Braat 2019 J1

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 14

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/332934009

Auditory distraction in open-plan study environments: Effects ofbackground


speech and reverberation time on a collaboration task

Article in Applied Acoustics · May 2019


DOI: 10.1016/j.apacoust.2019.04.038

CITATIONS READS

21 318

4 authors, including:

Marijke Keus van de Poll Maarten Hornikx


University of Gävle Eindhoven University of Technology
11 PUBLICATIONS 166 CITATIONS 130 PUBLICATIONS 1,779 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Armin Kohlrausch
Eindhoven University of Technology
221 PUBLICATIONS 5,786 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Maarten Hornikx on 09 May 2019.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Applied Acoustics 154 (2019) 148–160

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Applied Acoustics
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/apacoust

Auditory distraction in open-plan study environments: Effects of


background speech and reverberation time on a collaboration task
Ella Braat-Eggen a,b,⇑, Marijke Keus v.d. Poll c, Maarten Hornikx b, Armin Kohlrausch b
a
Avans University of Applied Sciences, Tilburg, the Netherlands
b
Eindhoven University of Technology, the Netherlands
c
University of Gävle, Sweden

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Previous research has shown that semantic-based tasks are negatively influenced by semantic aspects in
Received 13 May 2018 background speech. Collaboration is an important task in open-plan study environments and is a seman-
Received in revised form 15 March 2019 tic task which might be disrupted by background speech. Therefore, the aim of this study was to analyze
Accepted 29 April 2019
the influence of irrelevant background speech on student-collaboration.
Participants worked in pairs to solve spot-the-difference puzzles, by using the ’DiapixUK’ collaboration
task, while they were exposed to different background sound scenarios. The composed sound scenarios
Keywords:
varied in semantic content (mother tongue and foreign language background speech) and reverberation
Open-plan study environments
Collaboration task
time (short vs long), the latter affecting speech intelligibility.
Background speech Although a longer reverberation time decreases the intelligibility of background speech and a foreign
Reverberation time language decreases meaningfulness of speech, no significant changes in performance were found. On the
Language other hand, the data show an increased perceived disturbance for a longer reverberation time, which we
Task performance interpret as an increased difficulty of interpersonal communication in the collaboration task due to the
Noise disturbance increased level of the background speech. The quiet reference condition was the most preferred sound
condition which is in line with both the effect of a low background sound level and the absence of seman-
tic interference.
Ó 2019 Published by Elsevier Ltd.

1. Introduction classrooms and lecture halls suitable for teacher-centered instruc-


tions but also work environments for participatory learning, group
An acoustic environment should support people doing their work and individual work are needed [14,15]. Especially spaces
tasks without being disturbed and causing loss of performance. intended and designed to accommodate individual as well as small
Unfortunately, it is known that auditory distraction is a major group activities of students, so called open-plan study environ-
problem in all kinds of open-plan work environments [1–4]. For ments [3], have to support a diversity of tasks in the same environ-
instance, many studies show the detrimental effect of background ment. A situation of several groups working on their group
speech on performance of semantic tasks such as comprehensive assignment in the same open-plan study environment is a very
reading [5,6], proof reading [7–10] and writing [11,12]. Unfortu- common situation.
nately, background speech is common in an open-plan work envi- This so called collaborative learning can be defined as: ’working
ronment due to telephone calls and interactions between workers in a group of two or more to achieve a common goal, while respect-
[2–4]. These interactions might, in the best case, lead to communi- ing each individual’s contribution to the whole’ [16]. Collaboration
cation and collaboration. On the other hand, collaboration is also a therefore implies interaction among students to produce a com-
semantic task which might be disrupted by background speech. mon product and involves negotiations, discussions, and integrat-
The importance of collaboration tasks in open work spaces is ing others’ perspectives [17]. Verbal communication is a crucial
significant for open-plan study environments [3]. Due to new ways element of all those activities and will imply speech production.
of learning, informal learning places (e.g. libraries, study areas, lob- Therefore, background speech, due to speech production of other
bies, atria etc.) become more and more important [13,14]. Not only working groups in the same environment, is unavoidable and all
students working in an open-plan study environment will more
⇑ Corresponding author. or less be influenced by background speech while doing their tasks.
E-mail address: pe.braat-eggen@avans.nl (E. Braat-Eggen). Therefore, the focus in the present study was on the influence of

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apacoust.2019.04.038
0003-682X/Ó 2019 Published by Elsevier Ltd.
E. Braat-Eggen et al. / Applied Acoustics 154 (2019) 148–160 149

background speech on a collaboration task in an open-plan study fulness [5–7,11,19] of the background speech is related to an
environment. increase of disturbance and a decrease of performance on a seman-
tic task.
1.1. Why is background speech disruptive for cognitive performance? As mentioned earlier, a serial recall task is also disrupted by
background speech, although not due to its semantic properties
The characteristics of a sound in combination with the charac- but due to its auditory-perceptive characteristics [41]. Therefore,
teristics of a cognitive task can predict whether the sound will a serial recall task is as much disturbed by background speech in
impair cognitive performance in the task [18–20]. The ‘‘Duplex- an unknown foreign language as by background speech in the
Mechanism Account of Auditory Distraction” (DMAAD) [21,22] mother language [19]. Nevertheless, for tasks with semantic char-
describes two mechanisms through which sound can disrupt cog- acteristics, the semantics of a speech signal are decisive [41]. Since
nitive performance: attention distraction and specific collaboration has semantic characteristics, in the current study, we
interferences. expected a highly intelligible and meaningful background speech
The first mechanism, attention distraction, gives an explanation to be more disruptive compared to less intelligible and less mean-
of why an unexpected signal can disrupt performance (attentional ingful background speech.
capture). Sudden or abrupt changes in the signal capture attention
and draw it away from the focal task towards the background 1.2. Individual differences
sound signal [23]. For instance, a signal like ‘B B B B B B’ is not dis-
tracting, while the ‘K’ in the series ‘B B B K B B B’ is distracting and Another factor – in addition to the characteristics of the sound
decreases performance because of the violation of the expectation and the cognitive task – that can influence how an individual reacts
for another ‘B’ (i.e. the deviation effect) [21–24]. Besides an unex- to noise, is individual differences, like noise sensitivity. Individual
pected change in the sound signal, also other aspects in the sound differences are important to take into account when organizing
can capture attention and thereby disrupt performance, like hear- workplaces, as those individual differences might require different
ing one’s own name in a background conversation [25], or hearing work environments. It is reasonable to argue that individuals that
other interesting or relevant information [22,23,26,27,28,29]. are sensitive to noise will be more distracted by noise when under-
The second mechanism mentioned in the DMAAD to explain taking a cognitive task, and as a consequence perform worse com-
why background sound can disrupt cognitive performance is the pared to their less sensitive colleagues. However, studies that
occurrence of interference between similar cognitive processes investigated the relationship between subjective noise sensitivity
(interference-by-process). A classical way of studying this is mea- (i.e. noise sensitivity measured by self-rating) and cognitive perfor-
suring serial recall performance. In serial recall, a person has to mance have only found small correlations or no correlations at all
recall a series of visually presented items (e.g. numbers, words or [42–45]. In those studies, though, background sound consisted of
letters) in the correct serial order. Task-irrelevant sound that is different kinds of noise, or unintelligible speech. None of the stud-
played during the presentation of the visual stimuli impairs task ies used intelligible background speech. Neither has the correlation
performance. The magnitude of the impairment depends on the between noise sensitivity and performance on a verbal collabora-
acoustical variability of the sound [30,31]. A background sound tion task been investigated. In a recent field study in open-plan
consisting of almost no acoustical variability, like ‘B B B B B B’ (a study environments [3], noise sensitivity of students showed to
steady state signal) is almost not disruptive for the serial recall per- be related to the disturbance by noise. Therefore, in the current
formance. Performance in a background with a steady state signal study we investigated whether individual differences in subjective
is similar to performance in quiet. On the other side, a signal with noise sensitivity predict individual differences in susceptibility to
acoustical variability, like ‘B X K C M L’ (a changing state signal) the effects of background speech on collaboration.
impairs serial recall performance [32–35]. This changing state
effect [35] can be explained as an interference between voluntary 1.3. How to influence speech intelligibility and meaningfulness?
processes required to recall the serial order of the visually pre-
sented items and similar automatic processes required to analyse Room acoustics will influence the intelligibility of speech. A
the serial order of the items in the automatically incoming auditory widely used parameter to measure or describe speech intelligibil-
signal, i.e. interference-by-process [23,36]. For instance music and ity between a speaker and listener is the speech transmission index
speech can disrupt cognitive performance as long as there is acous- (STI), where a value of 1 indicates an excellent speech intelligibility
tical variability in the sound signal [35,37]. and a STI value below 0.3 indicates nearly unintelligible speech.
A property that distinguishes speech from other sounds is that The most important aspects that will influence speech intelligibil-
speech not only has acoustic characteristics, like changes in fre- ity in an environment are the speech level relative to the back-
quency and amplitude of the signal, but also has semantic charac- ground noise level at the listeners’ position and the reverberation
teristics, like the words and sentences that contain meaning [38]. time [46].
In line with the interference-by-process account, an interference A long reverberation time will decrease speech intelligibility
also occurs between semantic cognitive processes used to auto- due to the effect of smoothening the temporal profile of the wave-
matically analyse the meaning in the background speech signal form [46,47]. On the other hand, the level of speech at the listeners’
and similar semantic cognitive processes involved in the execution position is of great importance, a high speech to noise ratio will
of a semantic based task, like reading and writing [19]. This should result in more intelligible speech. The speech level in a room will
mean that speech intelligibility and meaningfulness of the back- decrease over distance between talker and listener due to sound
ground speech signal predict distraction on semantic tasks like absorbing materials in an environment. If more sound absorbing
reading and writing. As highly intelligible background speech con- materials are applied to walls, ceiling and floor, not only the intel-
tains more semantic information compared to a less intelligible ligibility will increase due to a shorter reverberation time but also
signal, a highly intelligible signal should be more disruptive. Fur- the speech level will be reduced at the listeners’ position resulting
ther, a highly intelligible speech signal will reasonably contain in a decrease of the intelligibility. Speech related room acoustic
more relevant or interesting information that can capture attention parameters as described in ISO 3382-3 [48], like spatial decay rate
and, as explained by the framework of attentional capture, disrupt of speech (D2,S) and sound pressure level of speech at 4 m (Lp,A,S,4m)
performance more than a less intelligible signal. Research indeed are correlated with intelligibility. A decrease of the speech level
shows that an increase of intelligibility [8,12,39,40] and meaning- over distance, expressed in a higher D2,S or lower Lp,A,S,4m value,
150 E. Braat-Eggen et al. / Applied Acoustics 154 (2019) 148–160

