1 s2.0 S0028393223000kkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkk57X Main

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 22

Neuropsychologia 184 (2023) 108523

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Neuropsychologia
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neuropsychologia

Review article

Handedness in twins reared apart: A review of the literature and new data
Gareth Richards a, *, Nancy L. Segal b
a
School of Psychology, Faculty of Medical Sciences, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, Tyne and Wear, NE2 4DR, UK
b
Department of Psychology, California State University, Fullerton, CA, 92831, USA

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Reared-apart twin studies are a powerful means for identifying the relative contributions of heredity and envi­
Behavioural genetics ronment to variation in human physical and behavioural traits. One such characteristic is handedness, for which
Handedness it has long been noted that approximately 20% of twin pairs are comprised of one right-handed cotwin and one
Review
left-handed cotwin. Reared-together twin studies suggest a slightly greater concordance in monozygotic (MZT)
Twins
than dizygotic (DZT) twins, implying that genetics influences hand preference. We report here two studies of
Twins reared apart
handedness in reared-apart twins. Study 1 synthesizes the available data and estimates that at least N = 560
same-sex reared-apart twin pairs (for which zygosity is known with reasonable confidence) have been identified.
Of these, handedness data are available for both members of n = 415 pairs. We observed similar levels of
concordance/discordance for reared-apart monozygotic (MZA) and dizygotic (DZA) twins. However, although
direction of handedness (right or left) has frequently been examined, strength of handedness (strong or weak) has
not. Study 2 examined strength of hand preference and relative hand skill, as well as right- and left-hand speed,
information available for participants in the Minnesota Study of Twins Reared Apart (MISTRA). We provide
evidence of heritability for right-hand and left-hand speed. We also found hand preference strength was more
alike than chance in DZA, but not MZA, twins. Findings are discussed in relation to genetic and environmental
influences on human handedness.

1. Introduction included mention of handedness.


The equal environments assumption (EEA) is important to explain as
Monozygotic (MZ) twins share 100% of their genes identical by violation can affect the findings and interpretations of twin-based ana­
descent, having formed when a single fertilised egg, or zygote, divided lyses. The EEA specifies that environmental factors relevant to traits of
during the first two weeks post conception. In contrast, dizygotic (DZ) interest are “equal” for MZ and DZ twins. Some critics of twin research
twins are no more alike than other full siblings in sharing, on average, assert that since MZ twins generally share more activities (e.g., activities
50% of their genes identical by descent. The classic twin method com­ related to the trait of sociability) than do DZ twins, this renders twin
pares the similarity (or concordance) of MZ and DZ twins, with a greater study findings invalid. However, as pointed out by Segal (2012, p. 3),
resemblance in the former relative to the latter indicating that genetic “this difference is problematic only if (1) MZ twins are required to share
factors affect the phenotype under study. However, as twins are typi­ … activities to a greater degree than DZ twins, and (2) sharing activities
cally raised together, they may share very similar early environments; affects sociability in a meaningful way. If sharing activities increases
this assumption is explicated in the paragraph that follows. One of the sociability, then the environments of the two types of twins would not be
most powerful methods through which behaviour geneticists can equivalent with respect to this behavior. The equal environments
ascertain the relative contributions of heredity and environment is to assumption would have been compromised in this hypothetical
study twins who have been reared apart from infancy (Segal, 2012). In example.” In fact, such objections have been tested empirically and have
such cases, MZ and DZ twins still share 100% and 50% of their genes been dismissed in most cases.
identical by descent, respectively, but genetic effects are no longer Other concerns of twin research involve twins’ sharing of prenatal
confounded by those of the shared environment. Although not typically environments. It is not widely appreciated that the prenatal circum­
their primary focus, case reports of reared-apart twins have often stances and adversities of MZ twins (e.g., twin-to-twin transfusion

* Corresponding author. School of Psychology, Newcastle University, 4.32 Dame Margaret Barbour Building, Wallace Street, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK.
E-mail address: gareth.richards@ncl.ac.uk (G. Richards).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108523
Received 13 September 2022; Received in revised form 14 February 2023; Accepted 20 February 2023
Available online 12 April 2023
0028-3932/© 2023 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
G. Richards and N.L. Segal Neuropsychologia 184 (2023) 108523

syndrome [TTTS]; positioning in the womb) lead mostly to differences, from k = 44 studies reported that the concordance rate for direction of
not similarities, in newborns. In contrast, DZ twins do not experience hand preference was slightly, but statistically significantly, higher in MZ
TTTS, but are subject to the vagaries of differential positioning in the (80.49%) than DZ (79.27%) twin pairs. However, to effectively partition
womb and related effects. Again, a classic comparison of MZ and DZ variance between genetic factors and shared environmental factors,
twins would be a violation of the EEA only if the effects were relevant to studies of MZ (MZA) and DZ (DZA) reared-apart twin pairs are required.
the trait in question. Direction of handedness may be one such trait. That
is because only MZ twins result from zygotic division and could be 2. Study 1: review of handedness in reared-apart twins
affected by differences in cell distribution, events possibly linked to
handedness. However, whether timing of zygotic division is associated Handedness was reported for the very first pair of MZA twins (Bessie
with handedness in MZ twins has recently been questioned (see Segal and Jessie1) to appear in the academic literature (Muller, 1925; Pope­
and Montoya, 2018). noe, 1922; Saudek, 1933). Muller (1925, p. 438) commented that “Both
The majority of humans are classified as right-handed, while 10.6% twins, moreover, had early shown lefthandedness, though B had been
prefer using their left hand for most unimanual and bimanual tasks broken of it.” A number of such case reports have since been published,
(Papadatou-Pastou et al., 2020). In fact, a population-level minority of yet the only near-systematic compilation of their findings, that of Farber
left-handers has been observed consistently throughout history and (1981), predates much of the relevant literature. We, therefore, sum­
across cultures (Ocklenburg and Güntürkün, 2018). However, the marise the available data here.
complexity of handedness is often underappreciated, and various as­
pects of this trait can be misunderstood. Although hand preference is 2.1. Literature search strategy
usually considered in terms of direction (left or right), it can also be
measured with reference to degree or strength (McManus and Bryden, Handedness in reared apart twin pairs is represented by a chal­
1992). For example, a given ‘right-hander’ may have a very strong lenging literature for several reasons. First, although in many cases
preference for using their right hand for a particular task or, alterna­ handedness has been reported, in no instance has it been the primary
tively, this preference might be relatively weak. Additionally, rather focus of the study. This can make identifying such research difficult.
than simply assessing an individual’s hand preference, behavioural Second, as reared-apart twins embody such a rare and engaging phe­
measures can be employed to quantify the difference in skill between the nomenon, entire books are often written about individual cases. Some­
two hands (i.e., relative hand skill) (McManus and Bryden, 1992). As times these appear outside of the academic literature (e.g., Lindeman,
with hand preference, relative hand skill can be considered in terms of 1969); in some cases, they are authored by the twins (e.g., Bordier and
both direction (right > left or left > right) and strength (strong or weak Futerman, 2014; Schein and Bernstein, 2007), and books are sometimes
lateralisation). self-published (e.g., Glasby et al., 2013). Third, cases of reared-apart
It has been suggested that birth stress/trauma can alter handedness, twins have sometimes been identified from twin case series of genetic
although empirical findings have been equivocal (Bakan et al., 1973; and environmental studies of psychiatric conditions (Essen-Möller,
Beaton, 2003; Coren and Porac, 1980; McManus, 1981; Nicholls et al., 1941; Tienari, 1963a), e.g., schizophrenia (Gottesman and Shields,
2012; Ooki, 2006; Satz, 1972; Searleman et al., 1989; Williams et al., 1972; Kallmann, 1938; Kurihara, 1959; Mitsuda and Sakai, 1968), bi­
1992). If such effects do occur, they would mostly be in the direction of polar disorder (Stenstedt, 1952), psychosis (Kringlen, 1967a; Slater,
right to left, i.e., ‘pathological left-handedness’ (Satz, 1972), given the 1953), as well as alcoholism (Kaij, 1960), lateral asymmetries (Bou­
higher frequency of left-handedness among individuals with brain terwek, 1943), criminal tendencies (Hayashi, 1963; Kranz, 1936; Lange,
damage or marked cognitive impairment (Papadatou-Pastou and Tom­ 1931; Yoshimasu, 1941), and the effects of head injury (Dencker, 1958).
prou, 2015). The birth circumstances of reared-apart twins are often In some instances, these data are complicated further by multiple
unknown—however, while birth trauma may precipitate shifts in reporting (often including books and monographs) of the same twin
handedness from right to left, it is unlikely that MZ and DZ twins would series (e.g., the case series reported on by Tienari [1963a; 1963b, 1966;
be affected equally since MZ twins experience more hazardous prenatal 1968]). Additionally, there are many relevant articles, again including
environments. There is no extant theory as to how birth trauma in twins books and monographs, published in languages other than English, e.g.,
would affect the strength of handedness, but it seems likely that MZ and German (Bouterwek, 1936, 1943; Essen-Möller, 1941; Kranz, 1936;
DZ twins would be equally affected. Prokop and Druml, 1973; Wagenseil, 1931), French (Franceschetti,
Hand preference runs in families, with the children of two left- 1948; Franceschetti et al., 1948), Japanese (Hayashi, 1963; Kurihara,
handed parents exhibiting higher rates of left-handedness (~26%) 1959; Yoshimasu, 1941), Chinese (Gao et al., 2011).
than the children of one left-handed and one right-handed parent Due to the above complications, search strings such as “Reared apart
(~19%), and higher rates than the children of two right-handed parents twins” AND “Handedness” would likely be unreliable, and many rele­
(~9%) (McManus and Bryden, 1992). Adoption studies indicate a ge­ vant articles would be missed. We, therefore, began by including all
netic basis, with the handedness of adoptees more closely resembling cases from the three ‘classic’ studies of reared apart twins, that is, those
that of their biological parents than adoptive parents; however, analyses of Newman et al. (1937), Shields (1962), and Juel-Nielsen (1965, 1980),
have often been underpowered, and findings have been somewhat and added to these data from the Minnesota Study of Twins Reared
mixed (Carter-Saltzman, 1980; Saudino and McManus, 1998). Simple Apart (MISTRA) (Segal, 2012), Swedish Adoption Twin Study of Aging
Mendelian inheritance (Jordan, 1914) clearly does not conform to (SATSA) (Pedersen, 2015), Fullerton Study of Chinese Twins Adopted
empirical observation (McManus and Bryden, 1992), a fact exemplified Apart (Segal et al., 2011), the Finnish Twin Cohort Study (Kaprio et al.,
by the approximately 20% of MZ twin pairs that are discordant for di­ 2019), and the Louise Wise Services/Child Development Center
rection of hand preference (Pfeifer et al., 2022). Single gene models (LWS/CDC) Twin Study (see Segal, 2021a). To identify other relevant
(Annett, 2002; McManus, 2002) positing two alleles at a single data, we examined all cases mentioned within reviews of reared apart
locus—one coding for right-handedness, the other for chance—have also twin studies/cases, e.g., Farber (1981), Segal (2011a, 2012, 2021a),
failed to account for this trait (McManus, 2022), especially as large-scale Price (1968), Gottesman and Shields (1972), Tucker (1997), Slater
molecular genetic studies have established that human handedness (1968), and Shields and Slater (1967). We returned to the original
shows polygenic effects (Cuellar-Partida et al., 2020). literature for each case identified and translated those reports which
More than a century ago, Danforth (1919, p. 400) commented that “a
surprising number of twin pairs seem to be composed of one right and
one left handed individual”. A recent meta-analysis (Pfeifer et al., 2022) 1
Please note that this pair is not the same as the Bessie and Jessie reported on
with N = 36,217 twin pairs (n = 15,506 MZ pairs; n = 20,711 DZ pairs) by Burlingham (1952).

2
G. Richards and N.L. Segal Neuropsychologia 184 (2023) 108523

were not published in English. We also checked reference lists for rele­ in the index case) – the number of DZA twin pairs included in the sample
vant articles, and contacted authors of largescale studies for which we is unclear – it is noted in the text that this “group of separated one-egg
knew reared-apart twin cases had been identified: that is, the SATSA pairs includes twins who developed schizophrenia at almost the same
(Pedersen, 2015), Finnish Twin Cohort Study (Kaprio et al., 2019), time, although their separation took place soon after birth and led to
Osaka University Aged Twin Registry (Hayakawa et al., 2002)/Japanese apparently very different life conditions.” (p. 316). Kallmann and Roth
Study of Adult Twins Reared Apart and Growing Old Separately (Hay­ (1956) also included data relating to four DZA twin pairs. Additionally,
akawa et al., 2006), and the Chinese National Twin Registry (CNTR) based on a 1957 personal communication with Niels Juel-Nielsen,
(Gao et al., 2011; Zhou et al., 2015). Therefore, although the current Shields (1958, p. 116) stated that “a small series is also under investi­
paper departs from the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Re­ gation by Professor Husén’s department in Stockholm.”; likewise, Con­
views and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) (Moher et al., 2009) criteria, to the way (1958, p. 184) mentions, “In Stockholm Dr. Husen is carrying out an
best of our knowledge we have retrieved all available studies/cases. analogous investigation.” However, further details of these twins appear
to be unavailable.
3. Study 1 results In addition to the n = 6 MZAm, n = 1 MZAf, and n = 2 DZAm “Twin
pairs who have been brought up in different homes” reported on by
3.1. Number of reared apart twin pairs identified Fukuoka (1937, p. 35), which are included in Table 1 of the current
paper, there is mention of n = 11 MZAm, n = 15 MZAf, n = 1 DZAm, and
We summarise case reports of same-sex reared-apart twins in n = 1 DZAf “Twin pairs who were brought up separately for some time,
Table 1. It is not possible to determine with certainty exactly how many then brought together and who have since been living in the same
reared-apart MZ male (MZAm), MZ female (MZAf), DZ male (DZAm), home”. We do not include these latter twin pairs in our study because
and DZ female (DZAf) pairs have been identified in the scientific liter­ degree of separation is unclear and appears likely to have been minimal.
ature and/or mainstream media. However, we were able to identify n = We also note that Komai (1937, p. 4) mentioned, “HAGI’S material is the
415 cases (n = 79 MZAm; n = 127 MZAf; n = 78 DZAm; n = 131 DZAf) same as FUKUOKA’S presented in this publication.” However, a Japanese
for which handedness data are available for both members of each twin speaking colleague’s (Dr E. Self) reading of the paper by Hagi (1931)
pair. This rises to n = 472 (n = 94 MZAm; n = 157 MZAf; n = 80 DZAm; revealed no mention of the study by Fukuoka (1937) or of reared-apart
n = 141 DZAf) when also considering those cases of Shields (1962) for twins. Additionally, Shields and Slater (1967, p. 581) noted that “In
which handedness data were missing or incomplete, and the n = 2 cases Japan, Mitsuda (personal communication) has observed a series of MZ
of Tienari (1963a) and n = 1 case of Slater (1953) for which (left-) twin pairs, in which one member had a schizophrenic or schizo-affective
handedness was specified for only one twin from each pair. This in­ illness; schizophrenia occurred in the co-twins in eight out of the 11
creases further, to N = 560 (n = 141 MZAm; n = 184 MZAf; n = 89 pairs brought up together, and in five out of the eight pairs brought up
DZAm; n = 146 DZAf), when also including other cases for which apart.” It is unclear if these might be the same cases as reported on by
handedness data are unavailable (see Table 1). Other possible cases of Inouye (1971, 1972), and so they are not considered separately within
same-sex twins reared apart were also identified from the literature but our study. Likewise, Fisher (1958, p. 596) presented data, provided to
not included in the current analysis; details of these and reasons for their him by Eliot Slater, for n = 27 MZ twin pairs “separated at or shortly
exclusion are provided below. after birth”, though additional details of this sample are unclear. Given
Sir Cyril Burt reported on a large sample of reared-apart twin pairs; the chronicity, location, size of the sample, and the established collab­
however, these twins are not considered here because the authenticity of oration between Eliot Slater and James Shields (Shields and Slater,
the data has been seriously questioned (for a discussion, see e.g., Tucker, 1967; Slater, 1953), it might be assumed that there is at least some
1997). The sample size increased across consecutive publications and overlap between this sample and that of Shields (1962). Although un­
was reported on as follows: Burt, 1943, p. 91) (N = 15 pairs); Burt (1955, certain from the information available from these sources, given the
p. 167–169) (N = 21 pairs); Burt (1958, p. 7) (“over 30 such cases”); apparent likelihood of this being the case, we do not consider these data
Conway (1958, p. 186) (N = 42); Burt (1966, p. 141) (N = 53 pairs). separately here.
Regardless, in the only mention of handedness, Burt (1966, p. 142)
stated that “Among our own cases we noted that mirror reversals 3.2. Hand preference of reared apart twins
(opposite handedness, reversed patterns in finger-prints and
palm-prints, contrary directions in the crown whorl of the hair, etc.) Concordance/discordance rates for direction of hand preference in
were nearly four times as frequent with monozygotic pairs as with MZA and DZA twin pairs identified from large studies, case series, and
dizygotic”. From the available data it is therefore impossible to ascertain case reports are presented in Table 2 and are summarized in Table 3. We
concordance/discordance figures for this sample. observed a slightly lower concordance rate in MZA (n = 206 pairs;
Although we do include in our review those cases in which zygosity 70.9% concordant, 29.1% discordant) than DZA (n = 209; 78.0%
can be assumed with a reasonable degree of confidence but for which it concordant, 22.0% discordant) pairs, although this effect was not sta­
is less than certain, we did not consider the following cases further, as tistically significant χ2 (1) = 2.763, p = .096, φ = 0.082. However, the
zygosity information was missing completely. More specifically, there direction of effect observed might relate to discordant handedness being
are n = 3 pairs from Qingdao reported on by Gao et al. (2011) as being more striking in MZA than DZA pairs, an observation which could lead
present within the Chinese National Twin Registry, n = 2 male to a greater likelihood of handedness being reported for the former than
twin-pairs from a triplet set in which one member was raised separately the latter. Concordance was also slightly lower for both MZA and DZA
to the other two (see Bordier and Futerman, 2014, p. 203), and n = 1 twins compared with the figures reported from the meta-analysis of
female twin-pair (Kathy and Betsy) for which no information on zygosity reared-together twins by Pfeifer et al. (2022) (MZT concordance =
was available (see Segal, 2021a, Chapter 5). Additionally, we are aware 80.49%; DZT concordance = 79.27%).
of n = 6 cases of twins reared apart (separated at < 5 years of age) in the It is worth noting that handedness was not reported systematically
Japanese Study of Adult Twins Reared Apart and Growing Old Sepa­ for the large cohort studied by Shields (1962), and it appears likely that
rately (Hayakawa et al., 2006), although we were unable to obtain this was only noted when left-handedness was present in at least one
further details. twin. Likewise, (left-) handedness for one twin, but no data for the other,
We do not consider the following cases further because details is reported for two MZAm pairs from Tienari (1963a) and one DZAm
regarding sex (and handedness) were not specified. Kallmann (1946) pair from Slater (1953). We, therefore, imputed these missing values as
reported on n = 59 MZA (Fig. 12., p. 317 in that paper) twin pairs who indicating right-handedness and present the aggregated data in Table 4.
were separated for more than five years (prior to onset of schizophrenia As we had no relevant information from which to go by, we did not

