Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

A study of right unilateral spatial neglect in

left hemispheric lesions: the difference


between right-handed and non-right-handed
-

post-stroke patients
Maeshima S, Shigeno K, Dohi N, Kajiwara T, Komai N. A study of right S.Maeshima ’, K. Shigeno’, N. Dohi ’,
unilateral spatial neglect in left hemispheric lesions: the difference T. Kajiwara’, N. Komai3
between right-handed and non-right-handed post-stroke patients. ’
Departments of Rehabilitation Medicine, Fujita
Acta Neurol Scand 1992: 85: 418-424. Health University, Hisai, Neurology, lzu
Nirayama Rehabilitation Hospital, Shizuoka,
Neurological Surgery, Wakayama
We report 20 cases of right unilateral spatial neglect caused by lesions in Medical College, Japan
the left cerebral hemisphere. Differences in neuropsychological symptoms and
lesion sites are discussed in connection with handedness. Of the
right-handed patients, 6 had severe aphasia, 4 had Gerstmann’s syndrome,
and 1 had pure agraphia, but unilateral spatial neglect in these cases Key words: unilateral spatial neglect:
disappeared after a number of months. Six of the non-right-handed patients cerebrovascular disorders; agnosia;
had moderate-to-severe aphasia, while the other 3 cases had no aphasia left-hemisphere: cerebral dominance.
at all. Eight of the 9 cases in this group continued to have right unilateral S. Maeshima, Department of Rehabilitation
spatial neglect for more than 6 months. Lesion site as determined by CT Medicine, Nanakuri-Sanatorium, Fujita Health
differed as to hemisphere, but all fell into the common area previously University, 424-1, Oodori-cho. Hisai, 514-12
mentioned in connection with such disorders: i.e.. the temporal, parietal Mie, Japan.
and occipital lobes. Accepted for publication November 14, 1991

Unilateral spatial neglect refers to a condition in in the right hemisphere. Meanwhile, Gainotti et al.
which the patient disregards objects located in one (8) studied cases of lesions in the right hemisphere,
side of the patient’s field of perception (1-3). Brain pointing out that there is no appreciable difference
(1) first reported on three cases of left unilateral between the hemisphere when it comes to omissions
spatial neglect and suggested that the condition was of portions of the figure at limited areas around the
related to lesions in the right hemisphere. Since then, peripheries; however, in right hemispheric lesions
similar impairments in visual disorientation and there is a temporary tendency to leave figures in-
hemi-inattention have been considered as symptoms complete. Caltagirone et al. (9) and Ogura et al. (10)
of lesions in the non-dominant hemisphere. Left uni- studied patients with lesions of both the right and left
lateral spatial neglect has also been noted in cases hemispheres. By comparing drawings, they found
of crossed aphasia in the dextral where it is consid- that neither side is omitted in cases of right unilat-
ered that lateralization of higher cortical function is eral spatial neglect caused by left hemispheric le-
incomplete (4). Such observations have led us to sions. There is, instead, distortion of the figure over-
believe that strong laterality exists for the right hemi- all. However their study dealt mainly with right-
sphere. Right unilateral spatial neglect is seen to handed cases. To date there are still few reports
appear occasionally in cases of left hemispheric le- concerning patients who are left-handed or at least
sions (5). There has also been extensive discussion not fully right-handed. Kawahara et al. (11) com-
of the qualitative differences between the two states. pleted a study of 100 cases of cerebral hemorrhage
Zarit & Kahn (6) took note of the fact that many and stated that left-handedness was thought to be a
such lesions in the left hemisphere are accompanied factor in all five cases of right unilateral spatial ne-
by aphasia and dementia, making it impossible to glect caused by left hemispheric lesions. Masure et al.
conduct thorough testing. They also postulate that (12) described that the hemispheric cerebral organi-
right hemispheric lesions occur far more frequently zation with respect to the mediation of visuospatial
than indicated by the literature. Albert (7) also main- performance does not differ between right-handers
tains that there is no difference in the frequency with and left-handers.
which lesions occur in the right or left hemispheres, Thus, many points remain to be clarified regard-
but that the more serious cases are generally found ing the relation among handedness, hemispheric