will result in a decrease of the speech to noise ratio and will there- ration of the participants accomplishing a collaborative task were
fore lower speech intelligibility. Reducing the speech level over used. Also, the noise sensitivity and strategy of the participants
distance can not only be accomplished by applying more acoustic was measured.
absorbing materials but also by installing high sound screens in
an open environment [49,50]. 2.2. Participants
The background noise level will also influence the intelligibility
of a speech signal because it reduces the signal to noise ratio, A total of 76 participants (37 male and 39 females, mean
therefore, if the background noise increases, speech intelligibility age = 24.5 years, SD = 4.9 years) took part in the experiment. The
decreases. In a multi-speaker background situation, the noise con- participants were Swedish, Belgian and Dutch students. In total
sists of speech and the loudest background voice is most intelligi- 46 Swedish students from the University of Gävle, 24 Dutch stu-
ble for a person doing a task. Also in this situation an increase of dents from Avans University of Applied Sciences and 6 Belgian vis-
the background speech will lead to a reduction of the intelligibility iting students at the University of Gävle in Sweden participated in
of the speech of the loudest speaker. this experiment. The Belgian students had the same native Dutch
A special situation occurs if the task of a worker requires verbal language as the Dutch students. The students worked in couples
communication, as in a collaboration task. An increase of the back- which resulted in 23 Swedish couples and 15 Dutch speaking cou-
ground noise level will lead to a louder speech level of the commu- ples. One Swedish couple was left out of the analyses because of
nicating participants in order to compensate the decrease of the technical errors. The Swedish and Belgian students received two
signal to noise ratio between them. This increase of the speech cinema tickets and the Dutch students received a financial contri-
level will implicate a higher background noise level for other work- bution to their study trip to Stockholm and Gävle as a reward for
ers in the environment. The latter effect is called the Lombard their participation.
effect [51], a well-known phenomenon that people in a room speak
at a higher sound level due to the background speech, which again 2.3. Acoustic conditions
leads to a higher background speech level. The Lombard effect was
found to start at an ambient noise level around 45 dB and a speech Five sound scenarios were composed, four comprising different
level of 55 dB [52]. background sounds and one quiet condition. In this study intelligi-
A poorly intelligible speech signal can also be less meaningful bility of background speech was influenced by manipulating the
for the listener, after all, unintelligible speech is also meaningless. reverberation time by changing the materials of the walls, floor
It should be noted that meaningless speech is not the same as irrel- and ceiling of the room. Although absorption of sound is not the
evant speech, because irrelevant speech can still have a meaning. only way to influence the intelligibility, it is the most common
In an experimental setting meaningfulness of speech can be influ- way to influence the acoustics of an environment. Adding masking
enced by for instance spectrally-rotating the speech [11], by play- sounds or screens to influence the speech intelligibility of an open
ing sentences in reverse [6,7] or by the language of the speech [7]. environment is another possibility to influence the acoustics, how-
Research has shown that background speech in an unknown for- ever, it takes architectural or electro-acoustical attributes to add to
eign language will decrease the disturbance and increase the per- the environment, while choosing materials for walls, floor and ceil-
formance of a semantic task compared to background speech in ing is a standard procedure of an architect designing an open-plan
the mother tongue [7,40]. study environment. To study the influence of intelligibility by
changing the reverberation time, two extreme sound absorbing
conditions were chosen: one condition with only sound absorbing
1.4. The aim of the study
materials on all walls, ceiling and floor and one condition with no
sound absorbing materials, resulting in a very short (T = 0.6 s) and
To the authors’ best knowledge, the influence of background
very long (T = 2.3 s) reverberation time. To study the influence of
speech on the perceived disturbance and performance of a collab-
meaningfulness of the background speech, the semantic content
oration task has not been previously investigated. Because of the
was manipulated by the language of the background speech. In this
importance of this task in an open-plan study environment, the
experiment Dutch and Swedish language was used due to the pos-
aim of this study is to analyze the influence of background speech
sibilities created by the cooperation between the University of
on the perceived disturbance and performance on a collaboration
Gävle (Sweden) and Avans University of Applied Sciences (The
task considering intelligibility and meaningfulness of the back-
Netherlands).
ground speech.
The hypothesis is that an increase of intelligibility and mean-
2.3.1. Acoustic models of two virtual open-plan study environments
ingfulness of the irrelevant background speech will lead to an
To create realistic sound scenarios, the acoustics of an open-
increase of disturbance and a decrease of performance in the col-
plan study environment was modelled with a software package
laboration task. Furthermore, noise sensitivity will be taken into
based on geometrical acoustics (Odeon version 12.12 [53]).
account in this study. The hypothesis is that highly sensitive indi-
For this purpose, the open-plan study environment of the 3rd
viduals will be more disturbed by the background speech and will
floor of the Vertigo building at the Eindhoven University of Tech-
show a decrease of performance in the collaboration task com-
nology was modelled. This study environment has a height of
pared to less sensitive individuals.
5.3 m and a shoe box shape with a volume of 2750 m3 (Fig. 1). A
picture of the open-plan study environment can be found in the
2. Materials and methods Appendix (Fig. A2). A comparison between the acoustic parameters
calculated using the room acoustics software and acoustic param-
2.1. Design eters measured in the study environment showed good agreement.
The differences between the calculated and measured reverbera-
A within-participants design was used with five different sound tion times (EDT and T30) were smaller than just noticeable differ-
scenarios with varying intelligibility and meaningfulness of the ences indicated in literature of 5–8.5% for T500Hz–1000Hz [54,55].
background speech. As dependent variables, the performance and For the purpose of the auralizations, two new virtual environments
self-estimated parameters such as disturbance, ability to ignore were created by changing the materials of the walls, ceiling and
the background speech, eagerness to go on and quality of collabo- floor. One model of the open-plan study environment was calcu-
E. Braat-Eggen et al. / Applied Acoustics 154 (2019) 148–160 151