3
G. Richards and N.L. Segal Neuropsychologia 184 (2023) 108523

Table 1
Handedness data for MZA and DZA twin pairs from case reports.
Case Case no. in Original reference(s) Sex Zygosity Handedness Evidence Coding in
Farber (1981) reported in Table 2
primary source (current
study)

A and B N/A Townsend (circa 1874–1875, as cited M MZ Unknown


in Segal, 2012, p. 3–4)
a
Paul C. and Paul O. 118 Newman (1929a); Newman et al. M MZ RH/NRH “They are both right-handed” (Newman RH/NRH
(aka C and O) (1937, Case III) et al., 1937, p. 179); “Rl” (ibid, Table
43).
Ferdinand 418 Lange (1931, Concordant MZ pairs M MZ Unknown
Schweizer and Case 6)
Luitpold
Schweizer
a
Raymond and 144 Newman (1933); Newman et al. M MZ RH/RH “Both are right-handed” (Newman RH/RH
Richard (1937, Case VII) et al., 1937, p. 213); “RR” (ibid, Table
43).
a
Harold and Holden 320 Newman (1934a); Newman et al. M MZ LH/RH “Harold is left-handed; Holden, right- NRH/RH
(1937, Case IX) handed.” (Newman et al., 1937, p.
230); “LR” (ibid, Table 43).
g
Ronald and Dennis 232 Saudek (1934) M MZ RH/RH “Dextrality … Right-handed” [Ronald]; RH/RH
“Same” [Dennis] (Saudek, 1934, p. 18).
John and William 180 Rosanoff et al. (1935, Case 1); Farber, M MZ Unknown
Shields, and Craig (1979, as cited in
Farber, 1981)
A and B 412 Bouterwek (1936, Case E81); M MZ NRH/NRH “Sie geben an, die Linke nicht gerade zu NRH/NRH
g
Bouterwek (1943, Case E81) bevorzugen. Doch teilen beide die
Spielkarten mit der Linken aus; A
springt Stabhochsprung bei gegenüber
der normalen verkehrter Armstellung
besser und B spannt beim
Bogenschieẞen mit der Linken.” [They
state that they do not exactly favour the
left. But both deal the playing cards
with their left hands; A pole vaults
better than the normal inverted arm
position and B braces with his left hand
in archery.] (Bouterwek, 1936, p. 266);
“beide linkshändig” [both left handed]
(Bouterwek, 1943, p. 146); same
quotation as noted above from
Bouterwek (1936, p. 266) is reproduced
in Bouterwek (1943, p. 147).
A and B N/A Fukuoka (1937, MZm1) M MZ Unknown
A and B N/A Fukuoka (1937, MZm2) M MZ Unknown
A and B N/A Fukuoka (1937, MZm3) M MZ Unknown
A and B N/A Fukuoka (1937, MZm4) M MZ Unknown
A and B N/A Fukuoka (1937, MZm5) M MZ Unknown
A and B N/A Fukuoka (1937, MZm6) M MZ Unknown
a
Kenneth and Jerry 142 Newman et al. (1937, Case XIII) M MZ RH/RH “Both are right-handed in everything.” RH/RH
(Newman et al., 1937, p. 267); “RR”
(ibid, Table 43).
a
Edwin and Fred 182 Newman et al. (1937, Case XV) M MZ RH/RH “Both are fully right-handed.” ( RH/RH
Newman et al., 1937, p. 283); “RR”
(ibid, Table 43).
a
Gene and James 162 Newman et al. (1937, Case XVII) M MZ RH/LH “Gene is right-handed, while James is RH/NRH
partially left-handed.” (Newman et al.,
1937, p. 301); “Rl” (ibid, Table 43).
a
James and Reece 154 Newman et al. (1937, Case XVIII) M MZ RH/LH “Reece’s left arm is 2 inches shorter RH/NRH
than his right and is slightly deformed,
yet he is fully left-handed; while James
is definitely right-handed.” (Newman
et al., 1937, p. 309); “RL” (ibid, Table
43).
Alfred and August 414 Essen-Möller (1941, Case 3); Kaij M MZ Unknown
(1960, Case 19); Essen-Möller (1970,
Case N. 3)
g
J.J. and J.B. 106 Yates and Brash (1941) M MZ RH/RH “Reversed asymmetry or mirror- RH/RH
imagery of various features is
confirmatory but not necessary
evidence of monozygocity. J.J. and J.B.
did not appear to show this in any way;
certainly they did not in handedness
(both were right-handed)” (Yates and
Brash, 1941, p. 90).
Kazuo and Takau N/A Yoshimasu (1941, Case XVII) M MZ (?) Unknown
(continued on next page)

4
G. Richards and N.L. Segal Neuropsychologia 184 (2023) 108523

Table 1 (continued )
Case Case no. in Original reference(s) Sex Zygosity Handedness Evidence Coding in
Farber (1981) reported in Table 2
primary source (current
study)

Millan and George 102 Stephens and Thompson (1943); M MZ RH/LH “George is left handed, while Millan is NRH/NRH
g
Stephens and Nunemaker (1950) right handed. Millan stated, however,
that he was originally left handed, but
when a youngster learned to use his
right hand because of an injury to his
left.” (Stephens and Thompson, 1943,
p. 111).
g
Philippe Joye and N/A Franceschetti (1948); Fraceschetti M MZ RH/RH “When going to sleep, both Ernstli and RH/RH
Charles Joye (né et al. (1948); McIndoe and Philippe suck their right thumbs” Joye
Ernstli Vatter) Franceschetti (1949); Joye (1954); (1954, p. 102); N. L. Segal (personal
(aka Victor J. and Segal (2009); Segal (2011a, p. communication with family members).
Eric V.) 40–53); Segal (2018, Case 1)
g
Earl and Frank 204 Burks and Roe (1949, Chapter 3) M MZ RH/RH “Both were righthanded.” (Burks and RH/RH
Roe, 1949, p. 17); Earl: “Right”; Frank:
“Right” (ibid, Table 5, p. 18).
James and Keith Group VI. Burks and Roe (1949, Chapter 5) M MZ (?) Not reported
(Doubtful
Zygosity)
c
Palle and Peter 108 Juel-Nielsen and Mogensen (1957); M MZ RH/RH “Both were right-handed.” ( RH/RH
Juel-Nielsen and Harvald (1958, Case Juel-Nielsen, 1980, Part 2, p. 25).
I); Mogensen and Juel-Nielsen
(1961a); Mogensen and Juel-Nielsen
(1961b); Mogensen and Juel-Nielsen
(1963); Juel-Nielsen (1965, Case I);
Juel-Nielsen (1980, Case I)
c
Kaj and Robert 206 Juel-Nielsen and Mogensen (1957); M MZ RH/RH “Both were right-handed.” ( RH/RH
Juel-Nielsen and Harvald (1958, Case Juel-Nielsen, 1980, Part 2, p. 139).
IV); Mogensen and Juel-Nielsen
(1961a); Mogensen and Juel-Nielsen
(1961b); Mogensen and Juel-Nielsen
(1963); Juel-Nielsen (1965, Case V);
Juel-Nielsen (1980, Case V)
g
A and B Group V Dencker (1958, Mz18) M MZ RH/RH “Left-handed”, “.” (Dencker, 1958, RH/RH
(Insufficient Table 35); note that although this only
Information) indicates the twins were alike for
handedness, it can be inferred from
elsewhere that they were both
right-handed as “The monozygotic
series contained 9 members who were
left-handed, and all of them had a
right-handed co-twin” (ibid, p. 100).
Peter and Bert 348 Kaij (1960, Case 17) M MZ Unknown
Philip and William Group V Kaij (1960, Case 18) M MZ Unknown
(Insufficient
Information)
Rudolph and Group VI Kaij (1960, Case 31) M MZ (?) Unknown
Edmund (Doubtful
Zygosity)
c
Viggo and Oluf 406 Mogensen and Juel-Nielsen (1961a); M MZ RH/RH “Both were right-handed.” ( RH/RH
Mogensen and Juel-Nielsen (1961b); Juel-Nielsen, 1980, Part 2, p. 285).
Mogensen and Juel-Nielsen (1963);
Juel-Nielsen (1965, Case XII);
Juel-Nielsen (1980, Case XII)
b
Alfred and Harry 174 Slater (1961, Case MZ 8); Shields M MZ RH/RH “Both right-handed.” (Slater, 1961, p. RH/RH
(1962, Case S m P 9) 375).
b
Herbert and 176 Slater (1961, Case MZ 11); Shields M MZ RH/RH “Both right-handed.” (Slater, 1961, p. RH/RH
Nicholas (1962, Case S m P 4); Gottesman and 376).
Shields (1972, Case MZ 17)
b
Richard and 236 Shields (1962, Case S m 1) M MZ Not reported RH/RH
Kenneth i
b
Bertram and 302 Shields (1962, Case S m 2) M MZ Not reported RH/RH
Christopher i
Russell and Tristram 126 Shields (1962, Case S m 3) M MZ LH (?)/LH “Both twins are left-handed.” (Shields, NRH/NRH
b
1962, p. 167). However, a hospital
report (from age 8) quoted (ibid, p. 166)
states Russell had “weakness of grip
and flexion of elbow on right side”; it is
stated elsewhere that an admission note
(age 14) mentioned he “had little use of
his right hand” (ibid, p. 166). He is also
noted to have had hemiplegia (this is
mentioned both in relation to the
admission note from age 14 [ibid, p.
(continued on next page)

5
G. Richards and N.L. Segal Neuropsychologia 184 (2023) 108523

Table 1 (continued )
Case Case no. in Original reference(s) Sex Zygosity Handedness Evidence Coding in
Farber (1981) reported in Table 2
primary source (current
study)

166], as well as in a general comment


by the author [ibid, p. 167]).
b
Frederick and Peter 322 Shields (1962, Case S m 5) M MZ Not reported RH/RH
i

Foster and Francis i 324 Shields (1962, Case S m 6) M MZ Not reported RH/RH b

Rodney and Barry i 220 Shields (1962, Case S m 7) M MZ Not reported RH/RH b

Edward and Keith i 190 Shields (1962, Case S m 8) M MZ Not reported RH/RH b
b
William and Stanley 246 Shields (1962, Case S m 10) M MZ Not reported RH/RH
i

Timothy and Kevin i 308 Shields (1962, Case S m 11) M MZ Not reported RH/RH b

James and Robert i 228 Shields (1962, Case S m 12) M MZ Not reported RH/RH b

Patrick and Victor i 334 Shields (1962, Case S m 13) M MZ Not reported RH/RH b

Hubert and Brian i 336 Shields (1962, Case S m 14) M MZ Not reported RH/RH b
b
Benjamin and 328 Shields (1962, Case S m 15) M MZ Not reported RH/RH
Ronald i
A and B i N/A Shields (1962, Suppl. Case 2) M MZ Not reported RH/RH b
g
A and B Group V Hayashi (1963, Case X); Hayashi M MZ LH/RH “Bは右利きであるが,Aは左利き。” [“B NRH/RH
(Insufficient (1967, Case 10) is right-handed, while A is left-
Information) handed.”] (Hayashi, 1963, p. 163).
A and B Group V Hayashi (1963, Case XIII); Hayashi M MZ Unknown
(Insufficient (1967, Case 13)
Information)
A and B Group V Hayashi (1963, Case XIV); Hayashi M MZ Unknown
(Insufficient (1967, Case 14)
Information)
A and B Group V Hayashi (1963, Case XV); Hayashi M MZ Unknown
(Insufficient (1967, Case 15)
Information)
A and B 222 Tienari (1963a, Case 173); M MZ Not reported
Tienari (1963b); Tienari (1966, Case
173)
A and B h 354 Tienari (1963a, Case 466); M MZ RH (?)/LH “HISTORY OF B. Left-handed.” ( RH/NRH g

Tienari (1963b); Tienari (1966, Case Tienari, 1963a, p. 133).


466)
A and B N/A Tienari (1963a, Case 703); M MZ Not reported
Tienari (1963b); Tienari (1966, Case
703)
A and B h N/A Tienari (1963a, Case 989); M MZ RH (?)/LH “HISTORY OF B. Left-handed.” ( RH/NRH g

Tienari (1963b); Tienari (1966, Case Tienari, 1963a, p. 86).


