Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 16

doi:10.

1093/brain/awr220 Brain 2011: 134; 3310–3325 | 3310

BRAIN
A JOURNAL OF NEUROLOGY

Acute visual neglect and extinction: distinct


functional state of the visuospatial
attention system
Roza M. Umarova,1,2 Dorothee Saur,2,3 Christoph P. Kaller,1,2 Magnus-Sebastian Vry,1,2
Volkmar Glauche,1,2 Irina Mader,2,4 Jürgen Hennig5 and Cornelius Weiller1,2

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on November 14, 2015


1 Department of Neurology, University Medical Centre Freiburg, Breisacherstr. 64, 79106 Freiburg, Germany
2 Freiburg Brain Imaging, University Medical Centre Freiburg, Breisacherstr. 64, 79106 Freiburg, Germany
3 Department of Neurology, University of Leipzig, Liebigstr. 20, 04103 Leipzig, Germany
4 Department of Neuroradiology, University Medical Centre Freiburg, Breisacherstr. 64, 79106 Freiburg, Germany
5 Department of Radiology, Medical Physics, University Medical Centre Freiburg, Breisacherstr. 60a, 79106 Freiburg, Germany

Correspondence to: Roza M. Umarova,


Department of Neurology,
University Medical Centre Freiburg,
Breisacher Strasse 64, 79106 Freiburg,
Germany
E-mail: roza.umarova@uniklinik-freiburg.de

The neural mechanisms underlying spatial neglect are still disputed. Abnormal left parietal hyperactivation is proposed to lead
to the rightward attentional bias, a clinical hallmark of neglect. Extinction, another deficit of visuospatial attention, is regarded
as either a ‘mild’ form of neglect or a distinct syndrome. Although both neglect and extinction are typical syndromes of acute
right hemispheric stroke, all imaging studies investigating these syndromes were conducted at least several weeks after stroke
onset, in a phase when brain reorganization has already progressed. The present study aimed at comparing the activation
patterns in acute stroke patients with neglect and extinction during visuospatial processing. Using functional magnetic reson-
ance imaging, we examined the functional state of the attention system in 33 patients with a first ever stroke (53  5 h after
stroke onset) and age-matched healthy subjects (n = 15). All patients had embolic infarcts within the territory of the right middle
cerebral artery. Patients were divided into three groups: (i) normal visuospatial processing (control patients, n = 11); (ii) patients
with visual extinction but with no signs of neglect (n = 9); and (iii) patients with visual neglect (n = 13). While undergoing
functional magnetic resonance imaging, patients performed a Posner-like task for visuospatial attention with detection of the
targets in the left and right visual hemifields. Patients with neglect showed the expected imbalance in the left versus right
parietal activation, which however, was present also in control and extinction patients, thus representing an epiphenomenon of
the acute structural lesion in the right hemisphere. Compared with control patients, neglect was characterized by reduced
activation in the right parietal and lateral occipital cortex, as well as in the left frontal eye field. In contrast, the activation
pattern in patients with extinction differed from all other groups by an increased activation of the left prefrontal cortex. In both
patients with neglect and extinction, detection of targets in the left hemifield correlated with an activation in the left prefrontal
and parietal cortex. Thus at least in acute stroke, a relative hyperactivation of the left parietal cortex is not a particular
characteristic of neglect. The specific signature of neglect is represented by the dysfunction of the right parietal and lateral
occipital cortex. The function of the left attentional centres might provide a compensatory role after critical right hemisphere
lesions and be relevant for the contralesional spatial processing.

Received January 2, 2011. Revised June 26, 2011. Accepted July 1, 2011. Advance Access publication September 23, 2011
ß The Author (2011). Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the Guarantors of Brain. All rights reserved.
For Permissions, please email: journals.permissions@oup.com
Acute visual neglect and extinction Brain 2011: 134; 3310–3325 | 3311

Keywords: neglect; extinction; visuospatial attention; parietal hyperactivation

Introduction lobule (Posner et al., 1984; Vallar et al., 1994; Rees et al.,
2000; Vuilleumier and Rafal, 2000).
Neglect is a visuospatial attention deficit, which is characterized The cognitive and neural mechanisms underlying spatial neglect
by the failure to report, respond or orient to novel or meaningful and extinction are reflected in the theories of spatial attention.
stimuli presented to the contralesional side, when this failure According to the theory of hemispheric rivalry, each hemisphere
cannot be attributed to either sensory or motor deficits (Brain, represents the contralateral space and inhibits another one
1941; Critchley, 1949; Heilman et al., 2003). It occurs in 70% through transcallosal pathways (Kinsbourne, 1970). Under
of patients with stroke with a right hemisphere lesion (Gottesman normal conditions, the activation of both hemispheres is balanced.
et al., 2008) and has a significant negative impact on functional In case of a structural lesion, the undamaged hemisphere is disin-
outcome and rehabilitation (Buxbaum et al., 2004; Jehkonen hibited from the damaged one and suppresses it. This functional
et al., 2006). Neglect is considered to be provoked by a structural imbalance between ipsi- and contralesional hemispheres would
lesion of the inferior parietal lobule, the temporoparietal junction provoke an attentional bias towards the ipsilesional space, and
or the inferior frontal cortex in the right hemisphere (Vallar and thus neglect.

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on November 14, 2015


Perani, 1986; Husain and Kennard, 1996; Marshall et al., 2002; Another model of neglect reflects the limited attentional cap-
Mort et al., 2003). In addition, the right superior temporal cortex acity after damage to the right hemisphere. The right hemisphere
is shown to be crucial for neglect (Karnath et al., 2001). This is considered to span the attentional functions for both hemi-
syndrome is discussed as a disconnection syndrome (Mesulam, spaces, while the left hemisphere seems to provide visuospatial
1990), which may be provoked by a disruption of association processing mostly of the contralateral space (Heilman and Van
tracks connecting the parietal and the frontal lobes (Bartolomeo Den Abell, 1980; Mesulam, 1981). According to this model, uni-
et al., 2007; Urbanski et al., 2007; Doricchi et al., 2008; Shinoura lateral lesions of the left hemisphere are unlikely to result in neg-
et al., 2009; Umarova et al., 2010) or the parahippocampal lect, since the intact right hemisphere can take over the task of
gyrus with the angular one (Bird et al., 2006). Although basal attending to the right side. In contrast, in the absence of similar
ganglia lesions may also be critical for the occurrence of compensatory mechanisms in the left hemisphere, right hemi-
neglect (Karnath et al., 2004; Ringman et al., 2004), such infarcts sphere lesions will result in a marked unilateral neglect
lead to neglect mainly through hypoperfusion of the cortical re- (Mesulam, 1981). This model considers extinction as a ‘mild’
gions mediating attention (Bogousslavsky et al., 1988; Weiller form of neglect, which follows less severe parietal lesions
et al., 1993; Hillis et al., 2005). The various aspects of lesion (Mesulam, 1981).
anatomy may determine the heterogeneity of neglect: ego- or A neglect model that is postulated by Corbetta and Shulman
allocentred; for personal-, peri- or extrapersonal space; in the (2002) includes elements of both theories: hemispheric rivalry
visual, sensory or motor modalities (Hillis et al., 2005, 2006; and right hemisphere dominance for spatial processing. Based on
Committeri et al., 2007; Verdon et al., 2009) and reflects its the results from neuroimaging studies, they subdivide the cortical
complexity. centres for spatial attention into a dorsal and a ventral attention
Extinction represents another type of visuospatial attention def- network (Corbetta and Shulman, 2002). The ventral attention
icit. It is characterized by the failure to respond to contralesional network is located predominantly in the right hemisphere and
stimulus, if it is presented simultaneously with an ipsilesional one. centred in the temporoparietal junction, anterior insula and ventral
While there are a number of studies investigating the anatomy of frontal cortex. It is specialized for target detection, particularly
visual neglect, the morphological correlates of extinction are less when they are salient or unexpected (Corbetta and Shulman,
known. Animal studies (Lynch and McLaren, 1989) and experi- 2002). The dorsal attention network includes bilaterally the intra-
ments with healthy subjects (Cicek et al., 2007) identified the right parietal sulcus, the frontal eye field and lateral occipital complex
inferior parietal cortex as an anatomical substrate for simultaneous (Corbetta and Shulman, 2002). It is activated by directional cues
processing of the targets in the left and right visual hemifield. and is involved in attention orientation: preparing and applying
In humans, a lesion or hypoperfusion of the right temporoparietal goal-directed selection for stimuli and responses. Lesions of the
cortex were related to extinction (Karnath et al., 2003; Hillis et al., ventral network provoke a dysfunction in the structurally intact
2006; Ticini et al., 2010). The pattern of lesions causing extinction dorsal network in the right hemisphere, resulting in a functional
is therefore similar to that of neglect. However, the relationship imbalance—a push-pull pattern—of evoked responses in the left
between neglect and extinction is still not fully understood. (hyperactive) and right (hypoactive) dorsal parietal cortex
Whether extinction is a ‘mild’ form of neglect or a separate (Kinsbourne, 1970; Corbetta et al., 2005). This parietal imbalance
form of attentional deficit is still an open question. Some data correlates with the degree of the rightward attentional bias and is
show that these syndromes are dissociable (Vallar et al., 1994; postulated as the pathomechanism of neglect. However, it is un-
Hillis et al., 2006; Vossel et al., 2011). On the other hand, clear whether the functional parietal imbalance is causative for
extinction is also described as a part of a neglect syndrome, es- neglect or reflects cortical reorganization that follows the large
pecially when the lesions are clustered in the inferior parietal right hemispheric lesions. To answer this question, it is necessary
3312 | Brain 2011: 134; 3310–3325 R. M. Umarova et al.

