Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 6

Neuroscience Letters 482 (2010) 117–122

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Neuroscience Letters
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neulet

Increased dopaminergic activity in socially isolated rats:


An electrophysiological study
Katrine Fabricius a,b , Lone Helboe a , Anders Fink-Jensen b,c , Gitta Wörtwein b,c ,
Björn Steiniger-Brach a , Florence Sotty a,∗
a
Discovery Pharmacology Research, H. Lundbeck A/S, Ottiliavej 7-9, 2500 Valby, Denmark
b
Faculty of health science, University of Copenhagen, Denmark
c
Laboratory of Neuropsychiatry, Mental Health Services Copenhagen. The Capital Region of Denmark, University of Copenhagen, Denmark

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: The development of animal models mimicking symptoms associated with schizophrenia has been a crit-
Received 13 April 2010 ical step in understanding the neurobiological mechanisms underlying the disease. Long-term social
Received in revised form 7 June 2010 isolation from weaning in rodents, a model based on the neurodevelopmental hypothesis of schizophre-
Accepted 8 July 2010
nia, has been suggested to mimic some of the deficits seen in schizophrenic patients. We confirm in the
present study that socially isolated rats display an increase in both spontaneous and d-amphetamine-
Keywords:
induced locomotor activity, as well as deficits in sensorimotor gating as assessed in a pre-pulse inhibition
Social isolation
paradigm. In addition, in vivo electrophysiological studies revealed changes in dopaminergic cell firing
Mesolimbic dopamine system
Schizophrenia
activity in the ventral tegmental area of isolated rats when compared to group-housed controls. These
Animal model alterations include an increase in the number of spontaneously active dopaminergic neurons, and a
In vivo electrophysiology change of firing activity towards a more irregular and bursting firing pattern. Taken together, our find-
ings suggest that the behavioral phenotype induced by social isolation may be driven by an overactive
dopamine system.
© 2010 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.

Symptoms associated with schizophrenia have been categorized morphologic alterations including reduced dendritic spine den-
into positive symptoms (e.g. delusions and hallucinations), nega- sity in the prefrontal cortex and hippocampus of isolated rats have
tive symptoms (e.g. blunting of affect, social withdrawal), cognitive been reported [23], further strengthening the neurodevelopmen-
deficits (e.g. impairment in memory and executive function) tal nature of the changes induced by social isolation. Moreover,
and affective (e.g. depressive) symptoms. Preclinical research has the critical role of stress in the isolation syndrome is of impor-
focused on the development of animal models mimicking these tance since stress in early life has been implicated in the later
symptoms as an attempt to elucidate the underlying neurobiolog- development of schizophrenia [4,17]. Thus, isolation rearing of rats
ical mechanisms, as well as to develop new pharmacotherapies. immediately after weaning seems to produce some changes in
Based on the neurodevelopmental hypothesis of schizophrenia, behavior related to core defects associated to schizophrenia, and
it has been reported that social isolation of rats immediately may therefore represent an animal model suitable for investigat-
after weaning induces an “isolation syndrome” involving sev- ing the neurobiological mechanisms underlying deficits associated
eral neurochemical and behavioral changes believed to resemble with schizophrenia, as well as to identify possible novel pharma-
some of the core symptoms observed in schizophrenic patients cotherapies for this disease [6,15].
[6,9,15]. For instance, novelty and psychostimulant-induced loco- Sensitization of the mesoaccumbal dopaminergic pathway has
motor hyperactivity [28] and deficits in sensorimotor gating [3,29] been suggested to be responsible for some of the behavioral
and a range of cognitive deficits [1,18,22] have been encoun- changes induced by social isolation [15]. It was further proposed
tered and linked to positive and cognitive symptoms associated that these behavioral alterations may result from changes in
with schizophrenia. Besides, neurochemical studies have revealed presynaptic dopamine function, including alterations in autore-
alterations in the reactivity of the dopaminergic system to psy- ceptor regulation, uptake efficiency, synthesis efficiency, and firing
chostimulants in socially isolated rats [12,15,28]. In addition, activity.
Since abnormal regulation of dopaminergic neurons in the ven-
tral tegmental area (VTA) is believed to be involved in the etiology
∗ Corresponding author at: H. Lundbeck A/S, Ottiliavej 9, DK-2500 Valby, Den- of psychotic symptoms [7], we investigated whether social isola-
mark. Tel.: +45 3643 3647; fax: +45 3643 8232. tion of rats post-weaning had an influence on the firing activity
E-mail address: FSOT@lundbeck.com (F. Sotty). of dopamine neurons recorded from the VTA in vivo. In addition,

