Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 3

Oxytocin Enhances the Encoding of Positive Social

Memories in Humans
Adam J. Guastella, Philip B. Mitchell, and Frosso Mathews
Background: In nonhuman mammals, oxytocin has a critical role in social recognition and the development of long-term bonds. There has
been limited research evaluating effects of oxytocin on the encoding and recognition of faces in humans.

Methods: In a double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled, between-subject design, we administered oxytocin (24 IU) or a placebo to 69
healthy human male volunteers and then presented 36 happy, angry, or neutral human faces. Participants returned the following day to
make “remember,” “know,” or “new” judgments for a mix of 72 new and previously seen faces.

Results: Oxytocin-administered participants were more likely to make remember and know judgments for previously seen happy faces
compared with angry and neutral human faces. In contrast, oxytocin did not influence judgments for faces that had not been presented
previously.

Conclusions: This study shows that the administration of oxytocin to male humans enhances the encoding of positive social information
to make it more memorable. Results suggest that oxytocin could enhance social approach, intimacy, and bonding in male humans by
strengthening encoding to make the recall of positive social information more likely.

Key Words: Emotion, face recognition, oxytocin, peptide, social research shows important differences between remember (“rec-
cognition ollection of detail”) and know (“feels familiar”) judgments (15),
and these seem to be based on discernable biological processes
(16). We selected three types of facial expressions for study:

T
he benefits of maintaining supportive social relationships
are well documented (1). Researchers suggest a role for the happy, angry, and neutral. Researchers suggest that OT may
neuropeptide oxytocin (OT) in this process by enhancing produce effects, in part, by enhancing the reward value for
social-reward and reducing stress and social-threat responding positive social stimulus (2,17) while decreasing responding to
(2,3). Animal studies provide strong evidence implicating OT in aggressive social stimulus (3,11). It was hypothesized, therefore,
social behaviour. For instance, mice given an injection of OT into that OT would enhance encoding and subsequent recognition
the medial-amygdala display improved recognition for peers for happy faces and reduce encoding and subsequent recogni-
(4,5). Although OT knockout mice fail to show evidence of tion for angry faces.
social-recognition (5), administration of OT to these animals
Methods and Materials
restores recognition (4). These effects are found when OT is
administered before social encounters but not after, suggest- Sixty-nine healthy young adult male students1 from the
ing that OT enhances encoding rather than retrieval (4). University of New South Wales (UNSW; aged 18 –30 years, M ⫽
Effects in animals also seem specific to rewarding social 19.98, SD ⫽ 2.27) were randomly assigned in a double-blind
stimuli (6). Oxytocin has amnesic effects on the consolidation manner to receive either 24 international units (IU) of OT (n ⫽
of nonsocial stimuli (7) and reduces social aggression and 35; Novartis, Basel, Switzerland) or a placebo (n ⫽ 34). Nasal
threat responses (6). sprays were developed by a compounding chemist (four puffs
Social recognition is essential for developing long-term bonds per nostril, each with 3 IU), with an identical placebo containing
(8). We could not locate any evaluation of OT influences on all ingredients except the active OT. Exclusion criteria included
recognition memory for faces in humans. Human research self-reported symptoms of depression, bipolar, panic, and psy-
suggests that OT impairs or does not influence memory for chotic disorders; substance dependence; epilepsy; and traumatic
nonsocial information, such as word lists or general emotional brain injury. Exclusion was determined from a brief self-report
information (9 –11). There is evidence, however, that OT en- interview by a trained experimenter. Participants were instructed to
hances the perception of faces in males. OT increases gaze to the abstain from alcohol and caffeine on the day of the procedure and
eye region of human faces (12), and improves the identification food and drink (except water) 2 hours before drug administration.
of emotion from the eyes of others (13). The aim of this study After a study description, written consent was obtained, and partic-
was to evaluate whether OT given before encoding would ipants were told they could withdraw at any time. Ethical approval
enhance face recognition memory long term. We used the was provided by the UNSW Ethics Committee (06074).
remember/know paradigm (14) because a substantial body of The Positive and Negative Affect Scale (PANAS) (18) was used
to track mood changes in positive and negative affect states. Face
stimuli consisted of 72 different male and female human black
From the Brain & Mind Research Institute (AJG), University of Sydney, School and white happy, angry, and neutral face expressions (24 of each
of Psychology (AJG, FM), and School of Psychiatry (PBM), University of expression). Seventy faces were taken from the Karolinska-
New South Wales, Sydney, Australia. Directed Emotional Faces database (19). Two additional faces
Address reprint requests to Adam Guastella, Ph.D., Brain & Mind Research Insti-
tute, University of Sydney, NSW, Australia; E-mail: aguastella@med.
1
usyd.edu.au. It is standard practice in the literature (12,13) to exclude females because
Received November 21, 2007; revised February 12, 2008; accepted February of variation in estrogen levels across the menstrual cycle and ethical
13, 2008. considerations relating to giving OT during pregnancy (32).

