Obedinta - Responsabilitate

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

Peace and Conflict: Journal of Peace Psychology © 2014 American Psychological Association

2014, Vol. 20, No. 2, 124 –134 1078-1919/14/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/pac0000021

Bad Examples: How Thinking About Blind Obedience Can Induce


Responsibility and Courage

Verena Graupmann Dieter Frey


DePaul University Ludwig Maximilian University

The effect of watching a documentary film about Milgram’s obedience experiment on


This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

personal responsibility, civil courage, and societal engagement in a student sample was
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

examined in two studies. In Study 1, after watching the film, the perception of personal
responsibility and the inclination to show civil courage were increased after watching
the film. The change in personal responsibility was associated with the experience of
higher negative affect after watching the film. In Study 2, instructions for students’
reflections on the film were varied from writing about reasons for the behavior of
Milgram’s participants as originating in the person versus in the situation. Intentions to
get involved in a volunteering project teaching civil courage to high-school students
were highest for those who reflected on reasons for Milgram’s participants’ behavior
originating in the person.

Keywords: civil courage, obedience, responsibility, attributions, Milgram experiment

In 2010, French television broadcasted a doc- 1974) famous series of experiments on obedi-
umentary on a fictitious game show that in- ence. In Milgram’s original study, participants
volved game show contestants giving increas- learned that they were involved in an experi-
ingly heavy electric shocks to another person ment about learning and punishment and under-
when the other person failed to give a correct went an allegedly random draw of who was
answer to a question. The media uproar that going to be the learner and who was going to be
followed the show was due to the alarming the teacher in the experiment. In fact, the actual
results of the show. The show contestants con- participant received the role of the teacher
tinued to administer electric shocks, even when whereas the other participant, a confederate of
the person, supposedly attached to the elec- the experimenter, took the role of the learner.
trodes, screamed in pain and begged them to The teacher was instructed to administer paired-
stop (Jarett, 2010). Learning about the extreme association word-learning tasks to the learner by
consequences of obedience never fails to shock intercom. For every mistake made, the teacher
the public and spark discussion. How do such was instructed to deliver electric shocks in in-
media-induced learning experiences affect the
crements to the learner by means of the inter-
willingness to assume personal responsibility
face of a shock machine, showing a range of
when basic values are violated?
shocks ranging from 15 to 450 V, the highest
The game show staged for the documentary
was based on Stanley Milgram’s (1963, 1965, stage indicated by “xxx.” There were no actual
electric shocks coming from the machine. The
learners in the experiment made many mistakes,
putting the participants under the pressure of
administering increasingly high shocks. When-
VERENA GRAUPMANN, Department of Psychology, DePaul ever a participant wanted to discontinue the
University.
DIETER FREY, Department of Psychology, Ludwig Maxi- experiment, the experimenter told them to con-
milian University, Munich Germany. tinue. Only when a participant had ignored four
CORRESPONDENCE CONCERNING THIS ARTICLE should be requests to continue was the experiment abort-
addressed to Verena Graupmann, DePaul University, Col-
lege of Science and Health, Department of Psychology,
ed. Otherwise, it was ended after the participant
2219 North Kenmore, Chicago, IL, 60614. E-mail: had pressed the button with the highest voltage
vgraupma@depaul.edu for the third time. In the described version of the
124
OBEDIENCE, RESPONSIBILITY, AND COURAGE 125

experiment (Milgram, 1974, Experiment 5), that exposes the degree to which participants
65% of participants carried on with the proce- gave up personal responsibility and the respec-
dure up to that point. The Milgram study has tive consequences (DeVos, 2009).
been replicated in many countries, finding very Despite the debates and criticisms in the lit-
similar results (e.g., Scotland [Burley & erature, the Milgram study is intriguing not only
McGuinness, 1977], Jordan [Shanab & Yahya, for its alarming results in regard to how easy it
1978], Spain [Miranda, Caballero, Gomez, & is to make people perform actions that are very
Zamorano, 1981], Holland [Meeus & Raaij- likely to be at odds with their values but (also)
makers, 1987], and Austria [Schurz, 1985]). for the simple fact of its immense notoriety
Not only the recent French documentary, but across and beyond the field of psychology,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

(also) a replication of the experiment from 2009 probably being one of the most famous psycho-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