989)
A and B 166 Tienari (1963a, Case 1080); M MZ Not reported
Tienari (1963b); Tienari (1966, Case
1080)
A and B 240 Kringlen (1964, Case V); Kringlen M MZ Unknown
(1967a, Case A); Kringlen (1967b,
Case 22)
A and B Group V Kringlen (1967a, Case C); Kringlen M MZ Unknown
(Insufficient (1967b, Case 66)
Information)
A and B N/A Kringlen (1967b, Case 50) M MZ Unknown
A and B m N/A Mitsuda and Sakai (1968, Case 16) M MZ Unknown
e
Tony Milsai and 202 Lindeman (1969) (also mentioned M MZ RH/RH ““Are you right-handed all the way? RH/RH
Roger Brooks k studies of these twins by Shields, and asked Tony.
by Machover [ibid, p. 201–202]) “I bat and throw righty,” Roger
answered, “but I kick lefty.”
“I do everything righty,” Tony said” (
Lindeman, 1969, p. 144).
A and B N/A Inouye (1971); Inouye (1972, Case M MZ Unknown
490)
A and B N/A Inouye (1971); Inouye (1972, Case M MZ Unknown
4143)
A and B N/A Inouye (1971); Inouye (1972, Case M MZ Unknown
4145)
A and B N/A Inouye (1971); Inouye (1972, Case M MZ Unknown
4155)
A and B N/A Inouye (1971); Inouye (1972, Case M MZ Unknown
4218)
A and B N/A Inouye (1971); Inouye (1972, no case M MZ Unknown
number, see p. 184–185)
Alois H. and Oskar 332 Prokop and Druml (1973) M MZ Unknown
H.
Twin 1 and Twin 2 N/A Sudarsky et al. (1983) M MZ Unknown
A and B N/A Gao et al. (2011, MZm1) M MZ Unknown
(continued on next page)

6
G. Richards and N.L. Segal Neuropsychologia 184 (2023) 108523

Table 1 (continued )
Case Case no. in Original reference(s) Sex Zygosity Handedness Evidence Coding in
Farber (1981) reported in Table 2
primary source (current
study)

A and B N/A Gao et al. (2011, MZm2) M MZ Unknown


A and B N/A Gao et al. (2011, MZm3) M MZ Unknown
A and B N/A Gao et al. (2011, MZm4) M MZ Unknown
A and B N/A Gao et al. (2011, MZm5) M MZ Unknown
A and B N/A Gao et al. (2011, MZm6) M MZ Unknown
A and B N/A Gao et al. (2011, MZm7) M MZ Unknown
A and B N/A Gao et al. (2011, MZm8) M MZ Unknown
A and B N/A Gao et al. (2011, MZm9) M MZ Unknown
A and B N/A Gao et al. (2011, MZm10) M MZ Unknown
A and B N/A Gao et al. (2011, MZm11) M MZ Unknown
Matías and Marcos N/A Segal (2011a, p. 79–80); Segal (2018, M MZ Unknown
Case 6)
Dan Matthews and N/A Bordier and Futerman (2014, p. M MZ Unknown
Twin 194–196)
AT and LT j N/A Segal and Diamond (2014) M MZ RH/A N. L. Segal (personal communication). NRH/NRH
g
g
William and Jorge N/A Segal et al. (2017); Segal (2018, Case M MZ RH/RH “All four twins are right-handed, but RH/RH
7a); Segal and Montoya (2018); Segal not to the same degree. William is the
et al. (2018); Segal et al. (2019) strongest right-hander, followed by
Wilber, Jorge, and Carlos in that order,
with Carlos on the brink of
ambidexterity (Segal and Montoya,
2018, p. 187–188).
g
Wilber and Carlos N/A Segal et al. (2017); Segal (2018, Case M MZ RH/RH See quotation in cell above. RH/NRH
7b); Segal and Montoya (2018); Segal
et al. (2018); Segal et al. (2019)
d
Howard and Doug N/A Segal (2021a) M MZ RH/LH “The twins appear to be opposite- RH/NRH
handed.” (Segal, 2021a, p. 430); “The
twins in the one male set also appear to
be opposite-handed.” (ibid, p. 208
[Footnote 38 from p. 167]).
e
Robert Shafran and N/A Segal (2021a) M MZ LH/RH “The triplets’ hand preference scores NRH/RH
Edward Gelland varied somewhat–Bob is left-handed,
(triplets) k and Dave and Eddy are both right-
handed” (Segal, 2021a, p. 184);
“Unseen films of Bob as a youngster,
made by researchers during home visits
and retrieved from the Yale archive,
show him drawing designs, sorting
pictures, and building a block tower,
mostly using his left hand.” (ibid, p.
184); “Bob’s score indicated
left-handedness, whereas Eddy’s and
David’s scores indicated
right-handedness. Attempts were made
to shift Bob’s handedness from left to
right when he learned to write in
school, but they were unsuccessful
(ibid, p. 435 [Footnote 60 from p. 184]);
“All three identical females sets and the
male triplets in the twin study had a
left-handed member.” (ibid, p. 208).
e
Robert Shafran and N/A Segal (2021a) M MZ LH/RH See quotations in cell above. NRH/RH
David Kellman
(triplets) k
e
Edward Gelland and N/A Segal (2021a) M MZ RH/RH See quotations in cell two above. RH/RH
David Kellman
(triplets) k
Sir Mohamed Farah N/A Burley (2022) M MZ RH/? Handedness for writing determined
(né Hussein from pictures of Sir Mohamed Farah
Kahin) and signing autographs.
Hassan Kahin
g
A and B N/A Fernandes et al. (in preparation) M MZ RH/RH RH/RH
Christian Immerlich N/A Kranz (1936) M DZ Unknown
and Florian
Immerlich
Ottokar Immel and N/A Kranz (1936) M DZ Unknown
Roland Immel
A and B N/A Fukuoka (1937, DZm1) M DZ Unknown
A and B N/A Fukuoka (1937, DZm2) M DZ Unknown
John and Terry h N/A Slater (1953, Case 236) M DZ RH (?)/LH “Terry is left-handed.” (Slater, 1953, p. RH/NRH g

46).
(continued on next page)

7
G. Richards and N.L. Segal Neuropsychologia 184 (2023) 108523

Table 1 (continued )
Case Case no. in Original reference(s) Sex Zygosity Handedness Evidence Coding in
Farber (1981) reported in Table 2
primary source (current
study)

A and B h N/A Shields (1962, Case Ds 1) M DZ LH/RH (?) “A, asthenic build, left-handed” ( NRH/RH b

Shields, 1962, p. 247).


A and B N/A Tienari (1966, Case 1011 [p. 22]) M DZ RH/? “In 1975, he replied that was right-
handed as a child and now writes with
his right-hand.” (J. Kaprio, personal
communication).
A and B N/A Gao et al. (2011, DZm1) M DZ Unknown
A and B N/A Gao et al. (2011, DZm2) M DZ Unknown
A and B N/A Gao et al. (2011, DZm3) M DZ Unknown
George Skrzynecky N/A Segal (2015) M DZ (?) Unknown
and Lucian
Poznanski
g
Greek twins N/A N. L. Segal (personal communication, M DZ (?) RH/RH (?) N. L. Segal (personal communication RH/RH
2022) with one twin).
Bessie and Jessie 116 Popenoe (1922); Muller (1925); F MZ RH/LH “Both twins, moreover, had early NRH/NRH
g
Saudek (1933) shown lefthandedness, though B had
been broken of it” (Muller, 1925, p.
438).
a
Alice and Olive (aka 210 Newman (1929b); Newman et al. F MZ LH/RH “Olive is right-handed and Alice is left- NRH/RH
A and O) (1937, Case I) handed.” (Newman et al., 1937, p.
165); “LR” (ibid, Table 43).
a
Eleanore and 208 Newman (1929c); Newman et al. F MZ RH/RH “Both are fully right-handed.” Newman RH/RH
Georgiana (aka E (1937, Case II) et al. (1937, p. 169); “RR” (ibid, Table
and G) 43).
Miriam and Mary Group V Wagenseil (1931) F MZ Unknown
(Insufficient
Information)
a
Mabel and Mary 318 Newman (1932a); Newman et al. F MZ RH/RH “Both are right-handed” (Newman RH/RH
(1937, Case IV) et al., 1937, p. 189); “RR” (ibid, Table
43).
a
Edith and Fay (aka B 158 Newman (1932b); Newman et al. F MZ RH/RH “both are fully right-handed” (Newman RH/RH
and D) (1937, Case V) et al., 1937, p. 198); “RR” (ibid, Table
43).
a
Ada and Ida 244 Newman (1932c); Newman et al. F MZ RH/RH “Both are right-handed” (Newman RH/RH
(1937, Case VI) et al., 1937, p. 206); “RR” (ibid, Table
43).
g
Anna and Corinne N/A Seeman and Saudek (1933, Case 1) F MZ RH/RH “Dextrality … Right-handed” [Anna]; RH/RH
“Same” [Corinne] (Seeman and Saudek,
1933, p. 271).
g
Leta and Lota N/A Seeman and Saudek (1933, Case 2) F MZ RH/RH “Dextrality … Right” [Leta]; “Same” RH/RH
[Lota] (Seeman and Saudek, 1933, p.
283).
a
Mildred and Ruth 150 Newman (1934b); Newman et al. F MZ LH/RH “Mildred is left-handed; Ruth, right- NRH/RH
(aka M and R) (1937, Case VIII) handed.” (Newman et al., 1937, p.
222); “LR” (ibid, Table 43).
A and B 234 Bouterwek (1936, Case E115); F MZ Not reported
Bouterwek (1943, Case E115)
A and B N/A Fukuoka (1937, MZf1) F MZ Unknown
a
Betty and Ruth 152 Newman et al. (1937, Case X) F MZ RH/A “Betty is fully right-handed, Ruth RH/NRH
distinctly ambidextrous.” (Newman
et al., 1937, p. 238); “AR” (ibid, Table
43).
a
Gladys and Helen 124 Newman et al. (1937, Case XI) F MZ RH/RH “Both are strongly right-handed.” ( RH/RH
Newman et al., 1937, p. 249); “RR”
(ibid, Table 43).
a
Thelma and Zelma 160 Newman et al. (1937, Case XII) F MZ A (LH?)/RH “Zelma is fully right-handed, and NRH/RH
Thelma partly left-handed or
ambidextrous.” (Newman et al., 1937,
p. 258); “AR” (ibid, Table 43).
a
Esther and Ethel 120 Newman et al. (1937, Case XIV) F MZ LH/RH “Ethel is fully right-handed, while NRH/RH
Esther is reported to be naturally left-
handed though trained to use the right
hand. Ethel has a short defective little
finger on her right hand, and Esther the
same deformity on her left hand.” (
Newman et al., 1937, p. 276); “Ethel is
much stronger in hand grip, registering
70 pounds with right hand and 68
pounds with left hand, while Esther
registered 62 pounds with right and 40
pounds with left hand.” (ibid, p. 275);
“It will be remembered that Esther is
reported to be naturally left-handed but
(continued on next page)

8
G. Richards and N.L. Segal Neuropsychologia 184 (2023) 108523

Table 1 (continued )
Case Case no. in Original reference(s) Sex Zygosity Handedness Evidence Coding in
Farber (1981) reported in Table 2
primary source (current
study)

trained to use her right hand.” (ibid, p.


279); “LR” (ibid, Table 43).
Maxine and Virginia 164 Newman et al. (1937, Case XVI) F MZ RH/RH “RR” (Newman et al., 1937, Table 43). RH/RH a
a
Augusta and Helen 408 Newman et al. (1937, Case XIX) F MZ LH/RH “Helen is fully right-handed; Augusta NRH/RH
was originally left-handed but has
learned to use her right hand for most
monomanual operations.” (Newman
et al., 1937, p. 318); “RL” (ibid, Table
43).
Mary and Gertrude 122 Rosanoff et al. (1937, Case 91) F MZ Unknown
Agnes Emmaline N/A Rosanoff et al. (1937, Case 119) F MZ Unknown
and Madeline
Jennie
Kaete St. and Lisa St. 226 Kallmann (1938, p. 207–211) F MZ Unknown
g
Lois and Louise 138 Gardner and Newman (1940) F MZ RH/RH “Both are strongly right-handed” ( RH/RH
Gardner & Newman, 1940, p. 122).
Adelaide and 242 Burks (1942) F MZ A/A Twin A: “Writes from copy … Right … NRH/NRH
g
Beatrice Tosses ball … RRR”; Twin B: “Writes
from copy … Right … Tosses ball …
LLL” (Burks, 1942, Table I, p. 38); “It is
interesting to note that both twins
showed early left-handed tendencies
but that A was changed to the right
hand for all unimanual activities,
whereas B’s parents, on physician’s
advice, permitted her to use the left
hand, the change to the right hand for
writing being made voluntarily.” (ibid,
p. 40–41); Twin A: “Handedness … Left
tendency as baby. Mother tied left
hand; did not punish. A uses right
hand”; Twin B: “Handedness … Left
tendency as baby. Mother tied left hand
but stopped on doctor’s order. Later B
learned to write with right hand of own
accord; uses left otherwise” (ibid,
Table II p. 47).
Florence and Edith 134 Craike and Slater (1945) F MZ Unknown
g
Clara and Doris 212 Burks and Roe (1949, Chapter 2) F MZ RH/A Clara: “Right”; Doris: “Right and left; RH/NRH
can use left to sew” (Burks and Roe,
1949, Table 1, p. 5).
g
Gertrude and Helen 186 Burks and Roe (1949, Chapter 4) F MZ RH/RH Gertrude: “Right”; Helen: “Right” ( RH/RH
Burks and Roe, 1949, Table 8, p. 31).
Esther and Elvira 330 Schwesinger (1952) F MZ Unknown
A and B 156 Stenstedt (1952, Case 106 [p. 74–75]) F MZ Unknown
A and B l 404 Kallmann and Roth (1956) F MZ Unknown
c
Olga and Ingrid 104 Juel-Nielsen and Mogensen (1957); F MZ RH/RH “Both were right-handed.” ( RH/RH
Juel-Nielsen and Harvald (1958, Case Juel-Nielsen, 1980, Part 2, p. 57).
II); Mogensen and Juel-Nielsen
(1961a) (?); Mogensen and
Juel-Nielsen (1961b); Mogensen and
Juel-Nielsen (1963); Juel-Nielsen
(1965, Case II); Juel-Nielsen (1980,
Case II)
c
Maren and Jensine 146 Juel-Nielsen and Mogensen (1957); F MZ RH/RH Jensine: “When she commenced school, RH/NRH
Mogensen and Juel-Nielsen (1961a) she had a pronounced tendency to use
(?); Mogensen and Juel-Nielsen her left hand for writing and drawing
(1961b); Mogensen and Juel-Nielsen but this was quickly corrected by her
(1963); Juel-Nielsen (1965, Case III); teacher, and since then she had felt
Juel-Nielsen (1980, Case III) naturally right-handed.” (Juel-Nielsen,
1980, Part 2, p. 74); “Both were
right-handed.” (ibid, Part 2, p. 77).
c
Ingegerd and 188 Juel-Nielsen and Mogensen (1957); F MZ RH/RH “Both were right-handed.” ( RH/RH
Monika Juel-Nielsen and Harvald (1958, Case Juel-Nielsen, 1980, Part 2, p. 106).
III); Mogensen and Juel-Nielsen
(1961a) (?); Mogensen and
Juel-Nielsen (1961b); Mogensen and
Juel-Nielsen (1963); Juel-Nielsen
(1965, Case IV); Juel-Nielsen (1980,
Case IV)
c
Martha and Marie 168 Juel-Nielsen and Mogensen (1957); F MZ RH/RH “Both were right-handed.” ( RH/RH
Mogensen and Juel-Nielsen (1961a) Juel-Nielsen, 1980, Part 2, p. 158).
(?); Mogensen and Juel-Nielsen
(continued on next page)

9
G. Richards and N.L. Segal Neuropsychologia 184 (2023) 108523

Table 1 (continued )
Case Case no. in Original reference(s) Sex Zygosity Handedness Evidence Coding in
Farber (1981) reported in Table 2
primary source (current
study)

(1961b); Mogensen and Juel-Nielsen


(1963); Juel-Nielsen (1965, Case VI);
Juel-Nielsen (1980, Case VI)
Kamma and Ella 114 Juel-Nielsen and Mogensen (1957); F MZ A/A “V female 49 … 2) hemicrania left- NRH/NRH
c
Juel-Nielsen and Harvald (1958, Case handed” (Juel-Nielsen and Harvald,
V); Mogensen and Juel-Nielsen 1958, Table 1, p. 58); “In two of the
(1961a) (?); Mogensen and pairs of twins, one of the twins (V2 and
Juel-Nielsen (1961b); Mogensen and VII1) was ambidextrous and probably
Juel-Nielsen (1963); Juel-Nielsen originally left-handed.” (ibid, p. 63);
(1965, Case VII); Juel-Nielsen (1980, Kamma: “She stressed that she was
Case VII) trained not to prefer writing with her
left hand and considered that she
originally had been left-handed. Since
leaving school she had used her right
hand in a natural manner.” (
Juel-Nielsen, 1980, Part 2, p. 169); Ella:
“She was left-handed, was taught to
write with her right hand but continued
to prefer her left when for example she
used an ax or saw, was rather clumsy in
some types of work, and was quite
certain that she was left-handed.” (ibid,
Part 2, p. 173); “Both were
ambidextrous, Ella probably
predominantly lefthanded.” (ibid, Part
2, p. 177).
c
Signe and Hanne 130 Juel-Nielsen and Mogensen (1957); F MZ RH/RH “Both were right-handed.” ( RH/RH
Juel-Nielsen and Harvald (1958, Case Juel-Nielsen, 1980, Part 2, p. 207).
VI); Mogensen and Juel-Nielsen
(1961a) (?); Mogensen and
Juel-Nielsen (1961b); Mogensen and
Juel-Nielsen (1963); Juel-Nielsen
(1965, Case VIII); Juel-Nielsen (1980,
Case VIII)
c
Karin and Kristine 178 Juel-Nielsen and Mogensen (1957); F MZ RH/RH “Both twins were right-handed.” ( RH/RH
Mogensen and Juel-Nielsen (1961a) Juel-Nielsen, 1980, Part 2, p. 224).
(?); Mogensen and Juel-Nielsen
(1961b); Mogensen and Juel-Nielsen
(1963); Juel-Nielsen (1965, Case IX);
Juel-Nielsen (1980, Case IX)
Petrine and Dorthe 184 Juel-Nielsen and Harvald (1958, Case F MZ RH/A “VII female 71 1) left-handed” ( RH/NRH c
VII); Mogensen and Juel-Nielsen Juel-Nielsen and Harvald, 1958,
(1961a) (?); Mogensen and Table 1, p. 58); “In two of the pairs of
Juel-Nielsen (1961b); Mogensen and twins, one of the twins (V2 and VII1)
Juel-Nielsen (1963); Juel-Nielsen was ambidextrous and probably
(1965, Case X); Juel-Nielsen (1980, originally left-handed.” (ibid, p. 63);
Case X) Dorthe: “She believed that she had been
left-handed as a child and preferred
using her left hand, but at school she
was forced to use her right hand. She
still wrote with her right hand but used
her left hand when working with a
hammer or throwing, and she felt that if
she lost her right hand, she “would
quickly be able to use the left”.” (
Juel-Nielsen, 1980, Part 2, p. 244);
“Petrine was right-handed, Dorthe
ambidextrous, probably originally
left-handed.” (ibid, Part 2, p. 247).
c
Astrid and Edith 170 Juel-Nielsen and Harvald (1958, Case F MZ RH/RH “Both were right-handed.” ( RH/RH
VIII); Mogensen and Juel-Nielsen Juel-Nielsen, 1980, Part 2, p. 268).
(1961a) (?); Mogensen and
Juel-Nielsen (1961b); Mogensen and
Juel-Nielsen (1963); Juel-Nielsen
(1965, Case XI); Juel-Nielsen (1980,
Case XI)
A and B 340 Kurihara (1959, 17-A and 17-B [also F MZ Unknown
referred to as MZ-4192]);
Inouye (1971); Inouye (1972, Case
4192)
b
Jessie and Winifred 238 Shields (1962, Case S f 1) F MZ Not reported RH/RH
i

