Ekman 1983

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 3

3. E. F.

Evans, in Neuronal Mechanisms of Hear- combined Poisson channels that would yield a remedy methodological problems that
ing, J. Syka and L. Aitkin, Eds. (Plenum, New threshold equal to that observed in the band-
York, 1981), p. 69; M. B. Sachs and E. D. reject condition. For a single channel the per- have lessened the impact of previous
Young, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 66, 470 (1979). formance measure, di', is approximately studies: (i) A broad sample of six emo-
4. J. E. Rose, J. E. Hind, D. J. Anderson, J. F. AIdBr (I)[T/r(I)]'n, where T is the interval over
Brugge, J. Neurophysiol. 34, 685 (1974); D. H. which events (spikes) are counted, r(I) is the tions was studied, rather than the two or
Johnson, thesis, Massachusetts Institute of mean firing rate at intensity I, r'(I) is the slope of three that are typical. (ii) Verification
Technology (1974); M. B. Sachs and E. D. the rate-intensity function at I, and AIdB is the
Young, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 68, 858 (1980). decibel difference between the two stimuli. For procedures were instituted to maximize
5. E. Javel, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 69, 1735 (1981). n independent, optimally combined channels the likelihood that each sample con-
6. N. F. Viemeister, ibid. 56, 1594 (1974); B. C. J. with equal d',, the resulting overall performance
Moore and D. H. Raab, ibid. 57, 400 (1975). is d' = n"'2d',. Thus, n = [d'IAldFr'()12r(I)IT. tained only the single target emotion and
7. D. H. Johnson, Biophys. J. 22, 413 (1978). [See J. P. Egan, Signal Detection Theory and
8. The final threshold for each subject was based ROC Analysis (Academic Press, New York, no other. (iii) A sufficiently broad sample
on the average of at least five separate threshold 1975), pp. 162-217, for an excellent treatment of of autonomic measures was obtained to
estimates. The standard deviation of these esti- Poisson observers.] At No = 50 dB in the band-
mates was typically less than 2 dB and ranged reject condition, AIdB = 4.8 for d' = 0.77. As- enable differentiation of multiple emo-
from 0.7 to 3.8 dB. suming a fixed counting interval of 100 msec and tions, with appropriate statistical protec-
9. The cutoff frequencies were chosen such that at a mean firing rate of 100 spikes per second at
the highest level of the band-reject masker, the No = 50 dB, then for slopes of five or two spikes tion against spurious findings due to mul-
bandpass noise was approximately 10 dB above per second per decibel, n is 1.03 or 6.4 fibers, tiple dependent measures. (iv) Autonom-
its masked threshold for all subjects. The noises respectively. These slopes are reasonable for
were produced by multiplying a lowpass and a rate-intensity functions obtained with broad- ic measures were taken from the onset of
bandpass noise by a 10-kHz sinusoid and adding band noise at lower spectrum levels [T. B.
a 20-kHz highpass noise. Schalk and M. B. Sachs, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. emotion production continuously until it
10. Audio Technica ATH-7 Electret headphones 67, 903 (1980)]. The assumption of optimum was terminated. More typical measures
were used. Since no high-frequency calibration combination requires, in effect, that saturated
standard exists for these or any headphones, the channels, those with d! = 0, be ignored. Wheth- taken before and after production of an
spectrum levels reported are the 1-kHz equiva- er the auditory system actually achieves or emotion may completely miss short-
lents measured with a flat plate coupler. The approximates optimum combination is not di-
overall level is approximately 43 dB greater than rectly relevant to the question of whether, at the lived target emotions. (v) Multiple elicit-
the reported spectrum level; the highest levels level of primary fibers, a rate code is even ing tasks were used with the same sub-
used in this experiment were in excess of 100-dB possible.
SPL overall. 15. I thank D. A. Burkhardt, W. D. Ward, M. jects. (vi) Professional actors (N = 12)
11. M. H. Lurie, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 11, 420 (1940); Ruggero, and G. Wakefield for their comments

Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on June 10, 2014


Y. Katsuki, N. Suga, Y. Kanno, ibid. 34, 1396 on the manuscript. I also thank H. S. Colburn and scientists who study the face
(1962); N. Y.-S. Kiang, Ann. Otol. Rhinol. Lar- and W. M. Rabinowitz for stimulating discus- (N = 4) served as subjects to minimize
yngol. 77, 656 (1968). sions on this topic. This work was supported by
12. M. C. Liberman, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 63, 442 grant NS12125 from the National Institute of contamination of emotion samples by
(1978); Science 216, 1239 (1982). Neurological and Communicative Disorders and extraneous affect associated with frus-
13. A. R. Palmer and E. F. Evans, Exp. Brain Res. Stroke.
Suppl. II (1979), p. 19; Hear. Res. 7, 305 (1982). tration or embarrassment.
14. This is the number of independent, optimally 8 February 1983; revised 24 May 1983 We studied six target emotions (sur-
prise, disgust, sadness, anger, fear, and
happiness) elicited by two tasks (direct-
ed facial action and relived emotion),
with emotion ordering counterbalanced
Autonomic Nervous System Activity within tasks. During both tasks, facial
Distinguishes Among Emotions behavior was recorded on videotape,
and second-by-second averages were ob-
Abstract. Emotion-specific activity in the autonomic nervous system was generat- tained for five physiological measures:
ed by constructing facial prototypes of emotion muscle by muscle and by reliving (i) heart rate-measured with bipolar
past emotional experiences. The autonomic activity produced distinguished not only chest leads with Redux paste; (ii) left-
between positive and negative emotions, but also among negative emotions. This and (iii) right-hand temperatures-mea-
finding challenges emotion theories that have proposed autonomic activity to be sured with thermistors taped to the pal-
undifferentiated or that have failed to address the implications of autonomic mar surface of the first phalanges of the
differentiation in emotion. middle finger of each hand; (iv) skin
resistance-measured with Ag-AgCl
For almost a century scientists have 2) presume undifferentiated autonomic electrodes with Beckman paste attached
argued about whether or not activity in arousal despite a number of reports of to the palmar surface of the middle pha-
the autonomic nervous system (ANS) is emotion-specific autonomic activity (3- langes of the first and third fingers of the
emotion-specific. Some of the most in- 5). We now report evidence of such nondominant hand; and (v) forearm flex-
fluential cognitive theories of emotion (1, specificity in an experiment designed to or muscle tension-measured with Ag-

Fig. 1. Frames from the videotape of one of the actor's performance of the fear prototype instructions: (A) "raise your brows and pull them
together," (B) "now raise your upper eyelids," (C) "now also stretch your lips horizontally, back toward your ears."
1208 SCIENCE, VOL. 221
AgCl electrodes with Redux paste and Skin directed facial action task we were able
High:
electronic integration of the electromyo- High - temperature Anger
to distinguish three subgroups of emo-
gram. / Low:tions (Fig. 2) on the basis of heart rate
Heart Fear
The directed facial action task com- rate and finger temperature differences (Fig.
Sad
prised six trials; in each a nonemotional 3). Additional differentiation in the re-
expression was performed and followed Low: lived emotions task enabled distinction
by an emotion-prototypic expression, Happy between sadness and other negative
Disgust
that is, an expression that theory and Surprise emotions on the basis of significantly
evidence indicate universally signals one larger decreases in skin resistance in
of the target emotions (6). Subjects were Fig. 2. Decision tree for discriminating emo- sadness [-12.6 ± 164.6 kilohm (8)] than
tions in direction facial action task.
not asked to produce an emotional in the others (fear, -0.37 ± 1.0 kilohm;
expression but instead were told precise- anger, -2.1 ± 3.7 kilohm; and disgust,
ly which muscles to contract (Fig. 1). +4.4 ± 6.6 kilohm).
Their attempts to follow these instruc- The nature of the emotion-specific There were also three negative find-
tions were aided by a mirror and coach- ANS activity was explored with t-tests ings of note. No significant differences
ing (by P.E.). The nonemotional expres- within significant univariate effects. Two were found between emotions on the
sion comprised two actions not included findings were consistent across tasks: (i) forearm flexor measure, thus indicating
in any of the emotional expressions to Heart rate increased more in anger that heart rate effects were not artifacts
control for ANS changes associated with (mean calculated across tasks + stan- of fist clenching or other related muscle
making any facial movement. Expres- dard error, +8.0 ± 1.8 beats per minute) activity. No statistically significant dif-
sions were held for 10 seconds. This task and fear (+8.0 ± 1.6 beats per minute) ferences were found between actors and
resembles a traditional emotion posing than in happiness (+2.6 ± 1.0 beats per scientists studying facial expression, in-
task (in which, for example, subjects are minute). (ii) Left and right finger dicating that the findings generalized to
asked to look fearful), but improves on it temperatures increased more in anger both of these populations. Finally, when
by precisely specifying for the subject, (left, +0.100C ± 0.0090; right, +0.080C ± the major analyses were rerun including
and for the experimenter's subsequent 0.008°) than in happiness (left, -0.07°C + all ANS data without regard to whether
verification, the exact set of muscle 0.0020; right, -0.03°C ± 0.002°). verification criteria were met, only the
movements that is required. Video rec- In addition to these differences be- negative versus positive emotional dis-
ords offacial expressions were measured tween the negative emotions of anger tinctions remained; all distinctions
(7) to ensure that autonomic data would and fear and the positive emotion of among negative emotions were lost. We
be included in the analyses only if the happiness, there were important differ- interpret this finding as supporting the
instructed set of actions had been made; ences among negative emotions. In the importance of verification of emotional
86.5 percent of the data were used.
In the relived emotion task, subjects
were asked to experience each of the six
emotions (in counterbalanced orders) by
12.0 0.16
reliving a past emotional experience for
30 seconds. This task resembles tradi-
tional imagery tasks, but more specifical- 10.0
ly focuses on reliving a past emotional
experience. After each trial, subjects rat- 0.12
ed the intensity of any felt emotion on a c 8.0 0-
scale from 0 to 8. Autonomic data were E 0
co
0

