Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 15

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/229187129

Typological mood-state factors measured in the Eight State Questionnaire

Article in Personality and Individual Differences · December 1987


DOI: 10.1016/0191-8869(87)90022-5

CITATIONS READS
22 1,686

1 author:

Gregory J. Boyle
University of Melbourne
224 PUBLICATIONS 5,341 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Role of Personality-Stress in Cancer and Coronary Heart Disease View project

Cognitive and affective components of empathy View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Gregory J. Boyle on 04 November 2017.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Bond University
From the SelectedWorks of Gregory J. Boyle

1987

Typological Mood-State Factors Measured in the


Eight State Questionnaire
Gregory J. Boyle, Bond University

Available at: http://works.bepress.com/greg_boyle/114/


1

Typological Mood-State Factors Measured in the Eight State

Questionnaire

Gregory J. Boyle

University of Melbourne

This research was supported by a Research Development Grant awarded to the author by the
University of Melbourne. Address correspondence to Gregory J. Boyle, University of
Melbourne, Parkville, Victoria 3052, Australia.
2

Abstract

Previous studies of the higher-order state-change factors in the Eight State

Questionnaire (8SQ) have suggested that at least three typological mood-state

dimensions are measured in the instrument. Nevertheless, the appropriate dR-

factor analytic methodology has not always been employed, and even when it has

(e.g. Boyle, 198Sa), the sample size has been sufficient to allow only preliminary

conclusions. The present study explores more thoroughly this issue of typological

mood-state factors in the 8SQ using a large sample of 470 subjects. A dR-

factoring of the intercorrelations for the subscale difference scores (across two

separate measurement occasions) clearly suggests the presence of four different

emotional/mood-type factors. These findings are compared with higher-order

factors obtained previously for the Differential Emotions Scale (DES), an

instrument purported to measure fundamental human emotions.


3

Introduction

Numerous multivariate self-report inventories have been devised to

measure fundamental emotional/mood states. Boyle (1985b) reviewed the

psychometric adequacy of a number of such instruments and concluded that most

have serious limitations in their present versions. The two most promising of these

mood-state measures, however, on the basis of Boyle's review, are the Eight State

Questionnaire (8SQ-Curran and Cattell, 1976) and the Differential Emotions Scale

(DES-Izard, Dougherty, Bloxom and Kotsch, 1974). The 8SQ is based on

extensive factor analytic work and purports to measure Anxiety, Stress,

Depression, Regression, Fatigue, Guilt, Extraversion and Arousal. Recently, the

question of the nature of the relationships between emotional states has received

some prominence in the literature (e.g. Schwartz and Weinberger, 1980; Sirota and

Schwartz, 1982). In this context, the investigation of typological (higher-order)

mood-state factors in the 8SQ seems warranted. Indeed, in some instances it might

be more convenient to score the 8SQ for secondary factors, thereby adding to the

usefulness of the instrument.

Previous studies of the higher-order factor structure of the 8SQ (e.g.

Stewart and Stewart, 1976; Cattell and Kline, 1977; Boyle, 1983, 1985a) have not

adequately resolved this issue. The dR-factoring procedure across two separate

measurement occasions (a methodology which is appropriate to the elucidation of

change dimensions and which has been supported by Nesselroade and Cable,

1974; Cattell, 1979, 1982; Lam, 1981) was employed in Boyle's (1985a) study on

a marginal sample of 258 young adults. Boyle employed an iterative principal

factoring procedure with oblique rotation and obtained a three-factor higher-order


4

solution for the 8SQ. According to Boyle (l985a, p. 372), "the first dR-factor

involved a combination of Anxiety, Stress, Depression, Regression and

Guilt ... reminiscent of Eysenck's (e.g. 1983) Neurotiscism dimension, but at the

state level. The second 8SQ factor clearly represented Eysenck's Extraversion

factor within the state realm, while the third dR-factor contrasted Fatigue with

Arousal, indicative of a physiological dimension pertaining to CNS

activation levels (cf. Meites, Lovallo and Pishkin, 1980)."