Fig. 1. Floor plan of the modelled open-plan study environment. Positions of receiver (Listener) and three sound sources (Talker 1–3), with the lines indicating the listening
and talking direction. Dimensions in mm.

lated with sound absorbing walls, ceiling and floor resulting in a have read different Hagerman or Matrix sentences aloud for at
very absorbing environment (reverberation time T30 = 0.6 s) and a least 5 min. From the recordings, three Dutch and three Swedish
second model was calculated with reflecting walls, ceiling and talkers were selected based on the intelligibility and normal pro-
floor resulting in a very reverberant environment (T30 = 2.3 s). In sody of the spoken sentences. By convolving these recorded speech
this study the first is called the absorbing model and the latter signals with the calculated impulse responses between the three
the reverberant model. The most important materials and absorp- talker positions and the receiver position (Fig. 1), four sound sce-
tion coefficients used in the models are presented in Table A1 of narios were created: Two absorbing Swedish and Dutch sound sce-
the Appendix. Three human talkers were modelled as sound narios and two reverberant Swedish and Dutch sound scenarios.
sources and one point as a human receiver. For creating auraliza- The sound signal in the quiet control scenario was a pink noise sig-
tions based on predictions using Odeon, impulse responses were nal at the same background noise level as measured in the real
computed for all source-receiver combinations in the two virtual study environment without people, 30 dB(A). The sound levels of
environments. the separate sources (talkers) at the receiver position as well as
the total sound levels for the different scenarios calculated by
2.3.2. Sound scenarios Odeon are described in Table 1. For the experiment with the collab-
In the scenarios, three talkers were implemented producing oration task, the five scenarios were played back through five loud-
Dutch or Swedish background speech. As a result, four sound sce- speakers in a highly absorbing room (T30 = 0.4 s, Appendix Fig. A1)
narios were created: creating a realistic sound environment. Odeon software was used
to create a 2D surround sound based on a specified speaker rig,
 Absorbing open-plan study environment (T30 = 0.6 s) with 3 therefore no HRTF was included in the auralization. Due to the
Swedish talkers 2D surround sound and the freedom of the subjects to move their
 Absorbing open-plan study environment (T30 = 0.6 s) with 3 heads, there will be some spread to the perception of the loudness
Dutch talkers of the background speech by the subjects and therefore some
 Reverberant open-plan study environment (T30 = 2.3 s) with 3 uncertainty in the calculated STI values.
Swedish talkers The reverberation time of a room will affect the intelligibility of
 Reverberant open-plan study environment (T30 = 2.3 s) with 3 background speech and therefore can influence the disturbance
Dutch talkers and performance of a semantic task [8,12,39,40]. A comparison of
the level of intelligibility of the background speech for the different
The position and speech direction of the three talkers and the sound scenarios will be based on a calculation of the speech trans-
listening direction of the listener can be seen in the floor plan of mission index (STI) of the background speech. A calculation of the
the open-plan study environment (Fig. 1). The talkers were posi- STI was done with talker 3, the nearest and loudest background
tioned at table groups close to the listener. This listener-talker con- voice, in accordance with the international standards IEC 60268-
figuration was chosen as an example of a realistic situation for
group work in an open-plan study environment.
Table 1
The background speech consisted of Hagerman sentences
Description of the sound sources (talkers) in the absorbing and reverberant model of
(Swedish) [56] and a Dutch version called Matrix sentences [57]. the open-plan study environment Vertigo floor 3.
These sentences have been developed for audiological tests mea-
suring speech intelligibility in noise. Each sentence is composed Talker Gender Sound level at Listener position
calculated by Odeon (dB(A))
with the same structure of five words (name, verb, number, adjec-
tive, object) and each word category can be filled by 10 different Dutch Swedish Absorbing model Reverberant model

words. In this way numerous sentences can be randomly com- 1 female male 46.1 54.7
posed. The sentences were recorded in a highly sound absorbing 2 female male 47.9 54.8
3 male female 52.2 56.3
setting and sampled at 44.1 kHz. Ten Dutch and six Swedish stu-
3 talkers 54.3 60.1
dents and employees from the Universities of Eindhoven and Gävle
152 E. Braat-Eggen et al. / Applied Acoustics 154 (2019) 148–160

Table 2
Estimated STI values for different conditions between talker 3 and the listener.

Absorbing model Reverberant model


STI LAeq background speech level 0.55 (fair) 0.37 (poor)
LA95 background speech level 0.83 (excellent) 0.56 (fair)
Without background speech 0.87 (excellent) 0.62 (good)

16 [58–60]. However, the STI is originally developed to measure


speech intelligibility in stationary noise, therefore a calculation of
the STI with background speech will be an estimation of the real
intelligibility [61]. The background noise level due to the irrelevant
background speech was computed as the LAeq and LA95 level of the
two other voices. LA95 is the sound level that is exceeded 95% of the
time, which is a method to quantify background noise levels of
fluctuating signals, i.e. it characterizes the general background
sound pressure level that excludes the particular local noise Fig. 3. Experimental setup. Dimensions in mm.
events. LA95 was also used in [62] to estimate the background
sound level in an open-plan office to calculate the STI value. Table 2
also shows the calculation of the STI value based on the usual pro-
cedure, which does not include the background voices only the sta-
tionary 30 dB(A) pink background noise. Due to the low number of University of Gävle. Indoor environmental conditions were kept
talkers and the differences in space angles and distances between constant throughout the experiments. The reverberation time of
listener and talkers (see Fig. 1) it is very difficult to estimate the the room was rather short (T30; = 0.4, Appendix Fig. A1). A realistic
stationary noise level, therefore a range of estimations is reported sound environment was created by a sound set-up consisting of a
in Table 2. laptop (Hp Zbook) generating the sound signal via an external
sound card (USB Sound Box ST Lab) connected to a sound amplifier
(Sherwood RD-7500) lined to five loudspeakers (Cambridge Audio
2.4. Collaboration task.
minX). The loudspeakers were set at a height of 1.6 m in a circle
surrounding the participants at a distance of 1.4 m (Fig. 3). A screen
The participating students worked in Swedish and Dutch/Bel-
was placed between the participants (h = 0.3 m above table hight)
gian couples on a collaboration task. For this purpose, the ’spot
in order to avoid them seeing each other’s pictures. The sound
the differences’ task, based on the ’DiapixUK’ pictures (Fig. 2)
levels of the sound scenarios were calibrated conform the calcu-
developed by Baker and Hazan [63] was used. Two participants
lated levels by recording the sound signal at both participant posi-
had to discover differences between two pictures without seeing
tions using a microphone (B&K 4189) and pre-amplifier (B&K type
each other’s pictures and only using verbal communication. A time
2671) and subsequently processed off-line with DIRAC 6.0 room
limit was set to three minutes, and a maximum of twelve differ-
acoustics software.
ences could be found in each picture pair. This collaboration task
was chosen due to the important vocal communication component
(e.g. listening and discussing) in the task in accordance with a col- 2.6. Dependent variables
laboration task in an open-plan study environment. Furthermore,
this task was suitable for repeated measurements due to the pro- 2.6.1. Performance measurements
ven equal difficulty of the picture pairs, no learning effect of com- As an objective variable of performance, the amount of found
pleting more than one picture and a balanced contribution of both differences for each puzzle was used.
participants [63].
2.6.2. Questionnaire scores
2.5. Experimental setup As subjective variables the self-rating scores on questions about
their experiences during the collaboration task were used. Besides
The participants carried out the test in pairs, seated at a round the questions concerning the noise disturbance, other subjective
table in a silent room (60 m2, Fig. 3) in a laboratory facility at the variables were chosen after a pilot study. A difference in eagerness