0304-3940/$ – see front matter © 2010 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2010.07.014
118 K. Fabricius et al. / Neuroscience Letters 482 (2010) 117–122

Fig. 1. Behavioral phenotype of 12 weeks SI and GH rats. (A) Amphetamine-induced locomotor hyperactivity. Mean activity count ± SEM (total beam breaks within a 5 min
epoch) revealed a significant difference between GH (n = 6, closed symbols) and SI rats (n = 9, open symbols) after d-amphetamine challenge. *p < 0.05; ***p < 0.001. (B) Pre-
pulse inhibition. Left: Mean % PPI ± SEM at pre-pulse (pp) intensities of 74–82 dB indicates a significant % PPI deficit in SI (n = 17) compared to GH (n = 15) rats *p < 0.05,
***p < 0.001. Right: Mean habituation of the startle response ± SEM to 8 + 8 startle-pulses before an after the testing paradigm *p < 0.05.

the influence of social isolation on dopamine-dependent behav- two rows of four infrared light sources and photocells. Record-
iors, e.g. spontaneous and d-amphetamine-evoked hyperlocomotor ing of a motility count required the interruption of adjacent light
activities, as well as pre-pulse inhibition (PPI) of the acoustic beams in the lower row, thus avoiding counts induced by station-
startle, were also evaluated to validate our procedure of social ary movements of the rat. Registration and timing of locomotor and
isolation. rearing activity were fully automated (custom-designed hardware
All experiments were carried out in accordance with the Euro- and software by Ellegaard Systems A/S, Faaborg, Denmark).
pean Communities Council Directive (86/609/EEC) for the care and The effect of 0.5 mg/kg base d-amphetamine on locomotor activ-
use of laboratory animals and the Danish legislation regulating ity was tested in the abovementioned photo beam set-up. The
animal experiments. The Danish Animal Experiments Inspectorate testing paradigm was divided into three phases: (1) Locomotor
approved the protocols (journal no. 2004/561-798). activity was measured for 60 min (habituation). (2) Subsequent to
Female Lister Hooded rats (Harlan, Netherlands) arrived at the the habituation, all animals received a saline injection (1 ml/kg, s.c.)
in-house facilities at postnatal day (PND) 8–9 with foster mothers and returned immediately to the test cages. (3) After 60 min ani-
[11 pups each which were cross-fostered randomly at birth]. At mals were administered d-amphetamine and locomotor activity
arrival, dams and pups were left undisturbed until PND 25 where recording continued for another 60 min.
the pups where randomly assigned to be singly housed (Social A group of rats was further tested for 3 consecutive days in a
Isolation rats, SI) or in groups of five (Group-housed controls, GH). PPI paradigm. PPI testing was performed using the Startle Monitor
Housing consisted of a transparent high grid lid macrolon cage type System (Kinder Scientific), which consisted of 8 sound-attenuated
III or IV and wood chip bedding (type IV GH; type III, SI). All animals startle chambers, all calibrated to the same sensitivity, and design
were housed in controlled temperature (22 ± 1.5 ◦ C) and humidity based computer software. The PPI regime consisted of a 5-min accli-
conditions (55–65%) and kept in a 12:12 hour light/dark cycle mation period with white background noise (70 dB), followed by
(lights on at 06:00 h). Handling and noise were kept to a minimum, randomized trials that consisted of: no pulse, startle-pulse alone
and cages were cleaned twice a week for GH and once a week for (120 dB, 40 ms duration), pre-pulse (4, 8 or 12 dB above background
SI. Food (altromin pills, and water were supplied ad libitum. SI and 30 ms duration) + startle-pulse, or finally pre-pulse (high) alone.
GH animals were kept in the same room facility and could smell, Inter-stimulus interval was set at 100 ms, whereas the inter-trial
hear and see each other). All behavioral testing took place in the interval varied between 9 and 15 s.
light phase between 09:00 a.m. and 5:00 p.m., after 12 weeks of In order to insure that the pre-pulses did not induce a star-
isolation. tle by themselves, 11 pre-pulse alone trials were included in the
All cohorts were tested for spontaneous locomotor activity. trial protocol, but were not used in the calculations of PPI. To esti-
For acclimation, all rats were brought to the laboratory one day mate the habituation to startle each trial session started and ended
prior to the test. On the test day, animals were placed in indi- with 8 startle-pulse alone which were calculated as percent change
vidual test cages and left undisturbed for 2 h (120 min). Each test from first 8 startle-pulses and 8 last pulses. Startle magnitude was
cage, (macrolon type III), was placed in a U-frame equipped with calculated as an average of “startle-pulse alone” trials.
K. Fabricius et al. / Neuroscience Letters 482 (2010) 117–122 119