(continued on next page)

10
G. Richards and N.L. Segal Neuropsychologia 184 (2023) 108523

Table 1 (continued )
Case Case no. in Original reference(s) Sex Zygosity Handedness Evidence Coding in
Farber (1981) reported in Table 2
primary source (current
study)

A and B h 326 Shields (1962, Case S f 2) F MZ RH (?)/LH “B is taller but lighter in weight and has RH/NRH b

had more illness; she is left-handed.” (


Shields, 1962, p. 192).
Valerie and Joyce h 338 Shields (1962, Case S f 3) F MZ LH/RH (?) “Valerie’s periods started a month or NRH/RH b

two before Joyce’s and she has usually


been bigger; she is left-handed.” (
Shields, 1962, p. 192).
A and B i 402 Shields (1962, Case S f 4) F MZ Not reported RH/RH b

Megan and Polly i 172 Shields (1962, Case S f 5) F MZ Not reported RH/RH b

A and B i 140 Shields (1962, Case S f 6) F MZ Not reported RH/RH b


b
Jenny and Kathleen 304 Shields (1962, Case S f 7) F MZ Not reported RH/RH
i

Olive and Madge i 112 Shields (1962, Case S f 8) F MZ Not reported RH/RH b
b
Madeline and Lilian 110 Shields (1962, Case S f 9) F MZ RH (?)/LH “Lilian is taller and heavier and is left- RH/NRH
h
handed.” (Shields, 1962, p. 201).
b
Marjorie and Norah 128 Shields (1962, Case S f 10) F MZ LH/RH “Marjorie went to a convent school NRH/RH
h
(family not R.C.) where she was a
boarder from 10 to 17½. Though
naturally left-handed she was taught to
write with her right hand.” (Shields,
1962, p. 203). However, in the
following section (without prior
mention of Norah’s handedness) it is
stated “As already mentioned Norah is
left-handed, except for writing.” (ibid,
p. 203). This is interpreted as being a
typo, and so this pair is coded as LH/RH
rather than RH/LH.
Molly and Dorothy i 416 Shields (1962, Case S f 11) F MZ Not reported RH/RH b
Pauline and Sally i 410 Shields (1962, Case S f 12) F MZ Not reported RH/RH b
Viola and Olga i 344 Shields (1962, Case S f 13) F MZ Not reported RH/RH b
b
Millicent and Edith 148 Shields (1962, Case S f 14) F MZ RH (?)/LH “Edith is left-handed.” (Shields, 1962, RH/NRH
h
p. 213).
Joan and Dinah i 352 Shields (1962, Case S f 15) F MZ Not reported RH/RH b

June and Clara i 224 Shields (1962, Case S f 16) F MZ Not reported RH/RH b
b
Jacqueline and 230 Shields (1962, Case S f 17) F MZ Not reported RH/RH
Beryl i
Christine and Nina i 312 Shields (1962, Case S f 18) F MZ Not reported RH/RH b

Herta and Berta i 136 Shields (1962, Case S f 19) F MZ Not reported RH/RH b
b
Charlotte and Laura 306 Shields (1962, Case S f 20) F MZ Not reported RH/RH
i
h b
Mary and Nancy 218 Shields (1962, Case S f 21) F MZ LH/RH (?) “Mary is left-handed.” (Shields, 1962, NRH/RH
p. 227).
b
Olwen and Gwladys 214 Shields (1962, Case S f 22) F MZ Not reported RH/RH
i

Annie and Trixie i 314 Shields (1962, Case S f 23) F MZ Not reported RH/RH b

Joanna and Isobel i 216 Shields (1962, Case S f 24) F MZ Not reported RH/RH b

Odette and Fanny i 342 Shields (1962, Case S f 25) F MZ Not reported RH/RH b

Amy and Teresa i 132 Shields (1962, Case S f 26) F MZ Not reported RH/RH b

Dora and Brenda i 350 Shields (1962, Case S f 27) F MZ Not reported RH/RH b

Maisie and Vera i 346 Shields (1962, Case S f 28) F MZ Not reported RH/RH b
b
Adeline and 422 Shields (1962, Case S f 29) F MZ Not reported RH/RH
Gwendolen i
A and B i N/A Shields (1962, Suppl. Case 1) F MZ Not reported RH/RH b

A and B 316 Kringlen (1967a, Case B); Kringlen F MZ (?) Unknown


(1967b, Case 48)
A and B 420 Kringlen (1967a, Case D); Kringlen F MZ Unknown
(1967b, Case 55)
A and B 310 Inouye (1971); Inouye (1972, Case F MZ Unknown
4138)
A and B N/A Inouye (1971); Inouye (1972, Case F MZ Unknown
4223)
A and B N/A Inouye (1971); Inouye (1972, Case F MZ Unknown
4317)
A and B N/A Chess, Ladimer, and Thompson F MZ Unknown
(unpublished, as cited in Thomas and
Chess, 1977)
d
Paula and Elyse N/A Schein and Bernstein (2007); Segal F MZ RH/RH “Elyse and I are both right-handed” ( RH/RH
(2021a) Schein and Bernstein, 2007, p. 63);
“They are both right-handed.” (Segal,
2021a, p. 271).
g
US and SK N/A Segal and Hur (2008) F MZ RH/RH Crovitz-Zener Self-Report Handedness RH/RH
Inventory: “Both twins were right-
(continued on next page)

11
G. Richards and N.L. Segal Neuropsychologia 184 (2023) 108523

Table 1 (continued )
Case Case no. in Original reference(s) Sex Zygosity Handedness Evidence Coding in
Farber (1981) reported in Table 2
primary source (current
study)

handed, although SK scored slightly


higher (27) than her twin sister (23),
indicating that she was less strongly
right-handed. Their handedness scores
place them both within the range
including 98% of right-handers” (Segal
and Hur, 2008, p. 544).
g
Begoña and Delia N/A Segal and Blandón-Gitlin (2010); F MZ RH/RH N. L. Segal (personal communication). RH/RH
Segal (2011a); Segal (2018, Case 5)
A and B N/A Gao et al. (2011, MZf1) F MZ Unknown
A and B N/A Gao et al. (2011, MZf2) F MZ Unknown
A and B N/A Gao et al. (2011, MZf3) F MZ Unknown
A and B N/A Gao et al. (2011, MZf4) F MZ Unknown
A and B N/A Gao et al. (2011, MZf5) F MZ Unknown
A and B N/A Gao et al. (2011, MZf6) F MZ Unknown
A and B N/A Gao et al. (2011, MZf7) F MZ Unknown
g
Nora Laster and N/A Segal (2011a, p. 26–27) F MZ RH/RH N. L. Segal (personal communication). RH/RH
Chun Hei
g
Kasia and Edyta N/A Segal (2011a, p. 59–66); Segal (2018, F MZ RH/LH “RL—MZAf (Poland, left twin was RH/NRH
Case 3) originally left, then some switch by
teachers” (N. L. Segal, personal
communication with parent).
Mari Tairí and Tairí N/A Segal (2011a, p. 67–78); Segal (2018, F MZ Unknown
Mari Case 4a)
g
Dorothy Loader and N/A Segal (2011b); Glasby et al. (2013) F MZ LH/LH “Dorothy is left-handed and I am too. I RH/NRH
Rosabelle (Belle) do recall however my foster Mum
Glasby telling me that when I was young that
my foster Dad held down my right hand
purposely so that I learned to write with
my left hand. I have no idea why he did
this and I didn’t think to ask. Out of
interest, I use scissors in my right hand
but I think that Dorothy still uses her
left hand for that purpose too.” (N. L.
Segal, personal communication with
Rosabelle Glasby).
AH and BH N/A Segal (2012, p. 266–267) F MZ Unknown
g
Samantha Futerman N/A Bordier and Futerman (2014); Segal F MZ RH/RH Anaïs: “Although I was ambidextrous, I RH/NRH
and Anaïs Bordier and Cortez (2014); Segal (2016) tended to use my left hand, but the
(aka US and FR) French educators were trained to
encourage us to be right-handed.” (
Bordier and Futerman, 2014, p. 61);
Samantha: “We were both
right-handed, although Anaïs could use
both hands.” (ibid, p. 282); Samantha:
“I am right-handed, and Anaïs is
ambidextrous. She only uses her right
hand about a quarter of the time. I don’t
know if Dr. Segal’s testing considered
that Anaïs had been encouraged to use
her right hand in school, part of French
educational aversion to
left-handedness. When she broke her
right arm riding her horse at age fifteen,
she had no choice but to write with her
left hand. Anyway, she is considered
ambidextrous, and I am right-handed.”
(ibid, p. 285); “Both twins were
right-handed, although FR uses both
hands. She had broken her right arm at
age fifteen, and used her left during
recovery” (Segal and Cortez, 2014, p.
102).
Siam and Fabienne N/A Segal (2014, Case 2) F MZ Unknown
g
US and SK N/A Segal et al. (2015a) F MZ RH/RH “Both SK and US use their right hand RH/RH
exclusively for unimanual tasks such as
writing and cutting” (Segal et al.,
2015a, p. 298).
f
Audrey and Gracie N/A Segal (2017) F MZ LH/RH “Audrey is left-handed and Gracie is NRH/RH
right-handed.” (Segal, 2017, p. 181).
d
Anne and Susan N/A Segal (2021a) F MZ LH/RH “Susan is right-handed and Anne is left- NRH/RH
handed.” (Segal, 2021a, p. 127).
(continued on next page)

12
G. Richards and N.L. Segal Neuropsychologia 184 (2023) 108523

Table 1 (continued )
Case Case no. in Original reference(s) Sex Zygosity Handedness Evidence Coding in
Farber (1981) reported in Table 2
primary source (current
study)
d
Melanie and Ellen N/A Segal (2021a) F MZ LH/RH “Melanie is left-handed and Ellen is NRH/RH
right-handed.” (Segal, 2021a, p. 139);
“they knew that they were
opposite-handed. Melanie, the
left-handed twin, had a longer left arm
and Ellen, the right-handed twin, had a
longer right arm.” (ibid, p. 147); “All
three identical females sets and the
male triplets in the twin study had a
left-handed member.” (ibid, p. 208).
d
Sharon and Lisa N/A Segal (2021a) F MZ LH/RH “Sharon is left-handed and Lisa is right- NRH/RH
handed.” (Segal, 2021a, p. 208); “All
three identical females sets and the
male triplets in the twin study had a
left-handed member.” (ibid, p. 208).
g
Dolores and Mirta N/A Segal (2021b, Case 1) F MZ RH/RH “Both twins are right-handed.” (Segal, RH/RH
2021b, p. 245).
g
Linh (Nugyen Thi N/A Segal (2021b, Case 2) F MZ RH/RH “Both twins are right-handed.” (Segal, RH/RH
Hoai Linh) and 2021b, p. 246).
Alice Cecilia Linh
Svensson
g
Elpis and Maria N/A Segal (2022) F MZ RH/RH “physical similarities, including right- RH/RH
hand preference” (Segal, 2022, p. 167)
g
SK and US N/A Segal and Hur (2022) F MZ RH/RH N. L. Segal (personal communication). RH/RH
g
A and B (born in N/A Segal (unpublished Case 1) F MZ RH/RH “RH/RH.” (N. L. Segal, personal RH/RH
Malaysia, raised communication).
in Singapore)
A and B N/A Segal (unpublished Case 2) F MZ LH/LH “some intentional switching by others NRH/NRH
g
later in life” (N. L. Segal, personal
communication).
g
A and B (born in N/A N. L. Segal (personal communication) F MZ RH/RH N. L. Segal (personal communication). RH/RH
South Korea,
raised in the US)
g
A and B N/A N. L. Segal (personal communication) F MZ RH/LH RH/NRH
Martha and Lydia N/A Slater (1953, Case 52) F DZ Unknown
Esther and Rachel N/A Slater (1953, Case 225) F DZ (?) LH/LH “This is the only pair where it is NRH/NRH
g
reported that both twins are left-
handed.” (Slater, 1953, p. 46); “Both
are left-handed.” (ibid, p. 334).
A and B N/A Juel-Nielsen and Harvald (1958, p. F DZ Unknown
63)
A and B h N/A Shields (1962, Case Ds 2) F DZ LH/RH (?) “A (like mother and aunt) has had NRH/RH b

migrainous headaches from age of 3,


liable to occur when excited; also a dry
skin conditions (familial), left-handed.”
(Shields, 1962, p. 247).
A and B i N/A Shields (1962, Case Ds 3) F DZ Not reported RH/RH b

A and B i N/A Shields (1962, Case Ds 4) F DZ Not reported RH/RH b

A and B i N/A Shields (1962, Case Ds 5) F DZ Not reported RH/RH b

A and B i N/A Shields (1962, Case Ds 6) F DZ Not reported RH/RH b

A and B i N/A Shields (1962, Case Ds 7) F DZ Not reported RH/RH b

A and B i N/A Shields (1962, Case Ds 8) F DZ Not reported RH/RH b

A and B i N/A Shields (1962, Case Ds 9) F DZ Not reported RH/RH b

A and B i N/A Shields (1962, Case Ds 10) F DZ Not reported RH/RH b

A and B i N/A Shields (1962, Case Ds 11) F DZ Not reported RH/RH b

A and B N/A Gao et al. (2011, DZf1) F DZ Unknown


A and B N/A Gao et al. (2011, DZf2) F DZ Unknown
g
Jennifer and N/A Segal (2011a, p. 67–78); Segal (2018, F DZ (?) Not reported N. L. Segal (personal communication). RH/RH
Samantha Case 4b)
g
US and UK N/A Segal et al. (2015b) F DZ RH/RH Crovitz and Zener Group Test for RH/RH
Assessing Hand and Eye Dominance:
“US was right-handed (score = 15),
while UK showed mixed handedness
(score = 34). UK, originally right-
handed, had broken her right wrist in
2010 and used her left hand during
recovery. However, she used her right
hand to place pegs in the dominant
hand condition.” (Segal et al., 2015b, p.
113).
d
Michele and Allison N/A Segal (2021a) F DZ RH/RH N. L. Segal (personal communication). RH/RH
Paula and Marjorie N/A Segal (2021a) F DZ Unknown
BL and Twin N/A N. L. Segal (personal communication) F DZ (?) Unknown “RH-/RH; BUT BOTH PARTLY AMBI” NRH/NRH
g
(N. L. Segal, personal communication).