to compare patients with right hemisphere lesions with and with- components of the visuospatial processing; and (iii) efficient
out neglect. enough to be sensitive for effects in acute stroke patients. We
Differences between neglect and extinction in terms of brain chose a Posner-like visual orienting and target detection task
activation pattern can be predicted from the models of spatial (Posner et al., 1984) that involves the dorsal and ventral attention
attention. The model of hemispheric rivalry predicts a left–right systems in healthy subjects and patients with neglect (Corbetta
parietal imbalance in both visual neglect and extinction. Since in and Shulman, 2002; Corbetta et al., 2005). Neuronal activations
neglect the spatial deficit is present during unilateral stimulation, in both hemispheres with right-hand button press for the targets
and in extinction only during bilateral stimulation, the functional detection served as a control condition to ensure a preservation of
left–right imbalance in neglect should be more severe than in ex- blood oxygen level-dependent response and to show that changes
tinction. In contrast, in the theory of the right hemisphere dom- in the attentional system are specific. We expected a wide range
inance in spatial processing (Mesulam, 1981), the problem is the in patients’ performance allowing a correlation analysis between
limited attentional capacity after right hemispheric lesions. functional MRI activation and behavioural performance. The block
Therefore, the activation in the left hemisphere may account for design of the paradigm ensured the high efficiency for detecting
a compensatory mechanism by assuming either a much weaker effects in acute stroke patients (Friston et al., 2007).
but existent spatial representation of both hemispaces in the left
attentional centres or an incomplete dominance of the right hemi-
sphere for spatial processing. Extinction might differ from neglect
by having a greater compensatory effect of the left hemisphere. In
Subject and methods

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on November 14, 2015


other words, while (absolute or relative) increased left hemispheric
activation would be seen as rather deleterious in the hypothesis of
Subjects
hemispheric rivalry, it may be seen as favourable in the second Patients were recruited from the stroke unit of the Department of
model. Neurology, University Medical Centre, Freiburg, Germany. For a
There are several neuroimaging and neurophysiological studies period of 18 months (January 2007–June 2009), we screened all
in the field of visuospatial attention, but only few neuroimaging patients with ischaemic stroke in the right hemisphere. Inclusion
experiments are performed in patients with neglect and extinction criteria consisted of ischaemic first-ever stroke in the right middle
(Rees et al., 2000, 2002; Vuilleumier et al., 2001, 2002, 2008; cerebral artery territory confirmed by MRI. Exclusion criteria were
Corbetta et al., 2005; Koch et al., 2008; Thimm et al., 2008). In as follows: (i) age 580 years; (ii) hemianopia and severe sight
these studies, patients with neglect are compared with healthy defects that prevented subjects from performing the functional
subjects (Corbetta et al., 2005) or include only a small number MRI task (object detection and recognition at a distance of at
of patients (Thimm et al., 2008). Critically, the patients are exam- least 2 m without glasses); (iii) inability to maintain alertness, low
ined at least several weeks after stroke onset, when major adap- conscious or arousal level; (iv) inability to tolerate the functional
tive and reorganizational processes have progressed substantially. MRI examination owing to reduced general health status; (v) pro-
Although the results of these studies are crucial, important ques- nounced small vessel disease or additional structural brain lesions;
tions concerning the functional neuroanatomy of acute neglect (vi) general contraindications for MRI examination; (vii) other
were not answered. neurological or psychiatric conditions that precluded active par-
The purpose of our study was to investigate: (i) if acute visual ticipation in research and/or altered the interpretation of the
neglect is characterized by the hyperactivation of the left parietal behavioural/imaging data (e.g. dementia, schizophrenia); (viii)
cortex; (ii) if acute stroke patients with visual extinction but with- severe stenosis (470%) of the carotid arteries and/or of the
out neglect have similar but less pronounced dysfunction of the middle cerebral arteries; and (ix) left-handedness.
visuospatial attention system compared with neglect, i.e. if extinc- Fifteen age-matched healthy right-handed subjects (eight
tion is a ‘mild’ form of neglect; and (iii) since patients with neglect males), 70  2 years old (mean  SEM) without any neurological
vary in the left-sided performance, what is the functional correlate or psychiatric history served as an additional control group.
of the detection of targets in the contralesional visual hemifield. Written informed consent was obtained from each subject in ac-
To answer these questions, we investigated and compared three cordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The Ethics Committee
groups of patients with acute right hemispheric stroke and of the University Medical Centre Freiburg approved the study.
age-matched healthy subjects. Patients had either visual neglect,
visual extinction without neglect or no visuospatial attention def-
icit. The last group of patients represented the most valid control
Clinical and behavioural testing
group, since they had lesions in the same vascular territory and All patients were examined for extinction and neglect on the day
were matched by age, time after stroke and stroke severity. of scanning. Visual fields were assessed by standardized neuro-
Patients were investigated within the first 2–4 days after stroke, logical bedside examination, since an instrumental perimeter
before the onset of major reorganization processes. All groups of examination was not feasible due to the early inclusion of the
patients were examined with a neglect test battery and a func- patients in the study. Visual neglect was diagnosed based on the
tional MRI experiment of visuospatial attention (Umarova et al., clinical signs (e.g. when the patient oriented towards the ipsile-
2010). The functional MRI paradigm was designed to be: (i) sional side, when addressed from the front or the left side and/or
simple and short enough to study acute stroke patients; (ii) well ignored contralesionally located persons or objects) and the results
established and include both visual orienting and target detection of the neuropsychological testing. The following tests were used.
Acute visual neglect and extinction Brain 2011: 134; 3310–3325 | 3313

Visual extinction sheet. Correctness of the general contour (1 score), details (1


The presence of extinction was tested clinically by wiggling fingers score) and their order (1 score) on the copied pictures are as-
for 2 s in one or both visual fields while controlling central gaze sessed. The maximal score for every figure is 3. A sum score
fixation. Fifteen trials were given in a fixed pseudo-randomized 56 was defined as deficient.
sequence including 10 unilateral trials (five on each side) and
five simultaneous bilateral trials. Extinction was considered if a
Analysis
patient failed to report at least two contralesional stimuli during All patients were divided into three groups: patients with visual
bilateral simultaneous presentation, while accurately detecting uni- neglect (neglect group), who failed to perform in at least two
lateral stimuli (Beis et al., 2004). neglect tests; patients with visual extinction (extinction group),
All patients performed standardized paper and pencil tests of who did not show visual neglect clinically nor in the neglect
visuospatial neglect in the peripersonal space (Neglect Test; Fels tests (only two patients of the group failed in one neglect test—
and Geissner, 1997), which is an adapted version of the copying of the presented pictures, all other patients succeeded in
Behavioural Inattention Test (Wilson et al., 1987). Visual neglect all tests); and patients without neglect or extinction, who repre-
was diagnosed when the patients failed to perform at least two sented the group of control patients, with normal performance in
of the following clinical tests: (i) the line cancellation test; (ii) the both extinction and neglect tests. Patients’ clinical data are pre-
letter cancellation test; (iii) the star cancellation test; (iv) text read- sented in Table 1. We used a one-way ANOVA to compare the
ing; and (v) picture copying task. Patients also performed the line clinical characteristics between groups of patients (age, time after
stroke onset, stroke severity in terms of NIH Stroke Scale assessed

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on November 14, 2015


bisection task, but the results were not taken into account, be-
cause this test was reported as non-specific to neglect (Rorden on admission). As the variance within neglect group was incom-
et al., 2006). All visual tests were performed with the targets parable with those within other groups of patients, we used the
distributed on an A4 sheet of paper, which was centred in front Kruskal–Wallis test to detect the differences between groups in
of the patients. neglect tests.