0006-3223/08/$34.00 BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2008;64:256 –258


doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.02.008 © 2008 Society of Biological Psychiatry
A.J. Guastella et al. BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2008;64:256 –258 257

Table 1. Percentage of Raw Remember, Know, and New Judgments Made for Old and New Face Expressions
Mean (SD)

Old Stimuli New Stimuli


Remember (%) Know (%) New (%) Remember (%) Know (%) New (%)

Angry Faces
OT M 58 (.23) 23 (.15) 19 (.17) 07 (.09) 20 (.14) 73 (.18)
Placebo M 55 (.18) 29 (.17) 16 (.12) 04 (.05) 21 (.15) 75 (.17)
Happy Faces
OT M 55 (.22) 30 (.18) 15 (.13) 04 (.07) 21 (.16) 75 (.19)
Placebo M 43 (.17) 31 (.15) 26 (.16) 03 (.05) 17 (.12) 80 (.15)
Neutral Faces
OT M 43 (.18) 29 (.18) 28 (.16) 06 (.09) 19 (.13) 75 (.16)
Placebo M 46 (.19) 31 (.15) 24 (.17) 07 (.10) 21 (.12) 72 (.16)
OT ⫽ oxytocin.

were edited and matched in appearance from the NimStim judgments for “new” happy, angry, and neutral faces showed no
Face-Stimulus database (20). No face identity was used more differences between drug groups4 [largest t (59) ⫽ 1.50, p ⫽ .14].
than once. Twelve happy, twelve angry, and twelve neutral face Scores for “old” stimuli were subjected to the independence
expressions were randomly selected to serve in Set A or Set B. of redundancy assumption (16,21): Independent Remember ⫽
Sets were counterbalanced as “old” (presented at study and test) (Hit Remember – False Alarm Remember); Independent Know ⫽
or “new” (presented only at test) for the remember/know Know/(1 – Remember). A 2 [Drug (OT, Placebo)] ⫻ 3 [Face
procedure. Expression (Happy, Angry, Neutral)] ⫻ 2 [Judgment (Indepen-
Participants completed the PANAS, the brief interview, and dent Remember, Independent Know)] repeated-measures multi-
then received the nasal spray. Forty-five minutes later (12,13), variate analysis of variance was run on responses to “old” face
participants completed the PANAS and viewed 36 faces (Set A or stimuli. Results indicated a main effect of Judgment [F (1,59) ⫽
Set B) for 2 sec on a standard 17-inch computer monitor. 18.55, p ⬍ .001], showing a reported higher percentage of
Participants were told that we were interested in how OT independent know judgments than independent remember judg-
influenced reactions to social information and asked to make ments. There was no main effect of Drug [F (1,59) ⫽ .01, p ⫽ .95],
ratings on a computer of how critical (1 ⫽ extremely critical to but a main effect of Face Expression [F (2,58) ⫽ 6.18, p ⫽ .004],
9 ⫽ extremely accepting) or trustworthy (1 ⫽ not at all to 9 ⫽ which was better accounted for by a Face Expression by Drug
extremely) each face appeared. Participants returned the follow- interaction [F (2,58) ⫽ 7.06, p ⫽ .002, (see Figure 1). Follow-up
ing day for a surprise memory test consisting of 72 randomized repeated-measure analyses of variance for each judgment
faces from Set A and B. Participants were asked to press the R showed that OT participants made more independent remember
(Remember) key if they could recollect specific details about a judgments for happy faces5 [F (2,58) ⫽ 2.71, p ⫽ .07]. An
face from the study session (such as an aspect of the face’s interaction was also found for independent know judgments
physical appearance), the K (Know) key if the face felt familiar [F (2,58) ⫽ 5.65, p ⫽ .006], showing that, in contrast to placebo,
but they could not recollect specific details, and the N (New) key OT participants displayed enhanced independent know re-
if they believed the face was not presented at study. Participants sponding to happy faces over angry and neutral faces. Interac-
completed self-reports (PANAS, possible drug side effects, beliefs tions between Judgment and Face Expression [F (2,58) ⫽ 1.69,
about drug assignment) and were debriefed. p ⫽ .19], Judgment and Drug [F (1,59) ⫽ .92, p ⫽ .34], and Face
Expression, Judgment, and Drug [F (2,58) ⫽ .67, p ⫽ .51] were
Results nonsignificant.
Eight participants were excluded due to equipment malfunc- Discussion
tion or failure to attend sessions, leaving 31 OT and 30 placebo
participants with no age difference [t (59) ⫽ .99, p ⫽ .33]. t tests This is the first study in humans to show that OT strengthens the
and chi-square analysis evaluating differences between drug encoding of positive social stimuli to make this information more
groups showed there were no differences in positive or negative memorable. Participants administered OT at study displayed im-
affect ratings at any session [largest t (59) ⫽ –1.58, p ⫽ .12], no proved “remember” responding for happy faces and a bias in
differences across critical and trustworthiness ratings of faces,2 “know” responding for happy over angry and neutral faces. In
and no differences in the number or type of side effects reported3 support of previous research (12,13), there was no clear influence of
or beliefs about drug allocation (p ⬎ .3). Table 1 presents the raw OT on subjective self-reports, such as mood, drug-identification,
percentages (before transformation) of remember, know, and and face-appraisal ratings. Findings support evolutionary-biological
new responses for “old” and “new” face stimuli. t tests run on perspectives (11) that emphasize OT’s role in enhancing processing
for cues important for bonding, while reducing the impact of
2
socially aversive cues. Our findings indicate OT may facilitate
A trend from the t tests run indicated OT participants could have rated
happy faces as more trustworthy than placebo participants [t (59) ⫽ 4
A 2 [Drug] ⫻ 3 [Face Expression)] ⫻ 2 [Judgment Type] repeated-
1.85, p ⫽ .07; OT M ⫽ 6.13; SD ⫽ 1.05; placebo M ⫽ 5.61; SD ⫽ 1.12]. measures multivariate of analysis was run on responses to “new” face
3
Side effects reported by OT participants included relaxation or tiredness stimuli. No significant main effects or interactions were found.
(n ⫽ 8), attentiveness (n ⫽ 2), headaches (n ⫽ 2), and blurred vision 5
When analysis was rerun using raw score remember judgments, the
(n ⫽ 1). Placebo participants reported relaxation or tiredness (n ⫽ effect was stronger [F (2,58) ⫽ 4.18, p ⫽ .02], showing OT enhanced
10), attentiveness (n ⫽ 1), and irritability (n ⫽ 1). memory for positive faces over placebo [t (59) ⫽ 2.38, p ⫽ .02].