(Burger, 2009), suggests the validity and actu- logical experiments. Given the effect of its rev-
ality of such alarming tendencies of obedience elations about human nature on public interest
and conformity in modern Western society. and discussion, which caused indignation and
Burger’s study was conducted with Californians shock as well as attempts to verify them by
recruited from different areas of life, and he replication again and again, there is an obvious
found very similar results in an—for ethical potential in making people aware of the exper-
reasons—abbreviated version of the original ex- iment’s implications. For example, being aware
periment that entailed recorded screaming from of automatic stereotyping and its negative con-
75 V and only going up to an alleged 150 V sequences has shown to be an ingredient in
(because 79% of Milgram’s participants that controlling those stereotypes (Bargh, 1999;
had continued up to 150 V followed through Lowery, Hardin, & Sinclair, 2001). This is con-
with the punishment until the end). In Burger’s gruent with the assumptions of social learning
recent study, 70% of participants were ready to theory (Bandura, 1975) that observing the neg-
go past 150 V, making it difficult to attribute ative consequences of a behavior should lead to
Milgram’s original results to different obedi- a lower probability of that behavior later shown
ence standards in the 1960s. by the observer.
Therefore, what happens when somebody
Effect of the Milgram Study learns about the Milgram experiment by watch-
ing a documentary film about it? Does such a
The Milgram experiment has received much learning experience raise feelings of personal
criticism, a great part of which was directed at responsibility as opposed to the blind obedience
the obvious ethical problems the study poses in of the majority showcased in the experiment?
regards to human participants’ well-being: Mil- Does it increase a person’s inclination to show
gram’s participants had to endure a great civil courage? Does a person’s interpretation of
amount of emotional stress in the experiment the situational and personal components of Mil-
itself and in the aftermath when having to rec- gram’s participants’ behavior have an effect on
oncile value-discrepant actual behavior (even if their subsequent behavioral tendencies? We ex-
under artificial conditions) with the desire for pected that watching a film about the Milgram
general moral integrity (e.g., Baumrind, 1964). experiment, showing models of obedience as
In addition, the internal logic of the study has well as models of disobedience, would affect
been questioned in terms of how the inconsis- perceptions of personal responsibility as well as
tencies in the situation (e.g., giving little reason the inclination to show civil courage and actual
for why the role of the teacher was not simply behavioral intentions to assume responsibility.
taken by the actual experimenter if what the
study allegedly looked at was only the learner’s The Role of Affect
learning behavior) made it more likely for par-
ticipants to obey because the usual context for Negative affect serves as indication to the
judgment was taken away (Orne & Holland, individual that something is not right (Schwarz,
1968; Ross & Nisbett, 1991). On the other hand, 1990). Consequently, negative affect has been
it has been argued that it is exactly this so-called proposed to play an important role in self-
“pact of ignorance” (i.e., the scientistic aspect regulation (Carver & Scheier, 1990). In the do-
of the situation) leading to “fake obedience” main of controlling automatic stereotypes in
126 GRAUPMANN AND FREY

favor of an equal treatment of every one, neg- However, when people perceive themselves
ative affect has been looked at as a consequence as part of a group that is responsible for helping
of becoming aware of one’s own automatic in a situation, they are more likely to actually
stereotyping, which stands in contrast to an in- show responsible behavior. In a study by Falo-
dividual’s desired nondiscriminatory behavior. mir-Pichastor and colleagues, not knowledge
In this context, negative affect functions as a about the flu virus and the vaccination, but the
cue for inhibiting automatic stereotyping of Af- identification with the own professional group,
rican Americans by White participants (Monte- predicted whether nurses decided to get a flu
ith, Ashburn-Nardo, Voils, & Czopp, 2002). In vaccination. This behavior was associated with
line with the notion that negative affect can be the motive of protecting patients (not the self)
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

used as a cue to the individual in guiding future from the virus (Falomir-Pichastor, Toscani, &
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

behavior while becoming aware of core values Huyghues Despointes, 2009).


such as responsibility, we expected negative Likewise, relating strongly to the effect of the
affect in response to watching a film about the social situation, behavioral models and norms
Milgram experiment to moderate the film’s ef- have shown to be important factors affecting
fect on responsibility. personal responsibility in a replication of the
In a related vein, research on the vicarious original experiment. Here, Milgram (1974)
experience of cognitive dissonance (Cooper & could show that when observing another partic-
Hogg, 2007) suggests that the arousal that is ipant disobeying to continue with the shocking,
associated with dissonance motivation and gen- the rate of obedience fell from 85% to 50%.
erally experienced as negative affect (Higgins, In a related vein, it has been demonstrated
Rhodewalt, & Zanna, 1979) is also experienced that playing prosocial games is associated with
when another person, perceived as an in-group more actual prosocial behavior (Greitemeyer &
member, engages in inconsistent behavior (Nor- Osswald, 2009, 2010, 2011). Furthermore, stud-
ton, Monin, Cooper, & Hogg, 2003). To the ies on superhero priming have shown that being
extent to which people identify with Milgram’s exposed to individuals acting responsibly leads
participants in the film, they might therefore to more prosocial behavior than being exposed
experience negative affect as a consequence of to the general idea of acting responsibly (Nelson
dissonance, resulting in a desire to rectify the & Norton, 2005).
imbalance by affirming their own responsibility
and civil courage inclination (cf. extent of en- Civil Courage
gagement in self-affirmation to reduce disso-
nance; Tesser, Crepaz, Collins Cornell, & What really makes Milgram’s experiments so
Beach, 2000). relevant for societal discourse is the clear result
of a lack of civil courage (i.e., an inclination to
intervene and/or oppose a majority opinion in a
Responsibility situation on the basis of one’s authentic values
while taking into account disapproval by au-
The social psychological take on responsibil- thorities and social rejection; Jonas & Brand-
ity is located in the domain of helping behavior. stätter, 2004; Lopez, O’Byrne, Koetting, & Pe-
The so-called “bystander effect” (Darley & La- terson, 2003). It has been empirically
tané, 1968) describes the phenomenon that peo- established that in contradistinction to pure
ple tend to watch an emergency without helping helping behavior, civilly courageous behavior is
with greater likelihood if more people (bystand- characterized by the negative social conse-
ers) are present. The central construct to explain quences the actor is willing to face (Greit-
this pattern is diffusion of responsibility. Ac- emeyer, Fischer, Kastenmüller, & Frey, 2006).
cording to Darley and Latané (1968), the re- Civil (or moral) courage relates to the motiva-
sponsibility of a person to intervene in an emer- tion to protect democratic and humane values.
gency is diffused when there are others because A lack of civil courage in a society is generally
then the person is not the only one to take associated with the oppression of minorities and
action, to be blamed for not taking action, and it the danger of a blind trust in authorities, result-
is no longer certain that action has not already ing in an imbalance of power in a society (Os-
been taken by somebody else. swald, Frey, & Streicher, 2012). Milgram’s re-
OBEDIENCE, RESPONSIBILITY, AND COURAGE 127

