Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 28

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/26890892

A developmental, mentalization-based approach to the understanding and


treatment of borderline personality disorder

Article in Development and Psychopathology · November 2009


DOI: 10.1017/S0954579409990198 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

856 6,885

2 authors, including:

Peter Fonagy
University College London
1,132 PUBLICATIONS 64,548 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Attachment and mentalization in family therapy for adolescent anorexia nervosa. View project

The Neurobiology of Mother-Infant Attachment View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Peter Fonagy on 10 July 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Development and Psychopathology 21 (2009), 1355–1381
Copyright # Cambridge University Press, 2009
doi:10.1017/S0954579409990198

A developmental, mentalization-based
approach to the understanding and treatment
of borderline personality disorder

PETER FONAGYa AND PATRICK LUYTENb


a
University College London; and b University of Leuven

Abstract
The precise nature and etiopathogenesis of borderline personality disorder (BPD) continues to elude researchers and
clinicians. Yet, increasing evidence from various strands of research converges to suggest that affect dysregulation,
impulsivity, and unstable relationships constitute the core features of BPD. Over the last two decades, the mentalization-
based approach to BPD has attempted to provide a theoretically consistent way of conceptualizing the interrelationship
between these core features of BPD, with the aim of providing clinicians with a conceptually sound and empirically
supported approach to BPD and its treatment. This paper presents an extended version of this approach to BPD based
on recently accumulated data. In particular, we suggest that the core features of BPD reflect impairments in different facets
of mentalization, each related to impairments in relatively distinct neural circuits underlying these facets. Hence, we
provide a comprehensive account of BPD by showing how its core features are related to each other in theoretically
meaningful ways. More specifically, we argue that BPD is primarily associated with a low threshold for the activation of
the attachment system and deactivation of controlled mentalization, linked to impairments in the ability to differentiate
mental states of self and other, which lead to hypersensitivity and increased susceptibility to contagion by other people’s
mental states, and poor integration of cognitive and affective aspects of mentalization. The combination of these
impairments may explain BPD patients’ propensity for vicious interpersonal cycles, and their high levels of affect
dysregulation and impulsivity. Finally, the implications of this expanded mentalization-based approach to BPD for
mentalization-based treatment and treatment of BPD more generally are discussed.

Borderline personality disorder (BPD) is a se- Links, Thabane, & Webb, 2008) and forensic
vere condition with a lifetime prevalence that populations (Black et al., 2007) where between
has been estimated to be as much as 6% 25 and 33% may be expected to meet criteria
(Grant et al., 2008). BPD is often comorbid for the diagnosis. In recent years, the develop-
with mood and anxiety disorders, bipolar disor- mental emergence of BPD has become a topic
der, and schizotypal and narcissistic personality of significant current interest (Cohen, 2008; Ro-
disorder (Grant et al., 2008). It may be particu- gosch & Cicchetti, 2005), particularly as there is
larly common in outpatient (Korzekwa, Dell, increasing evidence that the disorder has strong
roots in early development. This paper presents
a coherent, clinically relevant developmental
The ideas presented in this paper draw on the work of an
active group of clinicians and scientists. We thank Anthony model of BPD based on recently accumulated
Bateman, Linda Mayes, Mary Target, George Gergely, Jon data. We summarize the key features of the dis-
Allen, Efrain Bleiberg, Rudi Vermote, Benedicte Lowyck, order and link these features to impairments in
and Elizabeth Allison. specific facets of mentalization, each related to
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Peter relatively specific neural systems, and discuss
Fonagy, Research Department of Clinical, Educational
and Health Psychology, University College London, Gower
moderators of these relationships. Finally, we
Street, London WC1E 6BT, UK; E-mail: p.fonagy@ucl. outline the implications of these findings for
ac.uk. future research and clinical practice.

1355
1356 P. Fonagy and P. Luyten

Core Features of BPD al., 2008), includes an intense sense of shame


(Levy, Edell, & McGlashan, 2007; Rusch
Although the precise nature of BPD remains elu- et al., 2007), and can prompt dissociation (Phil-
sive, there is growing consensus that the core fea- ipsen et al., 2004) and self-harm (Coid, 1993).
tures of the disorder consist of emotional dysreg- If the unequivocal description of BPD in
ulation (e.g., Linehan, 1993; Reisch, Ebner- adulthood is problematic, its reliable descrip-
Priemer, Tschacher, Bohus, & Linehan, 2008), tion in adolescence is an even greater challenge
high levels of impulsivity (e.g., Grootens et al., and remains controversial in terms of the
2008), leading to self-harm and suicidality (Black, homogeneity of the category, comorbidity be-
Blum, Pfohl, & Hale, 2004), and disturbed inter- tween and within axes, arbitrary cutoffs, and
personal functioning (Hill et al., 2008), which is poor test–retest reliability (Miller, Muehlen-
also expressed in high levels of preoccupied and kamp, & Jacobson, 2008). In addition, current
disorganized attachment patterns in BPD patients dimensional models such as the five-factor
(for a review, see Levy, Meehan, Weber, Rey- model of personality may not necessarily be
noso, & Clarkin, 2005). In borderline patients the most suitable for a detailed clinical descrip-
we and others have noted a characteristic pattern tion of maladaptive features of personality pa-
of fearful attachment (attachment–anxiety and thology at different ages (Blatt & Luyten, in
relational avoidance), painful intolerance of alone- press; De Clercq, De Fruyt, Van Leeuwen, &
ness, hypersensitivity to social environment, ex- Mervielde, 2006; Westen, Dutra, & Shedler,
pectation of hostility from others, and greatly re- 2005), given the potentially different pheno-
duced positive memories of dyadic interactions typic expression of trait symptoms at younger
(e.g. Critchfield, Levy, Clarkin, & Kernberg, ages (Sharp & Romero, 2007). A classic 2-
2008; Gunderson & Lyons-Ruth, 2008). year prospective questionnaire study (Crick,
We underscore three further facets of BPD Murray-Close, & Woods, 2005) with a repre-
that emerge as aspects of disturbed social relat- sentative sample of 400 fourth to sixth graders,
edness from both factor analytic and clinical for instance, found that the developmental pre-
studies (e.g., Sanislow et al., 2002). First, disso- cursors of BPD features in adolescence showed
ciative comorbidity is more frequently reported moderate stability, although “emotion sensitiv-
by patients with BPD than other groups (e.g. ity” (intense unstable inappropriate emotion)
Sar, Akyuz, Kugu, Ozturk, & Ertem-Vehid, and “physical aggression” were no longer sig-
2006; Zanarini, Frankenburg, Jager-Hyman, nificant predictors once level of depression
Reich, & Fitzmaurice, 2008). It appears to be had been controlled for. The best predictors of
a response to increased stress (Stiglmayr et al., BPD features from fourth to sixth grade were in-
2008), to be associated with emotional neglect dicators of social dysfunction such as friend ex-
(Sar et al., 2006) and to be linked to suicidal clusivity (overly close relationship with friend),
ideation (Klonsky, 2008). Second, BPD is relational aggression (impulsivity), and cog-
also characterized by a disturbed sense of iden- nitive sensitivity (hostile, untrusting paranoid
tity (e.g., Blatt & Auerbach, 1988; Bradley & world view). The adaptive functioning and psy-
Westen, 2005), rooted in a dysfunction or defi- chiatric symptoms of adolescents who meet cri-
cit of a sense of agency or self-directedness teria for BPD are significantly worse than those
(e.g., Barnow, Ruge, Spitzer, & Freyberger, from clinic samples who meet criteria for other
2005; Bender & Skodol, 2007). Third, the ex- PDs or no PD criteria (Chanen, Jovev, & Jack-
perience of intense inner pain out of keeping son, 2007). Specific features of BPD such as
with the significance of the event may be a self-harm or traits of impulsivity and affective
core feature of BPD (Zanarini & Frankenburg, instability present during childhood and adoles-
2007) frequently associated with disturbed rela- cence are predictive of receiving an adult BPD
tionships (Gunderson & Lyons-Ruth, 2008), diagnosis mainly in retrospective studies (Zana-
and childhood abuse and neglect (Holm & Sev- rini et al., 2006). Both clinical and community
erinsson, 2008). It is commonly prompted by based studies have found only moderate stability
social situations that involve rejection, aban- of the diagnosis in adolescence (Chabrol &
donment, and/or isolation (e.g., Stiglmayr et Leichsenring, 2006) and the stability of these
Understanding and treatment of borderline personality disorder 1357

diagnoses into adulthood is suggestive rather agy, & Goodyer, 2008). We have consistently
than well-established (Cohen, 2008; Deschamps argued the following:
& Vreugdenhil, 2008). On balance, the best evi-
dence for developmental antecedents of BPD 1. Understanding the behavior of others in
comes from examinations of the particular type terms of their likely thoughts, feelings,
of social dysfunction also characteristic of the wishes, and desires is a not a constitutional
adult form of the disorder comprising lack of given but a developmental achievement.
trust and relational aggression. BPD probably 2. The acquisition of this capacity depends
consists of both stable and unstable features, ba- on the quality of attachment relationships,
sic temperamental characteristics that show sta- particularly but not exclusively, early attach-
bility over time, and more specific, potentially ments, as the latter reflect the extent to which
transient symptoms that are related to these per- our subjective experience was adequately
sonality traits in some yet to be specified way. mirrored by a trusted other.
BPD may consist of both symptoms that are man- 3. The quality of affect mirroring impacts on
ifestations of an acute illness and symptoms that the development of affect regulative pro-
represent more enduring aspects of the disorder, cesses and self-control (including attention
with many patients experiencing long term dis- mechanisms and effortful control) as well
ability. However, it would be too simplistic to as the capacity for mentalization.
consider early established traits as tantamount 4. Disruptions of early attachment and later
to inherited vulnerabilities. The interaction of un- trauma can disrupt the capacity for mentali-
folding constitutional givens and the psychoso- zation and, linked to this, the development
cial environment within which they manifest of a coherent self-structure.
and which they create is likely to be the leitmotif 5. The capacity to mentalize has both “trait”
of both this and other personality disorders. and “state” aspects that vary in quality in re-
lation to emotional arousal and interpersonal
context.
6. Mentalization and the associated capacities for
The Mentalization-Based Approach
affect representation, affect regulation, and
to BPD
attentional control normally obscure forms
It is our fundamental assumption that the core of subjectivity that developmentally antedate
features of BPD are not isolated characteristics, mentalization.
but are related on various levels. Over the last 7. The failure of mentalizing, in combination
two decades, we have attempted to provide a with profound disorganization of self-struc-
theoretically consistent way of conceptualizing ture, may account for the core features of
the development and interrelationship of these borderline personality functioning as de-
features, aiming to provide clinicians with a scribed above. In particular, we have argued
conceptually sound and empirically supported that borderline personality functioning can
approach to BPD. be understood as the consequence of (a)
Mentalization is a form of social cognition. the loss of mentalization in emotionally in-
It is the imaginative mental activity that enables tense relationship contexts, (b) the reemerg-
us to perceive and interpret human behavior in ence at these times of modes of thinking
terms of intentional mental states (e.g., needs, about subjective experience that antedate
desires, feelings, beliefs, and goals; Allen, Fon- full mentalization, and (c) the constant pres-
agy, & Bateman, 2008; Bateman & Fonagy, sure for externalization of internal states
2006). Previously we have argued that impair- (projective
ments in this capacity play an important role identification), which we conceive of as the
in the development of various psychiatric disor- reexternalization of disorganized intolerably
ders that involve pathology of the self (Bateman painful self-states (the self-destructive alien
& Fonagy, 2004; Fonagy & Bateman, 2006b, self).
2008; Fonagy & Target, 2006; Luyten, Fonagy, 8. Finally, we argue that a therapeutic interven-
Mayes, & Van Houdenhove, 2009; Sharp, Fon- tion that focuses on the patient’s capacity to
1358 P. Fonagy and P. Luyten

mentalize in the context of attachment rela- that requires attention, intention, awareness,
tionships can be helpful in improving both be- and effort. By contrast, automatic or implicit
havioral and affective aspects of the condition. mentalization is perceived, nonconscious, non-
verbal, and unreflective. It is typified by mirror-
This paper presents an expanded version of ing. It presumes parallel and therefore much
the mentalization-based approach to BPD based faster processing, is reflexive, and requires little
on recently accumulated data. In particular, we effort, focused attention or intention (Satpute &
reexamine the mentalization construct in the Lieberman, 2006). Developmentally, nonver-
light of current neuroimaging findings and sug- bally determined implicit mentalizing appears
gest a four-component developmental model to be robust early in the second year (Onishi,
(Luyten, Fonagy, et al., 2009) that might further Baillargeon, & Leslie, 2007), or perhaps earlier
increase our understanding of the relationships (Gergely, Bekkering, & Kiraly, 2002), whereas
among the core features of BPD from a develop- verbal recognition of another’s perspective is
mental perspective. reliable only in the fourth (Carpendale &
Lewis, 2006). Different brain areas may be re-
cruited for automatic and controlled mentaliz-
Mentalization as a Multidimensional
ing. Automatic social cognition involves the
Construct
amygdala, basal ganglia, ventromedial prefron-
The concept of mentalization has been appropri- tal cortex (VMPFC), lateral temporal cortex,
ately criticized as a marker of a specific form of and the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (ACC),
psychopathology such as BPD because in its ori- in general, phylogenetically older brain circuits
ginal formulation the concept was too broad and that rely heavily on sensory information. By con-
multifaceted to be operationalized (Choi-Kain & trast, controlled mentalization recruits the lateral
Gunderson, 2008; Holmes, 2005). Elsewhere prefrontal cortex (LPFC), medial prefrontal cor-
(Luyten, Fonagy, et al., 2009), based on extant tex (mPFC), lateral parietal cortex (LPAC), med-
neuroimaging, developmental social and cog- ial parietal cortex (mPAC), medial temporal lobe
nitive research, as well as heuristic considerations, (mTL), and rostral ACC (rACC), which are phy-
we have proposed that mentalization can be char- logenetically newer brain circuits involved in
acterized as organized along four polarities: auto- processing of linguistic and symbolic material.
matic/controlled, cognitive/affective, internal/ex- Arousal may facilitate automatic mentaliza-
ternal-based, and self/other focused. Each of tion and inhibit neural systems associated with
these dimensions reflects the involvement of controlled mentalization (Lieberman, 2007;
two relatively distinct neural systems. Below we Mayes, 2006), and BPD patients probably have
consider the relevance of each of these polarities a lower threshold for activation of flight–fight sys-
to the difficulties with mentalization experienced tems (Jogems-Kosterman, de Knijff, Kusters, &
by patients with BPD, and describe how these dif- van Hoof, 2007), although as we will see this
ficulties are related to the core features of BPD may be a more complex association than it ap-
discussed above. pears (Kahl et al., 2006). Abnormal stress regula-
tion and the developmental and neurological sep-
aration of these two systems, however, may
Implicit-automatic versus explicit-controlled
explain how emotional arousal in BPD may on
mentalization
the one hand cause the loss of explicit mentaliz-
The distinction between automatic and con- ing yet, on the other hand patients often appear
trolled social cognition has existed in both so- implicitly particularly attuned to the states of
cial and cognitive psychology for some time, mind of individuals around them. By the same to-
but it has only been applied relatively recently ken they appear able to perform experimental
in the field of mentalizing (Lieberman, 2007; mentalizing tasks relatively well under low
Satpute & Lieberman, 2006; Uddin, Iacoboni, arousal (Arntz, Bernstein, Oorschot, Robson, &
Lange, & Keenan, 2007). Explicit mentaliza- Schobre, 2006), but cannot explain the states of
tion is typically interpreted, conscious, verbal, mind they experience under high arousal and
and reflective. It is a serial and slow process show confusion about mental states at times
Understanding and treatment of borderline personality disorder 1359