Fig. 2. Example ’DiapixUK’ pictures for the ’spot the differences’ collaboration task.
E. Braat-Eggen et al. / Applied Acoustics 154 (2019) 148–160 153

and motivation during the test was observable, sometimes partic- utes after starting the puzzle a voice announced to stop searching
ipants didn’t want to stop after the stop signal. While motivation for differences and to fill in individually a short questionnaire
can influence how subjects react on background speech this ques- about their experiences during the collaboration task. After finish-
tion has been added [10]. A difference in collaboration style was ing the questionnaire an instruction on the computer screen indi-
also observed during the pilot, therefore a question has been added cated to press a button when they wanted to start the next puzzle.
that maps the self-assessed quality of the collaboration. A seven At the same time when pressing the button, a new sound environ-
points Likert scale, 1 until 7, was used for each question. ment started playing in the room. When finishing the last puzzle
The questions were: and the corresponding questionnaire the couples were asked to fill
in individually the noise sensitivity questionnaire. The experiment
- the disturbance by the background noise during the task: ’To lasted about 50 min for each participating couple.
what extent did you find the background noise disturbing dur-
ing the execution of the assignment?’ where 1 represented ‘to- 2.8. Statistical method
tally not disturbing’ and 7 ‘very disturbing’
- the ability to ignore the background noise during the task: ’To The data were analyzed with the statistical program SPSS 23.0.
what extent could you ignore the background noise?’ where 1 All questionnaire variables were analyzed taking into account indi-
represented ’it was very difficult to ignore’ and 7 ’it was very vidual participants (n = 74). The variables: performance and pro-
easy to ignore’. There was also a possibility to choose 0: ’there duced sound level, were measured and analyzed for participant
was no background noise’ couples (n = 37 for performance, n = 30 for produced sound level).
- the unwillingness to stop with the puzzle after a time limit set To study the impact of the background sound scenarios on the
to three minutes: ’How much did you want to continue with the collaboration task, for each dependent variable a single-factor
task despite the time was up?’ where 1 represented ‘not at all’ repeated measures ANOVA was used to analyze the significance
and 7 ‘very much’ of the differences between the means of the dependent variable
- the quality of the collaboration during the task: ’How did the due to the five sound scenarios. Furthermore, a follow-up pair-
collaboration go?’ where 1 represented ’very bad’ and 7 repre- wise comparison to examine where the differences occur was per-
sented ’very good’ formed by using post-hoc T-tests with Bonferroni correction.
To study the impact of language and reverberation time on the
Also, the self-rated noise sensitivity of the participants was collaboration task, for each variable a factorial 2(reverberation:
measured by a questionnaire developed by Weinstein [64,65]. absorbing vs. reverberant)  2(language of background speech:
The noise sensitivity was measured on the basis of eleven state- native vs. foreign) repeated measures ANOVA was used.
ments in which the participants had to indicate to what extent To verify the influence of the noise sensitivity a mean split was
they agreed with these statements (6-point scale). Furthermore, done to divide the subjects in two groups and a factorial 2(rever-
some couples showed changes in their strategy to find the differ- beration: absorbing vs. reverberant)  2(language of background
ences during the pilot. Therefore the participants were asked if speech: native vs. foreign)  2(noise sensitivity: low vs. high)
they used the same strategy for each picture to find the twelve repeated measures ANOVA was used to analyze the influence of
differences. the noise sensitivity on the dependent variables.
To verify the influence of using the same or different strategies a
2.6.3. Sound measurements factorial 2(reverberation: absorbing vs. reverberant)  2(language
During the execution of the collaboration task, the speech levels of background speech: native vs. foreign)  2(strategy: same vs.
of the participants in combination with the background sound sce- different) repeated measures ANOVA was used to analyze the
narios were measured. The sound was recorded at the middle of influence of the strategy on the dependent variables.
the round table at a distance of about 0.8 m from the participants
(Fig. 3) using a microphone (B&K 4189) and pre-amplifier (B&K
3. Results
type 2671) and subsequently processed off-line with DIRAC 6.0
room acoustics software. These sound measurements are lacking
3.1. Impact of background sound scenarios on a collaboration task
for the first seven Swedish participant couples.
The figures in this paragraph show the impact of the different
2.7. Procedure
sound scenarios on different dependent variables.
The participants worked on the collaboration task in couples.
These couples were participants with the same native language, 3.1.1. Performance
Swedish or Dutch. The participants had to solve ’spot-the- Fig. 4 shows the performance of participants working on the
difference’ puzzles [63] within a time limit of three minutes. Each ’DiapixUK’ collaboration task while being exposed to different
student was given one picture and was asked to identify the differ- sound scenarios. The different sound scenarios have no significant
ences through communication with his/her partner. The partici- effect on the performance of the participants (F(4,144) = 0.42,
pants were instructed to use the strategy to start in the left p = .791).
upper corner of the picture and to find the differences by a clock-
wise description of the pictures. The couples were allowed to 3.1.2. Questionnaire results
change their strategy during the experiment. After a short oral Fig. 5 shows the perceived disturbance of participants working
introduction, a training phase of three minutes started to solve on the ’DiapixUK’ collaboration task while being exposed to differ-
the first puzzle to get used to the procedure. The next ten puzzles ent sound scenarios. The different sound scenarios have a signifi-
were presented in the same order to all participants. The sequence cant effect on the perceived disturbance of the participants (F
of the sound scenarios was different for each couple, they were (4,292) = 63.64, p < .001, g2p = 0.466). The reverberant sound sce-
offered in a counterbalanced sequence using a Latin Square design. narios were reported as being the most disturbing. The quiet con-
The sound scenarios were presented two times after each other for dition was reported as being the most comfortable situation.
each couple. Each difference on which both participants in a couple Follow-up T-tests with Bonferroni adjustment showed significant
agreed on, was marked with a circle at their pictures. Three min- differences between the quiet condition and the four other condi-
154 E. Braat-Eggen et al. / Applied Acoustics 154 (2019) 148–160

Fig. 4. Mean values and confidence intervals (95%) of the performance of Fig. 6. Mean values and confidence intervals (95%) of the ability of participants
participant couples (n = 37) accomplishing the ’DiapixUK’ collaboration task with (n = 74) to ignore the background noise while accomplishing the ’DiapixUK’
different background sound scenarios: Quiet (Q), Reverberant & Foreign Language collaboration task with different background sound scenarios: Quiet (Q), Reverber-
(R&FL), Absorbing & Foreign Language (A&FL), Reverberant & Native Language ant & Foreign Language (R&FL), Absorbing & Foreign Language (A&FL), Reverberant
(R&NL), Absorbing & Native Language (A&NL). & Native Language (R&NL), Absorbing & Native Language (A&NL).

Fig. 7. Mean values and confidence intervals (95%) of eagerness of participants


(n = 74) to accomplish the ’DiapixUK’ collaboration task with different background
Fig. 5. Mean values and confidence intervals (95%) of the perceived disturbance of sound scenarios: Quiet (Q), Reverberant & Foreign Language (R&FL), Absorbing &
participants (n = 74) accomplishing the ’DiapixUK’ collaboration task with different Foreign Language (A&FL), Reverberant & Native Language (R&NL), Absorbing &
background sound scenarios: Quiet (Q), Reverberant & Foreign Language (R&FL), Native Language (A&NL).
Absorbing & Foreign Language (A&FL), Reverberant & Native Language (R&NL),
Absorbing & Native Language (A&NL).