search program was motivated by the desire to spective might result in the experience of more
understand how a collectively committed crime control over the situation and therefore more
against humanity such as the Holocaust could motivation to behave according to a standard of
be explained using a social psychological per- personal responsibility.
spective. The findings have added to under-
standing how the power of a social situation can The Current Research
lead to atrocious behavior in people who deem
themselves as innocent research participants Several psychological mechanisms are at
just following the instructions of an authority. work not only in the actual Milgram study but
However, none of the study variations show (also) in how it is perceived by people who
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

100% obedience (Milgram, 1965). Instead, the learn about it in the media or in educational
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

findings for those who did disobey clearly show contexts. Affective responses and active cogni-
that—apart from the experimentally devised tive processing of the value-discrepant behav-
variations in the situations that increased dis- iors shown by normal citizens under particular
obedience—what differentiated those who did circumstances can be assumed to play a role in
not follow the instructions from those who did how the awareness of Milgram’s findings af-
was the presence or lack of civil courage in fects individual thinking and behavior in terms
the individual: Those who did not obey with of responsibility and civil courage, the very
the instructions to carry on had to engage in qualities that seemed to be lacking in most of
behavior that opposed the implicit rules of the Milgram’s participants.
experimental situation against conformity To examine the effect of viewing a film about
pressures. How does learning about Mil- the Milgram experiment, we collected the ques-
gram’s study via watching a film about it tionnaire responses on feelings of responsibility
affect individual inclination to civil courage? and the inclination to show civil courage before
and after student participants had watched the
Attributions and Willingness to Take film. In a second study, we asked the students to
Responsibility elaborate on situational versus dispositional rea-
sons for the behavior of Milgram’s participants.
People’s judgment of and behavior toward We also assessed the effect of the induced at-
others who need help is influenced strongly by tributional style on behavioral intentions to pro-
the causes they think have led to the situation mote civil courage.
(i.e., by their attributions). When a situation is
attributed to a person as opposed to situation,
people are less likely to be willing to help Study 1: Responsibility and Civil Courage
alleviate the negative consequence brought
about (Reisenzein, 1986; Weiner, 1980). How Participants and Design
do attributions of events more generally affect
Fifty-two (46 female, 6 male; age M ⫽ 22.6)
behavioral intentions? In other words, how do
undergraduate students participated in the con-
people’s subjective theories about Milgram’s
text of an introductory psychology lecture at a
participants’ behavior influence their own be-
German university. The study was based on a
havioral intentions in terms of societal engage-
repeated-measures design; that is, data were
ment? When people watch the film about Mil-
collected at two points of time— before watch-
gram’s experiment, it is expected that they will
ing the film and directly after watching. The
not approve of participants’ compliant behavior.
dependent variables were at both times respon-
A situational interpretation of the behavior im-
sibility and civil courage. After watching the
plies that there is not much one could control
film (i.e., at the second measurement point)
about the behavior if the cause lies in the situ-
positive and negative affect were measured.
ation itself. However, dispositionally attributing
the behavior allows the observer to distance the Procedure and Materials
own person from the observed person. Seeing
examples of people showing disobedient behav- Milgram film. The students watched a
ior might even strengthen the inclination to not German 30-min black and white documentary
identify with those who obey. The latter per- about the version of the Milgram experiment
128 GRAUPMANN AND FREY

conducted by the Max-Planck Society in Mu- ative PANAS scores. It is interesting to note
nich, Germany, in 1970, titled “Abraham—An that although there were no significant correla-
Experiment.” The movie introduces the setup of tions with positive affect, there was a significant
the Milgram experiment and shows several par- positive correlation between the increase in
ticipants go through with the procedure, but feelings of responsibility after viewing the film
then it also shows participants who disobey the and the experienced negative affect, r ⫽ .32,
instructions and protest but then continue or p ⫽ .009. This indicates that the increase in
protest and then discontinue the experiment. feeling responsible after viewing the film was
Responsibility. Personal acceptance of re- associated with the negative affect experienced
sponsibility was measured as a within-subjects after the viewing.1
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

factor at both points of time with seven items Civil courage. The inclination to show
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