when they are dominated by reflexive assump- tion. We have suggested that mental interiors
tions about the internal states of others. Unfortu- are discovered by the infant through the contin-
nately, psychotherapists of many orientations gent interaction between external signs of affect
often attempt to provide mentalistic understand- in the child and their processed or “metabo-
ings for issues that trigger intense emotional reac- lized” or “marked” reflection by the caregiver
tions (challenging interpersonal situations, issues (Fonagy, Gergely, Jurist, & Target, 2002; Ger-
of shame, guilt, feelings of inadequacy, etc.) at a gely & Watson, 1996). In mirroring the infant
time when the capacity for effective explicit men- the caregiver must achieve more than contin-
talization is practically inaccessible (Fonagy & gency (in time, space, and emotional tone).
Bateman, 2006a). The mirroring must be “marked” (e.g., exagger-
ated), in other words, slightly distorted, if the
infant is to understand the caregiver’s display
Mentalization based on internal versus
as part of the infant’s emotional experience
external features of self and others
rather than an expression of hers (Fonagy et al.,
Different neural networks appear to be involved 2007; Gergely, 2004). We have speculated
depending on whether the focus of attention for that the origin of affect regulation difficulties
making imaginative inferences about both our in patients with BPD may be associated with
own and others’ states of mind is internal or exter- an early psychosocial environment where their
nal (Lieberman, 2007). A focus on “mental inte- internal experiences were not adequately mir-
riors” means directly considering thoughts, feel- rored. Noncontingent affect expressions by the
ings, and experiences, and is accompanied by parent will undermine the appropriate “labeling”
the activation of a medial frontoparietal network of the infant’s internal states (i.e., the establish-
(Satpute & Lieberman, 2006). Focusing mentali- ment of introspectively accessible second-order
zation on exteriors, on physical and visible fea- representations for them) that may, in turn, re-
tures, or actions of another’s or our own actions, main confusing, experienced as unsymbolized,
appears to recruit a more lateral frontotemporo- and hard to regulate. One recent study has sug-
parietal network. As the mental interior may be gested that individuals recruit the same neural
thought of as a second-order representation of tissue for both mother-referential and self-re-
the exterior (Fonagy, Gergely, & Target, 2007), ferential processing, indicating a strongly inte-
the internal/external distinction applies as much grated network for the two (Vanderwal, Hunyadi,
to self as other-related mentalization. Grupe, Connors, & Schultz, 2008), a finding
BPD patients primarily show deficits in that is strikingly consistent with the suggestion
making mental state judgments when these that affect mirroring plays a critical role in
call for a direct focus on mental interiors. self-development and development of mentaliz-
King-Casas and colleagues (2008) asked BPD ing capacity.
patients to engage in a neuroeconomic social Congruent with these assumptions, there is
exchange task and found that they had difficul- evidence that the quality of the caregiver’s mir-
ties in anticipating the likely impact of their ac- roring in the first 6 months may influence the
tions on a partner player they never met before. child’s capacity to regulate affect, as indicated
Results showed that they were poor at “coax- by the strange situation (Gergely, Koós, & Wat-
ing” their partner (i.e., generating a good feel- son, 2002; Koós & Gergely, 2001). However,
ing in them about the collaboration). By con- it is equally likely that temperamental character-
trast, the weight of evidence suggests that istics make certain infants’ affects hard to read
mentalizing tasks that focus attention on exter- and their experience of affect mirroring will be
nal features cause fewer problems for these pa- limited as a consequence. Such gene–environ-
tients, and BPD patients have actually been ment effects could be one way that the known
found to be hypersensitive to facial expressions influence of heredity (Distel et al., 2008; Lyons-
(e.g., Domes et al., 2008; Lynch et al., 2006). Ruth et al., 2007; Ni et al., 2007) is mediated in
These findings give rise to a model for the BPD. It may be of particular significance that
development of affect regulation based on the the polymorphism that appears to mark the in-
“external” versus “internal” attention distinc- fant’s openness to environmental influence
1360 P. Fonagy and P. Luyten

(maternal sensitivity) in developing secure at- Agent–attitude propositions are probably


tachment, the 5-HTTLPR short allele (Barry, subserved by several areas within the prefrontal
Kochanska, & Philibert, 2008), is also a marker cortex, particularly cortical midline structures,
for vulnerability for developing borderline whereas self-affective propositions appear to
and antisocial traits (Lyons-Ruth et al., 2007). be more likely to be processed by a more auto-
However, we have suggested that a major part matic, embodied, and lateralized system includ-
of the vulnerability that evidence suggests was ing parts of the VMPFC (Sabbagh, 2004; Sha-
caused by early neglect (Johnson, Cohen, Chen, may-Tsoory & Aharon-Peretz, 2007; Shamay-
Kasen, & Brook, 2006) is mediated by compro- Tsoory, Aharon-Peretz, & Levkovitz, 2007).
mise of normal affect mirroring in neglectful psy- The latter structure may be particularly impor-
chosocial environments (e.g., O’Connor, 2006). tant as TESS and TOMM representations
The concept of mutually responsive orientation come to be integrated, but it is currently not
advanced by Kochanska (1997) seems to us the known how these two systems might function
best depiction of the qualities of the “cooperative together to generate what we experience as em-
interpersonal set” that ultimately generate the pathy. If there are two systems, a more basic
child’s mental interior, which in turn normally “emotional contagion” system associated with
generates self-regulation and conscience. The de- the inferior prefrontal gyrus and a more advanced
velopmental experience of borderline children cognitive perspective-taking system associated
may fall short for reasons of constitution, neglect with the prefrontal cortex as a whole (Shamay-
and emotional abuse, or an interaction of these Tsoory, Aharon-Peretz, & Perry, 2009), then
factors. we might also expect that dysfunction in one sys-
tem might lead to overcompensation in the other.
In relation to emergent BPD, we might expect to
Cognitive versus affective mentalization
find an overly influential TESS system overcom-
The most commonly made developmental and pensating for a dysfunctional TOMM (VMPFC)
neuroscientific distinction in relation to mentali- system. This might yield characteristics such as
zation is between cognitive and affective content. susceptibility to emotional contagion, oversensi-
Baron-Cohen, Golan, Chakrabarti, and Belmonte tivity to some emotional cues, a predisposition to
(2008) identified two independent processing become overwhelmed by affect, and inability to
systems that might normally be charged with integrate affective knowledge with more reflec-
parsing these categories of content: (a) the theory tive and cognitive knowledge in relation to
of mind mechanism (TOMM) mediates agent– both other and self (Blatt, 2008). This is likely
attitude–proposition (or M-representations) such to have a particular impact on the individual’s ca-
as “Mother–believes Johnny–took the cookies,” pacity for genuine empathy if (as is usually the
and (b) the empathizing system (TESS) that uses case) the actual match of attitudes between self
self-affective state-proposition (E-representa- and other is imperfect. The constraint Baron-Co-
tions) such as “I am sorry–you feel hurt– hen suggested for the TESS system (requiring
by what I said.” The latter are appropriately la- consistency with the self-affective state) is likely
beled self-affective propositions because TESS often to lead to a state of affairs when the emo-
is normally constrained always to create repre- tional state of the self is inappropriately extended
sentations where emotion in the other is consis- to the other.
tent with the self-affective state. Thus, it will not
create “I am pleased that you are in pain” because
Mentalization with regard
it has to be a state that the self can generate in re-
to self versus others
lation to the presumed state in the other. This con-
straint may not be present in psychopaths (Blair, Under this heading we are less concerned with a
2008). Emotion understanding and belief–desire dichotomy. Rather, we consider the implica-
reasoning normally interact and only in combina- tions of the surprising commonality between
tion generate genuine social understanding in the developmental and brain processes under-
what we have termed mentalized affectivity or pinning mentalization of self and other. Neuro-
“the feeling of feeling” (Fonagy et al., 2002). imaging studies have consistently supported the
Understanding and treatment of borderline personality disorder 1361

assumption that envisioning the mind of an- observation priming actions while noncorre-
other is underpinned by the same brain systems sponding observation interferes with movement
as identify one’s own thoughts and feelings execution (e.g., Kilner, Paulignan, & Blake-
(Dimaggio, Lysaker, Carcione, Nicolo, & Se- more, 2003). Such conflations often appear to
merari, 2008; Lieberman, 2007; Uddin et al., occur in BPD (Allen et al., 2008). Developmen-
2007). The common circuitry used in mentaliz- tally we have tended to assume a state of ego-
ing self and other may explain the struggle of centrism in children; that is, that they are limited
the normally developing child to acquire a to interpreting the world from their own point of
sense of selfhood and the self-other confusions view (Piaget & Inhelder, 1948/1956). However,
in disorders such as BPD, which may be asso- motor neuron findings suggest the opposite;
ciated with the disruption of these neural sys- that is, that infants have to develop the capacity
tems. In BPD, the capacity to distinguish self to learn what is “me” as distinguished from
and other is severely impaired (e.g., Bender & states of mind assumed to be shared by others.
Skodol, 2007; Blatt & Auerbach, 1988; Fuchs, So how does the child learn to differentiate
2007). We have argued that “identity diffusion” self from others?
can be understood as an absence of a sense of A second, cortical midline system that con-
agency that reflects a more general failure of sists of the mPFC, ACC, and the temporal pa-
mentalization, because the continuity of our rietal junction in the LPAC (Lieberman, 2007;
sense of self is normally ensured via our sense Uddin et al., 2007), appears to play a central
of a connection between intentional mental role in this process. This system is less bodily
states and actions (Fonagy, 1991). based, and processes information about the
Reviews of the neuroimaging literature sug- self and others in more abstract and symbolic
gest that two distinct neural networks are shared ways (Frith, 2007; Uddin et al., 2007). Impor-
by self-knowing and knowing others (Lieber- tantly, unlike the frontoparietal system, it ap-
man, 2007; Uddin et al., 2007). The first system pears to be mainly shaped across development
involves a more body-based, frontoparietal mir- by interpersonal relationships.
ror neuron system that is involved in under- Recently the work of Marcel Brass and col-
standing the multimodal embodied self (e.g., leagues has offered a suggestion about how
face and body recognition) and understanding these two apparently independent systems,
others through motor-simulation mechanisms both of which process representations of both
(Gallese, Keysers, & Rizzolatti, 2004; Rizzo- self and other, may interact to create an experi-
latti & Craighero, 2004). This suggests that a ence of not-me and therefore me (Brass, Derr-
fundamental process that allows us to appreciate fuss, Forstmann, & von Cramon, 2005; Brass
the actions and emotions of others involves the & Haggard, 2008; Brass, Schmitt, Spengler,
activation of the mirror neuron system for actions & Gergely, 2007). If perceiving an intention
and the activation of visceromotor centers for the in another triggers the impulse to perform the
understanding of affect. This is thought to be one same behavior, the question of how automatic
of the key evolutionary mechanisms underpin- imitation can be avoided becomes a central is-
ning social empathy: knowing from the inside sue. Neuroimaging studies indicate that the
how someone else feels. This is an implicit, au- neural regions that are most often recruited in
tomatic system, providing physical other to self the inhibition of imitative behavior are the ante-
and self to other mapping, which is involved in rior frontomedial cortex (aFMC) and the tem-
the immediate understanding (or misunderstand- poroparietal junction (TPJ) area (Brass et al.,
ing) of self and others. 2005), which are cortical areas that are also re-
The sharing of structures not only implies a lated to mentalizing, self-referential processing,
model for acquisition but also highlights the po- and self-agency. The TPJ is involved in per-
tential for conflating the embodied simulation spective taking (e.g., Aichhorn, Perner, Kron-
of another person’s experience with one’s own bichler, Staffen, & Ladurner, 2006), in sense
experience. For instance, studies have shown of agency (e.g., Decety & Grezes, 2006), and
that observing an action has a powerful influ- mentalizing (Frith & Frith, 2006). The aFMC
ence on movement execution, with congruent is involved in mentalizing (Frith & Frith,
1362 P. Fonagy and P. Luyten

2006; Gilbert et al., 2007) and self-referential In summary, as noted earlier, knowing one-
processing (Northoff et al., 2006). In fact, re- self and others are two capacities that are clearly
cent work by Brass’s group, using a within-sub- connected developmentally and in terms of the
ject experimental design with reflective men- brain structures subserving these processes.
talizing and imitation–inhibition tasks, suggests This applies to both symbolic and automatic
that there is a functional relationship between ways of knowing. The shared ontogeny and
the extent an individual can inhibit imitative be- anatomy of these functions means that an inter-
havior and the capacity for belief–desire reason- vention that encourages self-reflection within
ing (Brass et al., 2007). Thus, it appears that the an interpersonal setting is well placed to modify
inhibition of imitative behavior involves cortical even deeply rooted dysfunctional processes.
areas that are also related to mentalizing, self- The overlap in ontogeny and neural structure
referential processing, and determining self- points to the interpersonal origins of the self
agency. We assume that this overlap reflects who experiences itself and other selves as mo-
common underlying processes such as self/other tivated by mental states: a developmental psy-
distinction and decoupling of self and other. chopathology approach that has increasingly
Hence, by reflecting on the frontoparietally strong empirical support (Hobson, 2002; Ro-
identified intent of the other, we can create a chat, 2009). The acute suffering that experi-
distinction between self and other experience ences of separation bring for BPD patients be-
and rapidly decouple the direct activation of comes more understandable if we assume that
corresponding motor representations. The ca- when mentalization fails the withdrawal or
pacity to inhibit imitative behavior may be key physical disappearance of the attachment figure
to enabling us to generate a sense of “me”-ness takes on catastrophic proportions. This is be-
through achieving a “not-other”-ness. In other cause in the absence of the capacity to mental-
words, each time we interpret the actions of an- ize, the individual is exposed to a direct and un-
other, there may be a sequence in which an in- mediated experience of vulnerability to others’
itial imitative matching response with the other reactions, now necessarily fragmented and im-
within a motor neuron self-other system inter- possible to differentiate from phenomenological
acts with the reflective mentalizing self-other self-experience.
system. This interaction inhibits the mirror sys-
tem and reduces the extent of “primary identifi-
Aspects of the Phenomenology of BPD
cation” with the other. To anticipate later con-
siderations concerning the emergence of BPD Thus far we have suggested that the failure of
slightly, we might hypothesize that the failure mentalization in BPD is specific to the explicit,
of medial prefrontal and temporoparietal men- internal, cognitive aspects. This impairs self–
talizing function in the course of development other differentiation, leaving the individual at
might leave the individual with difficulties in risk of being overwhelmed by others’ mental
decoupling their representations of another per- states. We have identified both constitutional
son’s experience from their self-representations. and social influences that might undermine
This would leave patients with BPD vulnerable the normal development of mentalization, par-
to losing a sense of self in interpersonal inter- ticularly the absence of marked mirroring.
change because they cannot adequately inhibit Does this model help us appreciate the subjec-
the alternative state of mind that is imposed on tive experience of individuals with BPD?
them through social contagion. Perhaps, then, We assume that the absence of fully function-
the evident determination to “manipulate and ing mentalization is most evident through the
control” the mind of others that is considered reemergence of prementalizing modes of repre-
so characteristic of BPD patients can be seen senting subjectivity. The clearest of these is the
as a self-preservative defensive reaction, defend- tendency to assume that mental states are direct
ing the integrity of the self within attachment representations of psychical reality, normal in a
contexts. Without such control, they might feel 20-month-old child (Gopnik & Meltzoff, 1997).
excessively vulnerable to losing their sense of The modes of social cognition that are charac-
separateness and individuality. teristic of the ways of thinking of BPD patients
Understanding and treatment of borderline personality disorder 1363