Fig. 2. Electrophysiological characteristics of dopaminergic neurons in the VTA of GH and SI rats. (A) mean locomotor activity count (±SEM) of GH and SI rats used for
electrophysiological recordings. The number of spontaneously active dopamine neurons in the VTA (B), percentage of spikes in burst (C) and CV ISI (D) were significantly
increased in SI compared to GH rats (n = 4–6 animals, 24–25 neurons in each group).

PPI was calculated as % PPI for each pre-pulse intensity occurring peaks were characteristic of the regular firing pattern.
as: [100 × (startle-pulse − pre-pulse + startle-pulse)/startle-pulse]. An initial trough that rose smoothly to a steady state was classified
A low percentage score indicates a deficit in PPI. as irregular firing pattern while an initial peak followed by decay
For electrophysiology experiments, animals were anaes- to a steady state was classified as bursty firing pattern (Fig. 3).
thetized with an initial intraperitoneal injection of chloral d-Amphetamine was obtained from Nomeco A/S (Nomeco A/S,
hydrate (400 mg/kg). An intraperitoneal catheter then enabled Copenhagen, Denmark), dissolved in saline, and administered at
continuous anesthetic infusion (120 mg/kg/h). Animals were a volume of 1 ml/kg. Doses are expressed in base concentrations.
mounted in a stereotaxic frame, the skull was exposed, and a Chloral hydrate was purchased from Sigma (Sigma–Aldrich Dan-
hole (0.5 cm × 0.5 cm) was drilled above the VTA (see coordinates mark A/S, Brøndby, Denmark).
below). Extracellular single-cell recordings were performed using All data are expressed as mean ± SEM. A two-way repeated
electrodes pulled from glass capillaries and filled with 2% Pon- measures (RM) analysis of variance (ANOVA) was applied to inves-
tamine Sky Blue in 0.5 M sodium acetate (impedance 2.0–8.0 M tigate statistical differences in the median values among SI and GH
at 135 Hz). The electrode was lowered to the dorsal border of the animals in spontaneous and d-amphetamine-induced locomotor
VTA, and advanced at a slow (1–3 ␮m/s), uniform speed using activity, as well as in percent PPI. Time and pre-pulse intensities
a hydraulic microdrive. The number of spontaneously active were used as the repeated measurement factor, and housing as
dopamine neurons was determined in 6–9 stereotaxic descents the between subject factor. Mean locomotor activity for all cohorts
[2,30] separated from each other by 200 ␮m, which sequence was tested with a two-way ANOVA with housing and cohort as
was kept constant from animal to animal. Descents were made between subject factors. If a significant difference was observed, a
in a stereotaxically defined block of tissue within the VTA area multiple comparisons design (Fishers LSD) was used to investigate
at the following coordinates: 5.6–5.2 mm posterior to bregma, statistical significance between SI and GH groups. Acoustic startle
0.5–0.9 mm lateral to the midline, and 6.5–8.5 mm ventral to the and habituation to the acoustic startle in PPI were analyzed with a
cortical surface, according to Paxinos and Watson [20]. Extracellu- Student’s t-test and paired t-test, respectively. Electrophysiological
lar action potentials were amplified, discriminated and monitored data were analyzed using a one-way ANOVA, except for the distri-
on an oscilloscope and an audiomonitor. Presumed dopaminergic bution of firing patterns which was analyzed using the Chi-square
neurons were characterized by (1) a slow and irregular firing test. For all tests, a p value ≤0.05 was considered significant. Sta-
pattern (0.5–10 Hz), and (2) triphasic action potentials with a tistical analysis was performed with SigmaStat (ver 3.0.1 Systat® ,
predominant positive component, a negative component followed USA).
by a minor positive component, with an overall duration >2.5 ms SI rats showed consistently increased spontaneous locomo-
[8]. Each spontaneously active dopamine neurons was counted tor activity as compared to GH controls in a novel test cage
and further recorded for 2–5 min for offline analysis of (i) their (F(1,71) = 58.39, p < 0.001). Overall, the mean locomotor activity
basal firing rate, (ii) the proportion of action potentials occurring count was 1460 ± 73 and 789 ± 50 in SI and GH rats, respectively.
in bursts (defined as the occurrence of two consecutive spikes with A two-way RM ANOVA revealed a significant effect of housing
an interspike interval (ISI) of less than 80 ms, and the termination (F(1,13) = 13.33, p = 0.003) and time (F(15,455) = 23.06, p < 0.001) with
of a burst defined as the occurrence of an ISI exceeding 160 ms) a significant interaction between housing and time (F(35,455) = 2.82,
[8], and (iii) the coefficient of variation of the ISI (CV ISI) defined p < 0.001). Further analysis showed that SI animals were signif-
as the ratio between the average ISI and the standard deviation of icantly more active than GH controls in the habituation period
the ISI × 100. In addition, autocorrelograms were constructed from (F(1,455) = 13.49, p = 0.002), but all animals habituated to the test
spike trains consisting of at least 300 consecutive spikes using a bin cage over time (F(1,455) = 34.3, p < 0.001). Further, SI animals exhib-
width of 10 ms for intervals up to 2000 ms and were used to quali- ited a significantly higher locomotor activity in response to
tatively classify neurons as firing in the regular, irregular, or bursty d-amphetamine as compared to GH (F(1,455) = 10.49, p < 0.001)
firing pattern [26]. Autocorrelograms showing 3 or more regularly (Fig. 1A).
120 K. Fabricius et al. / Neuroscience Letters 482 (2010) 117–122