13
G. Richards and N.L. Segal Neuropsychologia 184 (2023) 108523

Note. Other than the MZAm triplets, Robert Shafran, David Kellman, and Edward Gelland (who were also included in the Louise Wise Services [LWS]/Child
Development Center [CDC] Twin Study) and Tony and Roger (who are described in the book by Lindeman [1969]), case reports relating to twins in the MISTRA are not
included here (see Segal [2012] e.g., for Jim Lewis and Jim Springer, Oskar Stöhr and Jack Yufe, Dorothy and Bridget, Barbara and Judy, Mark Newman and Jerry
Levey, Jennifer Mitchell and Margaret Williams; Segal [2011a, p. 53–59, 2018, Case 2] for George and Brent; Bordier and Futerman [2014, p. 132–133] for DZAf pair
Kerrie and Amy).
a
Newman et al. (1937).
b
Shields (1962).
c
Juel-Nielsen (1965, 1980).
d
LWS/CDC Twin Study.
e
Minnesota Study of Twins Reared Apart (MISTRA).
f
Fullerton Study of Chinese Twins Adopted Apart.
g
Case reports.
h
Left-handedness was noted for one twin, but data were not reported for the other twin (assumed right-handed). Note that a preliminary report from Tienari’s
(1963b, p. 395) study stated, “In cases where one of the twins was left-handed and the other right-handed, dexterity tests were used as a check on this circumstance.”
i
Handedness not reported for this case, but left-handedness was noted in other cases in the same series (both twins assumed right-handed).
j
This pair is included in Table 2 as an MZAm pair; although assigned male at birth, both twins later assumed female gender identities.
k
Twin pairs included in Table 2 as aggregated data for the MISTRA rather than as case reports.
l
The sex of this pair is reported as female by Farber (1981, Table 3.1, p. 58–59).
m
It is unclear whether this pair is also included as one of the cases reported on by Inouye (1972), as it is stated in that publication that “Some pairs were also included
in a study by Mitsuda and Sakai (1968), who kindly permitted us to look through the case records. The present author also provided Dr. Mitsuda case records to avail in
his study.” (Inouye, 1972, p. 183).

impute values for the two cases (one MZAm [Burley, 2022]; one DZAm triplet sets: two sets consist of three MZA males (i.e., three MZA twin
[Tienari, 1966, Case 1011]) in which right-handedness was established pairs each), whereas the other set consists of a pair of MZA female twins
for one twin, but no details were available for the other. Concordance and their DZA male co-triplet (organised into one MZA female pair, and
rates were fairly similar to our original estimates: MZA (n = 251 pairs; two DZA male-female pairs). The age range of the N = 136 twin pairs
72.9% concordant, 27.1% discordant), DZA (n = 221; 77.8% concor­ included in the current study was 12–77 years (M = 41.87, SD = 13.93).
dant, 22.2% discordant), χ2 (1) = 1.526, p = .217, φ = 0.057.
5.2. Zygosity diagnosis
4. Study 2: Strength of hand preference and relative hand skill
in the Minnesota Study of Twins Reared Apart (MISTRA) When the MISTRA began in 1979, twins provided blood samples that
were analyzed across multiple serological systems by the Minneapolis
Strength of handedness, while an important manifestation of hand War Memorial Blood Bank. These data in combination with dermato­
preference, has received relatively little attention in the scientific liter­ glyphic measures, ponderal index and cephalic index yielded a proba­
ature, especially in studies of twins reared apart. Study 2, therefore, bility of less than .001 (Lykken, 1978). As genetic technology advanced
examined this variable as part of the relevant data gathered in the in later years, zygosity diagnosis was accomplished by DNA testing.
Minnesota Study of Twins Reared Apart (MISTRA), conducted by Bou­ Additional details about this process are reported by Segal (2012).
chard et al. (1990). The rearing environments of the MISTRA twins are
described briefly below, with more detailed description of participants,
zygosity diagnosis, tests, inventories, findings, and conclusions provided 5.3. Measures
by Segal (2012).
5.3.1. Strength of hand preference
5. Materials and methods The Crovitz-Zener Handedness Inventory (CZHI; Crovitz and Zener,
1962) is a self-report measure of hand preference. The current study
5.1. Participants employed a modified version of the original measure for which scores
were reported from 1 (left) to 9 (right), with those further from 0 indi­
Reared-apart twins in the MISTRA had to have been separated by cating stronger hand preference. Analyses presented here are based on
four years of age and to have spent their formative childhood years 14 items: preferred hand used for writing, holding a nail while
apart. The mean age at separation was 218.21 days (SD = 343.59) and hammering, throwing a ball, holding a bottle when removing the top,
ranged between 0 and 1644 days. None were raised in seriously drawing, peeling a potato, cutting with scissors, using a knife, holding a
impoverished homes, coming mostly from lower-middle class to middle needle when threading, holding a glass when drinking, holding a
class families. The majority of twins (70.2%) were reared by unrelated toothbrush, holding a dish when wiping, and using a tennis racquet.
individuals, whereas the remainder were variously reared by biological Note that five items (nail, bottle, peeling potato, needle, dish) are
relatives or multiple biological relatives, with some raised in “complex” reverse scored, as it is the non-dominant hand that is specified in their
situations. More mothers and fathers of MZA than DZA twins pursued response. Items from the CZHI are known to correlate strongly with
professional than non-professional occupations. Most family environ­ similar items from the more widely used Edinburgh Handedness In­
mental variables showed little correlation with twins’ measures of in­ ventory (Oldfield, 1971), r = .80–.90 (Bryden, 1977), and internal
telligence and other behaviours. consistency in the current study was high, Cronbach’s α = 0.942.
As part of the standard test battery, twins completed the Crovitz- To facilitate calculation of a laterality index (LI), scores for each of
Zener Handedness Inventory (CZHI) (Crovitz and Zener, 1962), a the 14 CZHI items were recoded as − 4 (left) to +4 (right), with 0 indi­
self-report assessment of hand preference, and the Purdue Pegboard cating no preference. LI was then calculated as: (R - L)/(R + L) × 100. In
(PPB) task (Tiffin, 1968), a behavioural measure of hand skill. Hand­ this formula, R represents the sum of positive values, and L represents
edness data (scores from at least one of the following: CZHI, PPB the sum of (absolute) negative values. Scores therefore range from
right-hand speed, or PPB left-hand speed) were available for the mem­ − 100, which indicates left preference (including items with no prefer­
bers of 81 MZA twin pairs (35 male pairs [43.2%]; 46 female pairs ence) for all items, to +100, which indicates right preference (including
[56.8%]) and 55 DZA twin pairs (12 male pairs [21.8%]; 26 female pairs items with no preference) for all items. To examine strength of hand
[47.3%]; 17 [30.9%] male-female pairs). Within these data are three preference (strong or weak), we removed the sign for the LI so that all
values were positive. Scores closer to 100 therefore indicate stronger

14
G. Richards and N.L. Segal Neuropsychologia 184 (2023) 108523

Table 2
Concordance/discordance for direction of hand preference in monozygotic (MZA) and dizygotic (DZA) twins reared apart.
Male Female

Study/cohort Measure(s) MZA DZA MZA DZA

RH/ NRH/ RH/ RH/ NRH/ RH/ RH/ NRH/ RH/ RH/ NRH/ RH/
RH NRH NRH RH NRH NRH RH NRH NRH RH NRH NRH

Newman et al. (1937) a Unspecified 3 0 4 0 0 0 6 0 6 0 0 0


Shields (1962) b Unspecified 15 1 0 0 0 1 24 0 6 9 0 1
Juel-Nielsen (1965, 1980) Unspecified 3 0 0 0 0 0 6 1 2 0 0 0
Louise Wise Services/Child Unspecified 0 0 1 0 0 0 1 0 3 2 0 0
Development Center (LWS/
CDC) Twin Study (Segal,
2021a) c
Minnesota Study of Twins Writing hand preference 20 2 8 7 0 4 28 2 9 18 0 4
Reared Apart (MISTRA) (from Crovitz-Zener
(current study) d Handedness Inventory)
Fullerton Study of Chinese Unspecified 0 0 1 1 0 0 7 2 5 0 1 3
Twins Adopted Apart (Segal
et al., 2011)
Finnish Twin Cohort Study ( Child handedness (right, 7 0 2 32 1 4 7 0 5 31 0 10
Kaprio et al., 2019) left, both)
Swedish Adoption Twin Study Dominant hand (right, 9 0 4 21 0 7 12 0 3 42 2 14
of Aging (Pedersen, 2015) left, both)
Case studies (see Table 1) Varied 7 3 4 1 0 1 14 3 5 2 2 0
Total (all data) 64 6 24 62 1 17 105 8 44 104 5 32
Total (excluding missing/ 51 6 22 62 1 15 81 8 38 95 5 31
incomplete data)

Note. More detailed handedness data for case reports are provided in Table 1. ‘Total (all data)’ aggregates all available data, including cases from Shields (1962) for
which handedness was not reported, as well as two cases from Tienari (1963a) and one case from Slater (1953) that reported (left-) handedness for only one twin from
each pair (all missing values from those cases have been inferred to indicate right-handedness); ‘Total (excluding missing/incomplete data)’ includes all available data
other than these cases.
a
Handedness data for all twin-pairs studied by Newman et al. (1937) are reported in Table 43 of that publication. However, these data are not entirely consistent
with those presented in the individual case reports (see Table 1). In particular, Case III is reported as “Rl” in Table 43 of Newman et al. (1937), but in the text of the case
report it is specified that both twins are right-handed (RH). Where non-right-handedness (NRH) is indicated (in Table 43 and/or the text of the case report of Newman
et al., [1937]), we have coded such cases as NRH.
b
Handedness was not systematically reported by Shields (1962), although is mentioned in case reports for 9 of the 57 twin-pairs. Within these case reports, it is
specified that 10 (8.8%), or possibly 11 (9.6%; see Case S f 10), of the 114 twins were left-handed. As this is very close to the overall 9.13% prevalence estimate for
left-handedness in reared-together twins reported by a recent meta-analysis (Pfeifer et al., 2022), it is conceivable that handedness had only been noted in the text in
cases of non-right-handedness. Further support for this idea is provided in that Slater (1961) reported on handedness for two of the pairs later studied by Shields (1962)
(but on which Shields did not specify handedness), and in each case both twins were right-handed.
c
Handedness data from the MZAm triplet set included as part of the LWS/CDC study are not included here because they are also included within the data from the
MISTRA. Additionally, Abrams (1986) reported on an MZA female pair referred to pseudonymously as ‘Amy and Beth’ (for Amy “it was reported that she was definitely
right-handed.” [p. 44]; Beth “became left-handed.” [p. 49]); it is unclear to which pair this corresponds, and these data are not included here. Likewise, Abrams and
Neubauer (1994) referred pseudonymously to one MZAf pair (‘Abby and Amy’) and one MZAm pair (‘Barry and Ben’), though did not comment on their handedness.
d
Handedness data for the MISTRA have also been presented by Segal (2012, p. 294–295). Table 13–1 of that publication (p. 294) reports the following: MZA twins
(n = 74 pairs): Right-Left, 15%; Right-Mixed, 8%; Left-Left, 0%; Left-Mixed, 1%; Mixed-Mixed, 3%; Right-Right, 73% (MZA hand-concordant = 76%; MZA hand-­
discordant = 24%); DZA twins (n = 55 pairs): Right-Left, 18%; Right-Mixed, 15%; Left-Left, 2%; Left-Mixed, 2%; Mixed-Mixed, 5%; Right-Right, 58% (DZA hand
concordant = 65%; DZA hand discordant = 35%). Data presented in Study 1 of the current paper differ for several reasons: (1) we included only same-sex twin-pairs,
(2) those who reported attempts at handedness switching (whether successful or not) are coded as NRH, and (3) these data relate specifically to writing hand, whereas
the data presented by Segal (2012) relate to Crovitz-Zener Handedness Inventory (Crovitz and Zener, 1962) scores.

hand preference, regardless of direction. 5.4. Statistics

5.3.2. Strength of relative hand skill Males are more likely than females to indicate being left-, mixed-, or
The Purdue Pegboard (PPB; Tiffin, 1968) task is a test of manual non-right-handed (Papadatou-Pastou et al., 2008). Cohort effects have
dexterity in which participants are timed while placing metal pins, one also been reported, with studies published earlier than 1976 finding a
at a time, into a row of holes in a pegboard, first with the dominant hand lower prevalence of left-handedness than later studies (Papadatou-Pas­
and then with the non-dominant hand. The measure is known to exhibit tou et al., 2020). Additionally, females are, on average, faster than males
high test-retest reliability (e.g., r = 0.76; Doyen and Carlier, 2002). As on the PPB, and performance on this task reduces with age for both
with the CZHI, we computed an absolute (i.e., unsigned) LI from this males and females (Agnew et al., 1988). For these reasons, we regressed
task as a measure of the strength of relative hand skill. In this case we sex, age, sex × age, age2, and sex × age2 on each of our outcome vari­
used absolute values of (L - R)/(L + R) × 100. That is because higher ables (unsigned CZHI score, unsigned PPB LI score, right-hand PPB
scores on the PPB indicate slower hand speed, i.e., low values for the speed, and left-hand PPB speed), and used the residual scores in sub­
right hand relative to the left hand signify right-handedness (opposite to sequent analyses to control for age and sex effects according to the
the scoring system for the CZHI). A score of 0, therefore, indicates no methods of McGue and Bouchard (1984).
difference in completion time for the right and left hands, whereas To determine MZA and DZA within-pair similarity for handedness
scores closer to 100 indicate greater disparity between the hands. In outcomes we computed intraclass correlation coefficients (ICC), using
addition to the absolute LI, we examined right-hand speed and left-hand two-way mixed effects models with single measures and absolute
speed as separate outcome variables. agreement definition. Given that shared environmental effects are ab­
sent for MZA twins, the ICC for this group can be considered a direct

15
G. Richards and N.L. Segal Neuropsychologia 184 (2023) 108523

Table 3 Table 4
Summary concordance/discordance (excluding missing/incomplete data) for Summary concordance/discordance (all data) for direction of hand preference in
direction of hand preference in twins reared apart. twins reared apart.
RH/RH NRH/NRH RH/NRH RH/RH NRH/NRH RH/NRH

Males (n ¼ 157 MZA (n = 51 (64.6%) 6 (7.6%) 22 Males (n ¼ 174 MZA (n = 64 (68.1%) 6 (6.4%) 24
pairs) 79) (27.8%) pairs) 94) (25.5%)
DZA (n = 62 (79.5%) 1 (1.3%) 15 DZA (n = 62 (77.5%) 1 (1.3%) 17
78) (19.2%) 80) (21.3%)
Females (n ¼ 258 MZA (n = 81 (63.8%) 8 (6.3%) 38 Females (n ¼ 298 MZA (n = 105 (66.9%) 8 (5.1%) 44
pairs) 127) (29.9%) pairs) 157) (28.0%)
DZA (n = 95 (72.5%) 5 (3.8%) 31 DZA (n = 104 (73.8%) 5 (3.5%) 32
131) (23.7%) 141) (22.7%)
All twins (n ¼ 415 MZA (n = 132 (64.1%) 14 (6.8%) 60 All twins (N ¼ 472 MZA (n = 169 (67.3%) 14 (5.6%) 68
pairs) 206) (29.1%) pairs) 251) (27.1%)
DZA (n = 157 (75.1%) 6 (2.9%) 46 DZA (n = 166 (75.1%) 6 (2.7%) 49
209) (22.0%) 221) (22.2%)
Concordant Discordant Concordant Discordant
Males (n ¼ 157 MZA (n = 57 (72.2%) 22 (27.8%) Males (n ¼ 174 MZA (n = 70 (74.5%) 24 (25.5%)
pairs) 79) pairs) 94)
DZA (n = 63 (80.8%) 15 (19.2%) DZA (n = 63 (78.8%) 17 (21.3%)
78) 80)
Females (n ¼ 258 MZA (n = 89 (70.1%) 38 (29.9%) Females (n ¼ 298 MZA (n = 113 (72.0%) 44 (28.0%)
pairs) 127) pairs) 157)
DZA (n = 100 (76.3%) 31 (23.7%) DZA (n = 109 (77.3%) 32 (22.7%)
131) 141)
All twins (n ¼ 415 MZA (n = 146 (70.9%) 60 (29.1%) All twins (N ¼ 472 MZA (n = 183 (72.9%) 68 (27.1%)
pairs) 206) pairs) 251)
DZA (n = 163 (78.0%) 46 (22.0%) DZA (n = 172 (77.8%) 49 (22.2%)
209) 221)