can be understood as prementalistic ways of so- processes. We believe that it is clinically help-
cial reasoning that reemerge with the disappear- ful to view the mental functioning in BPD pa-
ance of controlled mentalizing. Mentalization tients as a reemergence of developmentally ear-
gives way to a kind of “psychic equivalence” lier modes of mental functioning, because it
(Target & Fonagy, 1996), which clinicians often offers us a better perspective on their experi-
consider under the heading of “concreteness of ence of the world. To take just one example,
thought.” What is thought is real. The hypothesis both the young child and, at times, the individ-
that a situation is dangerous (“I am being victim- ual with BPD, can have an overriding sense of
ized” or “Everyone hates me”) demands extreme certainty in relation to their subjective experi-
measures of avoidance because experienced in ence. Considering this developmentally, we as-
the mode of psychic equivalence, even a passing sume that for good evolutionary reasons (see
thought feels real. No alternative perspectives are Csibra & Gergely, 2006) all young children
possible. There is a suspension of the experience consider the things that they are taught to be
of “as if.” Everything, sometimes frighteningly, shared cultural knowledge available to all oth-
appears to be “for real.” This can add drama as ers. The small child assumes that his knowledge
well as risk to interpersonal experience and pa- is knowledge held by all. Toddlers readily as-
tients’ exaggerated reactions are justified by the sume that other children will know facts that
seriousness with which they suddenly experience they themselves have just learned (Birch &
their own and others’ thoughts and feelings. The Bloom, 2003). They will therefore also assume
vividness and bizarreness of subjective experi- that there is nothing unique about their own
ence can appear as “quasipsychotic” symptoms thoughts or feelings (Fonagy et al., 2007). De-
(Zanarini, Gunderson, & Frankenburg, 1990), velopmentally, as they come to realize that not
and is reminiscent of the physically compelling all knowledge is shared by all, a key aspect of
memories associated with posttraumatic stress theory of mind (ToM; Bloom, 2004), children
disorder (PTSD; Morrison, Frame, & Larkin, normally learn the conditions under which
2003). For example, women with BPD not only this assumption should be suspended. Naı̈ve
report higher levels of proneness to shame and realism (Pronin, Gilovich, & Ross, 2004), the
guilt, but they also show greater shame proneness failure to perceive one’s own biases and to see
on implicit tests of self-concepts such as the Im- others as more susceptible to a host of cognitive
plicit Association Test (Rusch et al., 2007). and motivational distortions, and the “curse
Shame is felt as “more real” by these patients of knowledge bias’” (Birch & Bloom, 2004),
than anxious patients or normal controls, and thinking that if one knows something about
hence, the stronger association with self-esteem the world then everyone else knows it too,
and quality of life. The extent to which internal stem from the same developmental source pre-
experiences are experienced as if they are real dating the emergence of the self from this sense
events relates to psychotic features identified in of “oneness.” Developmentally it is not the
this group that have been shown to be mediators overvaluing of private knowledge, but rather
between histories of childhood sexual abuse and the undifferentiated experience of shared
suicidality (Soloff, Feske, & Fabio, 2008). Sim- knowledge that hinders perspective taking prior
ilar findings are also emerging in relation to anx- to the development of the PFC. It is social cen-
iety sensitivity in these patients (Gratz, Tull, & trism rather than egocentrism that remains a
Gunderson, 2008). challenge for the individual with BPD, because
Evidence for the continued influence on their unusually intense mirroring representa-
adults of developmentally earlier modes of tions of the other are not balanced by reflective
thought is available from studies of reasoning processes. It is important for the clinician to be
“errors” (e.g., hindsight bias, “the curse of aware that it is not the overvaluing of their own
knowledge,” “actions speak louder than perspective that can make BPD patients adopt
words”; Birch & Bloom, 2007; Blank, Nestler, an egocentric point of view; rather, their ex-
von Collani, & Fischer, 2008; Wertz & Ger- perience of their perspective feels universal.
man, 2007), which have been used to illuminate They expect other people, including their clini-
the architecture of the belief-desire reasoning cian, to know what they are thinking and
1364 P. Fonagy and P. Luyten

feeling, and to see situations in the same way The teleological mode, finally, refers to a
they do. Thus, thwarting their intentions seems mode of thinking that equates others’ desires
malign or willfully obtuse, rather than the result and feelings with their observable behavior.
of a different point of view, alternative priorities, Developmentally early modes of conceptualiz-
and so forth. This makes such frustrations not ing action solely in terms of that which is ap-
merely hurtful but intolerable and maddening, a parent can come to dominate motivation.
denial of what they believe to be a shared reality. Within this “teleological mode” there is a pri-
Although thoughts can feel too real for macy of the physical and observable. Experi-
the individual who has unreliable access to ence is only felt to be valid when its conse-
thoughts and feelings as second order repre- quences are apparent to all. Affection, for
sentations, they can come to be almost dissoci- example, is only felt to be genuine if accom-
ated to the point of near meaninglessness. The panied by a physical expression (e.g., a touch
young child creates mental models and pretend or caress). For many patients with BPD, one
worlds, but can maintain these only so long as can only be loved if one is also physically
they achieve complete separateness from the touched.
world of physical reality. Although psychic The most socially disruptive feature of bor-
equivalence makes subjective experience too derline cognition is the apparently unstoppable
real, the pretend mode severs its connection tendency to create unacceptable experience
with reality and may even lead to dissociative within the other. Externalization of the split-
experiences. Dissociation, which we have off parts of a disorganized self is desirable for
seen is sometimes a response to stress induced the child with a disorganized attachment but
loss of mentalization (Stiglmayr et al., 2008), is a matter of life and death for the traumatized
may be an extreme indication of such separate- individual who has internalized an abuser as
ness (Ross, 2007; Zanarini et al., 2008). In our part of the self. This is evident from the extreme
view, the chronic sense of emptiness that has levels of dysphoric affect reported by indi-
been found to characterize BPD is closely viduals with BPD, what Zanarini and col-
linked to both dissociation and suicidal ideation leagues (1998) have referred to as “the pain of
(Klonsky, 2008). In these states patients can being borderline.” The unbearable emotional
discuss experiences without contextualizing experience can include feeling abandoned,
them in any kind of physical or material reality. evil, betrayed, like a helpless child, misunder-
This could explain why psychophysiological stood, mistreated, victimized, inferior, mon-
reactivity to abandonment themes can be nor- strous, and can characterize these patients’ ex-
mal in BPD patients prone to dissociation perience on a day-to-day basis in a relatively
(Schmahl et al., 2004). Reality places fewer stable way. Patients can view themselves as per-
limits on the creation of mental representations. manently damaged, or rotten to the core (Zittel
Several studies using Rorschach, the Thematic Conklin & Westen, 2005).
Apperception Test, and other narrative methods The externalization of these internal states are
have provided evidence of hypercomplex repre- widely recognized in the common countertrans-
sentations of mental states of others that are of- ferential reactions of therapists working with
ten seen as malevolent and idiosyncratically borderline patients: anger and hatred, helpless-
elaborated (Stuart et al., 1990; Westen, Lu- ness and worthlessness, fear and worry, resent-
dolph, Lerner, Ruffins, & Wiss, 1990). There ment, and urges to save and rescue the patient
is also neuroimaging evidence that points to hy- (Gabbard & Wilkinson, 1994). Complications
peractive mentalizing (at least in terms of TPJ/ come when therapists identify with the projec-
temporal parietal sulcus activation) in BPD pa- tion and emotionally distance themselves from
tients in response to attachment related stimuli individuals with BPD (Aviram, Brodsky, &
(Buchheim et al., 2006). Attempting psycho- Stanley, 2006). In addition to being unusually
therapy with patients who are in this mode sensitive to rejection and abandonment, BPD pa-
can lead the therapist to lengthy but inconse- tients may react negatively (e.g., by harming
quential discussions of internal states that themselves or withdrawing from treatment)
have no link to genuine experience. when they (mis)perceive such distancing and
Understanding and treatment of borderline personality disorder 1365

rejection. As Aviram et al. (2006) point out, 2008b). It should be noted that not all studies
the stigmatizing attitude, engendered through have found a relationship between attachment
the externalization of an alien part of the self, classification and ToM tasks, and the associa-
can exacerbate the behaviors in the patient tion is somewhat more likely to be observed
that create the stigmatizing attitude. The result for emotion understanding than ToM (Meins
is a self-fulfilling prophecy and a cycle of stig- et al., 2002; Raikes & Thompson, 2006). The
matization to which both patient and therapist pathway connecting the two is unlikely to be
contribute. a direct one although recent evidence suggests
The alternative to projective identification is that the oxytocinergic system that is involved
obtaining relief from experiences of overwhelm- in caregiving behavior (Bartels & Zeki, 2004;
ing and intolerable emotion through the destruc- Champagne, Diorio, Sharma, & Meaney,
tion of the self in a teleological mode, that is, 2001) also enhances social awareness. Intrana-
physically, by self-harm and suicide (Kullgren, sal oxytocin, for instance, has been found to fa-
1988). These and other actions can also serve cilitate social function, including improving so-
to create a terrified alien self in the other (thera- cial memory, memory of facial expressions and
pist, friend, parent) who thus becomes the vehi- identity, enhancing the recognition of mental
cle for what is emotionally unbearable. The states revealed by facial expression, probably
need for this other who “uniquely understands” by causing selective fixation on the eye region
(and thereby suffers) the patient’s dysregulated when viewing faces, and increasing manifesta-
affect, not surprisingly, can become overwhelm- tions of trust (for a review, see Neumann,
ing and an adhesive, addictive pseudoattachment 2008). Oxytocin may link sensitivity to social
to this individual may develop. Indeed, studies of cues, such as infant facial expressions, with
adult attachment patterns of BPD patients repeat- dopamine-associated reinforcement pathways
edly highlight a combination of preoccupied and (Strathearn, Fonagy, Amico, & Montague, in
unresolved attachment associated with BPD press). Secure attachment and mentalization
(Agrawal, Gunderson, Holmes, & Lyons-Ruth, may both be facilitated by aspects of parent-
2004; Fonagy et al., 1996; Nakash-Eisikovits, ing. In particular, a psychological perspective
Dutra, & Westen, 2002). adopted by mothers in relation to their own ac-
tions or in relation to their child, including mater-
nal “mind-mindedness” and “reflective function”
Moderators of Mentalization
as they interact with or describe their infants, is
In this section, we consider the impact of poten- associated with both secure attachment and men-
tial moderators of the robustness of mentaliza- talization (for a review, see Sharp & Fonagy,
tion in relation to etiological factors of BPD 2008a). There is also a somewhat less well-estab-
from a developmental perspective. lished association between childhood adversity,
the potential to develop disorganized attachment,
and delays and problems with mentalization
Childhood attachment environment and
(Cicchetti, Rogosch, Maughan, Toth, & Bruce,
the emergence of mentalization
2003; Frodi & Smetana, 1984; Pears & Fisher,
A relation between attachment in infancy and 2005; Rogosch, Cicchetti, & Aber, 1995; Smith
early social understanding was first reported & Walden, 1999). Mind-mindedness is likely to
by Bretherton, Bates, Benigni, Camaioni, and be one of those parental attributes that is most
Volterra (1979), who found that children who adaptive in moderation. Although evidence on
were securely attached at age 12 months used this issue is still lacking, on the basis of clinical
more protodeclarative pointing at age 11 observations we have proposed that maladaptive
months than other infants. Subsequently, a aspects of parental mentalizing of a child can be
number of studies have reported associations either deficient (concrete and stimulus bound) or
between the quality of children’s primary at- excessive or hypermentalizing (necessarily going
tachment relationship and the passing of stan- beyond the data, often quite distorted and some-
dard ToM tasks somewhat earlier (see Fonagy, times paranoid; Fearon et al., 2006; Williams
Gergely, & Target, 2007b; Sharp & Fonagy, et al., 2006).
1366 P. Fonagy and P. Luyten