asking participants to indicate their responses to civil courage had been assessed before or di-
questions such as “ How responsible do you feel rectly after viewing the film, and, consistent
for solving societal problems?”, “How respon- with our hypotheses, there was greater inclina-
sible do you feel in respect to the maintenance tion to show civil courage after viewing the film
of human rights?”, and “How responsible do (M ⫽ 4.24, SD ⫽ 0.75) than before (M ⫽ 3.83,
you feel to help a stranger in an emergency?” SD ⫽ 0.94), t(51) ⫽ 1.76, p ⫽ .042, d ⫽ .48.
(1 ⫽ strongly disagree; 11 ⫽ strongly agree;
␣ ⫽ .67). Discussion
Civil courage. The inclination to show
civil courage was assessed with a scale devel- Seeing a film about other people’s irrespon-
oped by Kastenmüller, Greitemeyer, Fischer, sible behavior in the Milgram experiment had a
and Frey (2007) that asks participants to indi- positive effect on feelings of personal responsi-
cate how likely they would be to intervene in 13 bility and on the inclination to show civil cour-
situations requiring civil courage (i.e., helping a age. Providing some initial evidence for an in-
person or stand up for a value while risking crease in variables crucial for civic engagement
negative consequences for the own person). Ex- after watching a film about the Milgram exper-
amples of the situations include “Some of your iment, the results from Study 1 point at the role
colleagues are trying to isolate another col- of negative affect in fostering aspirations of
league” and “While riding on a train, you wit- responsibility and courage. This suggests that
ness a group of youngsters loudly exchanging negative evaluations of other people’s behavior
racist jokes.” (0 ⫽ not likely at all; 6 ⫽ very as depicted in the documentary are associated
likely; ␣ ⫽ .81). Civil courage was assessed with a motivation to be more responsible and
before or after watching the film. courageous personally.
Positive and negative affect. Participants’ However, the results clearly are affected by
affective reactions to watching the movie were social desirability at both points of measure-
assessed with the Positive and Negative Affect ment: Even at the first point of measurement
Schedule (PANAS; Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, students are likely to have a motivation to ap-
1988; positive affect: ␣ ⫽ .77; negative affect: pear responsible and socially courageous, and
␣ ⫽ .84). especially after having viewed the film they
might feel compelled to display the effect they
Results assume the film was supposed to have on them.
In a second study we explored how different
Responsibility. Feelings of responsibility styles of reflection about the film affect actual
were slightly higher after viewing the film (M ⫽
8.21, SD ⫽ 1.25) than before (M ⫽ 8.00, SD ⫽ 1
In a pilot study (N ⫽ 84; 70 female; Mage ⫽ 22.5 years),
1.25), t(51) ⫽ 2.21, p ⫽ .016, d ⫽ .17. we compared participants who had indicated responsibility
We also looked at how the difference be- either before and after or only after viewing the film. There
tween feelings of responsibility before and after were no significant differences in postfilm responsibility
viewing the film were related to affective reac- (p ⫽ .22) or postfilm civil courage inclination (p ⫽ .58),
suggesting that measuring responsibility at two points of
tions to the film and therefore correlated the time per se does not increase the extent to which partici-
difference score for responsibility after minus pants respond positively on measures of responsibility and
before viewing the film with positive and neg- inclination to show civil courage.
OBEDIENCE, RESPONSIBILITY, AND COURAGE 129

behavioral tendencies toward taking responsi- whereas a situational attribution emphasizes


bility. We were especially interested in how that the observed behavior is beyond individ-
attributing the cause of the “bad example” be- ual control.
havior displayed in the film to personal versus
situational reasons in the reflection affected how Participants and Design
the film influenced behavioral tendencies.
One-hundred and eleven (78 female, 32 male,
Study 2: Attributional Style and 1 missing data; age: M ⫽ 27 years) undergrad-
uate students participated in the context of an
Behavioral Intentions
introductory psychology lecture at a German
university. One week after participants had
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

The effect of watching a documentary on


watched the film about the Milgram experiment,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

experimentally induced failure to act morally


responsibly should be influenced by how it is students were instructed to engage in different
interpreted and reflected afterward. Previous styles of reflecting about the film that varied in
research has established that information attribution style (dispositional, personal, con-
given as a frame of an observed situation can trol). We assessed participants’ willingness to
have a huge effect on how this situation is volunteer in a project of teaching civil courage
evaluated and remembered. For instance, at schools.
knowing that a message is intended to per- Procedure and Materials
suade the audience to a behavior can affect
perception of that message even if it is given Milgram film. The students first watched
after the message has been communicated the German documentary “Abraham—An Ex-
(Silvia, 2006). Memory and the long-term periment” about the Milgram experiment in the
consequences of message processing have week before participating in the study as part of
been shown to be extremely malleable de- an introductory psychology lecture.
pending on the questions asked to retrieve Attributional style. Participants read one
memories of an event (e.g., Loftus, 1975). of the following instructions to reflect upon
In Study 2, we compared instructions of the movie, inducing an attributional style:
reflecting on the film, focusing on the situa- “Please elaborate on why one might argue
tional determinants or on the characteristics that it was primarily due to Milgram’s partic-
of the individual leading to the behavior of ipants’ own responsibility that the alleged
Milgram’s participants. These instructions are shocks were administered” (dispositional at-
intended to affect the attributional style used tribution style), “Please elaborate on why one
when thinking about the situation. Disposi- might argue that it was primarily due to the
tional attributions have been studied exten- extreme situation that the alleged shocks were
sively in regards to how observers construct administered” (situational attribution style),
them in light of the available situation (e.g., or “Please describe how the documentary film
Reeder & Brewer, 1979). It has further been was presented dramaturgically” (control con-
well established that situational attributions dition).
for causes of events are more associated with Behavior and behavioral intentions. At
willingness to help whereas personal attribu- the end of the questionnaire, there was a
tions lead to fewer helping intentions (Reisen- separate page on which participants were
zein, 1986). However, it is unclear how own asked to leave their e-mail addresses if they
future behavior is affected by attributional were interested in volunteering as trainers in
style; that is, when ascribing morally discrep- civil courage training at local schools (which
ant behavior to the individual observed rather the Psychology Department is actually con-
than the situation, how does this behavior ducting; i.e., this manipulation was a genuine
influence the observer’s future behavior in a effort to recruit interested trainers). They
related domain? We predict that a personal were informed that their e-mail information
attribution of the morally discrepant behavior would be separated from the other data to
will be associated with more intentions to act keep their answers anonymous. This informa-
responsibly oneself in the future because it tion served the additional purpose of empha-
allows detachment from the observed person sizing that this was indeed an unrelated proj-
130 GRAUPMANN AND FREY