tions and between the reverberant native and absorbing (native


and foreign) background speech conditions. The differences
between all other condition were not significant.
Fig. 6 shows the self-estimated ability of participants to ignore
the background noise while working on the ’DiapixUK’ collabora-
tion task and being exposed to different sound scenarios. The dif-
ferent sound scenarios have a significant effect on the ability of
the participants to ignore the background noise (F(4,292) = 45.39,
p < .001, g2p = 0.383). Post-hoc T-tests with Bonferroni adjustment
showed significant differences between the quiet condition and
the four other background speech conditions. The differences
between all the other conditions were not significant.
Fig. 7 shows the eagerness of participants to continue with the
’DiapixUK’ collaboration task while being exposed to different Fig. 8. Mean values and confidence intervals (95%) of the self-estimated quality of
sound scenarios. The different sound scenarios have no significant the collaboration of the participants (n = 74) while accomplishing the ’DiapixUK’
collaboration task with different background sound scenarios: Quiet (Q), Reverber-
effect on the eagerness of the participants (F(4,292) = 1.74,
ant & Foreign Language (R&FL), Absorbing & Foreign Language (A&FL), Reverberant
p = .141). & Native Language (R&NL), Absorbing & Native Language (A&NL).
Fig. 8 shows the self-estimated quality of the collaboration of
the participants while working on the ’DiapixUK’ collaboration task reported to have the highest quality. The sound scenarios with
and being exposed to different sound scenarios. The different background speech in the native language were reported to have
sound scenarios have a significant effect on the self-estimated the lowest quality. Post-hoc T-tests with Bonferroni adjustment
quality of the collaboration of the participants (F(4,292) = 3.11, showed significant differences between the means of the quiet
p = .016, g2p = 0.041). The results show that the quiet scenario was condition and the native background speech conditions.
E. Braat-Eggen et al. / Applied Acoustics 154 (2019) 148–160 155

of the language of the background speech (F(1,36) = 0.01,


p = .945) and no significant main effect of reverberation of the
study environment (F(1,36) = 1.57, p = .219). Also no interaction
effect of the reverberance of the study environment and the lan-
guage of the background speech on performance was found (F
(1,36) = 0.07, p = .788).

3.2.2. Questionnaire results


Fig. 10 shows the impact of different languages of background
speech and the reverberance of the study environment on per-
ceived disturbance of the ’DiapixUK’ collaboration task. A factorial
2(reverberation: absorbing vs. reverberant)  2(language: native
vs. foreign) repeated measures ANOVA with perceived disturbance
as dependent variable revealed a significant main effect of the lan-
guage of the background speech (F(1,73) = 4.45, p = .038, g2p = 0.058)
Fig. 9. Mean values and confidence intervals (95%) of produced sound levels by and a significant main effect of reverberation of the study environ-
speech of participants couples (n = 30) accomplishing the ’DiapixUK’ collaboration
task with different background sound scenarios: Quiet (Q), Reverberant & Foreign ment (F(1,73) = 11.23, p = .001, g2p = 0.133). Fig. 10a shows that
Language (R&FL), Absorbing & Foreign Language (A&FL), Reverberant & Native native background speech was reported as being more disturbing
Language (R&NL), Absorbing & Native Language (A&NL). than foreign background speech and Fig. 10b shows that a rever-
berant environment perceived more disturbance than an absorb-
3.1.3. Produced sound pressure levels
ing. No interaction effect of reverberance of the study
Fig. 9 shows the sound pressure levels produced by speech of the
environment and the language of the background speech on per-
participants working on the ’DiapixUK’ collaboration task while
ceived disturbance was found (F(1,73) = 0.35, p = .559).
being exposed to different sound scenarios. The different sound sce-
A factorial 2(reverberation: absorbing vs. reverberant)  2(lan-
narios have a significant effect on the produced sound pressure
guage: native vs. foreign) repeated measures ANOVA with the abil-
levels of the participants (F(4,116) = 109.58, p < .001, g2p = 0.791).
ity to ignore the background speech as dependent variable
The highest sound pressure levels are produced while being exposed revealed no significant main effect of language of the background
to the reverberant sound environments. The absorbing sound envi- speech (F(1,73) = 2.20, p = .143). On the other hand, a significant
ronments and quiet condition result in significantly lower sound main effect of the reverberance of the room on the ability to ignore
pressure levels as can be seen in Fig. 9. Post-hoc T-tests with Bonfer-
the background noise was shown (F(1,73) = 5.70, p = .002, g2p =
roni adjustment showed significant differences between the means
0.072) (Fig. 11). No interaction effect of the reverberance of the
of the quiet condition and all other conditions and between the
study environment and the language of the background speech
reverberant and absorbing background speech conditions. No signif-
on the ability to ignore the background noise was found (F(1,73)
icant difference were found between the other sound conditions.
= 0.14, p = .788).
A factorial 2(reverberation: absorbing vs. reverberant)  2(lan-
3.2. Impact of the language of the background speech and guage: native vs. foreign) repeated measures ANOVA with eager-
reverberation of the study environment on a collaboration task ness to go on as dependent variable revealed a significant main
effect of the language of the background speech (F(1,73) = 5.98,
3.2.1. Performance p = .017, g2p = 0.076) (see Fig. 12). On the other hand, no significant
A factorial 2(reverberation: absorbing vs. reverberant)  2(lan- main effect of the reverberance of the room on the eagerness to
guage: native vs. foreign) repeated measures ANOVA with perfor- go on with the task was showed (F(1,73) = 0.03, p = .868). Also no
mance as dependent variable revealed no significant main effect interaction effect of the reverberance of the study environment

Fig. 10. Mean values and confidence intervals of perceived disturbance of participants on the ’DiapixUK’ collaboration task in relation to language of background speech (a)
and reverberance of the study environment (b).
156 E. Braat-Eggen et al. / Applied Acoustics 154 (2019) 148–160

Fig. 13. Mean values and confidence intervals of produced sound pressure levels
Fig. 11. Mean values and confidence intervals of the self-estimated ability to ignore during the ‘DiapixUK’ collaboration task in relation to the reverberation of the
the background sound of participants on the ’DiapixUK’ collaboration task in environment.
relation to the reverberance of the study environment.
the reverberation of the study environment (reverberant, absorb-
ing) for the different background noise scenarios on the produced
sound pressure level during the ’DiapixUK’ communication task.
The produced sound level by the participants during the reverber-
ant sound scenarios was higher than the produced sound level dur-
ing the absorbing sound scenarios. No interaction effect of the
reverberance of the study environment and the language of the
background speech on the produced speech level was found (F
(1,29) = 0.76, p = .523).

3.3. Impact of the noise sensitivity and strategy of participants on the


dependent variables

To verify the influence of the noise sensitivity, a mean split was


done to divide the participants in two groups. The mean noise sen-
sitivity of all participants was 3.53 (SD = 0.56) on a 6-point scale.
The mean noise sensitivity of the group most sensitive participants
(noise sensitivity > 3.53) was 3.91 (SD = 0.34) and the mean noise
sensitive of the group participants with the lowest sensitivity
Fig. 12. Mean values and confidence intervals of the self-estimated eagerness to go
on with the ’DiapixUK’ collaboration task in relation to the language of the
(noise sensitivity < 3.53) was 3.03 (SD = 0.37). A factorial 2(rever-
background speech. beration: absorbing vs. reverberant)  2(language of background
speech: native vs. foreign)  2(noise sensitivity: low vs. high)
and the language of the background speech on the eagerness to go repeated measures ANOVA revealed no significant interactions of
on with the task was found (F(1,73) = 0.64, p = .427). the noise sensitivity with one of the dependent variables (perfor-
A factorial 2(reverberation: absorbing vs. reverberant)  2(lan- mance, perceived disturbance, ability to ignore the background
guage: native vs. foreign) repeated measures ANOVA on the self- noise, eagerness to go on with the task, self-estimated quality of
estimated quality of the collaboration as dependent variable the collaboration). Because noise sensitivity is an individual char-
revealed no significant main effect of the language of the back- acteristic of each participant, the impact of the noise sensitivity
ground speech (F(1,73) = 1.86, p = .177) and no significant main on performance and produced sound level, which both are vari-
effect of the reverberance of the room (F(1,73) = 0.02, p = .885). ables of couples, could not be checked.
No interaction effect of the reverberance of the study environment To verify the influence of changing the strategy of the couples to
and the language of the background speech on self-estimated qual- find the differences in the pictures, a factorial 2(reverberation:
ity of collaboration was found (F(1,73) = 0.10, p = .757). absorbing vs. reverberant)  2(language of background speech:
native vs. foreign)  2(strategy: no change vs. changing strategy)
3.2.3. Produced sound pressure levels repeated measures ANOVA was executed for all dependent vari-
A factorial 2(reverberation: absorbing vs. reverberant)  2(lan- ables. The results revealed no significant interactions of the change
guage: native vs. foreign) repeated measures ANOVA with pro- of strategy with one of the dependent variables.
duced sound pressure level as dependent variable revealed no
significant main effect of the language of the background speech 4. Discussion
(F(1,29) = 2.03, p = .164). On the other hand, a significant main
effect of reverberation of the study environment was revealed (F The aim of this study was to analyze the influence of back-
(1,29) = 93.09, p = .001, g2p = 0.762). Fig. 13 shows the impact of ground speech on performance of a student-collaboration task.
E. Braat-Eggen et al. / Applied Acoustics 154 (2019) 148–160 157