As we have already noted, retrospective eval- of suicide (Johnson et al., 2002) and personality
uations have led to recognition that disorgani- dysfunction (Zweig-Frank & Paris, 1991). In
zation of the attachment system is a key aspect one small longitudinal study, early maltreat-
of the psychopathology of BPD (Gunderson & ment and disrupted parent–infant communica-
Lyons-Ruth, 2008). Moreover, two longitu- tion predicted BPD symptoms (Lyons-Ruth
dinal studies following children from infancy et al., 2005), and in a larger study verbal (emo-
to early adulthood have found associations be- tional) abuse and neglect even more than phys-
tween insecure attachment in early childhood ical maltreatment marked out those who went
and BPD symptoms on follow-up (Lyons- on to develop BPD (Johnson et al., 2001,
Ruth, Yellin, Melnick, & Atwood, 2005; Ro- 2002, 2006). In addition, anomalies in parent-
gosch & Cicchetti, 2005; Sroufe, Egeland, ing and anxious attachment have been sug-
Carlson, & Collins, 2005). There is some sug- gested as possible mediating mechanisms be-
gestive evidence that the extent of the delay in tween low socioeconomic status and BPD
emotion-focused mentalization observed in symptoms (Cohen et al., 2008). Early neglect
maltreated children is of developmental signif- may indeed be an underestimated risk factor
icance relevant to BPD. For example, the qual- (Kantojarvi et al., 2008; Watson, Chilton, Fair-
ity of understanding of the possible situational child, & Whewell, 2006), as there is some evi-
determinants of sad and angry emotions at ap- dence from adoption and other studies to sug-
proximately 6 years of age was found to predict gest that early neglect interferes with emotion
social competence at 8 years of age, and the understanding (e.g. Shipman, Edwards, Brown,
experience of physical abuse was found to predict Swisher, & Jennings, 2005) and this plays a role
social isolation at 8 years of age to the extent that in the emergence of emotional difficulties in
it had impacted on emotion understanding, even preschool (Vorria et al., 2006) and even in ado-
controlling for verbal ability (Rogosch et al., lescence (Colvert et al., 2008). We have sug-
1995). Earlier, we discussed how emotion dys- gested that one developmental path to impair-
regulation and social competence problems are ments in mentalizing in BPD is a combination
among the early precursors of borderline PD. of early neglect, which might undermine the in-
Yet, as many patients with BPD do not expe- fant’s developing capacity for affect regulation,
rience sexual or physical abuse (Paris, 2004), with later maltreatment or other environmental
and the majority of abuse victims will not de- circumstances, including adult experience of
velop personality disorder (Binder, McNiel, & verbal, emotional, physical and sexual abuse,
Goldstone, 1996; Horwitz, Widom, McLaugh- that are likely to activate the attachment system
lin, & White, 2001), a model of BPD must be chronically (Fonagy & Bateman, 2008, and see
able to accommodate these findings. In our further discussion below). The finding of ele-
view, it is less the fact of maltreatment than vated PTSD scores among those 8.5-year-old
being in a family environment that discourages children exposed to violence who had been dis-
coherent discourse concerning mental states organized in their attachment with their mothers
that is likely to predispose the child to BPD. at 12 months of age is also consistent with this
In line with our emphasis on the importance suggestion (MacDonald et al., 2008). There is a
of a mirroring relationship for the establishment marked convergence between our formulation
of mentalization, a number of studies support concerning emotional abuse and the compel-
the view that neglect, low parental involvement, ling proposals advanced by Linehan concern-
and emotional maltreatment rather than the pre- ing the assumption of invalidating family envi-
sence of physical and sexual abuse are the ronments of the young person with prodromal
critical predictors of BPD (e.g. Johnson et al., BPD (Linehan, 1993).
2001; Ludolph et al., 1990). Studies that have
examined the family context of childhood
Stress, the attachment system, and
trauma in BPD tend to see the unstable, nonnur-
mentalization
turing family environment as the key social me-
diator of abuse (Bradley, Jenei, & Westen, Full mentalizing is likely to fail to dominate be-
2005) and underinvolvement the best predictor havior in the context of intense emotional
Understanding and treatment of borderline personality disorder 1367

arousal as the fight or flight response comes teractive neural systems with different neuro-
online. Earlier formulations (e.g., Fonagy & chemical substrates regulating specific and
Bateman, 2006b) proposed that, at extreme different aspects of prefrontal, posterior corti-
levels, the activation of the attachment system cal, and subcortical functions.
is associated with a deactivation of the mentali- Two points are critical for understanding im-
zation system along with other emotion-in- pairments in mentalizing in this context. First,
duced cognitive dysfunction. Our current for- owing to what can be construed as this neuro-
mulation emphasizes that in understanding the chemical switch associated with escalating
relation between emotional arousal and mental- levels of emotional stress, patterns of brain
izing, it is essential to go beyond a unitary con- functioning can shift from flexibility to automa-
cept of arousal (Robbins, 1997). Key neuro- ticity, that is, from relatively slow executive
modulators, for example, contribute to different functions mediated by the prefrontal cortex to
forms of arousal: norepinephrine contributes to faster habitual and instinctual behaviors medi-
alerting, vigilance, and controlled attentional ated by posterior cortical (e.g., parietal) and
processing in the face of stress; dopamine ener- subcortical structures (e.g., amygdala, hippo-
gizes approach behavior in response to poten- campus, and striatum). Concomitantly, mental-
tially rewarding incentives; and serotonin mod- izing appears to disappear as self-protective
ulates arousal in the norepinephrine and physical reactions (fight–flight–freeze) come
dopamine systems (Pliszka, 2003). Further- to dominate behavior. This has the presumed
more, the effects of arousal in any of these sys- evolutionary value of promoting immediate
tems vary not only with the extent of transmitter adaptive responses to danger. In situations of
secretion but also with the receptor subtype ac- interpersonal stress, where complex cognitive–
tivated (Arnsten, Mathew, Ubriani, Taylor, & emotional functioning (i.e., mentalizing) may
Li, 1999; Mayes, 2000). Following the model be helpful, however, the loss of mentalization
outlined by Mayes (2000, 2006) we suggest may be, to say the least, a significant incon-
that with increased arousal there is a switch venience. Thus, the degree of arousal generated
from cortical to subcortical systems, from con- by interpersonal situations is critical. More gen-
trolled to automatic mentalizing and subse- erally, there will be situational variations when
quently to nonmentalizing modes (see Fig- social stress triggers the threshold for switching
ure 1). Mayes (2000, 2006) proposed that from executive (mentalizing) to automatic (fight
stress regulation is a differential balance of ex- or flight) responding. We also assume, following
citation and inhibition involving multiple, in- Arnsten and Mayes, that the threshold for

Figure 1. A biobehavioral switch model of the relationship between stress and controlled versus automatic
mentalization (based on Luyten, Mayes, et al., 2009).
1368 P. Fonagy and P. Luyten

switching can be lowered as a result of exposure shown that activation of the attachment system
to early stress and trauma of the kind that has underpinning both caregiving and partner pref-
been documented for BPD patients, as noted ear- erence is associated with the activation of two
lier. specific systems that have consistently been
Second, both situational and more stable within- shown to play an important role in promoting
person variations may play a role in the switch and maintaining maternal behavior: (a) the do-
from more controlled to automatic mentalization. paminergic reward processing system (Cham-
This may also be a domain where genetic as well pagne et al., 2004; Strathearn, Li, Fonagy, &
as developmental influences make themselves Montague, 2008) and (b) the oxytocinergic sys-
felt. In particular, research suggests that in tem (Bartels & Zeki, 2004; Champagne et al.,
BPD normal stress regulation, with a main role 2001; Levine, Zagoory-Sharon, Feldman, &
for cortisol, is disturbed (see Jogems-Kosterman Weller, 2007). The functioning of the oxytocin
et al., 2007). Functional magnetic imaging mechanism provides an obvious way of ac-
(fMRI) studies of BPD patients that manipulated counting for the replicated association between
the background level of stress and/or attachment attachment security and the developmental ac-
system activation (e.g., Minzenberg, Fan, New, quisition of mentalization competencies. Vaso-
Tang, & Siever, 2007) confirm an abnormal pat- pressin may play an analogous role, perhaps es-
tern of frontal deactivation and associated hyper- pecially in males (Caldwell, Lee, Macbeth, &
responsiveness of the limbic system. For exam- Young, 2008; Lim et al., 2004).
ple, Silbersweig and colleagues (2007) reported Based on these findings, there are good rea-
that under conditions of negative emotion and sons to suppose that different attachment histories
behavioral inhibition, BPD patients showed rela- are associated with attachment styles that differ in
tively decreased ventromedial prefrontal activity terms of the associated background level of acti-
(including medial orbitofrontal and subgenual vation of the attachment system, and the point at
anterior cingulated) and increased amygdalar– which the switch from more prefrontal controlled
ventral striatal activity correlating with decreased to more automatic mentalizing occurs (Luyten,
constraint. Findings with implications for the hy- Fonagy, et al., 2009). In particular, neuroimaging
pothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis function have studies support the notion that attachment history
confirmed that BPD patients, at least those with affects the setting of the “switch,” which turns
explicit trauma history, show a reduction in the mentalizing system from planned, controlled
pituitary size (Garner et al., 2007), elevated cere- and organized cognition to automatic processing
brospinal levels of corticotropin releasing hor- with narrowed, poorly sustained attention, and in-
mone (Lee, Geracioti, Kasckow, & Coccaro, creased vigilance for attachment disruptions such
2005), dysfunctions of cortisol responsivity as rejection and abandonment.
(Jogems-Kosterman et al., 2007; Minzenberg For example, Gillath, Bunge, Shaver, Wen-
et al., 2006; Walter et al., 2008), and disturbed delken, and Mikulincer (2005) reported an
dexamethasone suppression test response (Win- fMRI study of women with high and low scores
genfeld et al., 2007). on attachment anxiety and avoidance who were
Although these studies require further repli- asked to think about or stop thinking about
cation in the light of sometimes quite small various relationship scenarios. When thinking
sample sizes, relatively disparate experimental about negative scenarios, women with high
paradigms, and considerable heterogeneity in levels of attachment anxiety showed more acti-
sample selection (e.g., comorbidity with de- vation in the hippocampus (memory retrieval)
pression, childhood abuse, PTSD, and copying and in emotion-related brain regions (the ante-
styles; Fertuck et al., 2006; Kahl et al., 2006), rior temporal pole and the dorsal anterior cingu-
research provides considerable evidence that late), and less activation in the orbitofrontal cor-
the activation and deactivation of the attach- tex. It seems that in contemplating negative
ment system is closely linked to arousal and scenarios those with an anxious attachment style
stress regulation (Heinrichs & Domes, 2008; may underrecruit the brain areas associated with
Lieberman, 2007; Mayes, 2006). Studies on ro- emotion regulation and show enhanced retrieval
dent models and neuroimaging studies have of negative memories. In a prospective study,
Understanding and treatment of borderline personality disorder 1369

Strathearn and colleagues (2008, in press) exam- The finding of reduced “reward” activation
ined whether differences in attachment security in mothers with insecure/dismissing attachment
of 30 first-time mothers assessed using the is also consistent with a recent study of re-
Adult Attachment Interview (AAI; George, sponses to smiling adult faces and positive
Kaplan, & Main, 1985) before the birth of their task feedback (Vrticka, Andersson, Grandjean,
child, were related to brain reward and pe- Sander, & Vuilleumier, 2008). This study re-
ripheral oxytocin response to infant cues. ported a negative correlation between dismiss-
Mothers about 10 months after birth of their ing attachment scores and activation of the ven-
child viewed their own or others’ infants’ smil- tral striatum. This is certainly congruent with
ing and crying faces during fMRI scanning. the typical distant and detached relational style
Mothers with secure attachment showed greater of avoidant individuals, and their tendency to
activation of brain reward regions, including the deny the importance of attachment relation-
ventral striatum, and the oxytocin-associated ships. In both studies, challenges to the attach-
hypothalamus/pituitary region. Peripheral oxy- ment system yielded greater ventral striatal
tocin response during infant contact was also activation in securely attached individuals,
significantly higher in secure mothers, and the whereas in avoidant/dismissing individuals it
size of change from baseline oxytocin levels was associated with a relative downregulation
was positively correlated with brain activation of reward-related activity. Consistent with the
to own infants in both regions. The important claims above about the complementary activa-
differences based on attachment history tion of the attachment and mentalizing systems,
emerged when the mothers viewed their in- in the Vrticka et al. study, avoidant attachment
fants’ sad faces. Securely attached mothers con- was positively related to activation in the mPFC
tinued to show greater activation in reward pro- and the ventral (vACC), areas that have been
cessing regions, whereas insecure/dismissing implicated in controlled social cognition and
mothers, congruent with findings described mentalization, social rejection, and emotion
earlier, showed reward system deactivation suppression. Moreover, Vrticka and colleagues
and insular activation in response to seeing their also reported that secure attachment was posi-
own infant’s sad faces. The insula may be a re- tively related to the activation of the ventral
gion associated with feelings of unfairness, striatum in response to positive reinforcement,
pain, and disgust (see review by Montague & but negatively with activation of the amygdala
Lohrenz, 2007). Mothers with insecure/dismiss- to negative reinforcement. This is in line with
ing attachment histories appeared less able to previous suggestions we have made that secure
downregulate the sad feelings evoked in them attachment requires the simultaneous (paradox-
by their infant’s sad faces, possibly because ical) activation of components that are normally
they felt overwhelmed by sad memories of their reciprocally activated, mentalizing and reward-
own past. These findings suggest that for se- salience associated regions of the brain (Fonagy
curely attached mothers, infant cues (whether & Bateman, 2006b). These observations are
positive or negative in affect) may act as an also congruent with the assumption that secure
important affective signal of “incentive salience” attachment consists of a combination of low
(Berridge, 2007), reinforcing and motivating re- anxiety and low avoidance.
sponsive maternal care. However, insecure Based on these and other findings, we pro-
mothers may experience a negative subjective re- pose that a combination of characteristics
action that would make them reflect (mirror) strongly associated with attachment history
their infant’s sadness without being able to create is likely to determine whether an individual
a symbolic/mentalizing distance between their “switches” in a particular context from more
infant’s and their own states of mind.1 controlled reflective to automatic mentalization
(see Figure 1, and for a detailed discussion, see
Luyten, Fonagy, et al., 2009).
1. The phenomenon of insecure mothers experiencing and
therefore perhaps directly mirroring their infant rather
than in a “marked” (nearly, but clearly not the same) generational transmission of patterns of attachment (van
manner may be one of the mechanisms behind the inter- IJzendoorn & Bakermans-Kranenburg, 1997).
1370 P. Fonagy and P. Luyten

The studies reviewed suggest that the anx- it is therefore possible not only that trauma-
ious–preoccupied attachment strategies that related rapid triggering of fright–flight may ac-
are characteristic of many BPD patients are as- count for the inhibition of mentalization, but
sociated with a lowered threshold for attach- more specifically that the hyperactivation of
ment system activation and, simultaneously, a the attachment system in BPD may be a conse-
lower threshold for controlled mentalization de- quence of maltreatment specifically in an at-
activation (see Figure 2). Thus, more automatic, tachment context. Attachment theorists, in par-
subcortical systems, including the amygdala, ticular Mary Main and Erik Hesse, have
have a low threshold for responding to stress suggested that maltreatment leads to the disor-
in BPD patients. This hypothesis in and of itself ganization of the child’s attachment to the care-
could offer a comprehensive explanation for giver because of the irresolvable internal con-
one of the central dynamic features of BPD pa- flict created by the need for reassurance from
tients, that is, their tendency to form attach- the very person who also (by association per-
ments easily and quickly, often resulting in haps) generates an experience of lack of safety.
many disappointments. This pattern would be The activation of the attachment system by the
because of their low threshold for activation threat of maltreatment is followed by proximity
of the attachment system, and their low thresh- seeking, which drives the child closer to an ex-
old for deactivation of neural systems associ- perience of threat leading to further (hyper)acti-
ated with controlled social cognition, including vation of the attachment system (Hesse, 2008).
neural systems involved in judging the trust- This irresolvable conflict leaves the child
worthiness of others. The vicious interper- with an overwhelming sense of helplessness
sonal cycles that are so characteristic of many and hopelessness, and leads to what has been
BPD patients thus can be understood in considered a hyperactivation of the attachment
terms of excitatory feedback loops leading to system. There can be no resolution of the
increased vigilance for stress-related cues in anx- anxiety, because looking for reassurance and
ious attachment, particularly attachment charac- protection generates more fear through the
terized by high anxiety and high avoidance (mental) proximity of the maltreating figure.
(Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007). These vicious cy- The ready triggering of the attachment system
cles are also related to their hypervigilance con- in BPD may be a residue of trauma history
cerning emotional states in others and their fail- and manifests both as the rapidly accelerated
ure to deactivate a hyperactive TESS system, tempo of intimacy in interpersonal relations
which further feeds into their lack of self-other and vulnerability to the temporary loss of
differentiation, setting up a likely sequence of mentalization.
further failures in understanding their own inter- In contrast, in individuals with secure at-
nal world, that of others, and the relationship be- tachment, the activation of the attachment sys-
tween the two (see Figure 2). tem predictably involves a relaxation of normal
We assume that the arousal of the attachment strategies of interpersonal caution. Congruent
system, beyond more general interpersonal with this assumption, expressions in most lan-
stress induced arousal, leads to a general loss guages associate love with various severe forms
of mentalization. Any trauma arouses the at- of sensory handicap, particularly blindness.
tachment system (seeking for protection) and There is good evidence that intense activation
attachment trauma may do so chronically. In of the neurobehavioral system underpinning at-
seeking proximity to the traumatizing attach- tachment is associated with deactivation of
ment figure as a consequence of trauma, the arousal and affect regulation systems (Luyten,
child may naturally be further traumatized. Mayes, et al., 2009), as well as deactivation of
The prolonged activation of the attachment sys- neurocognitive systems likely to generate inter-
tem may be an additional problem, because the personal suspicion, that is, those involved in
arousal of attachment may have specific inhib- social cognition or mentalization, including
itory consequences for mentalization in addi- the LPFC, mPFC, LPAC, mPAC, mTL, and
tion to that which might be expected as a conse- rACC (Bartels & Zeki, 2000, 2004; Lieberman,
quence of increased emotional arousal. In BPD 2007; Satpute & Lieberman, 2006). Moreover,
1371

Figure 2. A mentalization-based model of BPD.