www.sobp.org/journal
258 BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2008;64:256 –258 A.J. Guastella et al.

5. Young LJ (2002): The neurobiology of social recognition, approach, and


avoidance. Biol Psychiatry 51:18 –26.
6. Lim MM, Young L (2006): Neuropeptidergic regulation of affiliative be-
havior and social bonding in animals. Horm Behav 50:506 –517.
7. de Oliveira LF, Camboim C, Diehl F, Consiglia AR, Quillfeldt JA (2007):
Glucocorticoid-mediated effects of systemic oxytocin upon memory
retrieval. Neurobiol Learn Mem 87:67–71.
8. Young LJ, Wang Z (2004): The neurobiology of pair bonding. Nat Neuro-
sci 7:1048 –1054.
9. Pitman RK, Orr SP, Lasko NB (1993): Effects of intranasal vasopressin and
oxytocin on physiologic responding during personal combat imagery in
Vietnam veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychiatry Res 48:
107–117.
10. Bruins J, Hijman R, Van Ree JM (1992): Effect of a single dose of des-
glycinamide-[Arg8]vasopressin or oxytocin on cognitive processes in
young healthy subjects. Peptides 13:461– 468.
11. Heinrichs M, Meinlschmidt G, Wippich W, Ehlert U, Hellhammer DH
Figure 1. Percentages of independent remember and know judgments (2004): Selective amnesic effects of oxytocin on human memory. Physiol
across happy, angry, and neutral faces. Error bars represent standard error. Behav 83:31–38.
Rem ⫽ independent remember judgment; for independent remember 12. Guastella AJ, Mitchell PB, Dadds MR (2008): Oxytocin increases gaze to
judgments, analysis indicated oxytocin enhancement of remember re- the eye-region of human faces. Biol Psychiatry 63:3–5.
sponding to happy faces [t(59) ⫽ 2.11, p ⫽ .04]. 13. Domes G, Heinrichs M, Michel A, Berger C, Herpertz S (2007): Oxytocin
improves “mind-reading” in humans. Biol Psychiatry 61:731–733.
intimacy and bonding in male humans by making the encoding of 14. Rajaram S (1993): Remembering and knowing: two means of access to
positive social cues during interactions more memorable. the personal past. Mem Cogn 21:89 –102.
We propose two biological explanations. First, animal research 15. Diana RA, Reder LM, Arndt J, Park H (2006): Models of recognition: A review
shows that activation of OT receptors within the central amygdala of arguments in favor of a dual-process account. Psychon Bull Rev 13:1–21.
reduces fear (22). This has led to speculation (23) that OT reduces 16. Sharot T, Delgado M, Phelps E (2004): How emotion enhances the feel-
ing of remembering. Nat Neurosci 2:1376 –1380.
social-threat detection and associated memory consolidation in
17. Depue RA, Morrone-Strupinsky JV (2005): A neurobehavioral model of
humans. Oxytocin may reduce the salience of threatening-angry affiliative bonding: Implications for conceptualizing a human trait of
faces (24), allowing for enhanced processing of other salient cues affiliation. Behav Brain Sci 28:313–395.
(i.e., positive social cues). Second, OT may directly enhance pro- 18. Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A (1988): Development and validation of
cessing for positive social-cues by activating social-reward neural brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. J Pers
networks (25–27). The amygdala has rich links to reward-value Social Psychol 54:1063–1070.
pathways (28) that are involved in processing romantic and attrac- 19. Lundqvist D, Flykt A, Ohman A (1988): The Karolinska Directed Emo-
tive faces (29), as well as encoding and preference learning for tional Faces—KDEF. Stockholm: Karolinska Insitutet, Department of
positive faces (30). Further, direct eye gaze activates reward-value Clinical Neuroscience, Psychology Section.