The line cancellation test Functional magnetic resonance imaging


This test consists of 40 lines each with a length of 25 mm, which
paradigm
are distributed into three columns each on the right and left side
(each with six lines), and one column in the centre (four lines). We used a Posner-like paradigm in a mixed block design
All lines have to be cancelled. Patients were classified to fail the (Umarova et al., 2010). Stimuli were presented visually with the
test in case of (i) omitting at least four left-side targets; or (ii) if software Presentation (http://nbs.neuro-bs.com). Task blocks
right-side targets were also omitted, the difference between omis- (32 s), which consisted of 12 trials, were alternated with rest
sions on the left and right side was 44. (16 s). During a trial, a centrally located arrow (visual angle, 4 )
was presented for 800 ms, which pointed to the right or left side
The letter cancellation test indicating the location of the target (size, 1.3 ). For two-thirds of
Patients were presented with five rows of different letters, con- the trials, the target appeared at the indicated location for 400 ms
sisting of 34 letters per row. The task was to cancel the letters ‘E’ (at 11 of visual angle from the fixation cross). The remaining
and ‘R’, which are randomly distributed among other irrelevant one-third of the trials were null events, in which after the
letters that represented distractors. The interpretation of the re- cueing, no target appeared. The trials were separated by a fixation
sults was the same as in the line cancellation test. cross hair (visual angle 1.4 ) for a jittered period of 1000–2000 ms.
Each task block contained 12 randomly intermixed trials, from
Star cancellation test which eight were ‘valid’ (four events with targets in the right
The target stimuli are 56 small stars, which are interspersed be- and four in the left visual hemifield) and four were null-events
tween distracters that are represented by 52 large stars, 13 letters (two with left- and two with rightward arrow). A total of 192
and 10 short words. The task was to cross out the small stars. The stimuli were distributed over two sessions, each with eight task
interpretation of the results was the same as in the line cancella- blocks. Between sessions we assessed the subject’s performance.
tion test. The task was to detect left and right targets as fast as possible by
pressing a single button with the right non-paretic and
Reading non-neglected hand. In this way, the right arm and hand were
The patient is prompted to read a short text, which is set out in positioned in the right non-neglected hemispace to ensure that the
three columns (46, 47 and 46 words in the left, central and right response button would not be neglected. Subjects were also in-
columns, respectively). The number of words read is assessed. If structed to fixate on the cross-hair between trials.
one or more sentences were read incompletely on the left but not Patients and healthy controls performed the same cued visuo-
on the right side, patients were considered to fail. spatial functional MRI experiment. Before functional MRI examin-
ation, all patients were trained on the task outside the scanner. In
Copying of the presented pictures case of severe neglect, the training was first performed with the
Three pictures are presented to a patient (a four cornered star, a display screen positioned in the right hemispace, thus patients
rhomb and a flower), which are drawn on the left half of the were aware of both the left and right targets. Then the training
sheet. The task is to copy the figures on the right half of the was continued with the display positioned in the centre. Inside the
3314 | Brain 2011: 134; 3310–3325 R. M. Umarova et al.

Table 1 Clinical characteristics of patients and behavioural test results (all results are given in median and range unless
stated otherwise)

Control Patients with Patients with neglect


patients extinction (n = 13)
(n = 11) (n = 9)
Age (years), mean  SEM 67  2.8 69  3.3 65  3.0
Sex (female/male) 7/4 2/7 5/8
Time after stroke onset to functional MRI (h), 53  8.7 40  9.3 62  8.1
mean  SEM
NIHSS score, mean  SEM 91 61 81
Lesion volume* (ml) mean  SEM 29.8  4.3 35.1  9.2 62.4  9.0
Personal neglect (n) 0 0 2
Anosognosia (n) 1 1 9
Detected stimuli in extinction test# max. 5 5; –/5; –/5; – 5; 3–5/5; –/0; 0–3 0; 0–5/5; –/0; 0–5
(left/right/both)
Line bisection*, max 9 9; 6–9 7; 3–9 3; 0–9
Coping of drawings#, max 9 9; 6–9 9; 2–9 2; 0–8
Lines cancelation#, max 18 (left/right) 18; 16–18/18; 17–18 18; 17–18/18; – 5; 0–18/18; 5–18
Star cancelation#, max 27 (left/right)

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on November 14, 2015


27; 22–27/26; 22–27 26; 22–27/26; 22–27 0; 0–23/19; 6–26
Letters cancelation#, max 20 (left/right) 20; 16–20/19; 17–20 20; 18–20/19; 16–20 5; 0–19/15; 4–20
Reading#, max 47/46/47 words 47; 45–47/46; 45–46/47; – 47; 46–47/46; 43–46/47; 45–47 0; 0–47/44; 0–46/47; 10–47
(left/centre/right)

*Significant group effect in a one-way ANOVA; #significant group effect in the Kruskal–Wallis test.
NIHSS = NIH Stroke Scale.

scanner, the patients were first checked for the ability to fixate the Magnetic resonance imaging data
gaze on the cross hair between trials. Thereafter, they were asked
to perform a training session to ensure correct understanding of
acquisition
the task. Structural and functional MRI data were acquired on a 3 T
Behavioural performance was assessed by detection of right tar- TIM-TRIO MRI System (Siemens) with a standard head coil.
gets and non-responses to ‘null events’. We assumed efficient
visuospatial processing if a patient detected overall 470%
Diffusion weighted imaging and anatomical scan
(445) of the right targets and had 510% (56) false alarms.
From all patients, we obtained a diffusion weighted imaging and
Two-way mixed ANOVA was computed to examine reaction
T1 anatomical scan. Diffusion weighted imaging was obtained with
time for left and right targets between groups. Reaction times of
a T2*-weighted echoplanar images (EPI) sequence (repetition
detected targets in the left and right visual hemifield were treated
time = 3100 ms, echo time = 79 ms, b1 = 0 s/mm2, b2 = 1000 s/
as dependent variables (two levels: right and left) with group as a
mm2; matrix size = 64  64, voxel size 1.8  1.8  5 mm3,
between-subject factor (four levels: healthy subjects, control pa-
23 slices) with three diffusion directions. Anatomical scans were
tients, extinction group, neglect group). Conditional on significant
acquired with a sagittal magnetization-prepared rapid-acquisition
F-test results, we investigated the rightward processing bias in
gradient-echo sequence (repetition time = 2200 ms, echo
terms of reaction time differences for left versus right targets
time = 2.15 ms, flip angle = 12 , matrix = 256  256 pixel, voxel
(left reaction time right reaction time) between groups
size = 1  1  1 mm3, 176 slices).
(one-way ANOVA) with further contrasting of reaction time
lateralization between control patients and other groups. The sig-
nificance level was set at P 5 0.05. Because the behavioural per- Lesion mapping
formance was perfect in healthy subjects and nearly perfect in The lesions were delineated on the diffusion weighted images
control patients, the variance of data in them was skewed; there- using MRIcron Toolbox http://www.cabiatl.com/mricro/mricron/
fore, hit rates were compared between groups using the Kruskal– index.html. Lesion maps were normalized into the standard
Wallis test and the Mann–Whitney rank test for pairwise compari- Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space using normalization
sons. The control patients were contrasted with healthy subjects parameters from the coregistered anatomical T1 scans (Ashburner
and then with extinction and neglect groups, since we expected and Friston, 2005). Since the optimal algorithms for spatial nor-
the performance in healthy subjects and in control patients to be malization of structurally damaged brains are implemented in
superior to that of patients with visuospatial attention deficit. the current SPM8 version (Crinion et al., 2007), we did not
A Bonferroni correction was applied, thus all effects are reported mask the structural lesions. The lesion overlap was computed
at a 0.05/4 = 0.0125 level of significance. using MRIcron.
Acute visual neglect and extinction Brain 2011: 134; 3310–3325 | 3315

Functional magnetic resonance imaging scan laterality index (LI) between individual left (L) and right (R) intra-
acquisition and data analysis parietal sulcus activations: LI = (L R)/(L + R). Furthermore, the
During two functional MRI sessions, we acquired 330 axial blood oxygen level-dependent response related to the button
slices covering the whole brain using a T2*-weighted echoplanar press was explored and compared between groups (see
images with blood oxygen level-dependent contrast (repetition Supplementary Material).
time = 2190 ms, echo time = 30 ms, flip angle 75 , matrix The behavioural data and region of interest analysis were per-
size = 64  64 pixel, voxel size = 3  3  3 mm3, 36 slices, no formed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences
gap). The functional MRI data were corrected for motion and (SPSS, v.17) software.
distortion across runs using a reference volume acquired in a pre-
vious scan (Zaitsev et al., 2004, 2006). The first four volumes
were discarded to allow for T1 equilibration effects. The subse-
quent image processing and statistical analysis of the functional
Results
MRI data were performed with SPM8 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac. A total of 37 patients met the inclusion and exclusion criteria.
uk/spm/software/spm8/). The images were corrected for different Two patients had a haemorrhagic transformation of the infarct
acquisition times between slices with reference to the middle slice. in the frontal and parieto-temporo-occipital junction, i.e. the re-
Spatial normalization parameters were computed from the T1 scan gions of interest. They were excluded from further analysis be-
as detailed above and applied to the images, which were then cause of the possible false negative functional MRI group results in