1372 P. Fonagy and P. Luyten

the neuropeptides oxytocin and vasopressin second set of brain areas observed to be deacti-
play key roles in two aspects of creating attach- vated by the activation of attachment concerns,
ment relationships: (a) by activating the reward/ included the temporal poles, parietotemporal
attachment system (the “push” mechanism in- junction, amygdala, and mesial prefrontal cortex,
volved in attachment), and (b) by deactivating areas consistently linked to explicit and internally
neurobehavioral systems that are involved in focused mentalization including judgments of
mediating social avoidance (the “pull” mecha- social trustworthiness, moral judgments, ToM
nism involved in attachment). For instance, tasks, and attention to one’s own emotions.
oxytocin and vasopressin have been shown to Individuals who use attachment deactiva-
inhibit aversion of both female and male ro- tion strategies, typical of dismissive attached
dents to infant pups, as well as leading to a individuals, are able to keep the neural systems
number of affiliative behaviors, including care- involved in controlled mentalization “on-line”
giving behavior (Insel & Young, 2001). Oxyto- for longer, including neural systems involved
cin also reduces behavioral and neuroendocrine in judging the trustworthiness of other indi-
responses to social stress and seems both to en- viduals (i.e., the “pull mechanism” associated
able animals to overcome their natural avoid- with attachment; Vrticka et al., 2008). The dis-
ance of proximity and to inhibit defensive be- tinction from securely attached individuals is
havior, thereby facilitating approach behavior. clear. Secure individuals are able to keep the
Vasopressin has primarily been implicated in controlled mentalizing system “on line” even
male-typical social behaviors, including ag- in the context of increased stress, which is
gression and pair-bond formation, and mediates less likely to trigger the attachment system,
anxiogenic effects (Heinrichs & Domes, 2008). whereas dismissive individuals, for whom
Thus, in the context of secure attachment, the mild stress is not likely to trigger the attachment
activation of the attachment system will gener- system, may be able to keep mentalization
ate increased experience of reward, increased going until the stress becomes severe, at which
sensitivity, decreased social avoidance, but point the deactivating strategy is likely to fail. If
also the potential for the reward to override securely attached individuals are those who are
lack of trust. able to retain a relatively high activation of pre-
This complex set of associations with social frontal areas in the presence of the activation of
behavior may help us to account for the puz- the dopaminergic mesolimbic pathways (attach-
zling combination of facilitative and inhibitory ment and reward system), then differences in
associations between attachment history and mentalization between securely attached and
social cognition. In two separate imaging stud- avoidantly/dismissively attached individuals may
ies, Bartels and Zeki (2000, 2004), for instance, only show themselves under increasing stress,
reported that the activation of areas mediating and this seems concordant with experimental
maternal and/or romantic attachments appeared studies (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007). Although
simultaneously to suppress brain activity in the threshold of avoidant individuals for switching
several regions mediating different aspects of from controlled to automatic mentalization might
cognitive control and including those associ- be elevated, studies have shown that under in-
ated with making social judgments and mental- creasing levels of stress, these deactivating strate-
izing. The medial prefrontal, inferior parietal, gies tend to fail, leading to a strong reactivation
and medial temporal cortices mainly in the right of feelings of insecurity, heightened reactivation
hemisphere, as well as the posterior cingulate of negative self-representations, and increased
cortex, may be part of the circuitry specialized stress (Mikulincer, Gillath, & Shaver, 2002).
for attention and long-term memory (Cabeza By contrast, a low threshold for the stress-in-
& Nyberg, 2000), perhaps specifically respon- duced activation of the attachment system may
sible for integrating emotion and cognition translate as easy deactivation of the “pull-
(e.g., emotional encoding of episodic memo- mechanism” of attachment, and a low threshold
ries; Maddock, 1999) and subserving mood- for activation of the “push-mechanism.” In ad-
mediated inhibition or enhancement of cog- dition, we hypothesize that, if all other factors
nitive processing (Mayberg et al., 1999). The are constant, the greater an individual’s use of
Understanding and treatment of borderline personality disorder 1373

hyperactivating strategies, the lower will be 2001) cycles of attachment security, which re-
their threshold for the activation of automatic inforce feelings of secure attachment, personal
mentalization and thus the stronger the relation- agency, and affect regulation (“build”), and
ship between stress and a switch to automatic lead one to be pulled into different and more
mentalization will be (see Figure 2; Luyten, adaptive environments (“broaden”; Mikulincer
Fonagy, et al., 2009). Moreover, we predict & Shaver, 2007). Congruent with these as-
that greater use of hyperactivating strategies sumptions, studies on resilience have shown
will also be associated with increased time to that positive attachment experiences are related
recovery of mentalization and that deactivating to resilience in part through relationship recruit-
strategies might be associated with relatively ing, that is, the capacity of resilient individuals
rapid recovery of the capacity for mentalization to become attached to caring others (Hauser,
(Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007), but these predic- Allen, & Golden, 2006). Hence, high levels of
tions remain to be investigated. mentalization and the associated use of se-
However, this model would explain why curity-based attachment strategies when faced
mentalization deficits in BPD are more likely with stress are good candidates to explain the ef-
to be observed when the attachment system is fect of relationship recruiting and resilience in
triggered, such as in studies collecting AAI nar- the face of stress. Attachment hyperactivation
ratives (e.g., Fonagy et al., 1996), and why BPD and deactivation strategies, typically employed
patients who mix deactivating and hyperacti- by anxious–preoccupied and dismissively at-
vating strategies, as is characteristic of disorga- tached individuals respectively, in contrast,
nized attachment, show a tendency for both have been shown to limit the ability to “broaden
hypermentalization and a failure of mentaliza- and build” in the face of stress. Moreover, they
tion. Because attachment deactivating strategies have also been shown to inhibit other behavioral
are typically associated with minimizing and systems that are involved in resilience, such as
avoiding affective contents, BPD patients often exploration, affiliation, and caregiving (Insel &
have a tendency for hypermentalization, that is, Young, 2001; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007; Neu-
continuing attempts to mentalize, but without mann, 2008), which might also partially explain
integrating cognition and affect. At the same these individuals’ difficulties in entering lasting
time, as the use of hyperactivating strategies is relationships (including relationships with men-
associated with a decoupling of controlled men- tal health care professionals) and the intergenera-
talization, this leads to failures of mentalization tional transmission of psychopathology.
as a result of an overreliance on models of so- One major implication of these findings is
cial cognition that antedate full mentalizing that treatment should aim to find the optimal
(Bateman & Fonagy, 2006). Similar conclu- balance between attachment activation and
sions have been drawn from an fMRI study in mentalization (Fonagy & Bateman, 2006b).
BPD patients where TAT cards elicited hyper- Moreover, this implies that treatments that fo-
activation of the anterior cingulate and medial cus on gaining insight into one’s past, and
prefrontal cortices, suggesting an overly sensi- particularly into one’s traumatic past, which
tive switch between emotionally salient and typically involves high levels of stress, might
neutral information processing (Schnell, Die- be contraindicated in patients with serious im-
trich, Schnitker, Daumann, & Herpertz, 2007). pairments in mentalization. As trauma is typi-
Finally, it is important to note that BPD is in cally associated with attachment insecurity,
some ways at the opposite end of the spectrum and anxious and disorganized attachment in
from interpersonal resilience (Gunderson & particular, insight-oriented treatments might
Lyons-Ruth, 2008; Higgitt & Fonagy, 1992) be especially harmful for this subgroup of
and understanding the association between re- BPD patients (Fonagy & Bateman, 2006a).
silience and mentalization (Fonagy & Target, For instance, studies have shown that reflecting
1997) also provides helpful insight into BPD. on oneself and one’s autobiographical past and
Studies suggest that the ability to continue to self-concept, capacities that are typically called
mentalize even under considerable stress leads upon in many forms of insight-oriented psycho-
to so-called “broaden and build” (Fredrickson, therapy, are associated with activation of the
1374 P. Fonagy and P. Luyten

mPFC, which is deactivated under increasing As mentalization is likely to be a function


arousal levels (Lieberman, 2007). loosely coupled with the attachment system
(Fonagy & Target, 1997), it is doubtful that in-
dividual differences in mentalization are best
Relationship-specific mentalization:
thought of as essentially traitlike. Mentalization
Mentalization in context
is likely to show considerable fluctuations over
The association between mentalization, stress, time and across relationship contexts, not just as
and attachment reviewed in the previous section a function of stress but also as a function of the
suggests that we should expect differences in quality of particular relationships. Several sets
the quality of mentalization depending on the of empirical observations are relevant to this as-
quality of the relationship within which the as- sumption. First, general and relationship-spe-
pect of interpersonal cognition is observed (Al- cific mentalization, although overlapping, ap-
len et al., 2008; Luyten, Fonagy, et al., 2009). pear to be distinct (Happé & Frith, 1996;
This means that some revision of previously Humfress, O’Connor, Slaughter, Target, & Fo-
held assumptions is called for. Attachment the- nagy, 2002; O’Connor & Hirsch, 1999), and
ory and research is largely based on the as- there is increasing agreement that key mentali-
sumption that cognitive–affective models of re- zation-linked experiences associated with so-
lationships (internal working models Bowlby, cial relationships, such as trust and fairness,
1980) are relatively stable over time and are ac- are relationship-specific (Bugental & Johnston,
tivated in a wide array of interpersonal relation- 2000; Fiddick & Cummins, 2007). Second,
ships, including relationships with parents, several independent research traditions have fo-
partners, and friends (Fraley, 2007; Overall, cused on mentalization with regard to one’s
Fletcher, & Friesen, 2003). Similarly, mentali- own infant as a relationship-specific form of
zation has often been assumed to be invariant mentalization (for a review, see Sharp & Fon-
across different relationships. For instance, cur- agy, 2008a). Third, and most pertinent in the
rent assessment methods of mentalization, such present context, is the line of evidence suggest-
as the Reflective Functioning Scale as scored ing that quality of mentalization of therapists
on the AAI (Fonagy, Target, Steele, & Steele, may be specific to particular patients and,
1998), Child Attachment Interview (Ensink, even within that, may be variable across ses-
2003), and Parent Development Interview (Slade, sions of psychotherapy (Diamond, Stovall-
2005), involve the aggregation of mentalization McClough, Clarkin, & Levy, 2003; Diamond
across a number of attachment relationships. & Yeomans, 2008; Vermote et al., 2009).
Moreover, because the Reflective Functioning These findings are congruent with neuro-
Scale involves scoring mentalization based on imaging studies suggesting an inverse relation-
states of mind with regard to past attachment ship between amygdala activation and the acti-
experiences, research with this scale makes vation of mentalizing areas (particularly mPFC
the further assumption that mentalization elic- and TPJ) in response to pictures or names of
ited in relation to past relationships will be re- more or less familiar and personally connected
lated to current and even future relationships (Fo- individuals (Bartels & Zeki, 2000, 2004; Gob-
nagy, Steele, Moran, Steele, & Higgitt, 1991). bini & Haxby, 2007; Gobbini, Leibenluft, San-
However, research has called into question tiago, & Haxby, 2004; Ortigue, Bianchi-Demi-
the assumption that working models are trait- cheli, Hamilton, & Grafton, 2007). In line with
like. The substantial within-person variation these and the above observations concerning
in internal working models of others (e.g., fa- the situation and relationship specificity of
ther vs. mother) (e.g., Fraley, 2007; Pierce & mentalization, we suggest that any anomalies
Lydon, 2001) supports a view of internal work- in relation to mentalization are unlikely to be
ing models as hierarchically organized net- manifest in patients with BPD unless the rela-
works that contain both global and relation- tionship in which mentalization is being ob-
ship-specific representations (Fonagy, 2001; served “pulls” for the activation of these areas.
Overall et al., 2003) or as distributed processes The stronger the attachment in a particular rela-
within a connectionistic network (Fraley, 2007). tionship at a particular moment, the more likely
Understanding and treatment of borderline personality disorder 1375

that anomalies in mentalization will emerge in Conclusions


these patients. Certainly, clinical evidence
strongly implies that as the attachment bond be- Although there is increasing consensus regard-
tween therapist and client intensifies, the qual- ing the core features of BPD, there is much dis-
ity of BPD patients’ mentalization will tend to agreement over the features that should take pre-
deteriorate. Thus, initial assessment of clients dominance in explaining the etiopathogenesis of
can leave therapists with the impression that BPD. In this paper we suggest that the core fea-
they are working with an individual with rela- tures of BPD reflect impairments in different
tively high psychological mindedness and some- facets of mentalization, each related to impair-
one highly suitable for insight oriented psycho- ments in relatively distinct neural circuits under-
therapy. Typically, as treatment progresses and lying these facets. Hence, we aim to provide a
transference intensifies, activating the patient’s comprehensive account of the core features of
internal working models of particular child–par- BPD at multiple levels of analysis by showing
ent relationships and their attachment system in how these core features are related with each
general, the quality of psychological mindedness other in theoretically meaningful ways. More
is likely to deteriorate significantly and the pa- specifically, we argue that BPD is associated
tient’s capacity to perceive the therapist’s mind with low thresholds for activation of the attach-
as different from his or her own mental state ment system under stress, which, in combination
will be quite limited at times (Allen et al., 2008). with low thresholds for deactivation of the ca-
The perspective we are taking also implies that pacity for controlled mentalization, particularly
in both research and clinical practice, assessment with regard to differences in mental states of
of mentalization of patients with BPD will be po- self versus others, renders the interpersonal
tentially unhelpful without regard to context, or world incomprehensible and leads to a cascade
based on specific categories of relationships. In- of impairments in other types of mentalization.
terviews such as the AAI and the recently de- This theory offers a framework for understand-
veloped Child Attachment Interview (Shmueli- ing BPD patients’ propensity to become in-
Goetz, Target, Fonagy, & Datta, 2008), as well volved in vicious interpersonal cycles, their
as the Object Relations Inventory (Diamond, marked affective dysregulation, and high levels
Blatt, Stayner, & Kaslow, 1991) allow scoring of impulsivity, as well as identity diffusion, dis-
for mentalization with regard to self and others, sociative experiences, and profound feelings of
as well as mentalization in specific relationships, inner pain. Our formulations translate into a co-
as they elicit narratives about different significant herent treatment approach, which may also in-
others, and therefore provide a promising avenue form treatment of BPD across various theoretical
for further research in this area. orientations.