Note. Data included here are derived from the samples studied by Newman et al. Note. Data included here are derived from the cohorts studied by Newman et al.
(1937) (n = 7 MZAm pairs; n = 12 MZAf pairs), Shields (1962) (n = 3 MZAm (1937) (n = 7 MZAm pairs; n = 12 MZAf pairs); Shields (1962) (n = 16 MZAm
pairs), and Juel-Nielsen (1965, 1980) (n = 3 MZAm pairs; n = 9 MZAf pairs); the pairs, n = 30 MZAf pairs; n = 1 DZAm pair; n = 10 DZAf pairs [note that an
Louise Wise Services (LWS)/Child Development Center (CDC) Twin Study (see earlier paper by Shields, 1958, reports on n = 13 MZAm and n = 25 MZAf of
Segal, 2021a) (n = 1 MZAm pair; n = 4 MZAf pairs; n = 2 DZAf pairs); Minnesota these twin pairs); and Juel-Nielsen (1965, 1980) (n = 3 MZAm pairs; n = 9 MZAf
Study of Twins Reared Apart (MISTRA) (Segal, 2012) (n = 30 MZAm pairs; n = pairs); the Louise Wise Services (LWS)/Child Development Center (CDC) Twin
39 MZAf pairs; n = 11 DZAm pairs; n = 22 DZAf pairs); Fullerton Study of Study (see Segal, 2021a) (n = 1 MZAm pair; n = 4 MZAf pairs; n = 2 DZAf pairs);
Chinese Twins Adopted Apart (Segal et al., 2011) (n = 1 MZAm pair; n = 14 Minnesota Study of Twins Reared Apart (MISTRA) (Segal, 2012) (n = 30 MZAm
MZAf pairs; n = 1 DZAm pair; n = 4 DZAf pairs); Finnish Twin Cohort Study pairs; n = 39 MZAf pairs; n = 11 DZAm pairs; n = 22 DZAf pairs); Fullerton Study
(Kaprio et al., 2019) (n = 9 MZAm pairs; n = 12 MZAf pairs; n = 37 DZAm pairs; of Chinese Twins Adopted Apart (Segal et al., 2011) (n = 1 MZAm pair; n = 14
n = 41 DZAf pairs); Swedish Adoption Twin Study of Aging (SATSA) (Pedersen, MZAf pairs; n = 1 DZAm pair; n = 4 DZAf pairs); Finnish Twin Cohort Study
2015) (n = 13 MZAm pairs; n = 15 MZAf pairs; n = 28 DZAm pairs; n = 58 DZAf (Kaprio et al., 2019) (n = 9 MZAm pairs; n = 12 MZAf pairs; n = 37 DZAm pairs;
pairs); as well as all miscellaneous case reports (n = 12 MZAm pairs; n = 22 n = 41 DZAf pairs); Swedish Adoption Twin Study of Aging (SATSA) (Pedersen,
MZAf pairs; n = 1 DZAm pair; n = 4 DZAf pairs). Cases with missing or 2015) (n = 13 MZAm pairs; n = 15 MZAf pairs; n = 28 DZAm pairs; n = 58 DZAf
incomplete handedness data from Shields (1962) (n = 13 MZAm pairs; n = 30 pairs); as well as all available miscellaneous case reports (n = 14 MZAm pairs; n
MZAf pairs; n = 1 DZAm pair; n = 10 DZAf pairs), Tienari (1963a) (n = 2 MZAm = 22 MZAf pairs; n = 2 DZAm pairs; n = 4 DZAf pairs).
pairs), and Slater (1953) (n = 1 DZAm pair) are not included here.

estimate of heritability (Segal, 2012). We also compared the ICCs be­ Table 5
Reported writing hand stratified by sex and twin type in the MISTRA.
tween MZA and DZA twins using Fisher’s r-to-z tests. We did this because
a significantly higher ICC for MZAs than DZAs would provide further MZAm DZAm MZAf DZAf
evidence of genetic influence should such effects emerge. Statistical n % n % n % n %
analyses were conducted using IBM SPSS version 27 (IBM Corp, 2020), RH total 51 85.0% 17 77.3% 73 93.6% 43 95.6%
and data visualisation was accomplished with the ggplot2 package LH total 9 15.0% 5 22.7% 5 6.4% 2 4.4%
(Wickham, 2009) in RStudio version 1.3.1073 (R Core Team, 2020).
Note. 26 twins reported that they had been encouraged to switch from LH to RH,
although in 7 cases this proved unsuccessful. Handedness reported here relates
6. Results to preference for the writing hand at the time of measurement, regardless of
whether switching of handedness had been attempted in childhood. DZAf, fe­
Most participants, regardless of twin type, reported writing with male dizygotic reared-apart twins; DZAm, male dizygotic reared-apart twins;
their right hand (Table 5). Scores on the CZHI and PPB were uncorre­ MZAf, female monozygotic reared-apart twins; MZAm, male monozygotic
lated, whether signed, rs (214) = − 0.096, p = .161) or unsigned, rs (214) reared-apart twins.
= 0.102, p = .136.
Intraclass correlation coefficients (ICCs) were calculated for each excluding twin pairs for which one or both members reported attempts
handedness measure (Table 6) and scatterplots are displayed in Fig. 1. to switch their handedness during childhood and when restricting the
Strength of hand preference was significantly positively correlated analysis to only right-handed participants. The only notable differences
within DZA pairs, but not within MZA pairs. Strength of relative hand were that (1) the DZA ICC for strength of hand preference was not sig­
skill showed no evidence of correlation within MZA or DZA twin pairs, nificant when considering only those participants that did not report
but significant positive correlations for right-hand and left-hand speed attempted handedness switching during childhood, and (2) the DZA
on the PPB task were observed in MZA twins. No such effects were correlation was significantly stronger than the MZA correlation only
observed in DZA twins, although the difference in slopes was not sta­ when the analysis was restricted to right-handed participants (Table 6).
tistically significant. This pattern of findings remained similar when

16
G. Richards and N.L. Segal Neuropsychologia 184 (2023) 108523

Table 6
Intraclass correlation coefficients for handedness measures in MZA and DZA twins in the MISTRA.
MZA DZA Comparison

Inclusion criteria n ICC p n ICC p z p

Hand preference strength All participants 80 0.019 .433 54 0.333 .007 − 1.81 .070
Non-switched 62 0.043 .370 45 0.230 .064 − 0.95 .342
Non-switched RH 51 − 0.112 .781 37 0.490 < .001 − 2.89 .004
Relative hand skill strength All participants 74 0.043 .357 45 − 0.143 .842 0.96 .337
Non-switched 59 0.126 .171 38 − 0.138 .813 1.23 .219
Non-switched RH 49 0.131 .185 31 − 0.199 .862 1.39 .165
Right-hand speed All participants 76 0.332 .002 47 0.053 .362 1.53 .126
Non-switched 64 0.339 .003 41 0.065 .343 1.39 .165
Non-switched RH 55 0.384 .002 34 0.231 .089 0.75 .453
Left-hand speed All participants 76 0.318 .002 47 0.150 .157 0.93 .352
Non-switched 64 0.289 .009 41 0.199 .108 0.46 .646
Non-switched RH 55 0.380 .002 34 0.086 .316 1.38 .168

Note. Hand preference strength is the absolute (unsigned) laterality index (LI) for the Crovitz-Zener Handedness Inventory (CZHI; Crovitz and Zener, 1962); relative
hand skill strength is the absolute (unsigned) LI for the Purdue Pegboard (PPB; Tiffin, 1968) task; right- and left-hand speed are the times taken to complete the PPB
task with each hand, respectively (each outcome variable has been residualised to control for effects of age and sex). Statistically significant effects (p < .05, two-tailed)
are presented in bold. DZA, dizygotic reared-apart twins; MZA, monozygotic reared-apart twins.

Fig. 1. Scatterplots for similarity in (a) strength of hand preference (b) strength of relative hand skill, (c) right hand speed, and (d) left hand speed, stratified by
zygosity.
Note. CZHI, Crovitz-Zener Handedness Inventory; DZA, dizygotic reared-apart twins; LI, laterality index; MZA, monozygotic reared-apart twins; PPB, Purdue
Pegboard Task.

17
G. Richards and N.L. Segal Neuropsychologia 184 (2023) 108523

7. Discussion further large-scale research that includes reared-together and reared-


apart twin pairs, combined with molecular genetic techniques, could
The current research provides a synthesis of the reared-apart twin prove informative with respect to this question.
literature as regards hand preference (Study 1) and examines several A strength of the current research is that we used a reared-apart twin
features of handedness obtained from participants in the Minnesota study, representing one of the most powerful designs for examining the
Study of Twins Reared Apart (MISTRA) (Study 2). Study 1 identified that relative importance of genetic and environmental effects on behavioural
at least N = 560 MZA and same-sex DZA twin pairs have come to the traits. However, while this design allows researchers to separate genetic
attention of researchers. Direction of hand preference data were avail­ from shared environmental influences, reared-apart twin pairs are rare;
able for both members of n = 415 cases, and concordance was slightly thus, obtaining sufficient data to achieve adequate levels of statistical
higher in DZA (78.0%) than MZA (70.9%) twin pairs. Although the focus power is challenging. Nevertheless, Lykken et al. (1981) showed how
here is on handedness, it is hoped that our compilation of case reports statistical power increased by 20 percent when studying twins reared
can serve as a useful resource for researchers interested in studying apart versus twins reared together.
reared-apart twins. The findings from Study 2 offer evidence of mod­ Another possible concern is that our survey of the literature of
erate heritability for right-hand and left-hand completion speed for the reared-apart twins identified data for more MZA (n = 325) than DZA (n
Purdue Pegboard (PPB) task, implying genetic influence on motor skill. = 235) pairs, even though same-sex DZ twins are approximately as
However, MZA twin pairs were no more alike than chance for strength of common as MZ twins in the general population. Interestingly, this
hand preference or relative hand skill. This lack of MZA twin pair pattern was not observed when considering only those cases for which
resemblance could possibly be linked to unusual prenatal developmental handedness data were available (MZA, n = 206; DZA, n = 209). In some
influences unique to MZ twins (e.g., differential cell distribution asso­ research, e.g., Newman et al. (1937), DZA pairs were not studied, and in
ciated with zygotic division) that may affect cotwins differently other instances it is clear that DZA pairs were studied in less detail than
(McNamara et al., 2016; Segal, 2021a). We also observed statistically MZA pairs (e.g., Shields, 1962). Notably, although Tienari (1963a,
significant similarity for strength of hand preference in DZA twin pairs. 1963b) specified that all MZA pairs in his case series were investigated,
Further research will be needed to resolve these unexpected results. elsewhere it is mentioned that a DZAm pair, raised apart from age 2–15
The finding that MZA twin pairs were similar for right-hand and left- years, was not followed-up (Tienari, 1966). Likewise, although Juel-­
hand task completion speed for the PPB is consistent with both reared- Nielsen (1965, 1980) dedicated (two editions of) an entire book to the
together and reared-apart twin studies reporting that MZ twins are study of 12 pairs of MZA pairs, a DZAf pair, separated until age 28, was
more alike than chance for motor performance (Fox et al., 1996; Jäncke only mentioned briefly in a separate publication (Juel-Nielsen and
and Steinmetz, 1995) and dominant hand psychomotor speed (Simonen Harvald, 1958). Additionally, as Segal (2012) has pointed out, MZA
et al., 1998). These findings are also consistent with the observation that twins are more likely to find each another than DZA twins because
studies using the classic twin design have provided evidence of genetic confusion by others (due to physical resemblance) may facilitate their
influence for most measured human individual difference traits studied reunion. Consistent with this view, 65 of 89 (73.0%) DZAm and 99 of
thus far (Polderman et al., 2015). However, it should be noted that 146 (67.8%) DZAf cases identified in our review came from either the
heritability estimates for hand speed derived from MZA intraclass cor­ SATSA or the Finnish Twin Cohort Study, both of which are
relation coefficients (ICC) in the current study were moderate, implying population-based. Notably, we also identified only two case reports that
that considerable variance for this characteristic is explained by were dedicated to the study of a specific DZA twin-pair (one DZAm
non-shared environmental factors. [Segal, 2015]; one DZAf [Segal et al., 2015b]). Although the study of
Strength of relative hand skill, as measured via the PPB task, did not MZA twins is particularly compelling, we emphasise the need to also
show evidence of heritability. This finding is consistent with an adoption study DZA pairs to obtain adequate comparison. However, despite the
study (Saudino and McManus, 1998) that observed no resemblance foregoing, the body of work produced by the MISTRA yielded findings
between adopted children and their biological parents for strength of consistent with extant reared-together twin studies (Segal, 2012). The
handedness. It is also consistent with research showing no statistically applicability of most twin study findings to the non-twin population has
significant resemblance in strength of relative hand skill within MZ twin also been demonstrated for the majority of measured traits (Christensen
pairs reared together (Jäncke and Steinmetz, 1995). Although these et al., 1995).
findings suggest no genetic basis for strength of relative hand skill, We administered the Crovitz-Zener Handedness Inventory (CZHI;
McManus and Bryden (1992) estimated that only ~10% of the variance Crovitz and Zener, 1962) rather than the more widely used Edinburgh
in this factor is familial. Large sample sizes would, therefore, be required Handedness Inventory (EHI; Oldfield, 1971). However, although dif­
to detect statistically significant effects, implying that the current study ferences are noted, considerable consistency is generally observed
may lack adequate statistical power. across handedness questionnaires as regards their content and response
We observed an unexpected effect in that strength of hand preference formats (Papadatou-Pastou et al., 2013). Notably, items from the CZHI
on the CZHI was more similar than chance for DZA twin pairs, but not for and EHI that assess the same manual activities tend to show a high de­
MZA twin pairs. Considering the absence of shared environmental fac­ gree of agreement (Bryden, 1977). Likewise, although we utilised the
tors present for DZA twins, this effect could be attributed to genetic PPB task (Tiffin, 1968) as a measure of hand speed and strength of
influence, with the lack of significant effect in MZA twins representing a relative hand skill, alternative paradigms (e.g., Annett Pegboard task
Type 2 error. Alternatively, the significant DZA correlation could be a [Annett, 1970]; Tapley and Bryden circle-marking task [Tapley and
Type 1 error. The effect in DZA pairs was statistically significant when Bryden, 1985]; finger-tapping [Peters and Durding, 1978]) are some­
examining the whole cohort, or when the analysis was limited to right- times used in laterality research. Simultaneous consideration of multiple
handed participants who had not been subject to attempts at handedness measures of relative hand skill suggests that although direction (i.e.,
switching during childhood, but not when examining the subset of right or left) is consistent across individuals, the different tasks relate to
participants that included left-handers who had not experienced at­ separable processes (McManus et al., 2016). Further research may
tempts at handedness switching. Likewise, the difference in strength therefore consider administering a range of relative hand skill measures,
between the MZA and DZA correlations was notably only statistically as these could exhibit different levels of heritability. It would also be of
significant when the analysis was restricted to right-handers who had interest to examine genetic correlations between these distinct aspects of
not experienced attempts at handedness switching. It is unclear why the handedness.
findings differ in this manner. Regardless, considering the paucity of
literature relating to genetic and environmental influences on strength
of hand preference (regardless of direction) and relative hand skill,