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on November 14, 2015


smoothed with an isotropic Gaussian kernel of 9 mm full-width the area of haemorrhage. Two further patients were excluded due
at half maximum. A general linear model analysis was performed to poor behavioural performance: one pressed the button in
with a block design of the functional runs using the canonical 410% of null events, another detected 570% of targets in the
haemodynamic response function. The task blocks were modelled right visual hemifield. From the remaining 33 patients, 11 patients
as one main regressor to analyse the main effect of interest at the did not have any signs of visuospatial attention deficit and they
single-subject level. To capture the extra variance introduced by formed the group of control patients, nine patients presented with
the motor response, it was modelled as a stick function orthogo- visual extinction (extinction group) and 13 patients had visual neg-
nalized with respect to the main regressor of interest. The contrast lect (neglect group). All patients were examined in the acute stage
images of the motor response were further used as a control con- of stroke; the mean time between stroke onset and functional MRI
dition for the analysis of the blood oxygen level-dependent signal examination was 53  5 h (  SEM).
between groups. Six movement parameters were used as addition- Demographic and clinical data from all included patients are
al regressors. Data were high-pass filtered at 1/128 Hz. For the presented in Table 1. The three groups of patients did not differ
further second-level analysis, we used the images of the main with respect to age [one-way ANOVA, F(2,32) = 0.40, P = 0.675]
effect of interest, i.e. taking into account activation related to and time interval between the stroke onset and functional MRI
the visuospatial processing. examination [F(2,32) = 1.58, P = 0.222]. Stroke severity on
To evaluate the functional MRI activation in each group, we admission, measured with the NIH Stroke Scale, did not distinguish
performed a random-effect analysis treating subjects as a between groups [F(2,32) = 0.96, P = 0.395], neither did the
random variable. Group-specific main effects were characterized frequency of contralateral motor and somatosensory deficits
using a one-sample t-test. To detect differences in the attentional [F(2,32) = 1.92, P = 0.163 and F(2,32) = 2.40, P = 0.108,
activation between groups, they were contrasted with each other respectively].
using two-sample t-tests. To identify the regional differences in
task-related activations between all groups, we performed a
between-group ANOVA, modelling group condition as an inde-
Lesion anatomy
pendent factor (four levels: healthy subjects, control patients, ex- The location of the lesions was comparable in all groups,
tinction group, neglect group) and calculated the main effect of although lesions of patients with neglect were significantly
group as F-contrast. In additional analyses, we were interested in larger than those of all other patients’ groups [F(2,30) = 5.33,
correlates of differences in functional MRI performance in the P = 0.010] (Table 1). In control patients, the infarcts were located
contralesional space in patients with visuospatial attention deficit. in right fronto-temporo-parietal regions including the posterior
To this end, we performed a whole brain voxel-wise regression part of the inferior frontal gyrus [Brodmann area (BA) 44],
analysis using the individual performance data as covariates (reac- insula, frontal operculum, putamen and superior and middle tem-
tion time, hit rate). The anatomical location of the activated poral gyrus with adjacent white matter (Fig. 1). Patients with ex-
clusters was obtained using the Anatomy toolbox implemented tinction had either large cortico-subcortical infarcts with
in SPM8: http://www.fz-juelich.de/ime/spm_anatomy_toolbox involvement of the right insula, superior temporal gyrus and infer-
(Eickhoff et al., 2005). ior parietal lobule (six patients) or cortical inferior parietal lesions
Since the current hypothesis of neglect assumes an imbalance (three patients) (Fig. 1). Patients with neglect had infarcts within
between activity of the left and right parietal cortex, we explored the posterior middle and inferior frontal gyri (BA 44), frontal oper-
the functional MRI activation in the dorsal attention centres culum, insula, middle and superior temporal gyri and inferior par-
and primary visual cortex using a region of interest analysis ietal lobe (Fig. 1). All patients had an intact left hemisphere and
(see Supplementary Material). Additionally, we calculated a right occipital lobe. The optic radiation was also intact.
3316 | Brain 2011: 134; 3310–3325 R. M. Umarova et al.

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on November 14, 2015


Figure 1 Lesion overlap of control, extinction and patients neglect. Colour bar indicates the number of overlapping lesions.

Table 2 Hit rates and reaction times during functional MRI experiment for the targets presented in the left and right visual
hemifield

Healthy subjects Control patients Extinction patients Neglect patients


Left Right Left Right Left Right Left Right
Hits rate (%) median; range 100; – 100; – 98; 70–100 100; 80–100 92; 65–100 97; 92–100 25; 0–98 84; 75–100
Reaction time (ms) mean  SEM 324  9 320  9 516  37 477  39 522  56 420  29 641  63 543  36

Performance during functional magnetic the left hemifield was more heterogeneous. In neglect group,
hit rates for the targets in the left visual hemifield varied be-
resonance imaging tween 0 (four patients) and 98.4% (one patient), whereas
The behavioural performance during functional MRI examination patients of all other groups detected almost all of them
was assessed by hit rates, reaction time and number of false (Table 2). All three groups of patients but not healthy subjects
alarms (see Supplementary Material). Hit rates and reaction time were slower in response to the targets in the left than visual in
are shown in Table 2. Detection of the right targets was accurate the right hemifield (paired t-test, P 5 0.01). Control patients
(470%) in all groups. In contrast, the detection of the targets in did not differ from healthy subjects in left versus right
Acute visual neglect and extinction Brain 2011: 134; 3310–3325 | 3317

reaction time lateralization (reaction time for the left detected tar- ones (t = 1.81, P = 0.078), and patients with neglect compared
gets reaction time for the right ones; t = 1.17, P = 0.25), with control patients had a significant left–right reaction time lat-
whereas patients extinction compared with control ones showed eralization (t = 2.86, P 5 0.01) (Fig. 2). Thus, there was a congru-
a strong trend for processing the left targets slower than the right ency between behavioural performance in the functional MRI task
and behavioural neglect testing: patients showed a rightward at-
tentional bias of the visual processing that increased from control
to patients with neglect and manifested with more missed targets
and slower reaction time in the contra- than the ipsilesional visual
field.

Functional magnetic resonance


imaging results
Functional MRI activations within and between groups are shown
in Figs 3–7. The coordinates of peaks of cluster activation are
listed in Supplementary Table 1. Healthy subjects revealed a
widespread bilateral activation of the visuospatial attention

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on November 14, 2015


Figure 2 Differences in reaction time (RT) between targets system (Fig. 3). Activation within the visual system included bilat-
detected in the left versus right visual hemifield (mean  SEM). eral primary and visual association areas, including fusiform gyrus
There were no differences between healthy subjects and control and the lateral occipital cortex. Bilateral activation of the attention
patients. Compared with controls, patients with extinction system included intraparietal sulcus, frontal eye field, pre-
showed a tendency to process targets in the left visual hemifield
supplementary motor area, insula, supramarginal gyrus, posterior
slower than in the right one (P = 0.078); this difference was
inferior frontal gyrus and temporoparietal junction. The activation
significant in patients with neglect compared with control
ones (**P 5 0.01). was lateralized to the right hemisphere in the lateral occipital
cortex, supramarginal gyrus and temporoparietal junction.

Figure 3 Task activations (red) and lesion projections (black) in groups, one-sample t-test (healthy subjects P 5 0.01, family-wise error
corrected; in groups of patients P 5 0.001, uncorrected, extent threshold 10 voxels). While activation associated with the motor response
was similar in all groups (positive control), the pattern of activation in attention-relevant areas was distinct. All peaks of cluster activation
are listed in Supplementary Table 1. FEF = frontal eye field; IPS = intraparietal sulcus; LOC = lateral occipital complex; M1/S1 = primary
motor and sensory cortex; pre-SMA = pre-supplementary motor area; SMG = supramarginal gyrus; TPJ = temporoparietal junction.
3318 | Brain 2011: 134; 3310–3325 R. M. Umarova et al.

In addition to the whole brain contrasts, the activation in the although their visuospatial performance in the right visual field
dorsal attention system and primary visual areas was estimated was accurate and did not differ from patients with extinction.
using a region of interest analysis of parameter estimates in a Patients with neglect compared with healthy subjects showed a
one-way ANOVA and a post hoc two sample t-tests with small cluster of higher activation in the right gyrus angularis (peak
Bonferroni correction in case of a significant main effect of in 51, 69, 30, Fig. 4) that might reflect the perilesional activation
group. We restricted the analysis to regions preserved from as the sign of early reorganization. We would like to emphasize
lesions and thus excluded right temporoparietal junction as that there was no increase of contralesional activation in patients
well as the right posterior inferior frontal gyrus, which with neglect compared with control ones and healthy subjects
were damaged in five and four patients of neglect and extinc- according to both the whole brain and region of interest analyses
tion groups correspondingly. We found a significant group (Figs 4, 5 and 6). The whole-brain comparison between healthy
effect for all left and right regions of interest of the dorsal subjects and neglect groups revealed a reduced activation in the
attention system except for the left lateral occipital cortex: last one not only in the right-hemispheric attention network, but
for the left intraparietal sulcus [F(3, 44) = 2.95, P 5 0.05], also in the left lateral occipital cortex and left posterior inferior
left frontal eye field [F(3, 44) = 8.09, P 5 0.001], right lateral frontal gyrus (Fig. 4). The main difference between control and
occipital cortex [F(3, 44) = 5.85, P 5 0.01], right intraparietal neglect patients was hypoactivation of the right lateral occipital
sulcus [F(3, 44) = 4.23, P = 0.01] and right frontal eye field cortex (Fig. 4). The region of interest analysis showed reduction of
[F(3, 44) = 3.59, P 5 0.05] (Fig. 5). For the primary visual activation in the left frontal eye field, right lateral occipital cortex

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on November 14, 2015


areas, there was a significant group effect only for the right and right intraparietal sulcus compared with control patients
hemisphere [F(3, 44) = 7.86, P 5 0.001], but not for the left (t = 3.22, P 5 0.05; t = 3.55, P 5 0.01; and t = 2.75, P 5 0.05, re-
[F(3, 44) = 0.68, P = 0.57]. spectively; Figs 5 and 6). Neglect patients had a tendency to
Control patients and healthy subjects did not differ in the acti- reduced activation in the left intraparietal sulcus compared with
vation pattern according to both the whole brain and region of the control ones (t = 1.94, P = 0.059) and a significantly lower
interest analysis, except for the higher activation in the left extra- activation in the right primary visual area compared with patients
striate visual cortex (Fig. 6). Thus, in spite of the structural lesion with extinction (t = 4.16, P 5 0.01) and healthy subjects (t = 4.12,
in control patients, the key nodes of the attention system in them P 5 0.01; Supplementary Fig. 1).
were activated comparably to healthy subjects. In the whole-brain analysis, patients with extinction did not
Patients with neglect demonstrated a generally reduced activa- differ from control ones in the activation of the dorsal attention
tion in almost all regions relevant for attention (Figs 3 and 4), system, but they contrasted from all other groups by showing an

Figure 4 Contrasts between groups (two-sample t-test, P 5 0.001, uncorrected, extent threshold 10 voxels). There was no significant
difference between healthy subjects and control patients, healthy subjects 4 patients with extinction, control patients 4 patients with
extinction and patients with neglect 4 control patients.
Acute visual neglect and extinction Brain 2011: 134; 3310–3325 | 3319

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on November 14, 2015


Figure 5 Between group comparison in a region of interest analysis of activation in the left (L) and right (R) dorsal attention systems
(mean  SEM, *P 5 0.05; **P 5 0.01, ***P 5 0.001, after Bonferroni correction). FEF = frontal eye field; IPS = intraparietal sulcus;
LOC = lateral occipital complex.