References
Agrawal, H. R., Gunderson, J., Holmes, B. M., & Lyons- Aviram, R. B., Brodsky, B. S., & Stanley, B. (2006). Border-
Ruth, K. (2004). Attachment studies with borderline line personality disorder, stigma, and treatment implica-
patients: A review. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 12, tions. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 14, 249–256.
94–104. Barnow, S., Ruge, J., Spitzer, C., & Freyberger, H. J.
Aichhorn, M., Perner, J., Kronbichler, M., Staffen, W., & (2005). Temperament and character in persons with bor-
Ladurner, G. (2006). Do visual perspective tasks need derline personality disorder. Nervenarzt, 76, 838–846.
theory of mind? NeuroImage, 30, 1059–1068. Baron-Cohen, S., Golan, O., Chakrabarti, B., & Belmonte,
Allen, J., Fonagy, P., & Bateman, A. (2008). Mentalizing in M. K. (2008). Social cognition and autism spectrum con-
clinical practice. Washington, DC: American Psychiat- ditions. In C. Sharp, P. Fonagy, & I. Goodyer (Eds.),
ric Press. Social cognition and developmental psychopathology.
Arnsten, A. F. T., Mathew, R., Ubriani, R., Taylor, J. R., & Li, Oxford: Oxford University Press.
B.-M. (1999). Alpha-1 noradrenergic receptor stimulation Barry, R. A., Kochanska, G., & Philibert, R. A. (2008).
impairs prefrontal corical cognitive function. Biological G  E interaction in the organization of attachment:
Psychiatry, 45, 26–31. Mothers’ responsiveness as a moderator of children’s
Arntz, A., Bernstein, D., Oorschot, M., Robson, K., & genotypes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psy-
Schobre, P. (2006). Theory of mind in borderline and chiatry, 49, 1313–1320.
Cluster-C personality disorder: No evidence for defi- Bartels, A., & Zeki, S. (2000). The neural basis of romantic
cits. Unpublished manuscript, Maastricht University. love. NeuroReport, 11, 3829–3834.
1376 P. Fonagy and P. Luyten

Bartels, A., & Zeki, S. (2004). The neural correlates of ma- Brass, M., & Haggard, P. (2008). The what, when, whether
ternal and romantic love. NeuroImage, 21, 1155–1166. model of intentional action. Neuroscientist, 14, 319–325.
Bateman, A. W., & Fonagy, P. (2004). Psychotherapy for Brass, M., Schmitt, R. M., Spengler, S., & Gergely, G.
borderline personality disorder: Mentalization based (2007). Investigating action understanding: Inferential
treatment. Oxford: Oxford University Press. processes versus action simulation. Current Biology,
Bateman, A. W., & Fonagy, P. (2006). Mentalization based 17, 2117–2121.
treatment for borderline personality disorder: A practi- Bretherton, I., Bates, E., Benigni, L., Camaioni, L., & Vol-
cal guide. Oxford: Oxford University Press. terra, V. (1979). Relationships between cognition, com-
Bender, D. S., & Skodol, A. E. (2007). Borderline person- munication, and quality of attachment. In E. Bates, L.
ality as a self-other representational disturbance. Jour- Benigni, I. Bretherton, L. Camaioni, & V. Volterra
nal of Personality Disorders, 21, 500–517. (Eds.), The emergence of symbols: Cognition and com-
Berridge, K. C. (2007). The debate over dopamine’s role in munication in infancy (pp. 223–269). New York: Aca-
reward: The case for incentive salience. Psychopharma- demic Press.
cology (Berlin), 191, 391–431. Buchheim, A., Erk, S., George, C., Kachele, H., Ruchsow,
Binder, R. L., McNiel, D. E., & Goldstone, R. L. (1996). Is M., Spitzer, M., et al. (2006). Measuring attachment
adaptive coping possible for adult survivors of child- representation in an fMRI environment: A pilot study.
hood sexual abuse? Psychiatric Services, 47, 186–188 Psychopathology, 39, 144–152.
Birch, S. A. J., & Bloom, P. (2003). Children are cursed: An Bugental, D. B., & Johnston, C. (2000). Parental and child
asymmetric bias in mental-state attribution. Psycholog- cognitions in the context of the family. Annual Review
ical Science, 14, 283–286. of Psychology, 51, 315–344.
Birch, S. A. J., & Bloom, P. (2004). Understanding chil- Cabeza, R., & Nyberg, L. (2000). Neural bases of learning
dren’s and adults’ limitations in mental state reasoning. and memory: Functional neuroimaging evidence. Cur-
Trends in Cognitive Science, 8, 255–260. rent Opinion in Neurology, 13, 415–421.
Birch, S. A. J., & Bloom, P. (2007). The curse of knowledge Caldwell, H. K., Lee, H. J., Macbeth, A. H., & Young., W. S.
in reasoning about false beliefs. Psychological Science, 3rd. (2008). Vasopressin: Behavioral roles of an “origi-
18, 382–386. nal” neuropeptide. Progress in Neurobiology, 84, 1–24.
Black, D. W., Blum, N., Pfohl, B., & Hale, N. (2004). Sui- Carpendale, J. I. M., & Lewis, C. (2006). How children de-
cidal behavior in borderline personality disorder: Prev- velop social understanding (Understanding children’s
alence, risk factors, prediction, and prevention. Journal worlds). London: Blackwell.
of Personality Disorders, 18, 226–239. Chabrol, H., & Leichsenring, F. (2006). Borderline person-
Black, D. W., Gunter, T., Allen, J., Blum, N., Arndt, S., ality organization and psychopathic traits in nonclinical
Wenman, G., et al. (2007). Borderline personality disor- adolescents: Relationships of identity diffusion, primi-
der in male and female offenders newly committed tive defense mechanisms and reality testing with cal-
to prison. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 48, 400–405. lousness and impulsivity traits. Bulletin of the Mennin-
Blair, J. (2008). Empathic dysfunction in psychopathy. In ger Clinic, 70, 160–170.
C. Sharp, P. Fonagy, & I. Goodyer (Eds.), Social cogni- Champagne, F., Diorio, J., Sharma, S., & Meaney, M. J.
tion and developmental psychopathology. Oxford: Ox- (2001). Naturally occurring variations in maternal be-
ford University Press. havior in the rat are associated with differences in estro-
Blank, H., Nestler, S., von Collani, G., & Fischer, V. gen-inducible central oxytocin receptors. Proceedings
(2008). How many hindsight biases are there? Cogni- of the National Academy of Sciences of the United
tion, 106, 1408–1440. States of America, A 98, 12736–12741.
Blatt, S. J. (2008). Polarities of experience: Relatedness Champagne, F. A., Chretien, P., Stevenson, C. W., Zhang,
and self definiton in personality depvelopment, psycho- T. Y., Gratton, A., & Meaney, M. J. (2004). Variations
pathology, and the therapeutic process. Washington, in nucleus accumbens dopamine associated with indi-
DC: American Psychological Association. vidual differences in maternal behavior in the rat. Jour-
Blatt, S. J., & Auerbach, J. S. (1988). Differential cogntive nal of Neuroscience, 24, 4113–4123.
disturbances in three types of borderline patients. Jour- Chanen, A. M., Jovev, M., & Jackson, H. J. (2007). Adap-
nal of Personality Disorder, 2, 198–211. tive functioning and psychiatric symptoms in adoles-
Blatt, S. J., & Luyten, P. (in press). A structural–develop- cents with borderline personality disorder. Journal of
mental psychodynamic approach to psychopathology: Clinical Psychiatry, 68, 297–306.
Two polarities of experience across the life span. Devel- Choi-Kain, L. W., & Gunderson, J. G. (2008). Mentaliza-
opment and Psychopathology. tion: Ontogeny, assessment, and application in the
Bloom, P. (2004). Descartes’ baby. New York: Basic Books. treatment of borderline personality disorder. American
Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and Loss: Vol. 3. Loss: Sad- Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 1127–1135.
ness and depression. London: Hogarth Press and Insti- Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., Maughan, A., Toth, S. L., &
tute of Psycho-Analysis. Bruce, J. (2003). False belief understanding in mal-
Bradley, R., Jenei, J., & Westen, D. (2005). Etiology of bor- treated children. Development and Psychopathology,
derline personality disorder: Disentangling the contri- 15, 1067–1091.
butions of intercorrelated antecedents. Journal of Ner- Cohen, P. (2008). Child development and personality disor-
vous and Mental Disease, 193, 24–31. der. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 31, 477–493.
Bradley, R., & Westen, D. (2005). The psychodynamics of Cohen, P., Chen, H., Gordon, K., Johnson, J., Brook, J., &
borderline personality disorder: A view from develop- Kasen, S. (2008). Socioeconomic background and the
mental psychopathology. Development and Psychopa- developmental course of schizotypal and borderline
thology, 17, 927–957. personality disorder symptoms. Development and Psy-
Brass, M., Derrfuss, J., Forstmann, B., & von Cramon, D. chopathology, 20, 633–650.
Y. (2005). The role of the inferior frontal junction Coid, J. W. (1993). An affective syndrome in psychopaths
area in cognitive control. Trends in Cognitive Science, with borderline personality disorder? British Journal
9, 314–316. of Psychiatry, 162, 641–650.
Understanding and treatment of borderline personality disorder 1377

Colvert, E., Rutter, M., Beckett, C., Castle, J., Groothues, C., pact of borderline personality disorder and anxiety on
Hawkins, A., et al. (2008). Emotional difficulties in neuropsychological performance in major depression.
early adolescence following severe early deprivation: Journal of Personality Disorders, 20, 55–70.
Findings from the English and Romanian adoptees study. Fiddick, L., & Cummins, D. (2007). Are perceptions of fair-
Development and Psychopathology, 20, 547–567. ness relationship-specific? The case of noblesse oblige.
Crick, N. R., Murray-Close, D., & Woods, K. (2005). Bor- Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (Col-
derline personality features in childhood: A short-term chester), 60, 16–31.
longitudinal study. Development and Psychopathology, Fonagy, P. (1991). Thinking about thinking: Some clinical
17, 1051–1070. and theoretical considerations in the treatment of a bor-
Critchfield, K. L., Levy, K. N., Clarkin, J. F., & Kernberg, derline patient. International Journal of Psycho-Analy-
O. F. (2008). The relational context of aggression in sis, 72, 1–18.
borderline personality disorder: Using adult attachment Fonagy, P. (2001). Attachment theory and psychoanalysis.
style to predict forms of hostility. Journal of Clinical New York: Other Press.
Psychology, 64, 67–82. Fonagy, P., & Bateman, A. W. (2006a). Progress in the
Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2006). Social learning and social treatment of borderline personality disorder. British
cognition: The case for pedagogy. In M. H. Johnson & Y. Journal of Psychiatry, 188, 1–3.
M. Munakata (Eds.), Processes of change in brain and Fonagy, P., & Bateman, A. W. (2008). The development of
cognitive developement. Attention and performance borderline personality disorder—A mentalizing model.
(Vol. 21, pp. 249–274). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Journal of Personality Disorders, 22, 4–21.
De Clercq, B., De Fruyt, F., Van Leeuwen, K., & Mer- Fonagy, P., & Bateman, A. W. (2006b). Mechanisms of
vielde, I. (2006). The structure of maladaptive personal- change in mentalization-based treatment of BPD. Jour-
ity traits in childhood: A step toward an integrative devel- nal of Clinical Psychology, 62, 411–430.
opmental perspective for DSM-V. Journal of Abnormal Fonagy, P., Gergely, G., Jurist, E., & Target, M. (2002). Af-
Psychology, 115, 639–657. fect regulation, mentalization and the development of
Decety, J., & Grezes, J. (2006). The power of simulation: the self. New York: Other Press.
imagining one’s own and other’s behavior. Brain Re- Fonagy, P., Gergely, G., & Target, M. (2007). The parent–in-
search, 1079, 4–14. fant dyad and the construction of the subjective self. Jour-
Deschamps, P. K., & Vreugdenhil, C. (2008). Stability of nal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 288–328.
borderline personality disorder from childhood to adult- Fonagy, P., Leigh, T., Steele, M., Steele, H., Kennedy, R., Mat-
hood: A literature review. Tijdschrift voor Psychiatrie, toon, G., et al. (1996). The relation of attachment status,
50, 33–41. psychiatric classification, and response to psychotherapy.
Diamond, A., Blatt, S. J., Stayner, D., & Kaslow, N. (1991). Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 22–31.
Self–other differentiation of object representations. Un- Fonagy, P., Steele, H., Moran, G., Steele, M., & Higgitt, A.
published manuscript, Yale University. (1991). The capacity for understanding mental states:
Diamond, D., Stovall-McClough, C., Clarkin, J. F., & Levy, The reflective self in parent and child and its signifi-
K. N. (2003). Patient–therapist attachment in the treat- cance for security of attachment. Infant Mental Health
ment of borderline personality disorder. Bulletin of Journal, 13, 200–217.
the Menninger Clinic, 67, 227–259. Fonagy, P., & Target, M. (1997). Attachment and reflective
Diamond, D., & Yeomans, F. (2008). The patient–therapist function: Their role in self-organization. Development
relationship: Implications of attachment theory, reflec- and Psychopathology, 9, 679–700.
tive functioning, and research. Santé Mentale au Qué- Fonagy, P., & Target, M. (2006). The mentalization-fo-
bec, 33, 61–87. cused approach to self pathology. Journal of Personal-
Dimaggio, G., Lysaker, P. H., Carcione, A., Nicolo, G., & ity Disorders, 20, 544–576.
Semerari, A. (2008). Know yourself and you shall Fonagy, P., Target, M., Steele, H., & Steele, M. (1998). Re-
know the other . . . to a certain extent: Multiple paths flective-functioning manual, version 5.0, for application
of influence of self-reflection on mindreading. Con- to Adult Attachment Interviews. London: University Col-
scious Cognition, 17, 778–789. lege London.
Distel, M. A., Trull, T. J., Derom, C. A., Thiery, E. W., Fraley, R. C. (2007). A connectionist approach to the orga-
Grimmer, M. A., Martin, N. G., et al. (2008). Heritabil- nization and continuity of working models of attach-
ity of borderline personality disorder features is similar ment. Journal of Personality, 75, 1157–1180.
across three countries. Psychological Medicine, 38, Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in
1219–1229. positive psychology: The broaden-and-build theory of
Domes, G., Czieschnek, D., Weidler, F., Berger, C., Fast, positive emotions. American Psychologist, 56, 218–226.
K., & Herpertz, S. C. (2008). Recognition of facial af- Frith, C. D. (2007). The social brain? Philosophical
fect in Borderline Personality Disorder. Journal of Transactions of the Royal Society of London B Biolo-
Personality Disorders, 22, 135–147. gical Science, 362, 671–678.
Ensink, K. (2003). Assessing theory of mind, affective un- Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (2006). The neural basis of menta-
derstanding and reflective functioning in primary lizing. Neuron, 50, 531–534.
school aged children. Unpublished PhD dissertation, Frodi, A., & Smetana, J. (1984). Abused, neglected, and
University of London. nonmaltreated preschoolers’ ability to discriminate
Fearon, P., Target, M., Fonagy, P., Williams, L., McGregor, emotions in others: The effects of IQ. Child Abuse &
J., Sargent, J., et al. (2006). Short-term mentalization Neglect, 8, 459–465.
and relational therapy (SMART): An integrative family Fuchs, T. (2007). Fragmented selves: Temporality and iden-
therapy for children and adolescents. In J. Allen & tity in borderline personality disorder. Psychopathol-
P. Fonagy (Eds.), Handbook of mentalisaiton based ogy, 40, 379–387.
treatments. London: Wiley. Gabbard, G. O., & Wilkinson, S. M. (1994). Management
Fertuck, E. A., Marsano-Jozefowicz, S., Stanley, B., Tryon, of contertransference with borderline patients. Wash-
W. W., Oquendo, M., Mann, J. J., et al. (2006). The im- ington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
1378 P. Fonagy and P. Luyten