The present study attempts to more comprehensively examine typological

mood-state factors in the 8SQ on a considerably larger sample than previously

employed by Boyle (1985a) and to compare these findings with those reported by

Boyle (1986) for the DES-III (a 30-item version of lzard's scale) -- a 49-item

version, the DES-IV, has more recently been constructed comprising 12 separate

subscales.

The DES-III purports to measure 10 fundamental emotions discernible in the

facial expression -- namely, Joy, Surprise, Sadness, Anger, Disgust, Contempt,

Fear, Shame/Shyness and Guilt (cf. Izard and Buechler, 1979, 1980; Schwartz,

Ahem and Brown, 1979a; Schwartz, Weinberger and Singer, 1979b; Schwartz,

Fair, Salt, Mandel and Klerman, 1976; Schwartz, Brown and Ahem, 1980). Boyle

(1986) conducted a single-occasion R-factoring of the item intercorrelations for

the DES-III on a sample of 204 University of Delaware undergraduates, using an

iterative principal factoring procedure with rotation to oblique simple structure.

This was followed by a second-order analysis which produced a four-factor

solution. The first higher-order DES-III factor accounted for 48.2% of the variance

and contrasted Fear with Shame/Shyness, Disgust and Guilt. The second factor

accounted for 32.1% of the variance and contrasted Surprise with Joy and Interest.
5

The third factor accounted for 12.1% of the variance and appeared to represent a

combination of Contempt and Anger, while the fourth factor accounted for 7.6%

of the variance and represented Depressed Mood (i.e. the subjective-experience

component of the depression syndrome). The validity of the DES instrument has

been supported in several studies (e.g. Emde, 1980; Fuenzalida, Emde,

Pannabecker and Stenberg, 1981; Kotsch, Gerbing and Schwartz, 1982; Izard and

Blumberg, 1985, 1987; Izard, Blumberg and Oyster, 1985; Boyle, 1984), thereby

making it a useful instrument to compare with the 8SQ. The present paper

attempts to definitively examine the typological mood-state factors in the 8SQ and

to relate these to the extant knowledge above, concerning the typological mood-

factors in the DES-III.

Method

Subjects

The total sample comprised 470 male and female college students

attending either the Institute of Catholic Education, Melbourne, or the Melbourne

College of Advanced Education. Both colleges were situated in predominantly

middle-class socioeconomic areas. The age range of the sample varied between 18

and 50 years with the mean age being about 21 years.

Table 1
Means and standard deviations for 8SQ
subscale change scores (N=470)

Subscale M SD

Anxiety 5.87 5.14


Stress 3.85 3.15
Depression 5.36 4.46
Regression 4.02 3.54
Fatigue 5.88 5.16
Guilt 4.57 4.31
Extraversion -4.44 4.12
Arousal -4.57 4.11
6

Procedure and results

The 470 students responded to the 8SQ items on two separate occasions at

least l week apart. Although there was a wide range of scores, visual inspection of

the data suggested that floor and ceiling effects were not particularly a problem.

Both means and standard deviations for the 8SQ subscale change scores are

presented in Table l.

The intercorrelations of the 8SQ subscale change scores are presented in

Table 2. As is evident, the state-change dimensions measured in the 8SQ are

moderately intercorrelated (a usual finding with mood-state inventories-see Boyle,

1985b). Cattell (1979) has commented on the high intercorrelations between the

subscales of the 8SQ and has pointed out that a number of moods may vary

together in response to certain situational stimuli, as indeed the experience of

'mixed emotions' would predict, quite apart from the more common experience

that emotions such as Anxiety and Stress, for example, tend to fluctuate in similar

directions under various conditions.