418
Right unilateral spatial neglect

dominance, lesion site and accompanying neuropsy- Figure copying. This entailed a picture of a flower and
chological symptoms. Therefore, we investigated the perspective drawing of a 3-D object.
correlation with handedness in 20 cases of right uni- Controls were asked to perform tasks (b) (c) as
lateral spatial neglect caused by lesions in the left well, and no anomalies were observed.
hemisphere.
Anosognosia and hemiasomatognosia
Subjects and methods
All assessments were made by only one physician
Subjects included 20 CVA patients (14 of cerebral (SM) by Mori’s method (14).
hemorrhage and 6 of thrombosis) who, upon admis- The examiner asked questions such as, “What
sion to this facility, showed signs of right unilateral feels funny?” “Is there anything wrong with your leg
spatial neglect in standard neuropsychologcal tests or hand?” “Can you move your right leg or hand?”
(details to follow). Lesions located in the left hemi- (the examiner indicates the patient’s right leg or
sphere in all cases were later confirmed with CT. hand) “Is it hard to see things on the right side?” The
There were 13 men and 7 women ranging in age from patient’s responses were recorded. Patients who
31 to 77 years (mean: 60.8 years). The time from either were not aware of or who denied having visual
onset of the condition to admission varied from 2 problems were classified as anosognosic for visual
weeks to 6 months (mean: 7.31 weeks). Handedness field defect. Testing of this nature was not possible
was determined using the Kertesz & Sheppard test for the 11 cases with aphasia.
(13). Nine of the 20 subjects were left-handed from Disregard for one side of the body (hemiasomatog-
birth. Eight had a family member who was left- nosia) was observed by daily monitoring of patients
handed. Eight of these nine were ambidextrous, hav- in the hospital. For example, it was classified as
ing been trained in childhood to perform certain abnormal if patients disregarded one side of the face
tasks with the right hand, such as writing or using or body while shaving, applying make-up, arranging
chopsticks. The remaining 11 cases were all right- hair or washing the face, etc.
handed (Table 1).
In addition to neuropsychological examinations Aphasia
such as visual field (confrontation test), motor pa-
ralysis and sensory disturbance, the following neu- Aphasia was diagnosed using the SLTA (Standard
ropsychological tests were performed. Language Test for Aphasia) (15) which is the most
widely used in Japan. Results were evaluated ac-
cording to the scale devised by Hasegawa et al. (16)
Unilateral spatial neglect
based on degrees of severity ranging from O j l O (most
The following diagnostic tests were conducted to severe) to lOjl0 (mildest).
judge the degree of unilateral spatial neglect:
Apraxia
Line bisection test. The patient was asked to bisect
a 20 cm line down on a standard letter-size (A4, By evaluating the patient’s performance on a stan-
2Ox26cm) sheet of white paper. The same task dardized test of higher motor functions (17), we were
given to 34 controls (1 1 men, 23 women; mean age: able to judge attendant signs of apractognosia. Test
57.9 & 13.24 years) revealed a standard mean error involved buccofacial praxis such as clucking with the
of only 2.00 2.10 mm. The standard for task fail- tongue and clearing the throat, ritual praxis such as
ure was therefore set at an error of 1 6 . 5 mm or bowing (in greeting), use of everyday objects includ-
twice the mean error of the controls. Abnormalities ing a comb and toothbrush, and dressing oneself.
were assessed, as follows: 6.5 mm to 13 mm as mild, We evaluated each case for the above neuropsy-
13mm to 19.5 mm as moderate, and more than chological elements, then divided cases for compari-
19.5 mm as severe. son into a non-right-handed group, and a right-
handed group. All cases were examined by CT, and
Cancellation task. Based on Albert’s method (7),we the lesion site was classfied for correlations with
prepared a sheet of white A4 paper (20 x 26 cm) with handedness.
40 lines drawn thereon and asked patients to mark
the lines. Oversight of less than one fourth of A4 Results
paper was assessed as mildly abnormal, less than Neurological symptoms
half left oversight as moderately abnormal and ad-
ditional partial oversight of the right half as severely We investigated the neurological and neuropsycho-
abnormal. logical findings of the patients.
Initial neurological findings included, 18 cases of

419
Table 1, Clinical features and progress of the right unilateral spatial neglect
&
8'
P,
Handedness Neurological deficits Neuropsychological deficits z
Prognosis of unilateral g
Duration V S motor paresis Type of B I C D A A H L C F
Age after F aphasia F M spatial neglect
No. /Sex PH FH Etiology onset D I upper lower severity A A A A H N A B L C (observation period)