Fig. 3. Distribution of firing patterns of dopaminergic neurons in the VTA of GH and SI rats. Autocorrelograms of dopaminergic neurons in the VTA were constructed (A, B
and C, right panel) from spike trains (A, B and C, left panel) and allowed the classification of their firing pattern into regular (A), irregular (B) and bursty (C). The distribution
of these firing patterns was significantly different in SI when compared to GH rats, with a higher population of bursty neurons in SI rats (D).

In the PPI paradigm, increasing the pre-pulse volume signifi- tively, F(1,8) = 14.31, p = 0.005; Fig. 2B). Analysis of the firing rate of
cantly increased the % PPI of the acoustic startle in both GH and SI all recorded dopamine neurons did not show any significant differ-
rats (F(2,59) = 455, p < 0.001). However, SI animals exhibited a sig- ence between SI and GH rats (3.44 ± 0.46 Hz vs. 3.70 ± 0.41 Hz in SI
nificant PPI deficit when compared to GH controls (F(1,30) = 18.52, and GH rats, respectively, F(1,45) = 0.18, p = 0.67; not shown). Anal-
p < 0.001). Further, Fig. 1B analysis revealed that SI animals demon- ysis of the percentage of spikes in bursts of all recorded neurons
strated significantly reduced habituation of the startle response revealed a significant increase in SI rats when compared to GH rats
to the startle-pulse (p = 0.02, paired t-test), but no difference was (33.2 ± 5.4% vs. 19.9 ± 5.8% in SI and GH rats, respectively, q = 3.97,
observed in startle amplitude between SI and GH animals (p = 0.66, p = 0.005; Fig. 2C). A significant increase in the coefficient of varia-
t-test, data not shown). tion of the ISI was also found in SI compared to GH rats (94 ± 7.7%
All SI rats used in the electrophysiological studies exhibited a vs. 68 ± 7.4% in SI and GH rats, respectively, F(1,44) = 6.12, p = 0.017;
significant increase in locomotor activity in response to a novel test Fig. 2D). Construction of autocorrelograms for all recorded neurons
cage compared to GH rats (F(1,8) = 80.26, p < 0.001; Fig. 2A). SI ani- allowed their classification into regular (Fig. 3A), irregular (Fig. 3B)
mals showed a significant increase in the number of spontaneously or bursty (Fig. 3C). A significant change in the distribution of firing
active dopamine neurons recorded from the VTA when compared patterns was found in SI compared to GH rats (2 = 11.35, df = 2,
to GH animals (1.11 ± 0.12 vs. 0.69 ± 0.05 in SI and GH rats, respec- p = 0.003; Fig. 3D), e.g. a lower proportion of neurons exhibiting
K. Fabricius et al. / Neuroscience Letters 482 (2010) 117–122 121