Therefore, participants had to carry out a collaboration task while because negotiating and discussing are important components of
being exposed to four different background sound scenarios and a a student-collaboration task [17]. The ’DiapixUK’ task is an exam-
quiet sound environment in an open-plan study environment. ple of a problem-solving collaboration task and contains therefore
Speech in the background sound environment was varying in also an important role for speech communication [63]. Due to less
semantic content by changing the language in the background acoustic absorption materials in the reverberant model of the
speech, and in intelligibility by changing the reverberation time study environment, not only the reverberation time but also the
and, in consequence the sound level in the room (Table 2). sound pressure level due to noise in a room will increase (Table 2)
The results show no significant influence of the background which will lead to more demanding communication circum-
sound scenarios on the performance of the participants of the ‘Dia- stances. As for communication it is well known that with increas-
pixUK’ collaboration task. However, the influence of the back- ing background noise, speakers will raise their speech level to
ground sound scenarios on perceived disturbance, the ability to guarantee a sufficient signal to noise ratio for their communication
ignore the background speech and the quality of the collaboration partner [51]. The raised speech levels of the participants can be
of the participants was significant. Also, the produced speech seen in Fig. 13. So, in this collaboration task, not only semantic cog-
sound levels of the participants were significantly influenced by nitive processes but also speech communication is important and
the background sound scenarios. For all the self-estimated vari- both sub-components of the complex collaboration task will
ables the quiet background sound scenario was most appreciated. respond different to room acoustic parameters such as reverbera-
tion time and background sound level.
4.1. Impact of reverberation time of the open-study environment on Semantic cognitive processes prefer background speech with
the dependent variables low intelligibility, a result of a long reverberation time in a room,
however, speech communication prefers a good signal to noise
The reverberation time of a room will affect the intelligibility of ratio, a result of a short reverberation time. It seems that the higher
background speech and therefore could influence the disturbance sound level of background noise, an additional acoustic result of a
and performance of a semantic task [8,12,39,40]. Although a longer longer reverberation time in a room, has a dominant influence on
reverberation time decreases the intelligibility of speech (Table 2), disturbance due to the importance of the vocal communication
no significant influence of the reverberation time on performance component in the collaboration task.
was found. These findings do not fit within a semantic
’interference-by-process’ account, as, in line with this account, 4.2. Impact of language of the background speech on the dependent
the performance of a semantic task, such as a collaboration task, variables
should increase due to less intelligible background speech com-
pared to a more intelligible signal [39,40]. After all, less intelligible No significant impact of the meaningfulness of background
background speech contains less semantic information compared speech on performance was found. On the other hand, the lan-
to a more intelligible signal, therefore, a less intelligible signal guage of the background speech showed to be significantly impor-
should be less disruptive [19]. Furthermore, a less intelligible tant for the self-estimated disturbance of participants while
speech signal will reasonably contain less relevant or interesting performing a collaboration task (Fig. 10a). Participants were signif-
information that can capture attention and, as explained by the icantly more disturbed by background speech in the mother ton-
framework of attentional capture, will disrupt performance less gue than by background speech in a foreign language. This is in
than a more intelligible signal [23]. accordance with the ’interference-by-process’ account and the
Table 2 shows a decrease of the STI value due to the longer framework of attentional capture. The cognitive processes used
reverberation time, qualified from ’fair’ to ’poor’, from ’excellent’ to automatically analyse the unintended meaningful background
to ’fair’ or from ’excellent’ to ’good’. According to a model of Hon- speech in the mother tongue will interfere with the similar seman-
gisto, predicting the effect of speech of varying intelligibility on tic cognitive processes involved in the execution of a semantic
work performance of different tasks [66], performance starts to based collaboration task. The background speech in the mother
decrease when STI exceeds 0.2 and the highest performance tongue will also capture more attention than the meaningless
decrease is reached when the STI is 0.6. Estimated STI values based background speech in the foreign language. These findings corre-
on LAeq are lower than 0.6 (Table 2) and therefore a change in per- spond with research on reading comprehension [5,6] and proof-
formance can be expected. Estimated STI values based on LA95 and reading [7,11] which also show an increasing disturbance by
the regular STI values vary above 0.6 and despite of a change of increasing meaningful speech.
intelligibility no change of performance is expected based on Hon- The language of the background speech can only have an influ-
gisto’s model. Some studies have shown that for some complex ence on performance and disturbance if the speech intelligibility is
tasks, like writing [12], word memory and mathematics [67], the sufficient. Although the estimated STI values (Table 2) give no
largest drop of performance occurs for even lower values of the unambiguous assessment of the level of intelligibility of the back-
STI than the original model of Hongisto [66] predicted. In Jahncke ground speech, the observed impact of the language of background
et al. [67] and Keus van de Poll et al. [12], the largest drop of per- speech on disturbance indicates intelligible background speech.
formance occurred between STI values of 0.23 and 0.34. This could
be an explanation of the absence of an improvement of perfor- 4.3. Impact of background sound scenarios on a collaboration task
mance in this experiment although in any case a significant
improvement between the quiet and all other sound scenarios No significant performance differences were found between the
can be expected. Nevertheless, these effects were not found. sound scenarios (Fig. 4). Besides the earlier mentioned restricted
An explanation for the increase of disturbance due to a higher STI diversity between the sound scenarios and the relatively high
reverberation time could be found in the importance of different STI values (>0.6), these outcomes may also be the result of a lim-
task components in this complex collaboration task. Although it ited number of participant couples (n = 37), the limited semantic
is impossible to determine the influence of noise on a complex complexity of the collaboration task or the consequence of a ceiling
composed task based on the effects of noise on sub-components effect the ’DiapixUK’ task due the limit of 12 differences that could
[68], the analysis of sub-components can give possible explana- be found in a puzzle. The latter would imply a non-discriminatory
tions of unexpected results found by testing realistic (composed) performance measure. However, the statistical results do not pro-
tasks. Communication by a speech dialog will be very important vide convincing evidence for this. The median value of the found
158 E. Braat-Eggen et al. / Applied Acoustics 154 (2019) 148–160