Figure 6 The whole brain analysis of the main group effect: brain regions that show differences between groups in task-related blood
oxygen level-dependent signal changes during visuospatial processing (between-subject ANOVA, the main effect of group, F-contrast,
P 5 0.001, uncorrected; mean  SEM). IFG L = left posterior inferior frontal gyrus; IPS R = right intraparietal sulcus; LOC L/R = left/right
lateral occipital complex; MFG L = left posterior middle frontal gyrus; preSMA L = left pre-supplementary motor area.
3320 | Brain 2011: 134; 3310–3325 R. M. Umarova et al.

increased activation of the left prefrontal cortex, including poster- time lateralization. However, the hit rate for targets presented in
ior middle frontal gyrus (BA 6/8, peak in 21, 12, 48), posterior the left hemifield correlated with activation in the contralesional
inferior frontal gyrus (BA 44, peak in 42 12 33) and bilateral left hemisphere: posterior middle frontal gyrus (BA 6/8, peak in
pre-supplementary motor area (BA 6) (Figs 4 and 6). In the region 42, 0, 45, Spearman correlation r2 = 0.70, P 5 0.001,
of interest analysis, patients extinction had a significantly higher Supplementary Fig. 3), inferior frontal gyrus (BA 44/45 peak in
activation of the left frontal eye field compared with both healthy 48, 18, 27), prefrontal cortex (BA 10, peak in 30, 48, 9),
subjects and patients with neglect (t = 3.30, P 5 0.05 and pre-supplementary motor area (BA 6, peak in 6, 6, 48) and
t = 4.55, P 5 0.001, respectively), and higher activation of the left intraparietal sulcus (Fig. 7).
right frontal eye field compared with neglect patients (t = 3.15, It is critical to ensure that reduced activation within the atten-
P 5 0.05; Figs 4 and 5), while the right intraparietal sulcus and tion system in neglect group was not caused by an altered blood
right lateral occipital cortex activation did not differ significantly oxygen level-dependent response in the right middle cerebral
from control patients. Thus, patients with extinction were charac- artery territory owing for example to impaired neurovascular
terized by a preserved activation of the dorsal attention system in coupling in acute stroke. To compare the blood oxygen
the right hemisphere and abnormal hyperactivation of the left level-dependent response between groups, we analysed the
dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. motor activation evoked by the right-handed button press for tar-
At present, neglect is understood as the result of an imbalance gets detection. This motor effect showed an involvement of the
between hyperactive left parietal cortex and structurally or func- contralateral motor cortex, and also of the ipsilateral putamen and
tionally impaired right parietal one. To investigate this hypothesis, premotor cortex (between-group full-factorial ANOVA, Fig. 8).

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on November 14, 2015


we compared the activation in these regions in each group. The The latter two regions belong to the territory of the right middle
left parietal cortex was activated significantly stronger than the cerebral artery. We extracted the parameter estimates of the right
right in all groups of patients, including control patients without premotor cortex, right putamen and left primary motor cortex. For
visuospatial deficit (paired t-tests, for control patients t = 4.10, each region of interest, parameter estimates were compared be-
P 5 0.01; patients with extinction t = 3.84, P 5 0.01; patients tween groups using a one-way ANOVA. For the latter two re-
with neglect t = 3.90, P 5 0.01), but not in healthy subjects gions, there were no significant group effects. We found a
(paired t-tests, t = 1.04, P = 0.314). Further, we calculated the lat- significant group effect for the right premotor cortex [F(3,
erality index between left and right intraparietal sulcus activations 44) = 2.96, P = 0.043]. However, separate post hoc two sample
(Supplementary Fig. 2). There was no significant difference in be- t-tests comparing healthy subjects against each group of patients
tween groups [one-way ANOVA, F(3, 44) = 1.393, P = 0.257],
probably due to the high variability particularly in patients with
neglect. Correlation analysis (Pearson correlation) between lateral-
ity indices and left or right hit rates or with left versus right reac-
tion time lateralization also did not reveal any significant results.
Since the functional MRI behavioural performance in patients
with attentional deficit varied substantially, we performed an ex-
ploratory whole-brain voxel-wise correlation analysis with the be-
havioural parameters (left hit rate and left versus right reaction
time lateralization) across extinction and neglect groups. There
was no significant correlation with the left versus right reaction

Figure 8 (A) Motor activation by button press during functional


MRI experiment (main contrast of interest, between-subject
Figure 7 Whole brain voxel-wise correlation in patients with full-factorial ANOVA, family-wise error corrected, P 5 0.05). (B)
attentional deficit between blood oxygen level-dependent re- Plot of the per cent signal changes in the right premotor cortex,
sponse and hit rate for the targets presented in the left hemifield right putamen and left primary motor cortex (mean  SEM).
(P 5 0.001, uncorrected, extent threshold 10 voxels). The left There were no significant differences between groups in the
hit rate in extinction and neglect patients correlated with acti- activation within the left primary motor cortex and right puta-
vation in the contralesional left hemisphere: posterior middle men (one-way ANOVA). Though there was a significant group
frontal gyrus (BA 6/8), posterior inferior frontal gyrus (BA 44/ effect for the right premotor cortex, the t-test comparing those
45), pre-supplementary motor area, intraparietal sulcus and with and without stroke did not yield a significant difference.
prefrontal cortex (BA 10). PM = premotor cortex; M1 = primary motor cortex.
Acute visual neglect and extinction Brain 2011: 134; 3310–3325 | 3321

did not yield a significant difference (control patients t = 1.01, Left parietal hyperactivation is not
P = 0.32; patients with extinction t = 1.58, P = 0.12; patients
with neglect t = 1.65, P = 0.12). This provides evidence for a pre-
specific for acute neglect
served blood oxygen level-dependent response in the undamaged For a long time, neglect was believed to be a result of a functional
territory of the right and left middle cerebral artery in all groups of imbalance between undamaged left and impaired right parietal
patients. cortices that biases attention towards the ipsilesional side of
space (Kinsbourne, 1970). This model was supported by a func-
tional MRI study of Corbetta et al. (2005) showing an abnormal

Discussion pattern of activation in patients with neglect with increased acti-


vation in the left parietal cortex compared with the right. Further,
This is the first study assessing the functional state of the visuo- in this study the left parietal cortex showed a relatively higher
spatial attention system in patients with acute stroke before major response in patients, who detected fewer targets in the left
reorganization processes have progressed. We investigated pa- visual field and responded slower to left as compared with right
tients with comparable right hemisphere infarcts, who differed in visual field targets (Corbetta et al., 2005). It was concluded that
the degree of visuospatial attention deficit: (i) no deficit (control the relative hyperactivity of the left intraparietal sulcus provoked
patients); (ii) extinction; or (iii) neglect, and age-matched healthy such attentional imbalance and consequently a poor left-sided
volunteers. In accordance with the behavioural neglect testing, the performance.