As in Boyle's (1985a) study, an iterative principal factoring procedure was


employed (requiring 208 iterations, at which point the communality for the
Extraversion subscale reached one). Application of the Scree Test indicated that
four factors should be extracted (as Cattell and Vogelmann, 1977, along with
Hakstian, Rogers and Cattell, 1982, have shown, the Kaiser-Guttman eigenvalue
greater than unity underestimates the appropriate factor number when there are
fewer than about 20 variables in the analysis--cf. Child, 1970, pp. 43-44).
Accordingly, four higher-order 8SQ factors were extracted and rotated to oblique
simple structure via the direct Oblimin method in the SPSS statistical package (cf.
Nie, Hull, Jenkins, Steinbrenner and Bent, 1975). Othogonal rotation was not
employed in view of the reservations expressed by Loo (1979) as well as by
Gorsuch (1983) and Cattell (1978). Clearly, if a solution is truly orthogonal, the
7

oblique rotation will stop at the special orthogonal position, as Cattell has pointed
out. The factor pattern solution is presented in Table 3.
The results above support those already reported by Boyle (1985a) with the

exception that Depression has now shifted out of Factor I and formed the fourth

factor by itself (see Gorsuch, 1983, regarding the importance of a single high

loading with small factor matrices). The other results are similar to Boyle's earlier

findings with one factor representing Eysenck's Extraversion dimension at the

state level, while the other factor contrasted Fatigue with Arousal, and is clearly a

physiological activation dimension. Importantly, the state Neuroticism dimension

of Factor I is now clearly seen to involve Anxiety, Stress, Regression and Guilt,

while Depression appears to be a separate entity, as was found in the higher-order

R-factoring of the DES-III. This finding provides further evidence that Depressed

mood is a complex emotion involving more fundamental emotions such as

Sadness, Anger, Fear and Anxiety (cf. Izard et al., 1985; Cattell, 1979, pp. 76-79;

Price, Cattell and Patrick, 1981).

Table 2
Intercorrelations of subscale change scores
(N=470)

Anxiety Stress Depression Regression Fatigue Guilt Extraversion/Arousal


Anx.

Str. 0.52

Dep. 0.68 0.44

Reg. 0.56 0.41 0.52

Fat. 0.34 0.38 0.56 0.43

Gui. 0.60 0.37 0.57 0.43 0.31

Ext. -0.47 -0.27 -0.58 -0.44 -0.50 -0.41

Aro. -0.41 -0.37 -0.57 -0.40 -0.62 -0.37 -0.57


8

Table 3
Oblique factor pattern matrix for 8SQ

Subscale Factor1 Factor2 Factor3 Factor4 h2

Anxiety 0.77 0.22 -0.09 -0.21 0.82

Stress 0.65 -0.14 0.06 0.06 0.43

Depression 0.04 -0.18 -0.02 -0.81 0.87

Regression 0.50 -0.10 -0.14 -0.05 0.44

Fatigue 0.08 -0.76 -0.06 -0.12 0.77

Guilt 0.39 0.08 -0.10 -0.31 0.46

Extraversion 0.01 0.04 0.99 -0.04 1.00

Arousal -0.11 0.45 0.23 0.15 0.55

%Variance 75.1 13.0 8.5 3.5

Notes. Significant loadings underlined. Factor loadings rounded to two decimal


places. The association of Depression State with Neuroticism State appears to be
through the common loading of Guilt. Factor1 correlates -0.40 with Factor 2, -
0.48 with Factor 3, and -0.72 with Factor 4. Factor 2 correlates 0.42 with
Factor 3, and 0.33 with Factor 4, while Factor 3 correlates 0.60 with Factor 4.
Variation of the δ shift parameter in the SPSS package for greater or less
obliquity resulted in a poorer approximation to simple structure and reduced ±.10
hyperplane counts as compared with the present solution.

In conclusion, the present study suggests strongly that there are at least

four major typological mood-state factors: Neuroticism, Extraversion,

Arousal/Fatigue, and Depression. Only the Arousal/Fatigue state dimension was

not also represented in the higher-order findings for the DES-III. In the latter

analysis, a typological mood-state dimension pertaining to Anger/Contempt

emerged, which has no representation in the 8SQ instrument. Nevertheless,

research into typological mood-state factors seems well worth the effort, if in the

process, a smaller number of dimensions can be discerned as representing most of

the mood-state variance. Such research should enable the development of more

condensed and efficient multivariate mood-state instruments, which can more

readily be used in many applied situations.