regular firing in SI (12%) compared to GH (42%), and a higher pro- enhance accumbal dopamine levels as measured by microdialy-
portion of neurons exhibiting burst firing was found in SI (56%) sis, unless dopamine reuptake mechanisms were blocked [5]. Since
compared to GH (12%). burst firing activity in dopaminergic neurons is highly dependent
The aim of the present study was to investigate whether social on excitatory inputs [19,25], it would be essential to investigate
isolation of rats post-weaning had an influence on the firing activ- whether glutamatergic transmission is also affected by social iso-
ity of dopamine neurons recorded from the VTA in vivo. In addition, lation.
we confirmed that socially isolated rats exhibited an increased In conclusion, our in vivo electrophysiological recordings from
locomotor activity in response to a novel environment, as well as dopaminergic neurons in the VTA revealed for the first time an
to d-amphetamine, and a deficit in pre-pulse inhibition as earlier increase in the number of spontaneously active neurons and a
reported [3,12,21] further validating our social isolation procedure. higher proportion of dopamine neurons firing in bursts in socially
Locomotor hyperactivity in response to a novel environment isolated rats compared to group-housed rats. Together with the
has been consistently reported to be enhanced as a consequence assumption that an exacerbated burst firing activity in dopamine
of social isolation [6], a finding also observed in the present study. neurons underlies the hyperresponsive state of the mesolimbic
In addition, an enhanced locomotor response to d-amphetamine dopaminergic system thought to be associated with schizophre-
was also observed in our study, in agreement with reports from nia [7], our electrophysiological findings further strengthen the
the literature [12,24]. Locomotor hyperactivity in response to a usefulness of social isolation as a model for studying the patho-
novel environment and to psychostimulants has been suggested to physiological mechanisms underlying the disease.
be dependent on the mesolimbic dopaminergic system [13]. More
precisely, the enhanced locomotor response observed in socially
isolated rats has been suggested to be driven by a sensitization Acknowledgement
of the mesolimbic dopaminergic pathway [15]. In support of this
assumption, the locomotor response to intra-accumbal adminis- Technician Vibeke Nielsen is thanked for performing the
tration of amphetamine was enhanced by isolation rearing [12]. amphetamine locomotor study.
In addition, we confirmed in our study that socially isolated rats
displayed deficits in PPI as well as in habituation of the acous-
References
tic startle response, both of which also reported to be sensitive
to dopaminergic modulation [10,14,21]. For instance, PPI deficits [1] M. Bianchi, K.F. Fone, N. Azmi, C.A. Heidbreder, J.J. Hagan, C.A. Mars-
induced by social isolation were reversed by 6-hydroxydopamine- den, Isolation rearing induces recognition memory deficits accompanied by
cytoskeletal alterations in rat hippocampus, Eur. J. Neurosci. 24 (2006) 2894–
induced dopamine depletion in the nucleus accumbens [21] further
2902.
supporting a dysfunctional mesolimbic dopaminergic transmission [2] L.A. Chiodo, B.S. Bunney, Typical and atypical neuroleptics: differential effects
as a consequence of isolation rearing. Thus far, whether the alter- of chronic administration on the activity of A9 and A10 midbrain dopaminergic
ation in presynaptic dopamine function originates from changes in neurons, J. Neurosci. 3 (1983) 1607–1619.
[3] J. Cilia, P.D. Hatcher, C. Reavill, D.N. Jones, Long-term evaluation of
autoreceptor regulation, uptake efficiency, synthesis efficiency, or isolation-rearing induced prepulse inhibition deficits in rats: an update, Psy-
firing activity, has not been clearly established. chopharmacology (Berl.) 180 (2005) 57–62.
In the present study, we found for the first time an increased [4] C. Corcoran, E. Walker, R. Huot, V. Mittal, K. Tessner, L. Kestler, D. Malaspina,
The stress cascade and schizophrenia: etiology and onset, Schizophr. Bull. 29
number of spontaneously active dopamine neurons recorded from (2003) 671–692.
the VTA in socially isolated rats. It is noteworthy that a some- [5] S.B. Floresco, A.R. West, B. Ash, H. Moore, A.A. Grace, Afferent modulation of
what low number of active dopamine neurons were encountered dopamine neuron firing differentially regulates tonic and phasic dopamine