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on November 14, 2015


rightward attentional bias in the functional MRI experiment The present data are in line with those of Corbetta and Shulman
increased from control patients over those with extinction to pa- (2002), as neglect was provoked by a lesion of the ventral atten-
tients with neglect, while processing of targets in the right hemi- tion system with the resulting relative imbalance between activa-
field was almost preserved in all groups (470%). Although tions in the left and right parietal regions (Corbetta et al., 2005).
neglect patients detected fewer targets in the right hemifield com- However, first, all patients groups, including those without neg-
pared with control ones, they did not differ in the rightward spa- lect, demonstrated such a ‘push-pull’ pattern of parietal activation.
tial processing from patients with extinction. This demonstrates a Moreover, groups of patients did not differ between each other in
behavioural continuum in visuospatial abilities in these groups and the laterality indices. Secondly, the laterality indices of the parietal
indicates that the functional MRI experiment probed into a clinic- activation did not correlate with severity of the right attentional
ally relevant feature of all examined groups. The functional MRI bias. And finally, the left parietal activation was not higher in pa-
paradigm aimed at activating the visuospatial attention system as tients with neglect than in other groups and even tended to be
a whole, including both orientation and target detection compo- less pronounced than in control patients. Taken together, the rela-
nents. The obtained activation in healthy subjects and control pa- tive left–right parietal imbalance alone did not sufficiently explain
tients is generally in line with the results of the previous studies the rightward attentional bias, but rather represented a conse-
(Corbetta et al., 2000; Perry and Zeki, 2000). On the other hand, quence of the acute severe right hemisphere structural damage,
we used a Posner-like paradigm that did not examine the visual in other words an epiphenomenon but not the cause of neglect.
competition between targets and distractors, which might be con- Reduced activation of right parietal cortex and right lateral occipi-
trolled by the distinct brain structures (Vandenberghe et al., tal cortex were more specific neural correlates of neglect (Figs 3, 5
2005). Thus, the attention network, which is examined in the and 6).
present study, might not be completely identical to that one, In patients with neglect, activation of the left frontal eye field
which is pathologically impaired during neglecting of targets that was also reduced. This may explain why not only the responses to
presented with distractors. Although this issue may be addressed left targets, but also those to right ones, become progressively
in future works, the absence of functional MRI studies with visual slower as severity of neglect increases (Bartolomeo and Chokron,
competition in patients with neglect points to the experimental 1999); and why patients with left hemispatial neglect may also
difficulties especially in acute stroke patients. Furthermore, patients present the additional neglect in extreme right space (Ellis et al.,
with neglect did show the phenomenon of neglect during the 2006). The bilateral hypoactivation of the attentional centres may
present functional MRI experiment even without visual reflect a pathomechanism of the non-lateralized deficits in patients
competition. with neglect. For example, the non-spatial selective attention and
All three groups of patients were comparable in lesion anat- spatial working memory deficits, which are present in neglect, do
omy: they had embolic infarcts in the right middle cerebral not necessarily worsen towards one side of space (Husain et al.,
artery territory. Control patients had lesions in the right fronto- 1997; Malhotra et al., 2005). At the same time, there are reports
temporal cortex including the adjacent white matter, i.e. they of a pathological over-excitability of the left hemisphere in neglect
showed lesions typically associated with neglect (Husain and but not in control patients several weeks after stroke (Koch et al.,
Kennard, 1996; Karnath et al., 2004). The lesions found in 2008). This may potentially be explained through the differences
patients with extinction were in line with the literature findings in the lesion size between patients with and without neglect: pa-
(Lynch and McLaren, 1989; Ticini et al., 2010; Vossel et al., tients without large structural lesions and without neglect tend to
2011). In line with the previous data, in the present study, pa- recover faster. This might be associated with better restoration and
tients with neglect had significantly larger infarcts than other rebalancing of brain activation in control patients compared with
groups of patients. neglect ones.
3322 | Brain 2011: 134; 3310–3325 R. M. Umarova et al.

Distinct pattern of activation in insufficient time for such compensation or insufficient individual
neuroplastic resources might prevent patients with neglect in
extinction acute stroke to recruit the left hemisphere for impaired attentional
The activation pattern in patients with extinction was distinct from functions. Thus, we argue that the additional activation of the left
all other examined groups and showed an increased activation prefrontal cortex in patients with extinction was not the effect of
and additional involvement of the dorso- and ventrolateral pre- transcallosal disinhibiton, but rather reflected the functional com-
frontal cortex in the left hemisphere, including posterior middle pensation after critical damage of the attentional centres in the
frontal gyrus, posterior inferior frontal gyrus and bilateral right hemisphere. We cannot exclude that the additional pre-
pre-supplementary motor area. At the same time according to frontal activation in the left hemisphere modulated the right
the region of interest analysis, patients with extinction did not dorsal attention network and thus supported the contralesional
differ from control patients in the state of the dorsal attention visual processing. This is in line with the previous studies demon-
system including the left intraparietal sulcus. Compared with pa- strating that spontaneous recovery from neglect in the later stage
tients with neglect, those with extinction had a stronger activation was associated with increase of neural activity not only in the
of bilateral frontal eye field and left posterior inferior frontal gyrus. right parietal cortex but also in the left dorsolateral prefrontal
These findings suggest that extinction group was characterized by and left parietal cortex (Corbetta et al., 2005; Thimm et al.,
a distinct state of the attention system and not by its ‘milder’ 2008) or left temporo-occipital and medial temporal cortex
dysfunction compared with neglect. It may be argued that add- (Luaute et al., 2006).

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on November 14, 2015


itional activation of the left prefrontal cortex in patients with ex- Several studies demonstrated a positive effect of the contrale-
tinction reflected the transcallosal disinhibition with resulting sional activation on the motor and language recovery in stroke
hyperactivity of the left hemisphere. That would be in line with patients (Weiller et al., 1992, 1995; Musso et al., 1999;
the theory of hemispheric rivalry: the activation of undamaged Johansen-Berg et al., 2002; Gerloff et al., 2006; Saur et al.,
hemisphere suppresses the weaker, damaged one (Kinsbourne, 2006; Schaechter and Perdue, 2008; Bestmann et al., 2010;
1970). In this case, the contralesional prefrontal hyperactivation Riecker et al., 2010). As for the motor and language systems,
might represent a pathomechanism of extinction. However, ac- similar mechanisms may apply to recovery of visuospatial atten-
cording to this assumption, a greater release of the left hemisphere tion. We argue that the activity of the left hemisphere structures
and more severe left–right imbalance are to be expected after the may be functionally relevant for the recovery of visuospatial at-
larger right-sided strokes typical for neglect. But patients with neg- tention and may be a substrate for a functional compensation
lect did not show any absolute hyperactivation within the left after the large right hemispheric lesion.
hemisphere. Thus, visual extinction in the present study may not
be explained through the increased contralesional activation.
Activation of the visual cortex in spatial
The question remains as to what happens during bilateral stimu-
lation, which is specifically impaired in extinction. Does the hyper- attention deficit
function of the left prefrontal cortex suppress the perception of In the present study, the right lateral occipital cortex and V1,
the contralesional stimuli during bilateral stimulation? The func- which were spared and remote from lesions, had a pathologically
tional MRI paradigm in the present study was not specific for an reduced activation in patients with neglect. Similar results were
extinction deficit, since there were no trials with bilateral visual demonstrated in the previous studies (Lumer et al., 1998; Driver
stimulation. But the previous data with bilateral presentation of et al., 2001; Vuilleumier et al., 2001, 2008; Rees et al., 2002;
other kind of stimuli (faces) demonstrated that the conscious per- Corbetta et al., 2005). Studies on attentional modulation of
ception of faces in the left visual field increased activity in the left early visual areas (V1/V2) in healthy subjects showed that con-
frontal and parietal areas besides the right visual areas (Vuilleumier scious perception of stimuli is possible only through a functional
et al., 2001, 2002; Rees et al., 2002). Bilateral trials with an ex- interaction between the visual and frontoparietal cortex (Buchel
tinguished faces in the left visual field did not show activation of et al., 1998; Lumer et al., 1998; Lumer and Rees, 1999). In neg-
the left parietal or left frontal areas; in other words, activity in the lect and extinction patients, V1 was activated without visual
left attentional centres was not associated with the phenomenon awareness, but this activation was stronger for consciously de-
of extinction. tected versus extinguished stimuli (Rees et al., 2000, 2002;
In light of the theory of the limited attentional capacity after Driver et al., 2001; Vuilleumier et al., 2001, 2002). The abnormal
right hemisphere damage, the contralesional activation might rep- top–down modulation from the frontoparietal cortex in neglect
resent some residual visuospatial function sustaining contralesional might decrease both stimulus-evoked responses and spatial select-
visuospatial processing. In the present study, the left-lateralized ivity of visual neurons, weakening the relative salience of stimuli
activation correlated positively and strongly with the rate of re- presented in the left visual field (Corbetta et al., 2005).
sponses to the left targets (Fig. 7). This could not be explained by The study of Vuilleumier et al. (2008) also demonstrated
Kinsbourne’s hemispheric rivalry hypothesis and indicates a com- attention-dependent modulation of the extra- and striate visual
pensatory nature of the contralesional activations. The left hemi- cortex in patients with visual neglect. When the attention load
sphere functional compensation might fail in neglect due to the was high, the right visual cortex showed pathologically reduced
large lesions that provoke an extensive diaschisis and result in a responses particularly in the right V4/TEO to task-irrelevant
transhemispheric downregulation in acute stroke. In addition, checkerboards in the contralateral left hemifield, whereas left
Acute visual neglect and extinction Brain 2011: 134; 3310–3325 | 3323

visual areas showed no such reduction in their contralateral re-


sponse. Thus, the extrastriate areas, supporting higher order Supplementary material
visual processing, became ‘blind’ to the visual stimuli. Similarly in Supplementary material is available at Brain online.
animal studies, the different attentional loads under the same
visual input modulated the neuronal activity in the lateral occipi-
tal cortex (Treue and Maunsell, 1996, 1999). Hypoactivation
of the right extrastriate area, labelled as lateral occipital cortex,
References
as the most consistent correlate of neglect in the present Ashburner J, Friston KJ. Unified segmentation. Neuroimage 2005; 26:
839–51.
study was not previously described. Our data confirmed that
Baron JC. Depression of energy metabolism in distant brain structures:
lateral occipital cortex is a necessary node in the visuospatial studies with positron emission tomography in stroke patients. Semin
processing, through which the high-order attentional centres Neurol 1989; 9: 281–5.
modulate and receive bottom–up inputs from V1, and the lesion Bartolomeo P, Chokron S. Left unilateral neglect or right hyperattention?
Neurology 1999; 53: 2023–7.
of which may provoke neglect (Payne et al., 1996; Saalmann
Bartolomeo P, Thiebaut de Schotten M, Doricchi F. Left unilateral neglect
et al., 2007). as a disconnection syndrome. Cereb Cortex 2007; 17: 2479–90.
Beis JM, Keller C, Morin N, Bartolomeo P, Bernati T, Chokron S, et al.
Right spatial neglect after left hemisphere stroke: qualitative and quan-
Functional magnetic resonance titative study. Neurology 2004; 63: 1600–5.
Bestmann S, Swayne O, Blankenburg F, Ruff CC, Teo J, Weiskopf N,