pathways (31), and OT enhances eye gaze (12). Oxytocin may 20. Tottenham N, Tanaka JW, Leon AC, McCarry T, Nurse M, Hare TA, et al.
(2006): NimStim set of facial expressions. Boston: The MacArthur Foun-
enhance reward-value for positive social cues to increase salience
dation Research Network on Early Experience and Brain Development.
and encoding. Animal OT data shows the importance of reward 21. Yonelinas AP, Jacoby LL (1995): The relation between remembering and
pathways to bonding (8), but human OT research is needed. Taken knowing as bases for recognition: Effect of size congruency. J Mem Lang
together, these data suggest that OT may benefit psychiatric disor- 34:622– 643.
ders characterized by social-bonding deficits (i.e., autism) and 22. Hubber D, Veinante P, Stoop R (2005): Vasopressin and oxytocin excite
social-threat responses (i.e., social anxiety disorder) by strengthen- distinct neuronal populations in the central amygdala. Science 308:245–
ing the encoding of positive social cues in everyday interactions. 248.
Research is also required extending these findings to females, 23. Heinrichs M, Gaab J (2007): Neuroendocrine mechanisms of stress and
particularly given implications for mother-infant bonding. social interaction: Implications for mental disorders. Curr Opin Psychiatry
20:158 –162.
24. Fox E, Lester V, Russo R, Bowles RJ, Pichler A, Dutton K (2000): Facial
We thank Dr. Daniel Hermens, Dr. Gavan McNally, and expressions of emotion: Are angry faces detected more efficiently?
Professor Mark Dadds for helpful comments on this article, as Cogn Emotion 14:61–92.
well as Dr. Kevin Bird for statistical advice. We thank Siobhan 25. Cox SM, Andrade A, Johnsrude IS (2005): Learning to like: A role for human
Kelly and Caitlin Van Slooten for assistance with data collection. orbitofrontal cortex in conditioned reward. J Neurosci 25:2733–2740.
Dr. Guastella, Dr. Mitchell, and Miss Mathews report no 26. Balleine BW, Delgado MR, Hikosaka O (2007): The role of the dorsal
biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest. striatum in reward and decision-making. J Neurosci 27:8161– 8165.
27. Wickens JR, Horvitz JC, Costa RM, Killcross S (2007): Dopaminergic
mechanisms in actions and habits. J Neurosci 27:8181– 8183.
1. Uchino B (2006): Social support and health: A review of physiological
28. Senior C (2003): Beauty in the brain of the beholder. Neuron 38:525–528.
processes potentially underlying links to disease outcomes. J Behav Med
29. Aron A, Fisher HE, Mashek DJ, Strong G, Li HF, Brown LL (2005): Reward,
29:377–387.
2. Uvnas-Moberg K (1998): Oxytocin may mediate the benefits of posi- motivation and emotion systems associated with early-stage intense
tive social interaction and emotions. Psychoneuroendocrinology 23: romantic love. J Neurophysiol 94:327–337.
819 – 835. 30. Bray S, O’Doherty J (2007): Neural coding of reward-prediction error
3. Bartz JA, Hollander E (2006): The neuroscience of affiliation: Forging signals during classical conditioning with attractive faces. J Neuro-
links between basic and clinical research on neuropeptides and social physiol 97:3036 –3045.
behavior. Horm Behav 50:518 –528. 31. Kampe KK, Frith CD, Dolan RJ, Frith U (2002): Reward value of attractive-
4. Ferguson JN, Aldag JM, Insel TR, Young LJ (2001): Oxytocin in the medial ness and gaze. Nature 413:589.
amygdala is essential for social recognition in the mouse. J Neurosci 32. Campbell A (2008): Attachment, aggression, and affiliation: The role of
21:8278 – 8285. oxytocin in female social behaviour. Biol Psychol 77:1–10.

www.sobp.org/journal

You might also like