18
G. Richards and N.L. Segal Neuropsychologia 184 (2023) 108523

8. Conclusion Beaton, A.A., 2003. The nature and determinants of handedness. In: Hugdahl, K.,
Davidson, R.J. (Eds.), The Asymmetrical Brain. MIT Press, pp. 105–158. https://doi.
org/10.7551/mitpress/1463.003.0006.
We present a comprehensive summary of hand preference in reared- Bordier, A., Futerman, S., 2014. Separated @ Birth: a True Love Story of Twin Sisters
apart twins (Study 1) and the first study of strength of hand preference Reuinited. Berkley Books.
and relative hand skill in twin pairs from the MISTRA (Study 2). Bouchard Jr., T.J., Lykken, D.T., McGue, M., Segal, N.L., Tellegen, A., 1990. Sources of
human psychological differences: the Minnesota study of twins reared apart. Science
Although we observed evidence consistent with moderate genetic effects 250 (4978), 223–228. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.2218526.
on right-hand and left-hand speed, we did not find evidence of herita­ Bouterwek, H., 1936. Erhebungen an eineiigen Zwillingspaaren über Erbanlage und
bility for strength of relative hand skill. We also found greater than Umwelt als Charakterbildner. Z. Menschl. Vererb-u. Konstit-Lehre 20, 265–275.
Bouterwek, H., 1943. Erbe und Persönlichkeit: Charakterologische Ergebnisse der
chance similarity for strength of hand preference in DZA, but not MZA Zwillingsforschung. Franz Deuticke.
twins. Although this last effect was unexpected, it may reflect the rela­ Bryden, M.P., 1977. Measuring handedness with questionnaires. Neuropsychologia 15
tively small sample of DZA pairs present within the dataset. Further (4–5), 617–624. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(77)90067-7.
Burks, B.S., 1942. A study of identical twins reared apart under differing types of family
largescale molecular genetics and reared-together twin studies may offer relationships. In: Terman, L.M. (Ed.), Studies in Personality. McGraw-Hill,
a better understanding of the nature and underpinnings of strength of pp. 35–69. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1536-7150.1978.tb02790.x.
handedness. Burks, B.S., Roe, A., 1949. Studies of identical twins reared apart. Psychological
Monographs: General and Applied, 63(5), i–62. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0093608.
Burley, L., 2022. The Real Mo Farah. British Broadcasting Company.
Data sharing Burlingham, D., 1952. Twins: a study of three pairs of identical twins with 30 charts.
International Universities Press.
Burt, C., 1955. The evidence for the concept of intelligence. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 25 (3),
The datasets generated and/or analyzed during the current study are
158–177. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8279.1955.tb03305.x.
not publicly available due to limited ethics approval for the wider Burt, C., 1958. The inheritance of mental ability. Am. Psychol. 13 (1), 1–15. https://doi.
Minnesota Study of Twins Reared Apart (MISTRA) and to the specific org/10.1037/h0049002.
Burt, C., 1966. The genetic determination of differences in intelligence: a study of
consent provided by the participants. They may be available from the
monozygotic twins reared together and apart. Br. J. Psychol. 57 (1–2), 137–153.
second author on reasonable request and pending approval of any future https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1966.tb01014.x.
analyses by the MISTRA. SPSS syntax and R code used to run analyses for Burt, C., 1943. Ability and income. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 13 (2), 83–98. https://doi.org/
Study 2 are publicly available on the Open Science Framework (htt 10.1111/j.2044-8279.1943.tb02725.x.
Carter-Saltzman, L., 1980. Biological and sociocultural effects on handedness:
ps://osf.io/e4ysv/). comparison between biological and adoptive families. Science 209 (4462),
1263–1265. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7403887.
Christensen, K., Vaupel, J.W., Holm, N.V., Yashin, A.I., 1995. Mortality among twins
Declaration of competing interest
after age 6: fetal origins hypothesis versus twin method. BMJ 310 (6977), 432–436.
https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.310.6977.432.
The authors declare that they have no known competing financial Conway, J., 1958. The inheritance of intelligence and its social implications. British
interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence Journal of Statistical Psychology 11 (2), 171–190. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-
8317.1958.tb00012.x.
the work reported in this paper. Coren, S., Porac, C., 1980. Birth factors and laterality: effects of birth order, parental age,
and birth stress on four indices of lateral preference. Behav. Genet. 10 (2), 123–138.
Acknowledgments and funding sources https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01066263.
Craike, W.H., Slater, E., 1945. Folie à deux in uniovular twins reared apart. Brain: J.
Neurol. 68, 213–221. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/68.3.213.
Preliminary findings were presented on June 23rd, 2022, at the 52nd Crovitz, H.F., Zener, K., 1962. A group-test for assessing hand- and eye-dominance. Am.
Annual Meeting of the Behavior Genetics Association (Richards and Segal, J. Psychol. 75 (2), 271–276. https://doi.org/10.2307/1419611.
Cuellar-Partida, G., Tung, J.Y., Eriksson, N., Albrecht, E., Aliev, F., Andreassen, O.A.,
2022) and on August 25th, 2022, at the 8th North Sea Meeting on Lat­ Barroso, I., Beckmann, J.S., Boks, M.P., Boomsma, D.I., Boyd, H.A., Breteler, M.M.B.,
erality. The authors thank Prof. Nancy L. Pedersen for providing and Campbell, H., Chasman, D.I., Cherkas, L.F., Davies, G., de Geus, E.J.C., Deary, I.J.,
interpreting the SATSA data and Prof. Jaakko Kaprio for sharing the Deloukas, P., Medland, S.E., 2020. Genome-wide association study identifies 48
common genetic variants associated with handedness. Nat. Human Behav. https://
handedness data for reared apart twins from the Finnish Twin Cohort doi.org/10.1038/s41562-020-00956-y.
Study. For their assistance in interpreting research published in German Danforth, C.H., 1919. Resemblance and difference in twins: twins that look and act alike
and Japanese, we thank, respectively, Dr Patrick Rosenkranz and Dr attract attention first, while dissimilar ones are apt to be overlooked. J. Hered. 10
(9), 399–409. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordjournals.jhered.a101956.
Eriko Self. We also thank the team based at the Walton Library, New­
Dencker, S.J., 1958. A follow-up study of 128 closed head injuries in twins using co-twins
castle University, for their tireless efforts in searching for and locating as controls. Acta Psychiatrica et Neurologica 33 (Suppl. 123), 1–125.
many obscure academic articles that we could not otherwise have ob­ Doyen, A.-L., Carlier, M., 2002. Measuring handedness: a validation study of Bishop’s
tained. The SATSA was supported by the National Institutes of Health reaching card test. Laterality: Asymmetries Body, Brain Cognit. 7 (2), 115–130.
https://doi.org/10.1080/13576500143000140.
(grant numbers R01 AG04563, AG10175, AG028555), the MacArthur Essen-Möller, E., 1941. Psychiatrische Untersuchungen an einer Serie von zwillingen.
Foundation Research Network on Successful Aging; the Swedish Council Acta Psychiatrica et Neurologica, Suppl. 23, 1–200.
for Working Life and Social Research (FAS/FORTE) (grant numbers Essen-Möller, E., 1970. Twenty-one psychiatric cases and their MZ cotwins: a thirty
years’ follow-up. Acta Genet. Med. Gemellol.: Twin Res. 19 (1–2), 315–317. https://
97:0147:1B, 2009-0795), and the Swedish Research Council (grant doi.org/10.1017/s1120962300025798.
numbers: 825-2007-7460, 825-2009-6141). Support from the Academy Farber, S.L., 1981. Identical Twins Reared Apart: a Reanalysis. Basic Books, Inc.,
of Finland (grant #336823 to Jaakko Kaprio) enabled provision of the Publishers.
Fernandes, E. de S., Correia, J. L., Hellen, H. V. V., Silva, M. Jr., Segal, N. L., & Otta, E. (in
Finnish Twin Cohort Study data. preparation). Brazilian twins reared apart since newborns: A Case Study.
Fisher, R.A., 1958. Cancer and smoking. Nature 182 (4635). https://doi.org/10.1038/
References 182596a0, 596.
Fox, P.W., Hershberger, S.L., Bouchard Jr., T.J., 1996. Genetic and environmental
contributions to the acquisition of a motor skill. Nature 384 (6607), 356–358.
Abrams, S., 1986. Disposition and the environment. Psychoanal. Stud. Child 41 (1),
https://doi.org/10.1038/384356a0.
41–60. https://doi.org/10.1080/00797308.1986.11823450.
Fraceschetti, A., Bamatter, F., Klein, D., 1948. Valeur des tests cliniques et sérologiques
Abrams, S., Neubauer, P.B., 1994. Hartmann’s vision: identical twins and developmental
en vue de l’identification de deux jumeaux univitellins, dont l’un a été échangé par
organizations. Psychoanal. Stud. Child 49 (1), 49–59. https://doi.org/10.1080/
erreur. Bull. Schweiz. Akad. Med. Wiss. 4 (5–6), 433–444. https://doi.org/10.5169/
00797308.1994.11823050.
seals-306949.
Agnew, J., Bolla-Wilson, K., Kawas, C.H., Bleecker, M.L., 1988. Purdue pegboard age and
Franceschetti, A., 1948. L’importance déterminante des facteurs rhésus dans un cas
sex norms for people 40 years old and older. Dev. Neuropsychol. 4 (1), 29–35.
médico-légal. Arch. Julius Klaus-Stiftung für Vererbungsforsch. Sozialanthropol.
https://doi.org/10.1080/87565648809540388.
Rassenhyg. 23 (3–4), 460–464.
Annett, M., 1970. The growth of manual preference and speed. Br. J. Psychol. 61 (4),
Franceschetti, A., Bamatter, F., Klein, D., 1948. Valeur des tests cliniques et sérologiques
545–558. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1970.tb01274.x.
en vue de l’identification de deux jumeaux univitellins, dont l’un a été échangé par
Annett, M., 2002. Handedness and Brain Asymmetry: the Right Shift Theory. Psychology
erreur. Bull. Schweiz. Akad. Med. Wiss. 4 (5–6), 433–444. https://doi.org/10.5169/
Press.
seals-306949.
Bakan, P., Dibb, G., Reed, P., 1973. Handedness and birth stress 11, 363–366.

19
G. Richards and N.L. Segal Neuropsychologia 184 (2023) 108523

Fukuoka, G., 1937. Anthropometric and psychometric studies on Japanese twins. In: McManus, I.C., 2002. Right Hand, Left Hand: the Origins of Asymmetries in Brains,
Komai, T. (Ed.), Contributions to the Genetics of the Japanese Race II: Studies on Bodies, Atoms and Cultures. Weidenfeld & Nicolson.
Japanese Twins, pp. 9–47. McManus, I.C., 2022. Cerebral polymorphisms for lateralisation: modelling the genetic
Gao, W.-J., Li, L.-M., Cao, W.-H., Zhan, S.-Y., Lv, J., Pang, Z.-C., Chen, W.-J., Wang, S.-J., and phenotypic architectures of multiple functional modules. Symmetry, 14(4) 814.
Chen, R.-F., Hu, Y.-H., 2011. A comparative study on blood pressure, obesity, https://doi.org/10.3390/sym14040814.
smoking and alcohol drinking behavior in Chinese twins reared apart and together. McManus, I.C., Bryden, M.P., 1992. The genetics of handedness, cerebral dominance and
J. Peking Univ. (Heal. Sci.) 43 (3), 329–332. V43/I3/329. http://xuebao.bjmu.edu. lateralization. In: Rapin, I., Segalowitz, S.J. (Eds.), Handbook of Neuropsychology,
cn/EN/Y2011/. Vol. 6: Child Neuropsychology. Elsevier Science Publishers B.V., pp. 115–144
Gardner, I.C., Newman, H.H., 1940. Mental and physical traits of identical twins reared McManus, I.C., Van Horn, J.D., Bryden, P.J., 2016. The Tapley and Bryden test of
apart: case XX. Twins Lois and Louise. J. Hered. 31 (3), 119–126. https://doi.org/ performance differences between the hands: the original data, newer data, and the
10.1093/oxfordjournals.jhered.a104855. relation to pegboard and other tasks. Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and
Glasby, M., Loader, D., Glasby, R., 2013. Buy One Get One Free: A Book about Fact Being Cognit. 21 (4–6), 371–396. https://doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2016.1141916.
Stranger than Fiction. www.wanowandthen.com. McNamara, H.C., Kane, S.C., Craig, J.M., Short, R.V., Umstad, M.P., 2016. A review of
Gottesman, I.I., Shields, J., 1972. Schizophrenia and Genetics: a Twin Study Vantage the mechanisms and evidence for typical and atypical twinning. Am. J. Obstet.
Point. Academic Press. Gynecol. 214 (2), 172–191. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajog.2015.10.930.
Hagi, A., 1931. The blood types of twins as studied with saliva. Tokio Iji Shinshi 2731, Mitsuda, H., Sakai, T., 1968. Comparative studies with monozygotic twins discordant for
1455–1462. typical and atypical schizophrenia. Jpn. J. Hum. Genet. 13 (3), 183–188.
Hayakawa, K., Shimizu, T., Kato, K., Onoi, M., Kobayashi, Y., 2002. A gerontological Mogensen, A., Juel-Nielsen, N., 1961a. Factors influencing the selection and rejection of
cohort study of aged twins: the Osaka University Aged Twin Registry. Twin Res. 5 Szondi’s pictures: a study of uniovular twins brought up apart. Acta Psychiatr.
(5), 387–388. https://doi.org/10.1375/136905202320906147. Scand. 37 (1), 32–36. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.1961.tb06157.x.
Hayakawa, K., Kato, K., Onoi, M., Hayashi, C., Yang-Ping, C., Kanamori, M., Doi, S., Mogensen, A., Juel-Nielsen, N., 1961b. Monozygotic twins reared apart. Part 1.
Kikuchi, H., Nishihara, R., Kadota, K., 2006. The Japanese study of adult twins Intelligence. Acta Psychologica 91, 426–427. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0001-6918
reared apart and growing old separately. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 9 (6), 806–807. (61)80190-X.
https://doi.org/10.1375/twin.9.6.806. Mogensen, A., Juel-Nielsen, N., 1963. Proceedings of the Sixteenth International
Hayashi, S., 1963. A study of juvenile delinquency by twin method. Hanzaigaku Zasshi Congress of Psychology, Bonn 1960: Monozygotic Twins Reared Apart, vol. 3,
29, 153–172. pp. 22–23. I. Intelligence. Excerpa Criminologica.
Hayashi, S., 1967. A study of juvenile delinquency in twins. In: Mitsuda, H. (Ed.), Clinical Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., Altman, D.G., Antes, G., Atkins, D., Barbour, V.,
Genetics in Psychiatry: Problems in Nosological Classification. Igaku Shoin Ltd., Barrowman, N., Berlin, J.A., Clark, J., Clarke, M., Cook, D., D’Amico, R., Deeks, J.J.,
pp. 373–378 Devereaux, P.J., Dickersin, K., Egger, M., Ernst, E., Gøtzsche, P.C., Tugwell, P., 2009.
IBM Corp, 2020. IBM SPSS Statistics for MacOS. Version 27.0. IBM Corp. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA
Inouye, E., 1971. Monozygotic twins with schizophrenia reared apart in infancy. statement. PLoS Med. 6 (7), e1000097. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.
Excerpta Med. Int. Congr. Ser. 233, 93. pmed.1000097.
Inouye, E., 1972. Monozygotic twins with schizophrenia reared apart in infancy. Jpn. J. Muller, H.J., 1925. Mental traits and heredity: the extent to which mental traits are
Hum. Genet. 16 (3), 182–190. independent of heredity, as tested in a case of identical twins reared apart. J. Hered.
Jäncke, L., Steinmetz, H., 1995. Hand motor performance and degree of asymmetry in 16 (12), 433–448. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordjournals.jhered.a102537.
monozygotic twins. Cortex 31 (4), 779–785. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452 Newman, H.H., 1929a. Mental traits of identical twins reared apart: case III. Twins ‘C’
(13)80028-7. and ‘O’. J. Hered. 20 (4), 153–166. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordjournals.jhered.
Jordan, H.E., 1914. Hereditary lefthandedness, with a note on twinning. (Study III). a103166.
J. Genet. 4 (1), 67–81. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf02981820. Newman, H.H., 1929b. Mental and physical traits of identical twins reared apart: case I.
Joye, M., 1954. He Was Not My Son. Rinehart & Company, Inc. Twins ‘A’ and ‘O’. J. Hered. 20 (2), 49–64. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordjournals.
Juel-Nielsen, N., 1965. Individual and Environment: Monozygotic Twins Reared Apart. jhered.a103142.
International Universities Press. Newman, H.H., 1929c. Mental and physical traits of identical twins reared apart: case II.
Juel-Nielsen, N., 1980. Individual and Environment: Monozygotic Twins Reared Apart. Twins ‘E’ and ‘G’. J. Hered. 20 (3), 97–104. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordjournals.
International Universities Press. jhered.a103163.
Juel-Nielsen, N., Harvald, B., 1958. The electroencephalogram in uniovular twins Newman, H.H., 1932a. Mental and physical traits of identical twins reared apart: case IV.
brought up apart. Acta Genet. Stat. Med. 8 (1), 57–64. https://doi.org/10.1159/ Twins Mary and Mabel, and review of the first four cases studied. J. Hered. 23 (1),
000151054. 3–18. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordjournals.jhered.a103509.
Juel-Nielsen, N., Mogensen, A., 1957. Uniovular twins brought up apart: (preliminary Newman, H.H., 1932b. Mental and physical traits of identical twins reared apart: case V.
report of a psychiatric-psychological study). Acta Genet. Stat. Med. 7 (2), 430–433. Twins ‘B’ and ‘D’. J. Hered. 23 (8), 297–303. https://doi.org/10.1093/
Kaij, L., 1960. Alcoholism in twins: studies on the etiology and sequels of abuse of oxfordjournals.jhered.a103639.
alcohol. Almqvist & Wiksell. Newman, H.H., 1932c. Mental and physical traits of identical twins reared apart: case VI.
Kallmann, F.J., 1938. The genetics of schizophrenia. J. J. Augustin. Twins Ada and Ida. J. Hered. 23 (9), 369–377. https://doi.org/10.1093/
Kallmann, F.J., 1946. The genetic theory of schizophrenia: an analysis of 691 oxfordjournals.jhered.a103664.
schizophrenic twin index families. Am. J. Psychiatr. 103 (3), 309–322. https://doi. Newman, H.H., 1933. Mental and physical traits of identical twins reared apart: case VII.
org/10.1176/ajp.103.3.309. Twins Richard and Raymond. J. Hered. 24 (5), 209–214. https://doi.org/10.1093/
Kallmann, F.J., Roth, B., 1956. Genetics aspects of preadolescent schizophrenia. Am. J. oxfordjournals.jhered.a103778.
Psychiatr. 112 (8), 599–606. https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.112.8.599. Newman, H.H., 1934a. Mental and physical traits of identical twins reared apart: case IX.
Kaprio, J., Bollepalli, S., Buchwald, J., Iso-Markku, P., Korhonen, T., Kovanen, V., Twins Harold and Holden. J. Hered. 25 (4), 137–143. https://doi.org/10.1093/
Kujala, U., Laakkonen, E.K., Latvala, A., Leskinen, T., Lindgren, N., Ollikainen, M., oxfordjournals.jhered.a103900.
Piirtola, M., Rantanen, T., Rinne, J., Rose, R.J., Sillanpää, E., Silventoinen, K., Newman, H.H., 1934b. Mental and physical traits of identical twins reared apart: case
Sipilä, S., Waller, K., The older Finnish Twin Cohort — 45 years of follow-up, 2019. VIII. Twins ‘M’ and ‘R’. J. Hered. 25 (2), 55–60. https://doi.org/10.1093/
Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 22 (4), 240–254. https://doi.org/10.1017/thg.2019.54. oxfordjournals.jhered.a103878.
Komai, T., 1937. Review of literature on twin studies in Japan. In: Komai, T. (Ed.), Newman, H.H., Freeman, F.N., Holzinger, K.J., 1937. Twins: A Study of Heredity and
Contributions to the Genetics of the Japanese Race II: Studies on Japanese Twins, Environment. University of Chicago Press.
pp. 1–8. Nicholls, M.E.R., Johnston, D.W., Shields, M.A., 2012. Adverse birth factors predict
Kranz, H., 1936. Lebensschicksale krimineller Zwillinge. Verlag von Julius Springer. cognitive ability, but not hand preference. Neuropsychology 26 (5), 578–587.
Kringlen, E., 1964. Schizophrenia in male monozygotic twins. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 40 https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029151.
(Suppl. 178), 1–76. Ocklenburg, S., Güntürkün, O., 2018. The Lateralized Brain: the Neuroscience and
Kringlen, E., 1967a. Heredity and Environment in the Functional Psychoses. Heinemann. Evolution of Hemispheric Asymmetries. Academic Press.
Kringlen, E., 1967b. Case Histories Supplement. Norwegian Monographs on Medical Oldfield, R.C., 1971. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh
Science. Inventory. Neuropsychologia 9 (1), 97–113. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932
Kurihara, M., 1959. A study of schizophrenia by twin method. Psychiatr. Neurol. Jpn. 61 (71)90067-4.
(13), 1721–1741. Ooki, S., 2006. Nongenetic factors associated with human handedness and footedness in
Lange, J., 1931. Crime as Destiny: a Study of Criminal Twins. George Allen & Unwin Ltd. Japanese twin children. Environ. Health Prev. Med. 11 (6), 304–312. https://doi.
Lindeman, B., 1969. The twins who found each other. Pocket Books second ed. org/10.1007/bf02898021.
Lykken, D.T., 1978. The diagnosis of zygosity in twins. Behav. Genet. 8 (5), 437–473. Papadatou-Pastou, M., Tomprou, D.-M., 2015. Intelligence and handedness: meta-
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01067939. analyses of studies on intellectually disabled, typically developing, and gifted
Lykken, D.T., Geisser, S., Tellegen, A., 1981. Heritability Estimates from Twin Studies: individuals. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 56, 151–165. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
the Efficiency of the MZA Design. Unpublished Manuscript. University of Minnesota. neubiorev.2015.06.017.
McGue, M., Bouchard Jr., T.J., 1984. Adjustment of twin data for the effects of age and Papadatou-Pastou, M., Martin, M., Munafò, M.R., Jones, G.V., 2008. Sex differences in
sex. Behav. Genet. 14 (4), 325–343. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01080045. left-handedness: a meta-analysis of 144 studies. Psychol. Bull. 134 (5), 677–699.
McIndoe, A., Franceschetti, A., 1949. Reciprocal skin homographs in a medico-legal case https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012814.
of familial identification of exchanged identical twins. Br. J. Plast. Surg. 2, 283–289. Papadatou-Pastou, M., Martin, M., Munafò, M.R., 2013. Measuring hand preference: a
McManus, I.C., 1981. Handedness and birth stress. Psychol. Med. 11 (3), 485–496. comparison among different response formats using a selected sample. Laterality:
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291700052806. Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognit. 18 (1), 68–107. https://doi.org/10.1080/
1357650X.2011.628794.