transmission, Nat. Neurosci. 6 (2003) 968–973.
in control rats in the present study. Even though higher neuronal [6] K.C. Fone, M.V. Porkess, Behavioural and neurochemical effects of post-weaning
counts have generally been reported in the literature, none of social isolation in rodents—relevance to developmental neuropsychiatric dis-
these studies have used Lister Hooded rats. Interestingly, differ- orders, Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 32 (2008) 1087–1102.
[7] A.A. Grace, Phasic versus tonic dopamine release and the modulation of
ences in locomotor activity as well as sensitivity to dopaminergic
dopamine system responsivity: a hypothesis for the etiology of schizophrenia,
drugs have been reported in Lister Hooded compared to other rat Neuroscience 41 (1991) 1–24.
strains [11,27], and may rely on a different spontaneous activity [8] A.A. Grace, B.S. Bunney, Intracellular and extracellular electrophysiology of
nigral dopaminergic neurons. 1. Identification and characterization, Neuro-
of dopamine neurons. The increase in the VTA population activity
science 10 (1983) 301–315.
observed in socially isolated rats is in agreement with findings in [9] C.A. Heidbreder, I.C. Weiss, A.M. Domeney, C. Pryce, J. Homberg, G. Hedou, J.
another neurodevelopmental model of schizophrenia, the methy- Feldon, M.C. Moran, P. Nelson, Behavioral, neurochemical and endocrinologi-
lazoxymethanol (MAM) model [16]. In these studies, a hyperactive cal characterization of the early social isolation syndrome, Neuroscience 100
(2000) 749–768.
state of glutamatergic neurons in the ventral hippocampus was [10] T Humby, L.S. Wilkinson, T.W. Robbins, M.A. Geyer, Prepulses inhibit startle-
described and suggested to be responsible for the increased pop- induced reductions of extracellular dopamine in the nucleus accumbens of rat,
ulation activity in the VTA via disinhibition of the indirect ventral J. Neurosci. 16 (1996) 2149–2156.
[11] T.T. Iyaniwura, A.E. Wright, D.J. Balfour, Evidence that mesoaccumbens
striatopallidal pathway. We also found that spontaneously active dopamine and locomotor responses to nicotine in the rat are influenced by
dopamine neurons exhibited an exacerbated burst firing activity, pretreatment dose and strain, Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 158 (2001) 73–79.
as supported by an overall increased percentage of spikes in bursts [12] G.H. Jones, C.A. Marsden, T.W. Robbins, Increased sensitivity to amphetamine
and reward-related stimuli following social isolation in rats: possible dis-
and an increased CV of the interspike interval. Further classification ruption of dopamine-dependent mechanisms of the nucleus accumbens,
of the neurons according to their firing pattern revealed a higher Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 102 (1990) 364–372.
proportion of neurons firing in a burst mode in socially isolated [13] P.H. Kelly, P.W. Seviour, S.D. Iversen, Amphetamine and apomorphine
responses in the rat following 6-OHDA lesions of the nucleus accumbens septi
rats compared to group-housed rats, without affecting their over-
and corpus striatum, Brain Res. 94 (1975) 507–522.
all firing rate. These findings are of particular interest since burst [14] B.D. Kretschmer, M. Koch, The ventral pallidum mediates disruption of prepulse
firing in dopaminergic neurons has been suggested to be more inhibition of the acoustic startle response induced by dopamine agonists, but
not by NMDA antagonists, Brain Res. 798 (1998) 204–210.
potent to release dopamine at the terminal level than regular, tonic
[15] M.D Lapiz, A. Fulford, S. Muchimapura, R. Mason, T. Parker, C.A. Marsden,
firing activity [25]. Surprisingly, microdialysis studies have gen- Influence of postweaning social isolation in the rat on brain development, con-
erally reported unchanged basal dopamine levels in the nucleus ditioned behavior, and neurotransmission, Neurosci. Behav. Physiol. 33 (2003)
accumbens of socially isolated rats [31]. As a possible explana- 13–29.
[16] D.J. Lodge, A.A. Grace, Aberrant hippocampal activity underlies the dopamine
tion for this apparent inconsistency, it was reported that increasing dysregulation in an animal model of schizophrenia, J. Neurosci. 27 (2007)
burst firing in dopamine neurons from the VTA did not significantly 11424–11430.
122 K. Fabricius et al. / Neuroscience Letters 482 (2010) 117–122