differences was rather high (median = 9) but the mean number of ation time in a room, has a dominant influence on disturbance due
participant couples with a maximum score of 12 was only 3 (8%). to the importance of the signal to noise ratio for the vocal commu-
Although, no significant differences in performance were found nication sub-component in the collaboration task.
during different sound scenarios, disturbance and ignorance of The results showed the quiet sound scenario to lead to the low-
noise were found to be significantly influenced by the sound sce- est level of disturbance, a result in line with the sound level effect,
narios. Self-estimated disturbance differentiates to a greater extent but also in line with the absence of semantic interference. There-
and therefore provides interesting additional data to performance fore, it is doubtful if an open-plan study environment is suitable
data. An explanation of a more differentiated self-estimated distur- for student collaboration tasks. To create good learning environ-
bance and less differentiated performance might be an extra effort ments applying absorbing materials will not be enough, environ-
investment of participants in solving a task due to feeling dis- ments where students can choose to work in quiet zones or a
rupted in an adverse sound environment. Schlittmeier et al. [69] quiet room, as in activity based offices, might be a possible
call this the ’reactive effort enhancement’, and this effect can lead solution.
to reduced performance differences [69,70]. The ’DiapixUK’ task is limited in difficulty and performance
The eagerness to work on the task was not significantly influ- scale which might have had some influence on the research out-
enced by the sound scenarios (Fig. 7), for all other self-estimated comes. Furthermore, this research also shows that a real and com-
parameters the quiet scenario was significantly the most preferred plex sound environment must be taken into account. For instance,
sound environment. Unfortunately, a quiet sound scenario is sel- an increasing reverberation time in an environment implies also an
dom the case in open-plan study environments. Workspaces for increasing sound level of the background noise and this combina-
one group would be the best environment to work on a collabora- tion can have consequences for disturbance and performance of
tion task. Acoustic separated sections within an open-plan study people in work environments. In our setting with three spatially
environment would also be a better solution, for instance by using separated background speakers located at nearby table groups
high sound screens. Using acoustic absorbing materials is not we observed a strong effect on vocal communication aspects. It is
enough to create an optimal acoustic environment for a collabora- well possible that this effect was smaller if we had chosen a larger
tion task in an open-plan study environment. distance between speakers and listener which would have resulted
in a lower level of background speech.
4.4. Impact of the noise sensitivity and strategy of participants on the Therefore, more research is needed on the influence of the effect
dependent variables of background speech on the performance and disturbance of a col-
laboration task in open plan study environments. Moreover, this
Although in previous research the influence of noise sensitivity study indicates it is worth to perform more linked research on real-
of students on disturbance by noise in open-plan study environ- istic complex tasks, in real acoustic sound environments to bridge
ments was established [2], no influence of the noise sensitivity the gap which exists between laboratory findings and applications
score on disturbance or performance was found in this study. or practical relevance.
The influence of whether or not using different strategies to find
the differences in the ten pictures on the dependent variables was
not significant. This confirms the robustness of the ‘DiapixUK’ test. Acknowledgements
No difference was found between the two groups, which means no
learning effect was identified for the group that changed the strat- The authors would like to thank Marc Kalee for his contribution to
egy during the experiment. the digital model of the open-plan study environment.

5. Conclusion and outlook


Funding
The hypothesis ’an increase of intelligibility and meaningful-
ness of the irrelevant background speech will lead to an increase
This work was supported by a Doctoral Grant for Teachers by
of disturbance and a decrease of performance of a collaboration
the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO) under
task’ is not supported by the results of this study. The sound sce-
Grant number 023.006.025.
narios with background speech with a combined language and
reverberation time intervention showed that an increase of intelli-
gibility and meaningfulness did not lead to an increase of distur-
bance and a decrease of performance of the collaboration task. Appendix A. Information regarding the acoustic models of the
Although sound scenarios with a longer reverberation time open-plan study environments
result in less intelligible background speech, these background
sound environments were the most disturbing. We argue that Figs. A1 and A2
the sound level, an additional acoustic result of a longer reverber- Table A1

Table A1
Most important materials and absorption coefficients used in the Odeon models.

Materials Absorption coefficient


125 Hz 250 Hz 500 Hz 1000 Hz 2000 Hz 4000 Hz
Sound absorbing model:
Ceiling: sound absorbing ceiling 0.40 0.85 0.99 0.90 0.99 0.99
Floor: carpet 0.05 0.10 0.30 0.70 0.90 0.75
Walls: perforated panels 0.39 0.94 0.92 0.68 0.69 0.58
Reverberant model:
Ceiling: concrete 0.02 0.03 0.03 0.03 0.04 0.07
Floor: linoleum 0.02 0.02 0.03 0.04 0.04 0.05
Walls: unperforated panels 0.08 0.11 0.05 0.03 0.02 0.03
E. Braat-Eggen et al. / Applied Acoustics 154 (2019) 148–160 159

[12] Keus van de Poll M, Ljung R, Odelius J, Sörqvist P. Disruption of writing by


background speech: The role of speech transmission index. Appl Acoust
2014;81:15–8.
[13] Montgomery SE. Library space assessment: User learning behaviours in the
library. J Acad Librarianship 2013;40:70–5.
[14] Beckers R, van der Voordt T, Dewulf G. A conceptual framework to identify
spatial implications of new ways of learning in higher education. Facilities
2015;33(1/2):2–19.
[15] Pääkkönen R, Vehviläinen T, Jokitulppo J, Niemi O, Nenonen S, Vinha J.
Acoustics and new learning environment – A case study. Appl Acoust
2015;100:74–8.
[16] McInnerney J, Robert TS. In: Collaborative or cooperative learning? Online
collaborative learning: Theory and practice. Hershey, PA: Information Science
Publishing; 2004. p. 203–14.
[17] Kozar O. Towards better group work: Seeing the difference between
cooperation and collaboration. English Teach Forum 2010;2:16–23.
[18] Jones DM, Tremblay S. Interference in memory by process or content? A reply
to Neath. Psychon Bull. Rev. 2000;7:550–8.
[19] Marsh JE, Hughes RW, Jones DM. Interference by process, not content,
determines semantic auditory distraction. Cognition 2009;110:23–38.
[20] Sörqvist P, Marsh JE. How concentration shields against distraction. Curr
Directions Psychol Sci 2015;24:267–72.
[21] Hughes RW, Vachon F, Jones DM. Disruption of short-term memory by
changing and deviant sounds: support for a duplex-mechanism account of
auditory distraction. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 2007;33:1050–61.
Fig. A1. Reverberation Time (T30) calculated for the absorbing and reverberant
[22] Hughes RW, Hurlstone MJ, Marsh JE, Vachon F, Jones DM. Cognitive control of
models (by Odeon) and measured in the experimental environment.
auditory distraction: impact of task difficulty, foreknowledge, and working
memory capacity supports duplex-mechanism account. J Exp Psychol Hum
Percept Perform 2013;39(2):539.
[23] Hughes RW. Auditory distraction: a duplex-mechanism account. PsyCh J
2014;3:30–41.
[24] Hughes RW, Vachon F, Jones DM. Auditory attentional capture during serial
recall: Violations at encoding of an algorithm-based neural model? J Exp
Psychol: Learn Memory, Cognition 2005;31(4):736.
[25] Conway AR, Cowan N, Bunting MF. The cocktail party phenomenon revisited:
the importance of working memory capacity. Psychon Bull Rev 2001;8
(2):331–5.
[26] Dalton P, Hughes RW. Auditory attentional capture: Implicit and explicit
approaches. Psychol Res 2014;78(3):313–20.
[27] Lange EB. Disruption of attention by irrelevant stimuli in serial recall. J Mem
Lang 2005;53(4):513–31.
[28] Röer JP, Bell R, Buchner A. Self-relevance increases the irrelevant sound effect:
attentional disruption by one’s own name. J Cognit Psychol 2013;25
(8):925–31.
[29] Sörqvist P. Effects of aircraft noise and speech on prose memory: What role for
working memory capacity? J Environ Psychol 2010;30(1):112–8.
[30] Banbury SP, Macken WJ, Tremblay S, Jones DM. Auditory distraction and short-
term memory: phenomena and practical implications. Hum Factors 2001;43
(1):12–29.
[31] Beaman CP. The irrelevant sound phenomenon revisited: What role for
working memory capacity? J Exp Psychol: Learn Memory Cognition 2004;30
Fig. A2. Open-plan study environment, floor 3 Vertigo building, Eindhoven
(5):1106.
University of Technology.
[32] Bell R, Dentale S, Buchner A, Mayr S. ERP correlates of the irrelevant sound
effect. Psychophysiology 2010;47(6):1182–91.
[33] Campbell T, Beaman CP, Berry DC. Auditory memory and the irrelevant sound
References effect: further evidence for changing-state disruption. Memory 2002;10
(3):199–214.
[1] Kim J, Dear R. Workspace satisfaction: The privacy-communication trade-off in [34] Hughes RW, Tremblay S, Jones DM. Disruption by speech of serial short-term
open-plan offices. J Environ Psychol 2013;36:18–26. memory: The role of changing-state vowels. Psychon Bull Rev 2005;12
[2] Pierrette M, Parizet E, Chevret P, Chatillon J. Noise effect on comfort in open- (5):886–90.
space offices: development of an assessment questionnaire. Ergonomics [35] Jones DM, Macken WJ. Irrelevant tones produce an irrelevant speech effect:
2015;58(1):96–106. Implications for phonological coding in working memory. J Exp Psychol Learn
[3] Braat-Eggen PE, Heijst A, Hornikx M, Kohlrausch A. Noise disturbance in open- Mem Cogn 1993;19(2):369.
plan study environments: A field study on noise sources, student tasks and [36] Macken W, Tremblay S, Alford D, Jones D. Attentional selectivity in short-term
room acoustic parameters. Ergonomics 2017;60(9):1297–314. memory: similarity of process, not similarity of content, determines
[4] Scannell L, Hodgson M, Villarreal JGM, Gifford R. The role of acoustics in the disruption. Int J Psychol 1999;34(5–6):322–7.
perceived suitability of, and well-being in, informal learning spaces. Environ [37] Salamé P, Baddeley A. The effects of irrelevant speech on immediate free recall.
Behav 2016;48(6):769–95. Bull Psychonomic Soc 1990;28(6):540–2.
[5] Martin RC, Wogalter MS, Forlano JG. Reading comprehension in the presence of [38] A. Akmajian, RA Demers, AK Farmer, RM Harnish. Linguistics: An Introduction
unattended speech and music. J Mem Lang 1988;27(4):382–98. to Language and Communication. MIT 2001 Press. ISBN 0-262-51123-1.
[6] Oswald CJ, Tremblay S, Jones DM. Disruption of comprehension by the [39] Jones DM, Hughes RW, Macken WJ. Auditory distraction and serial memory:
meaning of irrelevant sound. Memory 2000;8(5):345–50. the avoidable and the ineluctable. Noise Health 2010;12:201–9.
[7] Jones DM, Miles C, Page J. Disruption of proofreading by irrelevant speech: [40] Marsh JE, Jones DM. Cross-modal distraction by background speech: what role
effects of attention, arousal or memory? Appl Cognit Psychol 1990;4 for meaning? Noise Health 2010;1:210–6.
(2):89–108. [41] Schlittmeier SJ, Weißgerber T, Kerber S, Fastl H, Hellbrück J. Algorithmic
[8] Venetjoki N, Kaarlela-Tuomaala A, Kerskinen E, Hongisto V. The effect of modeling of the Irrelevant Sound Effect (ISE) by the hearing sensation
speech and speech intelligibility on task performance. Ergonomics 2006;49 fluctuation strength. Attention, Percept Psychophysics 2012;74(1):194–203.
(11):1068–91. [42] Belojević G, Öhrström E, Rylander R. Effects of noise on mental performance
[9] Smith-Jackson TL, Klein KW. Open-plan office: task performance and mental with regard to subjective noise sensitivity. Int Arch Occup Environ Health
workload. J Environ Psychol 2009;29(2):279–89. 1992;64(4):293–301.
[10] Halin N, Marsh JE, Haga A, Holmgren M, Sörqvist P. Effects of speech on [43] Smith A, Stansfeld S. Aircraft noise exposure, noise sensitivity, and everyday
proofreading: can task-engagement manipulations shield against distraction? errors. Environ Behavior 1986;18(2):214–26.
J Exp Psychol Appl 2014;20(1):69–80. [44] Waye KP, Bengtsson J, Rylander R, Hucklebridge F, Evans P, Clow A. Low
[11] Sörqvist P, Nöstl A, Halin N. Disruption of writing processes by the semanticity frequency noise enhances cortisol among noise sensitive subjects during work
of background speech. Scand J Psychol 2012;53:97–102. performance. Life Sci 2002;70(7):745–58.
160 E. Braat-Eggen et al. / Applied Acoustics 154 (2019) 148–160