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on November 14, 2015


imaging in acute stroke et al. The role of contralesional dorsal premotor cortex after stroke as
studied with concurrent TMS-fMRI. J Neurosci 2010; 30: 11926–37.
Functional MRI examination of acute stroke patients and its inter- Bird CM, Malhotra P, Parton A, Coulthard E, Rushworth MF, Husain M.
pretation are difficult (Saur et al., 2006). In our study, several Visual neglect after right posterior cerebral artery infarction. J Neurol
measures were taken to reduce the danger of misinterpretations Neurosurg Psychiatry 2006; 77: 1008–12.
due to the blood oxygen level-dependent response changes in Bogousslavsky J, Miklossy J, Regli F, Deruaz JP, Assal G, Delaloye B.
Subcortical neglect: neuropsychological, SPECT, and neuropathological
ischaemic stroke. First, strict exclusion criteria were applied, so
correlations with anterior choroidal artery territory infarction. Ann
that patients with severe vessel stenosis or occlusion were not Neurol 1988; 23: 448–52.
included in the study. Secondly, we used an adequate reference Brain WR. Visual diisorientation with special reference to lesions of the
sample, since the structural lesion in acute stroke leads to a chain right cerebral hemisphere. Brain 1941; 64: 273–90.
Buchel C, Josephs O, Rees G, Turner R, Frith CD, Friston KJ. The func-
of metabolic and vascular brain changes in distant, undamaged
tional anatomy of attention to visual motion. A functional MRI study.
and even contralesional regions, which may also be an effect of Brain 1998; 121: 1281–94.
diaschisis (Monakow, 1906; Baron, 1989). We therefore included Buxbaum LJ, Ferraro MK, Veramonti T, Farne A, Whyte J, Ladavas E,
a control group of patients without any visuospatial deficit but et al. Hemispatial neglect: Subtypes, neuroanatomy, and disability.
with acute territorial right middle cerebral artery stroke, which Neurology 2004; 62: 749–56.
Cicek M, Gitelman D, Hurley RS, Nobre A, Mesulam M. Anatomical
demonstrated a strong and spared blood oxygen level-dependent
physiology of spatial extinction. Cereb Cortex 2007; 17: 2892–8.
response and a pattern of activation similar to that of healthy Committeri G, Pitzalis S, Galati G, Patria F, Pelle G, Sabatini U, et al.
subjects. Neural bases of personal and extrapersonal neglect in humans. Brain
On the other hand, patients with neglect had larger infarcts 2007; 130: 431–41.
Corbetta M, Kincade MJ, Lewis C, Snyder AZ, Sapir A. Neural basis and
that might provoke an abnormal blood oxygen level-dependent
recovery of spatial attention deficits in spatial neglect. Nat Neurosci
response in undamaged tissue of the right middle cerebral artery 2005; 8: 1603–10.
territory and vascular ischaemic effects might mimic a true hypo- Corbetta M, Kincade JM, Ollinger JM, McAvoy MP, Shulman GL.
function. Therefore, we investigated the blood oxygen Voluntary orienting is dissociated from target detection in human pos-
level-dependent response in the right middle cerebral artery terri- terior parietal cortex. Nat Neurosci 2000; 3: 292–7.
Corbetta M, Shulman GL. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven
tory to right-handed button presses. As was previously shown
attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 2002; 3: 201–15.
(Weiller et al., 1992, 1996; Johansen-Berg et al., 2002; Witt Critchley M. The phenomenon of tactile inattention with special refer-
et al., 2008), active movements (i.e. the button press) were asso- ence to parietal lesions. Brain 1949; 72: 538–61.
ciated with activation of ipsilateral premotor cortex and basal Crinion J, Ashburner J, Leff A, Brett M, Price C, Friston K. Spatial nor-
ganglia besides the contralateral motor areas (Fig. 8). The absence malization of lesioned brains: performance evaluation and impact on
fMRI analysis. Neuroimage 2007; 37: 866–75.
of differences between patients and healthy subjects provides evi- Doricchi F, Thiebaut de Schotten M, Tomaiuolo F, Bartolomeo P. White
dence for a preserved blood oxygen level-dependent response and matter (dis)connections and gray matter (dys)functions in visual neg-
indicates a true hypofunction of the bilateral attention system in lect: gaining insights into the brain networks of spatial awareness.
the present study. Cortex 2008; 44: 983–95.
Driver J, Mattingley JB. Parietal neglect and visual awareness. Nat
Neurosci 1998; 1: 17–22.
Driver J, Vuilleumier P, Eimer M, Rees G. Functional magnetic reson-
ance imaging and evoked potential correlates of conscious and uncon-
Funding scious vision in parietal extinction patients. Neuroimage 2001; 14:
S68–75.
Postdoctoral fellowship of the German Academic Exchange Service Eickhoff SB, Stephan KE, Mohlberg H, Grefkes C, Fink GR, Amunts K,
(DAAD). et al. A new SPM toolbox for combining probabilistic
3324 | Brain 2011: 134; 3310–3325 R. M. Umarova et al.

cytoarchitectonic maps and functional imaging data. Neuroimage Luaute J, Michel C, Rode G, Pisella L, Jacquin-Courtois S, Costes N, et al.
2005; 25: 1325–35. Functional anatomy of the therapeutic effects of prism adaptation on
Ellis AW, Jordan JL, Sullivan CA. Unilateral neglect is not unilateral: evi- left neglect. Neurology 2006; 66: 1859–67.
dence for additional neglect of extreme right space. Cortex 2006; 42: Lumer ED, Friston KJ, Rees G. Neural correlates of perceptual rivalry in
861–8. the human brain. Science 1998; 280: 1930–4.
Friston K, Ashburner JT, Kiebel S, Nichols T, Penny W, editors. Statistical Lynch JC, McLaren JW. Deficits of visual attention and saccadic eye
parametric mapping. The analysis of functional brain images. London: movements after lesions of parietooccipital cortex in monkeys.
Academic Press; 2007. p. 647. J Neurophysiol 1989; 61: 74–90.
Gerloff C, Bushara K, Sailer A, Wassermann EM, Chen R, Matsuoka T, Malhotra P, Jager HR, Parton A, Greenwood R, Playford ED,
et al. Multimodal imaging of brain reorganization in motor areas of the Brown MM, et al. Spatial working memory capacity in unilateral neg-
contralesional hemisphere of well recovered patients after capsular lect. Brain 2005; 128: 424–35.
stroke. Brain 2006; 129: 791–808. Marshall JW, Baker HF, Ridley RM. Contralesional neglect in monkeys
Giessing C, Thiel CM, Stephan KE, Rosler F, Fink GR. Visuospatial atten- with small unilateral parietal cortical ablations. Behav Brain Res 2002;
tion: how to measure effects of infrequent, unattended events in a 136: 257–65.
blocked stimulus design. Neuroimage 2004; 23: 1370–81. Mesulam MM. A cortical network for directed attention and unilateral
Glascher J. Visualization of group inference data in functional neuroima- neglect. Ann Neurol 1981; 10: 309–25.
ging. Neuroinformatics 2009; 7: 73–82. Mesulam MM. Large-scale neurocognitive networks and distributed pro-
Gottesman RF, Kleinman JT, Davis C, Heidler-Gary J, Newhart M, cessing for attention, language, and memory. Ann Neurol 1990; 28:
Kannan V, et al. Unilateral neglect is more severe and common in 597–613.
older patients with right hemispheric stroke. Neurology 2008; 71: Monakow CV. Aphasie und diaschisis. Neurologisches Centralbaltt 1906;
1439–44. 25: 1026–38.