9

References

Boyle G. J. (1983) Higher-order factor structure of Cattell's MAT and 8SQ.

Multivar. exp. clin. Res. 6, 119-127. Boyle G. J. (1984) Reliability and

validity of Izard's Differential Emotions Scale. Person. individ. Diff. 5,

747-750.

Boyle G. J. (1985a) A reanalysis of the higher-order factor structure of the

Motivation Analysis Test and the Eight State Questionnaire. Person.

individ. Diff. 6, 367-374.

Boyle G. J. (I985b) Self-report measures of depression: some psychometric

considerations. Br. J. clin. Psychol. 24, 45-59.

Boyle G. J. (1986) Higher order factors in the Differential Emotions Scale

(DE8-III). Person. individ. Diif. 7, 305-310.

Cattell R. B. (1978) The Scientific Use of Factor Analysis in Behavioral and Life

Sciences. Plenum Press, New York.

Cattell R. B. (1979) Personality and Learning Theory, Vol. I, The Structure of

Personality in its Environment. Springer, New York.

Cattell R. B. (1982) The clinical use of difference scores: Some psychometric

problems. Multivar. exp. clin. Res. 6, 87-98.

Cattell R. B. and Kline P. (1977) The Scientific Analysis of Personality and

Motivation. Academic Press, New York.

Cattell R. B. and Vogelmann S. (1977) A comprehensive trial for the scree and

KG criteria for determining the number of factors. Multivar. behav. Res.

12, 289-325.

Child D. (1970) The Essentials of Factor Analysis. Holt, Rinehart and Winston,

London.
10

Curran J. P. and Cattell R. B. (1976) Manual of the Eight State Questionnaire.

IPAT, Champaign, Illinois.

Emde R.N. (1980) Levels of meaning for infant emotions. In The Minnesota

Symposia on Child Psychology, Vol. 13, Development of Cognition,

Affect, and Social Relations (Edited by Collins W. A.). Erlbaum, Hillsdale,

N.J.

Eysenck H. J. (1983) Personality as a fundamental concept in scientific

psychology. Aust. J. Psychol. 35, 289-304.

Eysenck H. J. (1984) Cattell and the theory of personality. Multivar. behav. Res.

19, 323-336.

Fuenzalida C., Emde R.N., Pannabecker B. J. and Stenberg C. (1981) Validation

of the Differential Emotions Scale in 613 mothers. Motiv. Emotn. 5, 37-45.

Gorsuch R. L. (1983) Factor Analysis. 2nd edn. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, N.J.

Hakstian A. R., Rogers W. T. and Cattell R. B. (1982) The behavior of number-of

factors rules with stimulated data. Multivar. behav. Res. 17, 193-219.

Izard C. E. (1977) Human Emotions. Plenum Press, New York.

Izard C. E. (1978) On the development of emotions and emotion-cognition

relationships in infancy. In The Development of Affect (Edited by Lewis

M. and Rosenblum L.A.). Plenum Press, New York.

Izard C. E. (1979a) Emotions as motivations: an evolutionary-development

perspective. In Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (Edited by Dienstbier

R. A.). Univ. of Nebraska Press, Lincoln, Neb.

Izard C. E. (1979b) Facial expression, emotion, and motivation. In Nonverbal

Behavior (Edited by Wolfgang A.). Academic Press, New York.


11

Izard C. E. (1980) The emergence of emotions and the development of

consciousness in infancy. In The Psychobiology of Consciousness (Edited

by Davidson J. M. and Davidson R. J.). Plenum Press, New York.

Izard C. E. (1982) Measuring Emotions in Infants and Children. Cambridge

University Press.