20
G. Richards and N.L. Segal Neuropsychologia 184 (2023) 108523

Papadatou-Pastou, M., Ntolka, E., Schmitz, J., Martin, M., Munafò, M.R., Ocklenburg, S., Segal, N.L., Cortez, F.A., 2014. Born in Korea-adopted apart: behavioral development of
Paracchini, S., 2020. Human handedness: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 146 (6), monozygotic twins raised in the United States and France. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 70,
481–524. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000229. 97–104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.06.029.
Pedersen, N.L., 2015. Swedish adoption/twin study on aging (SATSA), 1984, 1987, 1990, Segal, N.L., Diamond, M., 2014. Identical reared apart twins concordant for
1993, 2004, 2007, and 2010. Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social transsexuality. J. Exp. Clin. Med. 6 (2), 74. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Research [distributor], 10.3886/ICPSR03843.2015-05-13, v2. jecm.2014.02.007.
Peters, M., Durding, B.M., 1978. Handedness measured by finger tapping: a continuous Segal, N.L., Hur, Y.-M., 2008. Reared apart Korean female twins: genetic and cultural
variable. Can. J. Psychol. 32 (4), 257–261. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0081694. influences on life histories, physical and health-related measures, and behavioral
Pfeifer, L.S., Schmitz, J., Papadatou-Pastou, M., Peterburs, J., Paracchini, S., traits. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 32 (6), 542–548. https://doi.org/10.1177/
Ocklenburg, S., 2022. Handedness in twins: meta-analyses. BMC Psychology 10 (1), 0165025408097133.
11. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-021-00695-3. Segal, N.L., Hur, Y.-M., 2022. Personality traits, mental abilities and other individual
Polderman, T.J.C., Benyamin, B., de Leeuw, C.A., Sullivan, P.F., van Bochoven, A., differences: monozygotic female twins raised apart in South Korea and the United
Visscher, P.M., Posthuma, D., 2015. Meta-analysis of the heritability of human traits States. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 194 (111643) https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
based on fifty years of twin studies. Nat. Genet. 47 (7), 702–709. https://doi.org/ paid.2022.111643.
10.1038/ng.3285. Segal, N.L., Montoya, Y.S., 2018. Accidental Brothers: the Story of Twins Exchanged at
Popenoe, P., 1922. Twins reared apart. J. Hered. 13 (3), 142–144. https://doi.org/ Birth and the Power of Nature and Nurture. St. Martin’s Press.
10.1093/oxfordjournals.jhered.a102184. Segal, N.L., Stohs, J.H., Evans, K., 2011. Chinese twin children reared apart and reunited:
Price, J., 1968. The genetics of depressive behavior. In: Coppen, A., Walk, A. (Eds.), first prospective study of co-twin reunions. Adopt. Q. 14 (1), 61–78. https://doi.org/
Recent Developments in Affective Disorders, British Journal of Psychiatry. Special 10.1080/10926755.2011.557953.
Publication No. 2, pp. 37–54 (Headley Bros). Segal, N.L., Hur, Y.-M., Graham, J.L., 2015a. Korean twins reared apart: genetic and
Prokop, H., Druml, W., 1973. Diskordantes Auftreten einer depressiv gefärbten Katatonie cultural influences on behavior and health. Adopt. Q. 18 (4), 291–310. https://doi.
bei eineiigen Zwillingen im Jugendalter. Wien. Z. Nervenheilkd. Deren Grenzgeb. 31 org/10.1080/10926755.2015.1088110.
(2), 156–166. Segal, N.L., Cortez, F.A., Zettel-Watson, L., Cherry, B.J., Mechanic, M., Munson, J.E.,
R Core Team., 2020. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Velázquez, J.M.A., Reed, B., 2015b. Genetic and experiential influences on behavior:
R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.r-project.org/. twins reunited at seventy-eight years. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 73, 110–117. https://doi.
Richards, G., Segal, N.L., 2022. Strength of hand preference and relative hand skill in org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.09.017.
twins reared apart. Behav. Genet. 52, 388. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-022- Segal, N.L., Montoya, Y.S., Loke, Y.J., Craig, J.M., 2017. Identical twins doubly
10119-6. exchanged at birth: a case report of genetic and environmental influences on the
Rosanoff, A.J., Handy, L.M., Rosanoff Plesset, I., 1935. The etiology of manic-depressive adult epigenome. Epigenomics 9 (1), 5–12. https://doi.org/10.2217/epi-2016-0104.
syndromes with special reference to their occurrence in twins. Am. J. Psychiatr. 91 Segal, N.L., Montoya, Y.S., Becker, E.N., 2018. Twins reared apart and twins in families:
(4), 725–762. https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.91.4.725. the findings behind the fascination. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 21 (4), 295–301. https://
Rosanoff, A.J., Handy, L.M., Rosanoff Plesset, I., 1937. The etiology of mental deficiency doi.org/10.1017/thg.2018.34.
with special reference to its occurrence in twins: a chapter in the genetic history of Segal, N.L., Montoya, Y.S., Peña, F.Y., Burgos, S., Katz, X., 2019. Eye refraction in doubly
human intelligence. Psychological Monographs, 48(4), i–137. https://doi.org/10.1037 exchanged monozygotic twins. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 22 (3), 177–182. https://doi.
/h0093544. org/10.1017/thg.2019.26.
Satz, P., 1972. Pathological left-handedness: an explanatory model. Cortex 8 (2), Shields, J., 1958. Twins brought up apart. Eugen. Rev. 50 (2), 115–123.
121–135. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(72)80013-3. Shields, J., 1962. Monozygotic Twins Brought up Apart and Brought up Together: an
Saudek, R., 1933. Identical twins reared apart: a comparative study of various tests of Investigation into the Genetic and Environmental Causes of Variation in Personality.
their intellectual, emotional, and social attitudes. Character and Personality 2 (1), Oxford University Press.
22–40. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1933.tb02079.x. Shields, J., Slater, E., 1967. Genetic aspects of schizophrenia. Hosp. Med. 1, 579–584.
Saudek, R., 1934. A British pair of identical twins reared apart. Character and Personality Simonen, R.L., Videman, T., Battié, M.C., Gibbons, L.E., 1998. Determinants of
3 (1), 17–39. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1934.tb01980.x. psychomotor speed among 61 pairs of adult male monozygotic twins. J. Gerontol.
Saudino, K., McManus, I.C., 1998. Handedness, footedness, eyedness and earedness in Series A: Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 53A (3), M228–M234. https://doi.org/10.1093/gerona/
the Colorado Adoption Project. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 16 (2), 167–174. https://doi.org/ 53A.3.M228.
10.1111/j.2044-835X.1998.tb00916.x. Slater, E., 1953. Psychotic and Neurotic Illnesses in Twins (Medical Research Council
Schein, E., Bernstein, P., 2007. Identical Strangers: A Memoir of Twins Separated and Special Report Series, No. 278). Her Majesty’s Stationery Office.
Reunited. Hachette Australia. Slater, E., 1961. The thirty-fifth Maudsley lecture: ‘Hysteria 311’. J. Ment. Sci. 107 (448),
Schwesinger, G.C., 1952. The effect of differential parent-child relation on identical twin 359–381. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.107.448.359.
resemblance in personality. Acta Genet. Med. Gemellol. 1 (1), 40–47. https://doi. Slater, E., 1968. A review of earlier evidence on genetic factors in schizophrenia.
org/10.1017/s1120962300042360. J. Psychiatr. Res. 6 (Suppl. 1), 15–26. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(68)
Searleman, A., Porac, C., Coren, S., 1989. Relationship between birth order, birth stress, 90005-8.
and lateral preferences: a critical review. Psychol. Bull. 105 (3), 397–408. https:// Stenstedt, Å., 1952. A study in manic-depressive psychosis: clinical, social and genetic
doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.105.3.397. investigations. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 28 (Suppl. 79), 1–111.
Seeman, E., Saudek, R., 1933. The handwriting of identical twins reared apart. Character Stephens, F.E., Nunemaker, J.C., 1950. Spondylitis in identical twins reared apart.
and Personality 1 (4), 268–285. J. Hered. 41 (11), 283–286. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordjournals.jhered.
Segal, N.L., 2009. Twins reared apart: a forgotten case. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 12 (1), a106058.
123–126. https://doi.org/10.1375/twin.12.1.123. Stephens, F.E., Thompson, R.B., 1943. The case of Millan and George: identical twins,
Segal, N.L., 2011a. Someone Else’s Twin: the True Story of Babies Switched at Birth. reared apart. J. Hered. 34 (4), 109–114. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordjournals.
Prometheus Books. jhered.a105259.
Segal, N.L., 2011b. Reunited twins: spouse relations. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 14 (3), Sudarsky, L., Myers, R.H., Walshe, T.M., 1983. Huntington’s disease in monozygotic
290–294. https://doi.org/10.1375/twin.14.3.290. twins reared apart. J. Med. Genet. 20 (6), 408–411. https://doi.org/10.1136/
Segal, N.L., 2012. Born Together—Reared Apart: the Landmark Minnesota Twin Study. jmg.20.6.408.
Harvard University Press. https://doi.org/10.4159/harvard.9780674065154. Tapley, S.M., Bryden, M.P., 1985. A group test for the assessment of performance
Segal, N.L., 2014. Art for twins: Yorùbá artists and their statues. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. between the hands. Neuropsychologia 23 (2), 215–221. https://doi.org/10.1016/
17 (3), 215–221. https://doi.org/10.1017/thg.2014.18. 0028-3932(85)90105-8.
Segal, N.L., 2015. The fourth International Network of Twin Registries: overview from Thomas, A., Chess, S., 1977. Temperament and Development. Brunner/Mazel.
Osaka. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 18 (6), 812–818. https://doi.org/10.1017/ Tienari, P., 1963a. Psychiatric illnesses in identical twins. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 39
thg.2015.81. (Suppl. 171), 9–195.
Segal, N.L., 2016. Twinsters the movie: reared apart twins in real time. Twin Res. Hum. Tienari, P., 1963b. A psychiatric twin study (preliminary report). Acta Psychiatr. Scand.
Genet. 19 (1), 80–85. https://doi.org/10.1017/thg.2015.98. 38 (Suppl. 169), 393–397.
Segal, N.L., 2017. Reared-apart Chinese twins: chance discovery. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. Tienari, P., 1966. On intrapair differences in male twins with special reference to
20 (2), 180–185. https://doi.org/10.1017/thg.2017.9. dominance-submissiveness. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 42 (Suppl. 188), 1–166. https://
Segal, N.L., 2018. Twins switched at birth: frequency, life histories, twin relationships, doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.1966.tb01912.x.
and critical issues. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 21 (5), 477–483. https://doi.org/ Tienari, P., 1968. Schizophrenia in monozygotic male twins. In: Rosenthal, D., Kety, S.S.
10.1017/thg.2018.48. (Eds.), The Transmission of Schizophrenia. Pergamon, pp. 27–36.
Segal, N.L., 2021a. Deliberately Divided: Inside the Controversial Study of Twins and Tiffin, J., 1968. Purdue pegboard examiner manual. Science Research Associates.
Triplets Adopted Apart. Rowman & Littlefield. Tucker, W.H., 1997. Re-reconsidering Burt: beyond a reasonable doubt. J. Hist. Behav.
Segal, N.L., 2021b. Twins separated at birth: across a country and around the world. Sci. 33 (2), 145–162. https://doi.org/10.1002/(sici)1520-6696(199721)33:2<145::
Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 24 (4), 244–250. https://doi.org/10.1017/thg.2021.30. aid-jhbs6>3.0.co;2-s.
Segal, N.L., 2022. Reared-apart and reunited: extraordinary dynamics. Twin Res. Hum. Wagenseil, F., 1931. Zwei Mitteilungen über die erbbiologische Bedeutung der eineiigen
Genet. 25 (3), 165–170. https://doi.org/10.1017/thg.2022.21. Mehrlinge. Z. Für Konstitutionslehere 15, 632–645.
Segal, N.L., Blandón-Gitlin, I., 2010. Twins switched at birth: a case from the Canary Wickham, H., 2009. ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis. Springer-Verlag.
Islands. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 13 (1), 115–119. https://doi.org/10.1375/ https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-98141-3.
twin.13.1.115. Williams, C.S., Buss, K.A., Eskenazi, B., 1992. Infant resuscitation is associated with an
increased risk of left-handedness. Am. J. Epidemiol. 136 (3), 277–286. https://doi.
org/10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a116493.

21
G. Richards and N.L. Segal Neuropsychologia 184 (2023) 108523

Yates, N., Brash, H., 1941. An investigation of the physical and mental characteristics of Zhou, B., Gao, W., Lv, J., Yu, C., Wang, S., Liao, C., Pang, Z., Cong, L., Dong, Z., Wu, F.,
a pair of like twins reared apart from infancy. Ann. Eug. 11 (1), 89–101. https://doi. Wang, H., Wu, X., Jiang, G., Wang, X., Wang, B., Cao, W., Li, L., 2015. Genetic and
org/10.1111/j.1469-1809.1941.tb02276.x. environmental influences on obesity-related phenotypes in Chinese twins reared
Yoshimasu, S., 1941. Psychopathie und Kriminalität. Die Bedeutung der Erbanlage und apart and together. Behav. Genet. 45 (4), 427–437. https://doi.org/10.1007/
Umwelt für die Entstehung von Verbrechen im Lichte der Zwillingsforschung. s10519-015-9711-0.
Psychiatr. Neurol. Jpn. 45 (9), 455–531.

22

You might also like