Gallese, V., Keysers, C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2004). A unify- Hauser, S. T., Allen, J. P., & Golden, E. (2006). Out of
ing view of the basis of social cognition. Trends in Cog- the woods: Tales of resilient teens. Cambridge, MA:
nitive Science, 8, 396–403. Harvard University Press.
Garner, B., Chanen, A. M., Phillips, L., Velakoulis, D., Heinrichs, M., & Domes, G. (2008). Neuropeptides and so-
Wood, S. J., Jackson, H. J., et al. (2007). Pituitary vol- cial behaviour: Effects of oxytocin and vasopressin in
ume in teenagers with first-presentation borderline per- humans. Progress in Brain Research, 170, 337–350.
sonality disorder. Psychiatry Research, 156, 257–261. Hesse, E. (2008). The Adult Attachment Interview: Proto-
George, C., Kaplan, N., & Main, M. (1985). The Adult At- col, method of analysis, and empirical studies. In J. Cas-
tachment Interview. Unpublished manuscript, Univer- sidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment
sity of California, Berkeley. theory and research (2nd ed., pp. 552–558). New York:
Gergely, G. (2004). The role of contingency detection in Guilford Press.
early affect–regulative interactions and in the develop- Higgitt, A., & Fonagy, P. (1992). The psychotherapeutic
ment of different types of infant attachment. Social treatment of borderline and narcissistic personality dis-
Behavior, 13, 468–478. order. British Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 23–43.
Gergely, G., Bekkering, H., & Kiraly, I. (2002). Rational Hill, J., Pilkonis, P., Morse, J., Feske, U., Reynolds, S.,
imitation in preverbal infants. Nature, 415, 755. Hope, H., et al. (2008). Social domain dysfunction
Gergely, G., Koós, O., & Watson, J. S. (2002). Perception and disorganization in borderline personality disorder.
causale et role des comportements imitatifs des parents Psychological Medicine, 38, 135–146.
dans le développement socio-émotionnel précoce [Con- Hobson, P. (2002). The cradle of thought: Explorations of
tingency perception and the role of contingent parental the origins of thinking. Oxford: Macmillan.
reactivity in early socio-emotional development]. In Holm, A. L., & Severinsson, E. (2008). The emotional
J. Nadel & J. Decety (Eds.), Imiter pour découvrir l’hu- pain and distress of borderline personality disorder: A
man: Psychologie, neurobiology, robotique et philoso- review of the literature. International Journal of Mental
phie de l’esprit (pp. 59–82). Paris: Presses Universi- Health Nursing, 17, 27–35.
taires de France. Holmes, J. (2005). Notes on mentalization—Old hat or new
Gergely, G., & Watson, J. (1996). The social biofeedback wine? British Journal of Psychotherapy, 19, 690–710.
model of parental affect-mirroring. International Jour- Horwitz, A. V., Widom, C. S., McLaughlin, J., & White, H.
nal of Psycho-Analysis, 77, 1181–1212. R. (2001). The impact of childhood abuse and neglect
Gilbert, S. J., Williamson, I. D., Dumontheil, I., Simons, J. on adult mental health: Prospective study. Journal of
S., Frith, C. D., & Burgess, P. W. (2007). Distinct re- Health and Social Behaviour, 42, 184–201.
gions of medial rostral prefrontal cortex supporting Humfress, H., O’Connor, T. G., Slaughter, J., Target, M., &
social and nonsocial functions. Social Cognitive and Fonagy, P. (2002). General and relationship-specific
Affective Neuroscience, 2, 217–226. models of social cognition: Explaining the overlap
Gillath, O., Bunge, S. A., Shaver, P. R., Wendelken, C., & and discrepancies. Journal of Child Psychology and
Mikulincer, M. (2005). Attachment-style differences in Psychiatry, 43, 873–883.
the ability to suppress negative thoughts: Exploring the Insel, T., & Young, L. (2001). The neurobiology of attach-
neural correlates. NeuroImage, 28, 835–847. ment. Nature Review Neuroscience, 2, 129–136.
Gobbini, M. I., & Haxby, J. V. (2007). Neural systems for Jogems-Kosterman, B. J., de Knijff, D. W., Kusters, R., &
recognition of familiar faces. Neuropsychologia, 45, van Hoof, J. J. (2007). Basal cortisol and DHEA levels
32–41. in women with borderline personality disorder. Journal
Gobbini, M. I., Leibenluft, E., Santiago, N., & Haxby, J. V. of Psychiatric Research, 41, 1019–1026.
(2004). Social and emotional attachment in the neural Johnson, J. G., Cohen, P., Chen, H., Kasen, S., & Brook,
representation of faces. NeuroImage, 22, 1628–1635. J. S. (2006). Parenting behaviors associated with risk
Gopnik, A., & Meltzoff, A. (1997). Words, thoughts, and for offspring personality disorder during adulthood.
theories. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63, 579–587.
Grant, B. F., Chou, S. P., Goldstein, R. B., Huang, B., Stin- Johnson, J. G., Cohen, P., Gould, M. S., Kasen, S., Brown,
son, F. S., Saha, T. D., et al. (2008). Prevalence, corre- J., & Brook, J. S. (2002). Childhood adversities, inter-
lates, disability, and comorbidity of DSM-IV borderline personal difficulties, and risk for suicide attempts dur-
personality disorder: results from the Wave 2 National ing late adolescence and early adulthood. Archives of
Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Condi- General Psychiatry, 59, 741–749.
tions. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 69, 533–545. Johnson, J. G., Cohen, P., Smailes, E., Skodol, A., Brown,
Gratz, K. L., Tull, M. T., & Gunderson, J. G. (2008). Pre- J., & Oldham, J. (2001). Childhood verbal abuse
liminary data on the relationship between anxiety sensi- and risk for personality disorders during adolescence
tivity and borderline personality disorder: The role of and early adulthood. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 42,
experiential avoidance. Journal of Psychiatric Re- 16–23.
search, 42, 550–559. Kahl, K. G., Bens, S., Ziegler, K., Rudolf, S., Dibbelt, L.,
Grootens, K. P., van Luijtelaar, G., Buitelaar, J. K., van der Kordon, A., et al. (2006). Cortisol, the cortisol–dehy-
Laan, A., Hummelen, J. W., & Verkes, R. J. (2008). In- droepiandrosterone ratio, and pro-inflammatory cyto-
hibition errors in borderline personality disorder with kines in patients with current major depressive disorder
psychotic-like symptoms. Progress in Neuropsycho- comorbid with borderline personality disorder. Biolog-
pharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 32, 267–273. ical Psychiatry, 59, 667–671.
Gunderson, J. G., & Lyons-Ruth, K. (2008). BPD’s inter- Kantojarvi, L., Joukamaa, M., Miettunen, J., Laksy, K.,
personal hypersensitivity phenotype: gene–environ- Herva, A., Karvonen, J. T., et al. (2008). Childhood
ment–developmental model. Journal of Personality family structure and personality disorders in adulthood.
Disorders, 22, 22–41. European Psychiatry, 23, 205–211.
Happé, F., & Frith, C. D. (1996). Theory of mind and social Kilner, J. M., Paulignan, Y., & Blakemore, S. J. (2003). An
impairment in children with conduct disorder. British interference effect of observed biological movement on
Journal of Developmental Psychology, 14, 385–398. action. Current Biology, 13, 522–525.
Understanding and treatment of borderline personality disorder 1379

King-Casas, B., Sharp, C., Lomax-Bream, L., Lohrenz, T., states: Hostile/helpless states of mind on the Adult At-
Fonagy, P., & Montague, P. R. (2008). The rupture and tachment Interview are associated with disrupted
repair of cooperation in borderline personality disorder. mother–infant communication and infant disorganiza-
Science, 321, 806–810. tion. Development and Psychopathology, 17, 1–23.
Klonsky, E. D. (2008). What is emptiness? Clarifying the MacDonald, H. Z., Beeghly, M., Grant-Knight, W., Augus-
7th criterion for borderline personality disorder. Jour- tyn, M., Woods, R. W., Cabral, H., et al. (2008). Longi-
nal of Personality Disorders, 22, 418–426. tudinal association between infant disorganized attach-
Kochanska, G. (1997). Mutually responsive orientation be- ment and childhood posttraumatic stress symptoms.
tween mothers and their young children: Implications Development and Psychopathology, 20, 493–508.
for early socialization. Child Development, 68, 94–112. Maddock, R. J. (1999). The retrosplenial cortex and emo-
Koós, O., & Gergely, G. (2001). The “flickering switch” tion: new insights from functional neuroimaging of
hypothesis: A contingency-based approach to the etiol- the human brain. Trends in Neuroscience, 22, 310–316.
ogy of disorganized attachment in infancy. Bulletin of Mayberg, H. S., Liotti, M., Brannan, S. K., McGinnis, S.,
the Menninger Clinic, 65, 397–410. Mahurin, R. K., Jerabek, P. A., et al. (1999). Reciprocal
Korzekwa, M. I., Dell, P. F., Links, P. S., Thabane, L., & Webb, limbic–cortical function and negative mood: Converg-
S. P. (2008). Estimating the prevalence of borderline per- ing PET findings in depression and normal sadness.
sonality disorder in psychiatric outpatients using a two- American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 675–682.
phase procedure. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 49, 380–386. Mayes, L. C. (2000). A developmental perspective on the
Kullgren, G. (1988). Factors associated with completed sui- regulation of arousal states. Seminars in Perinatology,
cide in borderline personality disorder. Journal of Ner- 24, 267–279.
vous and Mental Disease, 176, 40–44. Mayes, L. C. (2006). Arousal regulation, emotional flexibil-
Lee, R., Geracioti, T. D. Jr., Kasckow, J. W., & Coccaro, E. ity, medial amygdala function, and the impact of early
F. (2005). Childhood trauma and personality disorder: experience: Comments on the paper of Lewis et al. An-
Positive correlation with adult CSF corticotropin-releas- nals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1094,
ing factor concentrations. American Journal of Psychia- 178–192.
try, 162, 995–997. Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Wainwright, R., Das Gupta, M.,
Levine, A., Zagoory-Sharon, O., Feldman, R., & Weller, A. Fradley, E., & Tuckey, M. (2002). Maternal mind-mind-
(2007). Oxytocin during pregnancy and early postpar- edness and attachment security as predictors of theory
tum: Individual patterns and maternal-fetal attachment. of mind understanding. Child Development, 73,
Peptides, 28, 1162–1169. 1715–1726.
Levy, K. N., Edell, W. S., & McGlashan, T. H. (2007). De- Mikulincer, M., Gillath, O., & Shaver, P. R. (2002). Activa-
pressive experiences in inpatients with borderline per- tion of the attachment system in adulthood: Threat-related
sonality disorder. Psychiatric Quarterly, 78, 129–143. primes increase the accessibility of mental representa-
Levy, K. N., Meehan, K. B., Weber, M., Reynoso, J., & tions of attachment figures. Journal of Personality and
Clarkin, J. F. (2005). Attachment and borderline per- Social Psychology, 83, 881–895.
sonality disorder: Implications for psychotherapy. Psy- Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2007). Attachment in adult-
chopathology, 38, 64–74. hood: Structure, dynamics and change. New York:
Lieberman, M. D. (2007). Social cognitive neuroscience: A Guilford Press.
review of core processes. Annual Review of Psychology, Miller, A. L., Muehlenkamp, J. J., & Jacobson, C. M.
58, 259–289. (2008). Fact or fiction: Diagnosing borderline personal-
Lim, M. M., Wang, Z., Olazabal, D. E., Ren, X., Terwilli- ity disorder in adolescents. Clinical Psychology Review,
ger, E. F., & Young, L. J. (2004). Enhanced partner 28, 969–981.
preference in a promiscuous species by manipulating Minzenberg, M. J., Fan, J., New, A. S., Tang, C. Y., & Sie-
the expression of a single gene. Nature, 429, 754–757. ver, L. J. (2007). Fronto-limbic dysfunction in response
Linehan, M. M. (1993). Cognitive–behavioural treatment to facial emotion in borderline personality disorder: An
of borderline personality disorder. New York: Guilford event-related fMRI study. Psychiatry Research, 155,
Press. 231–243.
Ludolph, P. S., Westen, D., Misle, B., Jackson, A., Wixom, Minzenberg, M. J., Grossman, R., New, A. S., Mitropoulou,
J., & Wiss, F. C. (1990). The borderline diagnosis in V., Yehuda, R., Goodman, M., et al. (2006). Blunted
adolescents: Symptoms and developmental history. hormone responses to Ipsapirone are associated with
American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 470–476. trait impulsivity in personality disorder patients. Neu-
Luyten, P., Fonagy, P., Mayes, L., & Van Houdenhove, B. ropsychopharmacology, 31, 197–203.
(2009). Mentalization as a multidimensional concept. Montague, P. R., & Lohrenz, T. (2007). To detect and cor-
Manuscript submitted for publication. rect: Norm violations and their enforcement. Neuron,
Luyten, P., Mayes, L., Fonagy, P., & Van Houdenhove, B. 56, 14–18.
(2009). The interpersonal regulation of stress. Unpub- Morrison, A. P., Frame, L., & Larkin, W. (2003). Relation-
lished manuscript. ships between trauma and psychosis: A review and inte-
Lynch, T. R., Rosenthal, M. Z., Kosson, D. S., Cheavens, J. gration. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 42,
S., Lejuez, C. W., & Blair, R. J. (2006). Heightened sen- 331–353.
sitivity to facial expressions of emotion in borderline Nakash-Eisikovits, O., Dutra, L., & Westen, D. (2002). Re-
personality disorder. Emotion, 6, 647–655. lationship between attachment patterns and personality
Lyons-Ruth, K., Holmes, B. M., Sasvari-Szekely, M., Ro- pathology in adolescents. Journal of the American Acad-
nai, Z., Nemoda, Z., & Pauls, D. (2007). Serotonin emy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 1111–1123.
transporter polymorphism and borderline or antisocial Neumann, I. D. (2008). Brain oxytocin: A key regulator of
traits among low-income young adults. Psychiatric emotional and social behaviours in both females and
Genetics, 17, 339–343. males. Journal of Neuroendocrinology, 20, 858–865.
Lyons-Ruth, K., Yellin, C., Melnick, S., & Atwood, G. Ni, X., Sicard, T., Bulgin, N., Bismil, R., Chan, K.,
(2005). Expanding the concept of unresolved mental McMain, S., et al. (2007). Monoamine oxidase a gene
1380 P. Fonagy and P. Luyten