[17] J.L. Lukkes, M.V. Mokin, J.L. Scholl, G.L. Forster, Adult rats exposed to early-life dopaminergic neurons during local in vivo application of amino acids in the
social isolation exhibit increased anxiety and conditioned fear behavior, and ventral tegmental area, Neuroscience 49 (1992) 63–72.
altered hormonal stress responses, Horm. Behav. 55 (2009) 248–256. [26] J.M. Tepper, L.P. Martin, D.R. Anderson, GABAA receptor-mediated inhibition
[18] S McLean, B. Grayson, M. Harris, C. Protheroe, S. Bate, M. Woolley, J. Neill, of rat substantia nigra dopaminergic neurons by pars reticulata projection
Isolation rearing impairs novel object recognition and attentional set shifting neurons, J. Neurosci. 15 (1995) 3092–3103.
performance in female rats, J. Psychopharmacol. (2008) 57–63. [27] I.C. Weiss, I.L. Di, J. Feldon, A.M. Domeney, Strain differences in the isolation-
[19] P.G. Overton, D. Clark, Burst firing in midbrain dopaminergic neurons, Brain induced effects on prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle response and on
Res. Brain Res. Rev. 25 (1997) 312–334. locomotor activity, Behav. Neurosci. 114 (2000) 364–373.
[20] G. Paxinos, C. Watson, The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates, Academic Press, [28] I.C. Weiss, A.M. Domeney, C.A. Heidbreder, J.L. Moreau, J. Feldon, Early social
New York, 1998. isolation, but not maternal separation, affects behavioral sensitization to
[21] S.B. Powell, M.A. Geyer, M.A. Preece, L.K. Pitcher, G.P. Reynolds, N.R. Swerd- amphetamine in male and female adult rats, Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 70
low, Dopamine depletion of the nucleus accumbens reverses isolation-induced (2001) 397–409.
deficits in prepulse inhibition in rats, Neuroscience 119 (2003) 233–240. [29] I.C Weiss, C.R. Pryce, A.L. Jongen-Relo, N.I. Nanz-Bahr, J. Feldon, Effect of social
[22] N.C. Schrijver, H. Wurbel, Early social deprivation disrupts attentional, but not isolation on stress-related behavioural and neuroendocrine state in the rat,
affective, shifts in rats, Behav. Neurosci. 115 (2001) 437–442. Behav. Brain Res. 152 (2004) 279–295.
[23] A.B Silva-Gomez, D. Rojas, I. Juarez, G. Flores, Decreased dendritic spine den- [30] F.J. White, R.Y. Wang, Differential effects of classical and atypical antipsy-
sity on prefrontal cortical and hippocampal pyramidal neurons in postweaning chotic drugs on A9 and A10 dopamine neurons, Science 221 (1983) 1054–
social isolation rats, Brain Res. 983 (2003) 128–136. 1057.
[24] J.K. Smith, J.C. Neill, B. Costall, Post-weaning housing conditions influence [31] L.S. Wilkinson, S.S. Killcross, T. Humby, F.S. Hall, M.A. Geyer, T.W. Robbins,
the behavioural effects of cocaine and d-amphetamine, Psychopharmacology Social isolation in the rat produces developmentally specific deficits in prepulse
(Berl.) 131 (1997) 23–33. inhibition of the acoustic startle response without disrupting latent inhibition,
[25] M.F. Suaud-Chagny, K. Chergui, G. Chouvet, F. Gonon, Relationship between Neuropsychopharmacology 10 (1994) 61–72.
dopamine release in the rat nucleus accumbens and the discharge activity of

You might also like