ect (the credibility of this project being nificant, ␹2 ⫽ 3.75, p ⫽ .044; also the compar-
unrelated to the study was validated by the ison between situational and dispositional attri-
reception of a considerable number of e-mails butions conditions, yielded a significant
after the study that requested details regard- difference, ␹2 ⫽ 4.20, p ⫽ .035, whereas the
ing the selection for participation in the train- control and situational attributions conditions
ing project). Also, we asked participants to did not differ, ␹2 ⬍ 0.40, p ⬎ .52.
indicate how likely they thought they were to Looking at how likely participants thought
actually get involved in the trainings on a they would be to get involved in a volunteering
scale from 1 (not at all likely) to 11 (ex- program, such as the one described, we found
tremely likely). that those who had written about dispositional
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

attributions for why Milgram’s participants


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Results acted the way they did expressed the highest


personal likelihood to get involved in volunteer-
Samples of attribution narratives.
ing to train civil courage, (M ⫽ 8.38, SD ⫽
Dispositional attribution style. “Possibility
2.60) whereas those who had written about sit-
to stop the participation was always there. Ev-
uational reasons expressed less personal likeli-
erybody has a conscience. No absolute order—
hood (M ⫽ 7.09, SD ⫽ 2.95), t(84) ⫽ 20.49,
participants did not have to show obedience and
there would have been no negative conse- p ⬍ .001, d ⫽ .46. Those who wrote about
quences”; “There was no actual obligation for dramaturgical style lay in between the two ex-
the participants to execute the electric shocks, perimental conditions (M ⫽ 7.37, SD ⫽ 2.60)—
they were merely told to continue by the exper- not significantly different from the dispositional
imenter. If they had stopped, there would not attribution style condition, p ⬎ .12, but margin-
have been any negative consequences. The par- ally different from the situational attribution
ticipants had to press the button that gave the style condition, t(84) ⫽ 1.9, p ⫽ .059, d ⫽ .39.2
shocks to the alleged ‘learner’ themselves.”
Situational attribution style. “People are Discussion
not responsible for their behavior in the situa-
tion of a scientific experiment, since they cannot In Study 2, students were more likely to leave
assume that the experimenter would allow their e-mail address and therefore more likely to
something bad to happen”; “Pressure from ex- get involved with a volunteering program when
perimenter. No possibility to reflect (time!). they had reflected on the Milgram experiment as
Trust in research/ science. Persuasive power of represented in the film in terms of the personal
experimenter. No visual representation of vic- responsibility of Milgram’s participants.
tim/ no eye-contact/gaze. One goes through all We have conceptualized writing about “per-
the steps of the experiment (without thinking sonal responsibility” as making a dispositional
about it, like a “fish in the stream”).” attribution. However, although it implies that
Main analyses. Forty-four percent (i.e., 40 the individual himself or herself made a deci-
people) left their e-mail address to be contacted sion to act in a certain way, this decision might
about volunteering in civil-courage training in not only be influenced by personality traits but
schools. This was dependent on the attribution (also) by other factors such as experiences
condition they had been assigned to, reaching growing up or a particular belief system. On the
marginal significance, ␹2 ⫽ 5.42, p ⫽ .066. other hand, thinking about the personal respon-
Only among those participants who had written sibility might have induced a sense of counter-
about dispositional attributions did more (20) factual thinking (Roese, 1997). These counter-
than probabilistically expected (14) indicate factuals might have served as guidance to what
their e-mail address. In the control condition should have happened and how the current par-
and the situational attribution condition, how- ticipants should behave.
ever fewer than expected (control: 16; situa-
tional: 14) actually indicated their e-mail ad- 2
The decrease in the size of the sample here is due to
dresses (control: 13; situational: 11). some participants not leaving their e-mail address or not
Comparing the control with the dispositional filling out the last item regarding their general likelihood to
attributions condition, this difference was sig- participate in such a project.
OBEDIENCE, RESPONSIBILITY, AND COURAGE 131