is associated with borderline personality disorder. Psy- Rogosch, F. A., & Cicchetti, D. (2005). Child maltreatment,
chiatric Genetics, 17, 153–157. attention networks, and potential precursors to border-
Northoff, G., Heinzel, A., de Greck, M., Bermpohl, F., Do- line personality disorder. Development and Psychopa-
browolny, H., & Panksepp, J. (2006). Self-referential thology, 17, 1071–1089.
processing in our brain—A meta-analysis of imaging Rogosch, F. A., Cicchetti, D., & Aber, J. L. (1995). The role
studies on the self. NeuroImage, 31, 440–457. of child maltreatment in early deviations in cognitive
O’Connor, T. (2006). The persisting effects of early experi- and affective processing abilities and later peer rela-
ences on social development. In D. Cicchetti & D. J. tionship problems. Development and Psychopathology,
Cohen (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology: Vol. 3. 7, 591–609.
Risk, disorder and adaptation (2nd ed., pp. 202–234). Ross, C. A. (2007). Borderline personality disorder and dis-
New York: Wiley. sociation. Journal of Trauma and Dissociation, 8,
O’Connor, T. G., & Hirsch, N. (1999). Intra-individual dif- 71–80.
ferences and relationship-specificity of social under- Rusch, N., Lieb, K., Gottler, I., Hermann, C., Schramm, E.,
standing and mentalising in early adolescence. Social Richter, H., et al. (2007). Shame and implicit self-con-
Development, 8, 256–274. cept in women with borderline personality disorder.
Onishi, K. H., Baillargeon, R., & Leslie, A. M. (2007). 15- American Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 500–508.
Month-old infants detect violations of pretend scenar- Sabbagh, M. A. (2004). Understanding orbitofrontal contri-
ios. Acta Pychologica, 124, 106–128. butions to theory-of-mind reasoning: Implications for
Ortigue, S., Bianchi-Demicheli, F., Hamilton, A. F., & autism. Brain Cognition, 55, 209–219.
Grafton, S. T. (2007). The neural basis of love as a sub- Sanislow, C. A., Grilo, C. M., Morey, L. C., Bender, D. S.,
liminal prime: An event-related functional magnetic Skodol, A. E., Gunderson, J. G., et al. (2002). Confir-
resonance imaging study. Journal of Cognitive Neu- matory factor analysis of DSM-IV criteria for borderline
roscience, 19, 1218–1230. personality disorder: Findings from the collaborative
Overall, N. C., Fletcher, G. J., & Friesen, M. D. (2003). Map- longitudinal personality disorders study. American
ping the intimate relationship mind: Comparisons be- Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 284–290.
tween three models of attachment representations. Per- Sar, V., Akyuz, G., Kugu, N., Ozturk, E., & Ertem-Vehid,
sonality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 1479–1493. H. (2006). Axis I dissociative disorder comorbidity in
Paris, J. (2004). Sociocultural factors in the treatment of borderline personality disorder and reports of childhood
personality disorders. In W. J. Livesley (Ed.), Hand- trauma. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 67, 1583–1590.
book of personality disorders: Theory and practice Satpute, A. B., & Lieberman, M. D. (2006). Integrating
(pp. 135–147). New York: Wiley. automatic and controlled processes into neurocognitive
Pears, K. C., & Fisher, P. A. (2005 ). Emotion understand- models of social cognition. Brain Research, 1079,
ing and theory of mind among maltreated children in 86–97.
foster care. Development and Psychopathology, 17, Schmahl, C. G., Elzinga, B. M., Ebner, U. W., Simms, T.,
47–65. Sanislow, C., Vermetten, E., et al. (2004). Psychophys-
Philipsen, A., Richter, H., Schmahl, C., Peters, J., Rusch, iological reactivity to traumatic and abandonment
N., Bohus, M., et al. (2004). Clonidine in acute aversive scripts in borderline personality and posttraumatic stress
inner tension and self-injurious behavior in female pa- disorders: A preliminary report. Psychiatry Research,
tients with borderline personality disorder. Journal of 126, 33–42.
Clinical Psychiatry, 65, 1414–1419. Schnell, K., Dietrich, T., Schnitker, R., Daumann, J., &
Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1956). The child’s conception of Herpertz, S. C. (2007). Processing of autobiographical
space. London: Routledge. (original work published memory retrieval cues in borderline personality disor-
1948) der. Journal of Affective Disorders, 97, 253–259.
Pierce, T., & Lydon, J. E. (2001). Global and specific rela- Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., & Aharon-Peretz, J. (2007). Disso-
tional models in the experience of social interactions. ciable prefrontal networks for cognitive and affective
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, theory of mind: A lesion study. Neuropsychologia, 45,
613–631. 3054–3067.
Pliszka, S. R. (2003). Non-stimulant treatment of attention- Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Aharon-Peretz, J., & Levkovitz, Y.
deficit/hyperactivity disorder. CNS Spectrums, 8, 253– (2007). The neuroanatomical basis of affective men-
258. talizing in schizophrenia: Comparison of patients with
Pronin, E., Gilovich, T., & Ross, L. (2004). Objectivity in the schizophrenia and patients with localized prefrontal le-
eye of the beholder: Divergent perceptions of bias in self sions. Schizophrenia Research, 90, 274–283.
versus others. Psychological Review, 111, 781–799. Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Aharon-Peretz, J., & Perry, D.
Raikes, H. A., & Thompson, R. A. (2006). Family (2009). Two systems for empathy: A double dissocia-
emotional climate, attachment security, and young chil- tion between emotional and cognitive empathy in infer-
dren’s emotion knowledge in a high-risk sample. Brit- ior frontal gyrus versus ventromedial prefrontal lesions.
ish Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24, 89–104. Brain, 132, 617–627.
Reisch, T., Ebner-Priemer, U. W., Tschacher, W., Bohus, Sharp, C., & Fonagy, P. (2008a). The parent’s capacity to
M., & Linehan, M. M. (2008). Sequences of emotions treat the child as a psychological agent: Constructs,
in patients with borderline personality disorder. Acta measures and implications for developmental psycho-
Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 118, 42–48. pathology. Social Development, 17, 737–754.
Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron Sharp, C., & Fonagy, P. (2008b). Social cognition and at-
system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192. tachment-related disorders. In C. Sharp, P. Fonagy &
Robbins, T. W. (1997). Arousal systems and attentional pro- I. Goodyer (Eds.), Social cognition and developmental
cesses. Biological Psychology, 45, 57–71. psychopathology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Rochat, P. (2009). Others in Mind—Fear of rejection and Sharp, C., Fonagy, P., & Goodyer, I. (Eds.). (2008). Social
the social origin of self-consciousness. Cambridge: cognition and developmental psychopathology. Oxford:
Cambridge University Press. Oxford University Press.
Understanding and treatment of borderline personality disorder 1381

Sharp, C., & Romero, C. (2007). Borderline personality dis- Vorria, P., Papaligoura, Z., Sarafidou, J., Kopakaki, M.,
order: A comparison between children and adults. Bul- Dunn, J., Van Ijzendoorn, M. H., et al. (2006). The de-
letin of the Menninger Clinic, 71, 85–114. velopment of adopted children after institutional care: A
Shipman, K., Edwards, A., Brown, A., Swisher, L., & Jen- follow-up study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psy-
nings, E. (2005). Managing emotion in a maltreating chiatry, 47, 1246–1253.
context: A pilot study examining child neglect. Child Vrticka, P., Andersson, F., Grandjean, D., Sander, D., &
Abuse and Neglect, 29, 1015–1029. Vuilleumier, P. (2008). Individual attachment style
Shmueli-Goetz, Y., Target, M., Fonagy, P., & Datta, A. modulates human amygdala and striatum activation
(2008). The Child Attachment Interview: A psychomet- during social appraisal. PLoS ONE, 3, e2868.
ric study of reliability and validity. Developmental Psy- Walter, M., Bureau, J. F., Holmes, B. M., Bertha, E. A.,
chology, 44, 939–956. Hollander, M., Wheelis, J., et al. (2008). Cortisol re-
Silbersweig, D., Clarkin, J. F., Goldstein, M., Kernberg, sponse to interpersonal stress in young adults with bor-
O. F., Tuescher, O., Levy, K. N., et al. (2007). Failure derline personality disorder: A pilot study. European
of frontolimbic inhibitory function in the context of Psychiatry, 23, 201–204.
negative emotion in borderline personality disorder. Watson, S., Chilton, R., Fairchild, H., & Whewell, P.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 1832–1841. (2006). Association between childhood trauma and dis-
Slade, A. (2005). Parental reflective functioning: An introduc- sociation among patients with borderline personality
tion. Attachment and Human Development, 7, 269–281. disorder. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psy-
Smith, M., & Walden, T. (1999). Understanding feelings chiatry, 40, 478–481.
and coping with emotional situations: A comparison Wertz, A. E., & German, T. C. (2007). Belief-desire reason-
of maltreated and nonmaltreated preschoolers. Social ing in the explanation of behavior: Do actions speak
Development, 8, 93–116. louder than words? Cognition, 105, 184–194.
Soloff, P. H., Feske, U., & Fabio, A. (2008). Mediators of Westen, D., Dutra, L., & Shedler, J. (2005). Assessing ado-
the relationship between childhood sexual abuse and lescent personality pathology. British Journal of Psy-
suicidal behavior in borderline personality disorder. chiatry, 186, 227–238.
Journal of Personality Disorders, 22, 221–232. Westen, D., Ludolph, P., Lerner, H., Ruffins, S., & Wiss, F.
Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., Carlson, E., & Collins, W. A. C. (1990). Object relations in borderline adolescents.
(2005). The development of the person: The Minnesota Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adoles-
Study of Risk and Adaptation From Birth to Adulthood. cent Psychiatry, 29, 338–348.
New York: Guilford Press. Williams, L., Fonagy, P., Target, M., Fearon, P., Sargent, J.,
Stiglmayr, C. E., Ebner-Priemer, U. W., Bretz, J., Behm, R., Bleiberg, E., et al. (2006). Training psychiatry residents
Mohse, M., Lammers, C. H., et al. (2008). Dissociative in mentalization-based therapy. In J. Allen & P. Fonagy
symptoms are positively related to stress in borderline (Eds.) Handbook of mentalisation based treatments.
personality disorder. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, London: Wiley.
117, 139–147. Wingenfeld, K., Lange, W., Wulff, H., Berea, C., Beblo, T.,
Strathearn, L., Fonagy, P., Amico, J., & Montague, R. (in Saavedra, A. S., et al. (2007). Stability of the dexametha-
press). Adult attachment predicts maternal brain and oxy- sone suppression test in borderline personality disorder
tocin response to infant cues. Neuropsychopharmacology. with and without comorbid PTSD: A one-year follow-
Strathearn, L., Li, J., Fonagy, P., & Montague, R. (2008). up study. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 63, 843–850.
What’s in a smile? Maternal brain responses to infant Zanarini, M., Gunderson, J. G., & Frankenburg, F. R.
facial cues. Pediatrics, 122, 40–51. (1990). Discriminating borderline personality disorder
Stuart, J., Westen, D., Lohr, N. E., Benjamin, J., Becker, S., from other Axis II disorders. American Journal Psy-
& Vorus, N. (1990). Object relations in borderlines, chiatry, 147, 161–167.
depressives, and normals: An examination of human Zanarini, M. C., & Frankenburg, F. R. (2007). The essential
responses on the Rorschach. Journal of Personality nature of borderline psychopathology. Journal of Per-
Assessment, 55, 296–318. sonality Disorders, 21, 518–535.
Target, M., & Fonagy, P. (1996). Playing with reality II: Zanarini, M. C., Frankenburg, F. R., DeLuca, C. J., Hennen, J.,
The development of psychic reality from a theoretical Khera, G. S., & Gunderson, J. G. (1998). The pain of being
perspective. International Journal of Psycho-Analysis, borderline: Dysphoric states specific to borderline person-
77, 459–479. ality disorder. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 6, 201–207.
Uddin, L. Q., Iacoboni, M., Lange, C., & Keenan, J. P. Zanarini, M. C., Frankenburg, F. R., Jager-Hyman, S.,
(2007). The self and social cognition: the role of cortical Reich, D. B., & Fitzmaurice, G. (2008). The course of
midline structures and mirror neurons. Trends in Cog- dissociation for patients with borderline personality dis-
nitive Science, 11, 153–157. order and Axis II comparison subjects: A 10-year fol-
van IJzendoorn, M. H. & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. low-up study. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 118,
(1997). Intergenerational transmission of attachment: 291–296.
A move to the contextual level. In L. Atkinson & Zanarini, M. C., Frankenburg, F. R., Ridolfi, M. E., Jager-
K. J. Zucker (Eds.), Attachment and psychopathology Hyman, S., Hennen, J., & Gunderson, J. G. (2006). Re-
(pp. 135–170). New York: Guilford Press. ported childhood onset of self-mutilation among bor-
Vanderwal, T., Hunyadi, E., Grupe, D. W., Connors, C. M., derline patients. Journal of Personality Disorders, 20,
& Schultz, R. T. (2008). Self, mother and abstract other: 9–15.
An fMRI study of reflective social processing. Neuro- Zittel Conklin, C., & Westen, D. (2005). Borderline person-
Image, 41, 1437–1446. ality disorder in clinical practice. American Journal of
Vermote, R., Fonagy, P., Vertommen, H., Verhaest, Y., Stroo- Psychiatry, 162, 867–875.
bants, R., Vandeneede, B., et al. (2009). Outcome and out- Zweig-Frank, H., & Paris, J. (1991). Parents’ emotional ne-
come trajectories of personality disordered patients during glect and overprotection according to the recollections
and after a psychoanalytic hospitalization-based treatment. of patients with borderline personality disorder. Ameri-
Journal of Personality Disorder, 23, 293–306. can Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 648–651.

View publication stats

You might also like