used only when the relived emotion was n CD


felt at the midpoint of the scale or greater .0
%
6.0
and when no other emotion was reported
at a similar strength; 55.8 percent of the Zs,0.0
Cu
data were used. ec 4.0
Change scores were computed for
each emotion on each task (directed fa- 4) 0.04
co Ee 0
cial action: averaged data during emo- I 2.0
tional face minus that during nonemo-
tional face; relived emotion: averaged
data during relived emotion minus that -2. 0
during the preceding 10-second rest peri-
od). The experiment was analyzed in a 2
by 2 by 6 (actors versus scientists by task
by emotion) multivariate analysis of vari- D co 0
_ ;

ance. Our hypothesis that there are auto-


0
-0.04 e &
0 %.. L C. 0)
nomic differences among the six emo- C
<
e) Cu3
CI)
cu
CD)
9
a < U. co
C
X u. -

co 0

tions was supported [emotion main ef- Emotions


fect, F(25, 317) = 2.51, P < 0.001].
There were differences in emotion-spe- Fig. 3. Changes in (A) heart rate and (B) right finger temperature during the directed facial
action task. Values are means + standard errors. For heart rate, the changes associated with
cific autonomic patterns between the anger, fear, and sadness were all significantly greater (P < 0.05) than those for happiness,
two eliciting tasks [task by emotion in- surprise, and disgust. For finger temperature, the change associated with anger was significant-
teraction, F(25, 62) = 2.0, P = 0.014]. ly different (P < 0.05) from that for all other emotions.
16 SEPTEMBER 1983 1209
state and as indicating one reason previ- its own right, begets the question of how changes in the central nervous system,
ous studies that failed to include verifica- that activity was generated. Particularly cognitive processes, motor behaviors,
tion procedures have been unable to intriguing is our discovery that produc- and the subjective experience of emo-
distinguish so many negative emotions. ing the emotion-prototypic patterns of tion; it also underscores the centrality of
Combining the results from the two facial muscle action resulted in autonom- the face in emotion as Darwin (11) and
tasks, this experiment provides the first ic changes of large magnitude that were Tomkins (12) suggested.
evidence (to our knowledge) of autonom- more clear-cut than those produced by PAUL EKMAN
ic differences among four negative emo- reliving emotions (a more naturalistic ROBERT W. LEVENSON*
tions (disgust and anger distinguished process). With this experiment we can- WALLACE V. FRIESEN
from each other and from fear or sadness not rule out the possibility that knowl- Department of Psychiatry,
in the directed facial action task; sadness edge of the emotion labels derived from University of California School of
distinguished from disgust, anger, or fear the facial movement instructions or see- Medicine, San Francisco 94143
in the relived emotion task) as well as ing one's own or the coach's face was
References and Notes
showing general distinctions between directly or indirectly responsible for the
positive and negative emotions in both effect. We find this unlikely since it 1. S. Schachter and J. E. Singer, Psychol. Rev. 69,
379 (1962).
tasks. In addition to this new evidence, would indicate either (i) that just viewing 2. G. Mandler, Mind and Emotion (Wiley, New
York, 1975).
we replicated with the directed facial an emotional face directly produced 3. A. F. Ax, Psychosom. Med. 15, 433 (1953).
action task the single most reliable find- autonomic patterning or (ii) that subjects 4. D. H. Funkenstein, S. H. King, M. Drolette.
ibid. 16, 404 (1954); R. A. Sternbach, J. Psycho-
ing from past studies: anger and fear inferred the "correct" set of autonomic som. Res. 6, 87 (1962).