Without direct additional measures on how finding is reflected further in more general ob-
much control participants attributed to the obe- servations regarding the function of negative
dient participants in the Milgram study or how affect, in which negative emotions are defined
much counterfactual thinking was evoked, it is as signaling that some action is required to
difficult to conclude which cognitive route was uphold efficient interaction with the social and
taken here. However, the content of partici- physical environment (Rozin & Royzman,
pants’ writing in response to the personal re- 2001).
sponsibility prompt that sought to induce a dis- In a meta-analysis, mood induction via film
positional attributional style seems to point to a has emerged as the most efficient method of
greater reliance on the idea of actual control in manipulating affective states (Westermann,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

the situation and lack of real situational forces Spies, Stahl, & Hesse, 1996). This suggests
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

in the experimental situation than to the use of that the association of an increase in respon-
counterfactuals. sibility with negative emotion after learning
Nevertheless, both explanations concur in the about the Milgram experiment might be par-
conclusion that thinking about individual re- ticularly strong because of the use of film as a
sponsibility in somebody else’s immoral behav- medium.
ior can help the individual to detach from the Reminding people of what other people are
possibility to engage in such behavior oneself. capable of when they negate personal respon-
This suggests that the instruction to reflect on sibility and showing people who do take re-
the Milgram experiment in terms of personal sponsibility, even as they are being dissuaded
responsibility, and for that matter, being re- to do so, as achieved by a film on the Milgram
minded of individual causality rather than the experiment, appear to have beneficial effects
power of the situation, might be a meaningful regarding individual aspirations of civic re-
intervention when trying to increase levels of sponsibility and civil courage. Therefore, in
societal engagement. addition to its value of providing scientific
insight into the influence of authority on obe-
General Discussion dience, the Milgram experiment can serve as
a reminder to think of the personal responsi-
Viewing a film about the Milgram experiment bility of the individual and activate civilly
had an effect of increasing personal feelings of courageous thinking and behavior.
responsibility and the inclination to show civil Still, there is some reason to be cautious about
courage. Reflecting about dispositional reasons for generalizing our findings regarding the responsi-
the morally discrepant behavior shown in the film bility increases after viewing the Milgram film.
increased intentions to act more responsibly in Because the current studies do not provide data
terms of societal engagement. from a control condition in which a neutral-
In addition, in Study 1, we saw more negative content film is shown, we cannot completely rule
affect after watching the film being related to an out the influence that demand characteristics
increase in feelings of personal responsibility— might have played in people’s responses to the
this suggests an emotionally activating element questionnaire items.
of watching a film about the Milgram experi- It is important to note that we saw that the
ment: Higher negative emotion has shown to be specific interpretation of the material in this con-
related to higher responsibility at after viewing text plays a role in how subsequent behavior is
the film. This finding is in accordance with affected: Thinking about Milgram’s participants
research on the motivating nature of negative as responsible individuals, in contrast to individ-
affect—for instance, in research on cognitive uals merely influenced by the situation, made peo-
dissonance, and in particular, in this case, vicar- ple more likely to want to engage in action. This
ious dissonance (i.e., observing another person shows how easily personal responsibility can be
showing inconsistent behavior; here: value- evoked and increased (or reduced) by suggesting a
discrepant; Cooper & Hogg, 2007). Although specific perspective of attribution. Attributions to
dissonance arousal can be framed as positive stable attributes of a person (e.g., in the form of
affect (e.g., Cooper et al., 1978), the motivation spontaneous trait inferences) have been linked
to reduce dissonance is only associated with with stereotyping (Moskowitz, 1993) and dys-
negative arousal (Higgins et al., 1979). This functional reactions to failure (Weiner, 1985);
132 GRAUPMANN AND FREY

however, in the current context they seem to pos- Burger, J. M. (2009). Replicating Milgram: Would
itively affect people’s motivation to compensate people still obey today? American Psychologist,
the discrepancy between the viewed behavior and 64, 1–11. doi:10.1037/a0010932
their own standards and values for behavior. In a Burley, P. M., & McGuinness, J. (1977). Effects of
social intelligence on the Milgram paradigm. Psy-
broader sense, this relates to the notion of values
chological Reports, 40, 767–770. doi:10.2466/pr0
as an internal compass that can guide a person .1977.40.3.767
despite the pressures of conformity and authority Carver, C. S., Lawrence, J. W., & Scheier, M. F.
in a given situation. (1999). Self-discrepancies and affect: Incorporat-
Our findings resonate with findings on the dif- ing the role of feared selves. Personality and So-
ferential effect of feared selves, depending on how cial Psychology Bulletin, 25, 783–792. doi:
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