1 56/M LR t t infarction 7 wks - t severe severe - - - t t t N V t t t t remained 9 months post onset


2 7l/M LR t t infarction 7 wks t t severe severe - - - t t t t t t t t remained 9 months post onset
3 75/F LR t t hemorrhage 7 wks t t severe severe - - - t t t t t t t t remained 6 months post onset
4 70/M LR t t hemorrhage 24 wks t t moderate moderate T 1/10 t - t t N E N E t t t t remained 6 months post onset
5 54/M LR t t infarction 12 wks t t severe severe T 2/10 t - t t NENE t t t t remained 6 months post onset
6 7 1/M LR t t infarction 16 wks t t severe severe T Oil0 t t t t N E N E t t t t remained 9 months post onset
7 49/M LR t t hemorrhage 7wks t t mild mild B 6/10 - - t t N E N E t t + t relrlained 6 months post onset
8 54/M LR t - hemorrhage 20wks t t mild mild W 3/10 t t t t N E N E t t t t remained 9 months post onset
9 57/F L - t hemorrhage 7 wks - - severe moderate B 2/10 t - t t N E N E t t t t disappeared within 3 months
10 7 1/M R - - infarction 12 wks t t severe severe T Oil0 t t t t NENE - t t t disappeared within 5 months
11 69/F R - infarction 2 wks t t severe severe T Oil0 t t t t N E N E - t t t disappeared within 2 months
12 59/F R - hemorrhage 4 wks t t severe severe C 6/10 t t t t N E N E - t t t disappeared within 3 months
13 44/M R - hemorrhage 3 wks t t severe severe B 1/10 t t t t NENE - t t t disappeared within 2 months
14 55/F R - hemorrhage 2 wks t t severe mild T 2/10 t t t t NENE - t t t disappeared within 2 months
15 55/F R - hemorrhage 8 wks t t severe severe B 2/10 t t t t N E N E - t t t disappeared within 6 months
16 65/M R - hemorrhage Zwks t - - - G - - t - N P t - t t t disappeared within 3 weeks
17 77/M R - hemorrhage 3wks t - - - G t t t - N P t - t t t disappeared within 4 weeks
18 62/F R - hemorrhage 1 wks t - - - G t t t - N P t - t t t disappeared within 3 weeks
19 48/M R - hemorrhage Zwks t - - - G t - t - N P t - t t t disappeared within 4 weeks
20 54/M R - hemorrhage 0.5 wks t - - - AG - - t - N P t - t t t disappeared within 2 weeks

PH: Past history of corrective training T: total aphasia BF A: bucco-facial apraxia


FH: Family history of the left-haders B: Broca's syndrome IM A ideomotor apraxia
W Wernicke's aphasia C A: constructional apraxia
V FD: visual, field defect C: conduction aphasia D A: dressing apraxia
S I: sensory impairment G: Gerstmann's syndrome A H: anosognosia for hemiparesis
AG: agraphia A V anosognosia for visual field defect
NE: not examined for aphasia H A: Hemiasomatognosia
NP: no paresis
NV: no visual field defect L B: line bisection
C L: cancellation task
F C: figure copying
Right unilateral spatial neglect