[45] Zimmer K, Ellermeier W. Psychometric properties of four measures of noise [57] Houben R, Koopman J, Luts H, Wagener KC, Wieringen A, Verschuure H, et al.
sensitivity: a comparison. J Environ Psychol 1999;19(3):295–302. Development of a Dutch matrix sentence set to assess speech intelligibility in
[46] Houtgast T, Steeneken HJM, Plomp R. Predicting speech intelligibility in rooms noise. Int J Audiol 2014;53(10):760–3.
from the modulation transfer function I. General room acoustics. Acustica [58] IEC 60268-16:2011 Sound system equipment – Part 16: Objective rating of
1980;46(1):60–72. speech intelligibility by speech transmission index. International
[47] Beaman CP, Holt NJ. Reverberant auditory environments: the effects of Electrotechnical Commission, Geneva, Switzerland (2009).
multiple echoes on distraction by irrelevant speech. Appl Cognit Psychol [59] Schroeder MR. Modulation Transfer Functions: Definition and Measurement.
2007;21:1077–90. Acustica 1981;49:179–82.
[48] ISO 338-3:2012. Acoustics - Measurements of room acoustic parameters - Part [60] Zaglauer M, Drotleff H, Liebl A. Background babble in open-plan offices: a
3: Open-plan offices. Brussels: International Organization for Standardization natural masker of disruptive speech? Appl Acoust 2017;118:1–7.
[49] R.H.C. Wenmaekers, C.C.J.M. Hak. Spatial decay rate of speech in open plan [61] American National Standards Institute (1997). ANSI-S3.5, Methods for
offices: the use of D2,S and Lp, A,S,4m as building requirements Euronoise Calculation of the Speech Intelligibility Index (American National Standards
2015, the 10th European Congress and Exposition on Noise Control Institute, New York).
Engineering 2015 Maastricht, The Netherlands 1e6. [62] T Vervoort, M. Vercammen Background noise level to determine the speech
[50] Keranen J, Hongisto V, Oliva D, Hakala J. The effect of different room acoustic privacy in open plan offices. Euronoise 2015, 3 May - 3 June, Maastricht.
elements on spatial decay of speech - a laboratory experiment. In: Broth_anek [63] Baker R, Hazan V. DiaPixUK: task materials for the elicitation of multiple
M, editor. Proceedings of Euronoise 2012. Prague Czech Republic: European spontaneous speech dialogs. Behav Res Methods 2011;43:761–70.
Acoustics Association; 2012. p. 10624–3629. [64] Weinstein ND. Individual differences in reactions to noise: A longitudinal
[51] Pick Jr HL, Siegel GM, Fox PW, Garber SR, Kearney JK. Inhibiting the Lombard study in a college dormitory. J Appl Psychol 1978;63(4):458–66.
effect. J Acoust Soc Am 1989;85(2):894–900. [65] Nordic S, Palmquist E, Cleason AS. Metric properties and normative data for
[52] Rindel JH, Christensen CL, Gade AC. Dynamic sound source for simulating the brief noise and electromagnetic field sensitivity scales. Scand J Public Health
Lombard effect in room acoustic modeling software. New York, 2013;41:293–301.
USA: Internoise; 2012. p. 19–22. [66] Hongisto V. A model predicting the effect of speech of varying intelligibility on
[53] Rindel JH. Computer simulation techniques for acoustical design of rooms. work performance. Indoor Air 2005;15:458–68.
Acoust Aust 1995;23(3):81–6. [67] Jahncke H, Hongisto V, Virjonen P. Cognitive performance during irrelevant
[54] Meng Z, Zhao F, He M. The just noticeable difference of noise length and speech: effects of speech intelligibility and office-task characteristics. Appl
reverberation perception. International Symposium on Communications and Acoust 2013;74(3):307–16.
Information Technologies, ISCIT; 2006. p. 418–21. [68] Sörqvist P. On interpretation and task selection: the sub-component
[55] Martellotta F. The just noticeable difference of centre time and clarity index in hypothesis of cognitive noise effects. Front Psychol 2015;5:1–3.
large reverberant spaces. J Acoust Soc Am 2010;128(2):654–63. [69] Schlittmeier SJ, Hellbrück J, Thaden R, Vorländer M. The impact of background
[56] Hagerman B. Sentences for testing speech intelligibility in noise. Scand Audiol speech varying in intelligibility: Effects on cognitive performance and
1982;11(2):79–87. perceived disturbance. Ergonomics 2008;51(5):719–36.
[70] Kahneman D. Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall; 1973.

View publication stats

You might also like