show similar heart rate increases but changes from the label and then some- 5. R. J. Roberts and T. C. Weerts, Psychol. Rep.
50, 219 (1982).
differ in peripheral vascular function (in- how produced these complex patterns. 6. P. Ekman and H. Oster, Annu. Rev. Psychol.
dicated by our finding of colder fingers in The biofeedback literature (10) suggests 30, 527 (1979).
7. P. Ekman and W. V. Friesen, The Facial Action
fear than in anger). The magnitude of that people cannot voluntarily produce Coding System (Consulting Psychologists Press,
Palo Alto, Calif., 1978). To avoid directly sug-
these heart rate increases, both mean such complex patterns of autonomic ac- gesting emotion labels, we did not obtain emo-
(Fig. 3) and maximum (fear, +21.7; an- tivity. tion ratings, relying instead on facial measure-
ment for verification in this task.
ger, +25.3 beats per minute) are compa- We propose instead that it was con- 8. This large value reflects the influence of one
rable to other such findings (9). tracting the facial muscles into the uni- outlying subject.
9. Distinctions between fear and anger were based
Further research is needed to choose versal emotion signals which brought on changes in heart rate and diastolic blood
between two alternative explanations of forth the emotion-specific autonomic ac- pressure and on the magnitude of heart rate
change (3, 5). Increases were also comparable to
the differences in the results we obtained tivity. This might occur either through heart rate change in Schachter and Singer's (1)
with the two eliciting tasks: (i) the tasks peripheral feedback from making the fa- anger-epinephrine condition.
10. G. E. Schwartz [Science 175, 90 (1972)] reported
involve different neural substrates, cial movements, or by a direct connec- one of the few instances in which biofeedback
produced complex patterns of two ANS func-
which generate different patterns of emo- tion between the motor cortex and hypo- tions-heart rate and blood pressure. There are
tion-specific autonomic activity; or (ii) thalamus that translates between emo- a few reports of different patterns of one auto-
nomic and one somatic function (for example,
the tasks differ in the extent of emotion tion-prototypic expression in the face heart rate and respiration rate) [D. Newlin and
blending they produce. Further work is and emotion-specific patterning in the R. Levenson, Biol. Psychol. 7, 277 (1978)].
11. C. Darwin, The Expression of the Emotions in
also needed to demonstrate that emo- ANS. Although further studies are need- Man and Animals (Univ. of Chicago Press,
tion-specific autonomic activity is not ed to verify this hypothesis and to deter- Chicago, 1965).
12. S. S. Tomkins, Affect, Imagery, and Conscious-
unique to actors and scientists, although mine the pathways involved, the fact ness (Springer, New York, 1962).
13. We thank E. Callaway, R. Lazarus, P. Tannen-
the possibility that training in either pro- that emotion-specific autonomic activity baum, and S. Tomkins for their comments on
fession would have such a profound ef- occurred is of fundamental theoretical this report; the John D. and Catherine T. Mac-
Arthur Foundation for research funding; and L.
fect on autonomic patterning in emotion importance, no matter what the underly- Temoshok, J. Kamiya, and L. Zegans for the
seems unlikely. ing mechanisms may turn out to be. It use of equipment.
* Present address: Department of Psychology,
Our finding of emotion-differentiated raises the question of how such complex Indiana University, Bloomington 47405.
autonomic activity, albeit important in patterns of autonomic activity relate to 3 May 1983

1210 SCIENCE, VOL. 221

You might also like