right homonymous hemianopsia, 15 cases of right right-handers, “mild” and “moderate” was null; and
hemiparesis involving the face, and 14 cases in which “severe”, 9, while among the cases of right-handers,
sensory impairment for the right side. “mild” was nil; “moderate”, 4; “severe”, 7. In can-
Neuropsychological findings included, in addition cellation task, among the cases of none-right-
to right unilateral spatial neglect, 12 cases of apha- handers, “mild” was 1; “moderate”, 2; and “severe”,
sia (four Broca, one Wernicke, one Conduction, and 6, while among the cases of right-handers, “mild”
four Total aphasias), four cases of Gerstmann’s syn- was 2; “moderate”, 4; “severe”, 5 . In figure copying,
drome, and one case of pure agraphia. There were among the cases of none-right-handers, “mild” was
also ten cases of ideomotor apraxia, 14 cases of null; “moderate”, 1; and “severe”, 8, while among
bucco-facial apraxia, 20 cases of constructional the cases of right-handers, “mild” was 1;“moderate”,
apraxia, and 15 cases with dficulty in dressing. The 4; “severe”, 6.
breakdown for anosognosia in the eight cases with- On picture problems, they made marked omis-
out aphasia was three cases of anosognosia for sions on the right side (Fig. la). The right-handers,
hemiplegia and eight cases of anosognosia for visual conversely, disregarded the right side of figures to a
field defect. There were nine cases in hemiasomatog- lesser extent, and errors on picture tests for the most
nosia for the right side of the body was observed part were limited to distortions of the right side
(Table 1). (Fig. lb). In 12 cases, symptoms of right unilateral
In studying the relationship between handedness spatial neglect disappeared within six months. In
and neurological symptoms, we discovered that 6 of another eight cases, however, symptoms persisted
the 11 cases in the right-handed group had severe for more than six months. In particular, the eight
aphasia, while four had Gerstmann’s syndrome and non-right-handed patients who had been trained in
one had pure agraphia. The six cases with severe the past to use the right hand suffered from right
aphasia were also marked by severe hemiplegia, uni- unilateral spatial neglect well past six months. The
lateral sensory dysfunction, hemianopsia, bucco- other one patient who had not received such cor-
facial apraxia, ideomotor apraxia, constructional rective training were free of symptoms within six
apraxia and difficulty with dressing. There was no months. By contrast, symptoms in the right-handed
clearly defined paralysis or sensory impairments in group disappeared within two weeks to six months
the four cases with Gerstmann’s syndrome and one after admission.
case with pure agraphia, but hemianopsia and Sex, age, etiology, handedness, focal lesion were
anosognosia for visual field defect were observed. compared between the group in which right unilat-
Three of these cases also showed signs of bucco- eral spatial neglect disappeared (disappeared group)
facial apraxia and two of these cases showed ideo-
motor apraxia. All five cases had some construc-
tional apraxia, but none of these patients had
difficulty with dressing.
Meanwhile, all nine cases in the non-right-handed
group had hemiplegia, with unilateral sensory dys-
function in eight cases and hemianopsia in seven
cases. Moderate-to-severe aphasia was present in !.lode1

six cases. No aphasia was observed in the other


three cases. Of the six cases of aphasia, four showed
bucco-facial apraxia, and two ideomotor apraxia.
The three cases with no aphasia were marked by
anosognosia for visual field defect and/or hemiple-
gia, but there was neither bucco-facial apraxia nor
ideomotor apraxia. In all nine cases, we found uni- Case 2 care 1 rare 7

lateral personal neglect, constructional apraxia and Fig. l a . Figure copying by non-right-handed group
difficulty with dressing.

Qualitative differences and prognosis in unilateral spatial neglect


We investigated qualitative differences and progno-
sis in unilateral spatial neglect between handedness,
sex, age and etiology.
In comparison to the right-handed group, the non-
right-handers exhibited a greater degree of unilateral case 10 Case 13 Cane 16

neglect. In line bisection, among the cases of none- Fig. Ib. Figure copying by right-handed group