close or distant these are perceived to be to the 10.1177/0146167299025007002


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

current self (Carver, Lawrence, & Scheier, 1999). Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and
More approach motivation was observed when a functions of positive and negative affect: A con-
negatively discrepant actualization of the self was trol-process view. Psychological Review, 97, 19 –
perceived as being relatively distant from the cur- 35. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.97.1.19
Cooper, J., & Hogg, M. A. (2007). Feeling the an-
rently experienced self, whereas more avoidance guish of others: A theory of vicarious dissonance.
motivation was observed when the feared self was Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 39,
perceived as close to the current self. This can be 359 – 403. doi:10.1016/S0065-2601(06)39007-7
transferred to our participants engaging in dispo- Cooper, J., Zanna, M. P., & Taves, P. A. (1978).
sitional attributions that allow them to detach from Arousal as a necessary condition for attitude
the obeying participants in Milgram’s study. Con- change following induced compliance. Journal of
sequently, such attributional processing results in Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 1101–
intentions to actively engage in the promotion of 1106. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.36.10.1101
civil courage, moving toward a more moral self. Darley, J. M., & Latané, B. (1968). Bystander inter-
On the other hand, the situational attributional vention in emergencies: Diffusion of responsibil-
style should lead to more perceived closeness or ity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
8, 377–383. doi:10.1037/h0025589
identification with the obeying Milgram partici- De Vos, J. (2009). Now that you know, how do you
pants as “feared selves” because the emphasis on feel? The Milgram Experiment and psychologiza-
the power of the situational factors suggests that tion. Annual Review of Critical Psychology, 7,
one might not be immune to such behavior one- 223–246.
self. Therefore, fewer intentions to engage in civil Falomir-Pichastor, J. M., Toscani, L., & Huyghues
courage training programs in this condition can be Despointes, S. (2009). Determinants of flu vacci-
interpreted as avoidant behavior. Being exposed to nation among nurses: The effects of group identi-
the morally discrepant behavior of Milgram’s par- fication and professional responsibility. Applied
ticipants via film and given the opportunity to Psychology, 58, 42–58. doi:10.1111/j.1464-0597
draw a line between oneself and the observed .2008.00381.x
Greitemeyer, T., Fischer, P., Kastenmüller, A., &
participants using guided reflection can make this
Frey, D. (2006). Civil courage and helping behav-
famous demonstration of the fatal consequences iour: Differences and similarities. European Psy-
of obedience a powerful lesson in civil courage chologist, 11, 90 –98. doi:10.1027/1016-9040.11
even today. .2.90
Greitemeyer, T., & Osswald, S. (2009). Prosocial
References video games reduce aggressive cognitions. Journal
of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 896 –900.
Bandura, A. (1975). Social learning & personality de- doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2009.04.005
velopment. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart & Winston Greitemeyer, T., & Osswald, S. (2010). Effects of
Bargh, J. A. (1999). The cognitive monster: The case prosocial video games on prosocial behavior.
against controllability of automatic stereotype ef- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98,
fects. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual 211–221. doi:10.1037/a0016997
process theories in social psychology (pp. 361– Greitemeyer, T., & Osswald, S. (2011). Playing prosocial
382). New York, NY: Guilford Press. video games in- creases the accessibility of prosocial
Baumrind, D. (1964). Some thoughts on ethics of thoughts. The Journal of Social Psychology, 151, 121–
research: After reading Milgram’s “Behavioral 128. doi:10.1080/00224540903365588
Study of Obedience.” American Psychologist, 19, Higgins, E. T., Rhodewalt, F., & Zanna, M. P.
421– 423. doi:10.1037/h0040128 (1979). Dissonance motivation: Its nature, persis-
OBEDIENCE, RESPONSIBILITY, AND COURAGE 133