Izard C. E. and Blumberg S. (1985) Affective and cognitive characteristics of

depression in ten- and eleven-year-old children. J. Person. soc. Psychol.

48, 194--202.

Izard C. E. and Blumberg S. (1987) Discriminating patterns of emotions in anxious

and depressed ten and eleven year old children. J. Person. soc. Psychol. In

press.

Izard C. E., Blumberg S. H. and Oyster C. K. (1985) Age and sex differences in

the pattern of emotions in childhood anxiety and depression. In Motivation,

Emotion and Personality (Edited by Spence J. T. and Izard C. E.). North

Holland, Amsterdam.

Izard C. E. and Buechler S. (Eds) (1979) Emotion expressions and personality

intergration in infancy. In Emotions in Personality and Psychopathology.

Plenum Press, New York.

Izard C. E. and Buechler S. (1980) Aspects of consiousness and personality in

terms of differential emotions theory. In Emotion: Theory, Research and

Experience, Vol. 1 (Edited by Plutchik R. and Kellerman H.). Academic

Press, New York.

Izard C. E., Dougherty F. E., Bloxom B. M. and Kotsch N. E. (1974) The

Differential Emotions Scale: a method of measuring the subjective


12

experience of discrete emotions. Unpublished manuscript, Vanderbilt

Univ., Nashville, Tenn.

Kotsch W. E., Gerbing D. W. and Schwartz G. E. (1982) The construct validity of

the Differential Emotions Scale as adapted for children and adolescents. In

Measuring Emotions in Infants and Children (Edited by Izard C. E.).

Cambridge University Press.

Lam T. C. M. (1981) The unreliability of difference scores for the measurement of

change: a paradox. Proc. Conf of the American Educational Research

Association, Los Angeles, Calif.

Loo R. (1979) The orthogonal rotation of factors in clinical research: a critical

note. J. clin. Psychol. 35, 762-765.

Meites K., Lovallo W. and Pishkin V. (1980) A comparison of four scales for

anxiety, depression, and neuroticism. J. clin. Psycho/. 36, 427-432.

Nesselroade J. R. and Cable D. G. (1974) Sometimes, it's okay to factor different

scores: the separation of state and trait anxiety. Multivar. behav. Res. 9,

273-281.

Nie N.H., Hull C. H., Jenkins J. G., Steinbrenner K. and Bent D. H. (1975)

Statistical Package for the Social Sciences. McGraw-Hill, New York.

Price P. L., Cattell R. B. and Patrick S. V. (1981) A check on the factor-analytic

structure of four 0-A Kit source traits, independence, evasiveness, realism

and dissofrustance, found diagnostic of depression. Multivar. exp. clin.

Res. 5, 81-90.

Schwartz G. E. and Weinberger D. A. (1980) Patterns of emotional responses to

affective situations: relations among happiness, sadness, anger, fear, depression,

and anxiety. Motiv. Emotn. 4, 175-191.


13

Schwartz G. E., Ahern G. L. and Brown S. L. (1979a) Lateralized facial muscle

response to positive versus negative emotional stimuli. Psychophysiology

16, 561-571.

Schwartz G. E., Weinberger D. A. and Singer J. A. (1979b) Cardiovascular

differentiation of emotion during imagery and exercise. Psychosom. Med.

41, 583.

Schwartz G. E., Brown S. L. and Ahern G. L. (1980) Facial muscle patterning and

subjective experience during affective imagery: sex differences.

Psychophysiology 17, 75-82.

Schwartz G. E., Fair P. L., Salt P., Mandel M. R. and Klerman G. L. (1976) Facial

muscle patterning to affective imagery in depressed and non-depressed

subjects. Science 192, 489-491.

Sirota A. D. and Schwartz G. E. (1982) Facial muscle patterning and lateralization

during elation and depression imagery. J. abnorm. Psychol. 91, 25-34.

Stewart D. W. and Stewart L. F. (1976) An application of P-technique to the Eight

State Questionnaire. Multivar. exp. clin. Res. 2, 135-139.

View publication stats

You might also like