421
Maeshima et al.

and the other group in which right unilateral spatial frontal lobe. Sites of cerebral hemorrhage in the other
neglect remained (remained group) (Chi-square). five cases were in the left putamen, capsula interna
While the number of non-right handers was signifi- or thalamus, and the area extending from the parietal
cantly larger in the disappeared group than the re- lobe (Fig. 2). In the right-handed group, there were
mained group, there were no significant relations two cases of infarction with lesions found in an ex-
between sex, age, and etiology. tensive area extending from the left frontal lobe
As to responsible lesion, the number of non-right through the temporal, parietal and occipital lobes.
handers was also significantly larger i i the remained Of another 9 cases of cerebral hemorrhage, 4 were
group than in the disappeared group, but there were found in a wide area centered on the left putamen,
no significant relation between responsible lesion. and the other 5 were of subcortical hemorrhage in
the left parietal and occipital lobes (Fig. 2). As to
Prognosis in the other neurological symptoms focal lesion, the number of non-right handers was
also significantly larger in the remained group than
Anosognosia faded within two weeks to three months in the disappeared group, but there were no signifi-
regardless of handedness or corrective training. All cant relations between handedness and focal lesion.
nine cases with right hemiasomatognosia belonged
to the non-right-handed group. Hemiasomatognosia
Discussion
of the right side eventually disappeared in one case
(Case 9) in which unilateral spatial neglect also irn- Nine of the 20 cases covered in our study were also
proved. All six total aphasics did not improve ap- not right-handed, indicating that handedness possi-
parently. Though their aphasic symptoms still re- bly affects changes in laterality pertaining to higher
mained, another six cases improved generally, cortical function. In Cases 1, 2 and 3 , extensive le-
regardless of handness or corrective training. sions in the left hemisphere had damaged portions
of the language centers; however, there was no apha-
Lesion site sia, ideomotor apraxia or bucco-facial apraxia. The
symptoms, including unilateral spatial neglect, per-
Fig. 2 shows a superposition of CT images at initial sonal neglect, and anosognosia, are those associated
examination. Extensive infarctions were seen in the with the non-dominant hemisphere. As a conse-
area of the brain supplied by the left middle cerebral quence, we were led to assume that functions nor-
artery (five cases) and the area supplied by the left mally belonging to the dominant hemisphere in these
posterior and left middle cerebral arteries (one case). three cases were operating from the right hemisphere
There were nine cases of hemorrhage in the left basal and that certain visuo-spatial cognitive functions
ganglia, and five of subcortical hemorrhage in the left which are normally assigned to the non-dominant
parietal and occipital lobes. The nine cases in the hemisphere were located in the left hemisphere.
non-right-handed group were broken down into four Cases 4-9 were marked by right unilateral spatial
of cerebral infarction, all in the left temporal, parietal neglect, hemiasomatognosia and aphasia. In five of
and occipital lobes or the area extending from the six cases there was also bucco-facial apraxia. The
speculation in these six cases is that, in addition to
language centers, the left hemisphere was also the

f
site of certain functions associated with the non-
dominant hemisphere which relate to visuo-spatial
perception, perception of physical space and control
of bucco-facial praxis. Ideomotor apraxia seen in
only two cases may indicate a strong probability that
dominant hemispheric functions related to engrams
C T lesion in non-right-handed group
of limb movements were relegated to the opposite
side (right side). Also, despite the fact that all nine
of these non-right-handed patients suffered from le-
sions in the left hemisphere, their mistakes in figure
copying tasks were notably closer to errors of uni-
lateral omission made by right-handed right-
hemispheric lesion cases rather than overall distor-
tions.
A review of prognosis in these cases of right uni-
CT lesion in right-handed group lateral spatial neglect shows that lesions were com-
Fig. 2. CT lesion in non-right-handed group (upper), in right- paratively limited in Case 9 where symptoms disap-
handed group (lower) peared after three months and recovery from aphasia