tence, and reinstatement. Journal of Experimental havior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychol-
Social Psychology, 15, 16 –34. doi:10.1016/0022- ogy, 41, 423– 430. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2004.08.003
1031(79)90015-5 Norton, M. I., Monin, B., Cooper, J., & Hogg, M. A.
Jarett, C. (April 19, 2010). Game of death. The Psy- (2003). Vicarious dissonance: Attitude change
chologist. Retrieved from http://www.thepsychologist from the inconsistency of others. Journal of Per-
.org.uk/blog/11/blogpost.cfm?threadid⫽1533& sonality and Social Psychology, 85, 47– 62. doi:
catid⫽48. retrieved: 02/24/2014 10.1037/0022-3514.85.1.47
Jonas, K.-J., & Brandstätter, V. (2004). Zivilcour- Orne, M. T., & Holland, C. H. (1968). On the eco-
age—Definition, befunde und mabnahmen. logical validity of laboratory deceptions. Interna-
Zeitschrift für Sozialpsychologie, 35, 185–200. tional Journal of Psychiatry, 6, 282–293.
doi:10.1024/0044-3514.35.4.185 Osswald, S., Frey, D., & Streicher, B. (2012). Moral
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Kastenmüller, A., Greitemeyer, T., Fischer, P., & courage. In E. Kals & J. Maes (Eds.), Justice and
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Frey, D. (2007). Das Münchener Zivilcourage- conflicts (pp. 391– 405). Berlin, Germany: Springer.
Instrument: Entwicklung und erste validierung. Reeder, G. D., & Brewer, M. B. (1979). A schematic
Diagnostica, 53, 205–217. doi:10.1026/0012-1924 model of dispositional attribution in interpersonal
.53.4.205 perception. Psychological Review, 86, 61–79. doi:
Loftus, E. F. (1975). Leading questions and the eye- 10.1037/0033-295X.86.1.61
witness report. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 560 –572. Reisenzein, R. (1986). A structural equation analysis
Lopez, S. J., Koetting O’Byrne, K., & Petersen, S. of Weiner’s attribution-affect model of helping
(2003). Profiling courage. In S.J. Lopez, K. behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
O’Byrne Koetting, S. Petersen (Eds.), Positive chology, 50, 1123–1133. doi:10.1037/0022-3514
Psychological Assessment: A handbook of models .50.6.1123
and measures., (pp. 185–197). Washington, DC: Roese, N. J. (1997). Counterfactual thinking. Psycho-
American Psychological Association. logical Bulletin, 121, 133–148. doi:10.1037/0033-
Lowery, B. S., Hardin, C. D., & Sinclair, S. (2001). 2909.121.1.133
Social influence effects on automatic racial preju-
Ross, L., & Nisbett, R. E. (1991). The person and the
dice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
situation: Perspectives of social psychology. New
ogy, 81, 842– 855.
York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
Meeus, W. H. J., & Raaijmakers, Q. A. W. (1986).
Rozin, P., & Royzman, E. (2001). Negativity bias,
Administrative obedience: Carrying out orders to
negativity dominance, and cognition. Personality
use psychological-administrative violence. Euro-
and Social Psychology Review, 5, 296 –320. doi:
pean Journal of Social Psychology, 16, 311–324.
doi:10.1002/ejsp.2420160402 10.1207/S15327957PSPR0504_2
Milgram, S. (1963). Behavioral study of obedience. Schurz, G. (1985). Experimentelle Überprüfung des
The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, Zusammenhangs zwischen Persönlichkeitsmerkmalen
67, 371–378. doi:10.1037/h0040525 und der Bereitschaft zum destruktiven Gehorsam
Milgram, S. (1965). Some conditions of obedience gegenüber Autoritäten. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle
and disobedience to authority. Human Relations, und Angewandte Psychologie, 32, 160 –177.
18, 57–76. doi:10.1177/001872676501800105 Schwarz, N. (1990). Feelings as information: Infor-
Milgram, S. (1974). Obedience to authority. New mational and motivational functions of affective
York, NY: Harper & Row. states. In E. T. Higgins, & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.),
Miranda, F. S. B., Caballero, R. B., Gomez, M. N. G., Handbook of motivation and cognition: Founda-
& Zamorano, M. A. M. (1981). Obediencia a la tions of social behavior (Vol. 2, pp. 527–561).
autoridad. Psiquis: Revista de Psiquiatría, Psi- New York, NY: Guilford Press.
cología y Psicosomática, 2, 212–221. Shanab, M. E., & Yahya, K. A. (1978). A cross-
Monteith, M. J., Ashburn-Nardo, L., Voils, C. I., & cultural study of obedience. Bulletin of the Psy-
Czopp, A. M. (2002). Putting the brakes on prej- chonomic Society, 11, 267–269. doi:10.3758/
udice: On the development and operation of cues BF03336827
for control. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- Silvia, P. J. (2006). Reactance and the dynamics of
chology, 83, 1029 –1050. doi:10.1037/0022-3514 disagreement: Multiple paths from threatened free-
.83.5.1029 dom to resistance to persuasion. European Journal
Moskowitz, G. B. (1993). Individual differences in of Social Psychology, 36, 673– 685. doi:10.1002/
social categorization: The influence of personal ejsp.309
need for structure on spontaneous trait inferences. Tesser, A., Crepaz, N., Collins, J. C., Cornell, D., &
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, Beach, S. R. (2000). Confluence of self-esteem reg-
132–142. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.65.1.132 ulation mechanisms: On integrating the self-zoo.
Nelson, L. D., & Norton, M. I. (2005). From student Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26,
to superhero: Situational primes shape helping be- 1476 –1489. doi:10.1177/01461672002612003
134 GRAUPMANN AND FREY

Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). De- Weiner, B. (1985). An attributional theory of
velopment and validation of brief measures of posi- achievement motivation and emotion. Psycholog-
tive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal ical Review, 92, 548 –573. doi:10.1037/0033-295X
of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063– .92.4.548
1070. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063 Westermann, R., Spies, K., Stahl, G., & Hesse, F. W.
Weiner, B. (1980). A cognitive (attribution)- (1996). Relative effectiveness and validity of
emotion-action model of motivated behavior: mood induction procedures: A meta-analysis. Eu-
An analysis of judgments of help-giving. Jour- ropean Journal of Social Psychology, 26, 557–
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 580. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-0992(199607)26:
186 –200. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.39.2.186 4⬍557::AID-EJSP769⬎3.0.CO;2-4
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Members of Underrepresented Groups:


Reviewers for Journal Manuscripts Wanted
If you are interested in reviewing manuscripts for APA journals, the APA Publications and
Communications Board would like to invite your participation. Manuscript reviewers are vital to the
publications process. As a reviewer, you would gain valuable experience in publishing. The P&C
Board is particularly interested in encouraging members of underrepresented groups to participate
more in this process.

If you are interested in reviewing manuscripts, please write APA Journals at Reviewers@apa.org.
Please note the following important points:

• To be selected as a reviewer, you must have published articles in peer-reviewed journals. The
experience of publishing provides a reviewer with the basis for preparing a thorough, objective
review.

• To be selected, it is critical to be a regular reader of the five to six empirical journals that are most
central to the area or journal for which you would like to review. Current knowledge of recently
published research provides a reviewer with the knowledge base to evaluate a new submission
within the context of existing research.

• To select the appropriate reviewers for each manuscript, the editor needs detailed information.
Please include with your letter your vita. In the letter, please identify which APA journal(s) you
are interested in, and describe your area of expertise. Be as specific as possible. For example,
“social psychology” is not sufficient—you would need to specify “social cognition” or “attitude
change” as well.

• Reviewing a manuscript takes time (1– 4 hours per manuscript reviewed). If you are selected to
review a manuscript, be prepared to invest the necessary time to evaluate the manuscript
thoroughly.

APA now has an online video course that provides guidance in reviewing manuscripts. To learn
more about the course and to access the video, visit http://www.apa.org/pubs/authors/review-
manuscript-ce-video.aspx.

You might also like