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on November 14, 2015


Halligan PW, Fink GR, Marshall JC, Vallar G. Spatial cognition: evidence Mort DJ, Malhotra P, Mannan SK, Rorden C, Pambakian A, Kennard C,
from visual neglect. Trends Cogn Sci 2003; 7: 125–33. et al. The anatomy of visual neglect. Brain 2003; 126: 1986–97.
Hamzei F, Knab R, Weiller C, Rother J. The influence of extra- and Musso M, Weiller C, Kiebel S, Muller SP, Bulau P, Rijntjes M.
intracranial artery disease on the BOLD signal in FMRI. Neuroimage Training-induced brain plasticity in aphasia. Brain 1999; 122: 1781–90.
2003; 20: 1393–9. Payne BR, Lomber SG, Geeraerts S, van der Gucht E, Vandenbussche E.
Heilman KM, Van Den Abell T. Right hemisphere dominance for atten- Reversible visual hemineglect. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1996; 93:
tion: the mechanism underlying hemispheric asymmetries of inatten- 290–4.
tion (neglect). Neurology 1980; 30: 327–30. Perry RJ, Zeki S. The neurology of saccades and covert shifts in spatial
Heilman K, Watson R, Valenstein E. Neglect and related disorders. In: attention: an event-related fMRI study. Brain 2000; 123: 2273–88.
Heilman K, Valenstein E, editors. Clinical neuropsychology. Oxford Posner MI, Walker JA, Friedrich FJ, Rafal RD. Effects of parietal injury on
University Press; 2003. p. 296–346. covert orienting of attention. J Neurosci 1984; 4: 1863–74.
Hillis AE, Chang S, Heidler-Gary J, Newhart M, Kleinman JT, Davis C, Rees G, Wojciulik E, Clarke K, Husain M, Frith C, Driver J. Neural cor-
et al. Neural correlates of modality-specific spatial extinction. J Cogn relates of conscious and unconscious vision in parietal extinction.
Neurosci 2006; 18: 1889–98. Neurocase 2002; 8: 387–93.
Hillis AE, Newhart M, Heidler J, Barker PB, Herskovits EH, Degaonkar M. Rees G, Wojciulik E, Clarke K, Husain M, Frith C, Driver J. Unconscious
Anatomy of spatial attention: insights from perfusion imaging and activation of visual cortex in the damaged right hemisphere of a par-
hemispatial neglect in acute stroke. J Neurosci 2005; 25: 3161–7. ietal patient with extinction. Brain 2000; 123: 1624–33.
Husain M, Kennard C. Visual neglect associated with frontal lobe infarc- Riecker A, Groschel K, Ackermann H, Schnaudigel S, Kassubek J,
tion. J Neurol 1996; 243: 652–7. Kastrup A. The role of the unaffected hemisphere in motor recovery
Husain M, Shapiro K, Martin J, Kennard C. Abnormal temporal dynamics after stroke. Hum Brain Mapp 2010; 31: 1017–29.
of visual attention in spatial neglect patients. Nature 1997; 385: Ringman JM, Saver JL, Woolson RF, Clarke WR, Adams HP. Frequency,
154–6. risk factors, anatomy, and course of unilateral neglect in an acute
Jehkonen M, Laihosalo M, Kettunen JE. Impact of neglect on functional stroke cohort. Neurology 2004; 63: 468–74.
outcome after stroke: a review of methodological issues and recent Rorden C, Fruhmann Berger M, Karnath HO. Disturbed line bisection is
research findings. Restor Neurol Neurosci 2006; 24: 209–15. associated with posterior brain lesions. Brain Res 2006; 1080: 17–25.
Johansen-Berg H, Rushworth MF, Bogdanovic MD, Kischka U, Saalmann YB, Pigarev IN, Vidyasagar TR. Neural mechanisms of visual
Wimalaratna S, Matthews PM. The role of ipsilateral premotor attention: how top-down feedback highlights relevant locations.
cortex in hand movement after stroke. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA Science 2007; 316: 1612–5.
2002; 99: 14518–23. Saur D, Lange R, Baumgaertner A, Schraknepper V, Willmes K,
Karnath HO, Ferber S, Himmelbach M. Spatial awareness is a function of Rijntjes M, et al. Dynamics of language reorganization after stroke.
the temporal not the posterior parietal lobe. Nature 2001; 411: 950–3. Brain 2006; 129: 1371–84.
Karnath HO, Fruhmann Berger M, Kuker W, Rorden C. The anatomy of Schaechter JD, Perdue KL. Enhanced cortical activation in the contrale-
spatial neglect based on voxelwise statistical analysis: a study of 140 sional hemisphere of chronic stroke patients in response to motor skill
patients. Cereb Cortex 2004; 14: 1164–72. challenge. Cereb Cortex 2008; 18: 638–47.
Karnath HO, Himmelbach M, Kuker W. The cortical substrate of visual Shinoura N, Suzuki Y, Yamada R, Tabei Y, Saito K, Yagi K. Damage to
extinction. Neuroreport 2003; 14: 437–42. the right superior longitudinal fasciculus in the inferior parietal lobe
Kinsbourne M. A model for the mechanism of unilateral neglect of space. plays a role in spatial neglect. Neuropsychologia 2009; 47: 2600–3.
Trans Am Neurol Assoc 1970; 95: 143–6. Stone SP, Halligan PW, Marshall JC, Greenwood RJ. Unilateral neglect:
Koch G, Oliveri M, Cheeran B, Ruge D, Lo Gerfo E, Salerno S, et al. a common but heterogeneous syndrome. Neurology 1998; 50:
Hyperexcitability of parietal-motor functional connections in the intact 1902–5.
left-hemisphere of patients with neglect. Brain 2008; 131: 3147–55. Thimm M, Fink GR, Sturm W. Neural correlates of recovery from acute
Krug K. A common neuronal code for perceptual processes in visual hemispatial neglect. Restor Neurol Neurosci 2008; 26: 481–92.
cortex? Comparing choice and attentional correlates in V5/MT. Ticini LF, de Haan B, Klose U, Nagele T, Karnath HO. The role of
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2004; 359: 929–41. temporo-parietal cortex in subcortical visual extinction. J Cogn
Liu TT. Efficiency, power, and entropy in event-related fMRI with mul- Neurosci 2010; 22: 2141–50.
tiple trial types. Part II: design of experiments. Neuroimage 2004; 21: Treue S, Maunsell JH. Attentional modulation of visual motion processing
401–13. in cortical areas MT and MST. Nature 1996; 382: 539–41.
Acute visual neglect and extinction Brain 2011: 134; 3310–3325 | 3325

Treue S, Maunsell JH. Effects of attention on the processing of motion in fMRI in a parietal patient with visual extinction and spatial neglect.
macaque middle temporal and medial superior temporal visual cortical Neuropsychologia 2002; 40: 2156–66.
areas. J Neurosci 1999; 19: 7591–602. Vuilleumier P, Sagiv N, Hazeltine E, Poldrack RA, Swick D, Rafal RD,
Umarova RM, Saur D, Schnell S, Kaller CP, Vry MS, Glauche V, et al. et al. Neural fate of seen and unseen faces in visuospatial neglect: a
Structural connectivity for visuospatial attention: significance of ventral combined event-related functional MRI and event-related potential
pathways. Cereb Cortex 2010; 20: 121–9. study. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2001; 98: 3495–500.
Urbanski M, Thiebaut De Schotten M, Rodrigo S, Catani M, Vuilleumier P, Schwartz S, Verdon V, Maravita A, Hutton C, Husain M,
Oppenheim C, Touzé E. Brain networks of spatial awareness: et al. Abnormal attentional modulation of retinotopic cortex in parietal
Evidence from diffusion tensor imaging tractography. J Neurol patients with spatial neglect. Curr Biol 2008; 18: 1525–9.
Neurosurg Psychiatry 2008; 79: 598–601. Weiller C, Chollet F, Friston KJ, Wise RJ, Frackowiak RS. Functional re-
Vallar G. Extrapersonal visual unilateral spatial neglect and its neuroanat- organization of the brain in recovery from striatocapsular infarction in
omy. Neuroimage 2001; 14: S52–8. man. Ann Neurol 1992; 31: 463–72.
Vallar G, Perani D. The anatomy of unilateral neglect after right- Weiller C, Isensee C, Rijntjes M, Huber W, Müller S, Bier D, et al.
hemisphere stroke lesions. A clinical/CT-scan correlation study in Recovery from Wernicke’s aphasia: a positron emission tomographic
man. Neuropsychologia 1986; 24: 609–22. study. Ann Neurol 1995; 37: 723–32.
Vallar G, Rusconi ML, Bignamini L, Geminiani G, Perani D. Anatomical Weiller C, Juptner M, Fellows S, Rijntjes M, Leonhardt G, Kiebel S, et al.
correlates of visual and tactile extinction in humans: a clinical CT scan Brain representation of active and passive movements. Neuroimage
study. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1994; 57: 464–70. 1996; 4: 105–10.
Vandenberghe R, Geeraerts S, Molenberghs P, Lafosse C, Weiller C, Willmes K, Reiche W, Thron A, Isensee C, Buell U, et al. The
Vandenbulcke M, Peeters K, et al. Attentional responses to unattended case of aphasia or neglect after striatocapsular infarction. Brain 1993;
stimuli in human parietal cortex. Brain 2005; 128: 2843–57. 116: 1509–25.

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on November 14, 2015


Verdon V, Schwartz S, Lovblad KO, Hauert CA, Vuilleumier P. Wilson B, Cockburn J, Haligan PW. Behavioural inattention test.
Neuroanatomy of hemispatial neglect and its functional components: Titchfield, Hants: Thames Valley Company; 1987.
a study using voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping. Brain 2010; 133: Witt ST, Laird AR, Meyerand ME. Functional neuroimaging correlates of
880–94. finger-tapping task variations: an ALE meta-analysis. Neuroimage
Vossel S, Eschenbeck P, Weiss PH, Weidner R, Saliger J, Karbe H, et al. 2008; 42: 343–56.
Visual extinction in relation to visuospatial neglect after right-hemispheric Zaitsev M, Dold C, Sakas G, Hennig J, Speck O. Magnetic resonance
stroke: quantitative assessment and statistical lesion-symptom mapping. imaging of freely moving objects: prospective real-time motion correc-
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2011; 82: 862–8. tion using an external optical motion tracking system. Neuroimage
Vuilleumier PO, Rafal RD. A systematic study of visual extinction. 2006; 31: 1038–50.
Between- and within-field deficits of attention in hemispatial neglect. Zaitsev M, Hennig J, Speck O. Point spread function mapping with par-
Brain 2000; 123: 1263–79. allel imaging techniques and high acceleration factors: fast, robust, and
Vuilleumier P, Armony JL, Clarke K, Husain M, Driver J, Dolan RJ. Neural flexible method for echo-planar imaging distortion correction. Magn
response to emotional faces with and without awareness: event-related Reson Med 2004; 52: 1156–66.

You might also like