422
Right unilateral spatial neglect

progressed simultaneously. Based on these observa- other words, unilateral spatial neglect is a form of
tions, it does not seem unreasonable to assume that perceptual impairment that arises when elemental
the part of the brain responsible for spatial neglect perceptual disorders such as hemianopsia are com-
was not the site directly damaged by the lesion. pounded by slight deterioration in attention and
However, symptoms also disappeared within several arousal functions. The six right-handed patients with
months despite the presence of extensive lesions severe aphasia also suffered from complications of
throughout the right hemisphere. Perhaps this signi- hemianopsia, hemiplegia and sensory impairment.
fies that a lesser degree of strict lateralization of vi- These observations lead us to believe that right uni-
sual spatial functions makes it easier for the oppo- lateral spatial neglect might be caused by a similar
site hemisphere to take over tasks. In the eight cases mechanism. The difference is that symptoms persist
where symptoms persevered for a longer period we for several months longer when the area affected by
also saw greater lateralization into the left hemi- lesions is large and deterioration of arousal func-
sphere. This may be due from more direct damage tions and inattention are long. This also suggests the
occurring in the area of the cerebrum that controls possibility that the mechanism by which right uni-
these processes. Whatever the reason, it is of par- lateral spatial neglect appears in purely right-handed
ticular interest in terms of laterality of visuo-spatial cases is different from that for cases in which left-
perception to note that these were patients who had handedness is a factor. It has been reported that the
been constrained in childhood to use the right hand severe cases of unilateral spatial neglect are more
for activities such as holding chopsticks or writing. frequently seen in men than in women, and that
In three of nine cases there was no aphasia what- laterality for visuo-spatial perception is hgher (19).
ever, leading us to speculate that language function In this study, there were no noticeable differences
in these cases might reside in the right hemisphere. between sex, age, and etiology. The number of the
In summary, it seems probable that the lateraliza- cases in which right unilateral spatial neglect re-
tion of such higher cortical functions as visuo-spatial mained was much larger in men. This seemed to be
perception into the right and left hemispheres be- because the number of non-right handers was larger
come precisely reversed in non-right-handed patients in men among the subjects studied.
(mirror image), or that some areas overlap (anom- Lesion sites thought to be responsible in cases of
alous patterns). left unilateral spatial neglect include, in addition to
In the 11 cases where left-handedness was not a the right temporo-parieto-occipital junction (20, 21),
factor, we found a consistent pattern of hemispheric the right frontal lobe (22), the right occipital lobe
dominance: aphasia was present in 6 cases, Gerst- (23), the right thalamus (24, 25), and right basal gan-
mann’s syndrome in 4, and pure agraphia in 1. Ma- glia (26). Cases of right unilateral spatial neglect
nifestations of right unilateral spatial neglect, mostly have been reported as being caused by lesions in the
ideomotor apraxia and bucco-facial apraxia, disap- left temporo-parieto-occipital junction (1, 27), the
peared within six months after onset. In these cases, left occipital lobe (20), the left basal ganglia (20) and
it is thought that dominant-hemisphere functions the left frontal lobe (28), in addition to the frequently-
such as language and limb praxis are sited as usual mentioned left parietal and temporal regions. CT
in the left hemisphere, and the temporary appear- images of the 20 cases in this study showed that in
ance of right unilateral spatial neglect is due to some 6 there were infarctions in the area supplied by the
other factor. However, the fact that even such right- left middle cerebral artery, ranging extensively across
handed patients are prone to occasional unilateral the left temporal, parietal, occipital and frontal lobes.
omissions implies that the accepted qualitative dif- In another nine cases there was extensive hemor-
ferences between the hemispheres are insufficient to rhagmg in the area extending from the left basal
explain all phenomena. We must also consider the ganglia through the capsula interna and thalamus.
possibility that lateralization of visuo-spatial func- Also, in five cases there was subcortical hemorrhag-
tion is not absolute. Four of the patients with Ger- ing in the left parietal and occipital lobes. From these
stmann’s syndrome and one of the patients with observations, we confirmed that although lesions
pure agraphia also suffered from right unilateral spa- may be seen in either hemisphere, the sites respon-
tial neglect accompanied by hemianopsia and sible for appearance of unilateral spatial neglect all
anosognosia for visual field defect: however, symp- fall in the parietal and occipital lobes, which are
toms were temporary and disappeared in a few regions generally-cited in connection with such dis-
weeks. What is more, these symptoms were not ac- orders. When we compared right-handers with non-
companied by the classic signs of dysfunction in the right-handers, we found that lesions are more com-
non-dominant hemisphere such as personal neglect monly located in the parietal and temporal lobes.
and difficulty with dressing. The pattern we have just Only in a few cases were lesions found to be re-
described concurs well with the theory of sensory stricted to a small area in the parietal and occipital
impairments proposed by Batterby et al. (18). In lobes.

