Litter on Beaches in the OSPAR Region. OSPAR Island, South Georgia 1990–1995. Mar. Pollut. Bull. 34, 61–65.
Commission, London. Williams, A.T., Tudor, D.T., 2001. Litter burial and exhumation: spatial and temporal
Ribic, C.A., Sheavly, S.B., Rugg, D.J., Erdmann, E.S., 2010. Trends and drivers of distribution on a cobble pocket beach. Mar. Pollut. Bull. 42, 1031–1039.
marine debris on the Atlantic coast of the United States 1997–2007. Mar. Pollut.
Bull. 60, 1231–1242.

You might also like