423
Maeshima et al.

Right unilateral spatial neglect due to left hemi- 12. MASUREMC. BENTONAL. Visuospatial performance in
sphere lesion was clinically investigated. The num- left-handed patients with unilateral brain lesions. Neuropsy-
chologia 1983: 21: 179-181.
ber of cases of right unilateral spatial neglect due to 13. KERTESZ A. SHEPPARD A. The epidemiology of aphasia and
left hemisphere lesion was unexpectedly small, but it cognitive impairment in stroke. Brain 1981: 104: 117-128.
was also a fact that severe cases existed in which 4. MORIE. Anosognosia and hemiasomatognosia in stroke pa-
such neglect was persistent. To determine such tient with right-hemisphere damage. Clin Neurol (Tokyo)
1982: 881-890.
cases, it is desirable to perform more in-depth in- 5. Nirayama Conference. Standard language test of aphasia.
vestigation after confirming handedness (including Manual of directions. Tokyo, Homeido 1975.
family members). 6. HASEGAWAT, KISHIH, SHICENO K e t al.Three-dimensional
structure in language test of aphasia. Folia phoniat 1985: 37:
246-258.
References 17. HASEGAWA T, ASAGAWA K, ABEK et al. Standardized test
of higher motor functions. Higher Brain Function Research
1. BRAINWR. Visual disorientation with Special reference to (Tokyo) 1985: 5: 865-886.
lesions ofthe right cerebral hemisphere. Brain 1941: 64: 244- 18. BATTERSBY WS, BENDERMB, POLLACKM, KAHNRL.
2722. Unilateral “spatial agnosia” (“inattention”) in patient with
2. PATERSON A, ZANGWILL OL. Disorders of visual percep- cerebral lesions. Brain 1956: 79: 68-93.
tion associated with lesions of the right cerebral hemisphere. 19. MCGLONEJ, KERTESZA. Sex differences in cerebral pro-
Brain 1944: 67: 331-358. cessing of visuospatial tasks. Cortex 1973: 9: 313-320.
3. MCFIE J, PIERCYMF, ZANGWILL OL. Visual-spatial ag- 20. HEILMAN KM, VALENSTEIN E, WATSONRT. Localization
nosia associated with lesions of the right cerebral hemisphere. of neglect. In: Localization in Neuropsychology, edited by
Brain 1950: 73: 167-190. KERTESZA, New York, Academic Press, 1983: 471-492.
4. ALEXANDER MP, FISCHETTE MR, FISCHER RS. Crossed 21. KUBOH. Unilateral spatial agnosia (neglect). Advances in
aphasias can be mirror image or anomalous. Brain 1989: 112: N e u r o l o g d Sciences (Tokyo) 1980: 24: 598-609.
953-973. 22. HEILMANN MH, VALENSTEIN E. Frontal lobe neglect in
5. DENNY-BROWN D, MEYERJS, HORENSTEIN S. The sig- man. Neurology 1972: 22: 660-664.
nificance of perceptual rivalry resulting from parietal lesion. 23. KOBAYASHI S, YAMAGUCHI S, FUJIWARA S, TAZAKIY.
Brain 1952: 75: 433-471. Hemispatial agnosia (neglect) in cerebral infarction in the
6 . ZARITSH, KAHNRL. Impairment and adaptation in chronic territory of posterior cerebral artery. Japanese Journal of
disabilities: spatial inattention. J Nerv Ment Dis 1974: 159: Neuropsychology (Tokyo) 1986: 2: 41-47.
63-72. 24. WATOSON RT, HEILMANKM. Thalamic neglect. Neurol-
7. ALBERTML. A simple test of visual neglect. Neurology 1973: ogy 1979: 29: 690-694.
23: 658-664. 25. VALLARG, PERANID, CAPPA SF etal. Recovery from
8. GAINOTTI G, TIACCIC. The relationships between disorders aphasia and neglect after subcortical stroke: neuropsycholog-
of visual perception and unilateral spatial neglect. Neuropsy- ical and cerebral perfusion study. J Neurol Neurosurg Psy-
chologia 1971: 9: 451-458. chiat 1988: 51: 1269-1276.
9. CALTAGIRONE C, MICELIG, GAINOTTI G. Distinctive fea- 26. FERROJM, KERTESZ A, BLACKSE. Subcortical neglect;
tures of unilateral spatial agnosia in right and left brain- quantitation, anatomy and recovery. Neurology 1987: 37:
damaged patients. Eur Neurol 1977: 16: 121-126. 1487-1492.
10. OGURAJ, YAMADORI A. On the hemispheric asymmetry of 27. DELISDC, KNIGHTRT, SIMPSON GV. Reversed hemi-
unilateral spatial neglect. Brain and Nerve (Tokyo) 1984: 36: spheric organization in a left-hander. Neuropsychologia 1983:
131-135. 21: 13-24.
11. KAWAHARA N, SATOK, SHIMADA T et al. The incidence 28. DRONKERS NF, KNIGHTRT. Right-sided neglect in a left-
and the recovery rate of unilateral spatial neglect in 100 cases hander: evidence for reversed hemispheric specialization of
with right or left putaminal hemorrhage: with special reference attention capacity. Neuropsychologia 1988: 27: 729-735.
to the cerebral lateralization. Higher Brain Function Research
(Tokyo) 1984: 4: 70-74.

424

You might also like