Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

Journal of Anxiety Disorders 22 (2008) 56–66

Recognition of facial expressions in


obsessive–compulsive disorder
Kathleen M. Corcoran a, Sheila R. Woody a,*, David F. Tolin b
a
University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada
b
The Institute of Living, Hartford, CT, United States
Received 8 September 2006; received in revised form 22 December 2006; accepted 26 January 2007

Abstract
Sprengelmeyer et al. [Sprengelmeyer, R., Young, A. W., Pundt, I., Sprengelmeyer, A., Calder, A. J., Berrios, G., et al. (1997).
Disgust implicated in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, 264, 1767–1773] found that
patients with OCD showed severely impaired recognition of facial expressions of disgust. This result has potential to provide a
unique window into the psychopathology of OCD, but several published attempts to replicate this finding have failed. The current
study compared OCD patients to normal controls and panic disorder patients on ability to recognize facial expressions of negative
emotions. Overall, the OCD patients were impaired in their ability to recognize disgust expressions, but only 33% of patients
showed this deficit. These deficits were related to OCD symptom severity and general functioning, factors that may account for the
inconsistent findings observed in different laboratories.
# 2007 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Disgust perception; Obsessive–compulsive disorder; Facial expression recognition

1. Introduction sadness, and surprise (Ekman, Levenson, & Friesen,


1983; Ekman, Sorenson, & Friesen, 1969; Izard, 1971),
Accurate recognition of facial expressions is a a finding that is reliable across numerous procedural
critical element of humans’ social structure (Ekman, variations (Boucher & Carlson, 1980; Izard, 1971).
1992), serving as a guide for social behaviour. Even Despite this apparent universal ability, individuals
toddlers gain important information from the facial with some forms of psychopathology are impaired in
expressions of others (La Barbera, Izard, Vietze, & their recognition of facial expressions, although the
Parisi, 1976; Sorce, Emde, Campos, & Klinnert, 1985; specific quality and meaning of these deficits is still
Young-Browne, Rosenfeld, & Horowitz, 1977). Adults poorly understood. Researchers have documented
across various cultures recognize six basic facial abnormalities in facial expression recognition in
expressions of emotion: anger, disgust, fear, happiness, alcoholism (Kornreich et al., 2001a, 2001b), Alzhei-
mer’s disease (Hargrave, Maddock, & Stone, 2002),
anorexia nervosa (Zonnevijlle-Bendek, van Goozen,
Cohen-Kettenis, van Elburg, & van Engeland, 2002),
* Corresponding author at: Department of Psychology, University of
British Columbia, 2136 West Mall, Vancouver, BC, V6T 1Z4 Canada.
bipolar disorder (Ketter & Lembke, 2002), criminal
Tel.: +1 604 822 2719; fax: +1 604 822 6923. psychopathy (Kosson, Suchy, Mayer, & Libby, 2002),
E-mail address: swoody@psych.ubc.ca (S.R. Woody). social phobia (Simonian, Beidel, Turner, Berkes, &

0887-6185/$ – see front matter # 2007 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2007.01.003
K.M. Corcoran et al. / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 22 (2008) 56–66 57

Long, 2001), major depression (Rubinow & Post, 8 anxiety disorder controls, and 40 normal controls on
1992), schizophrenia (Addington & Addington, 1998; two tasks of facial expression recognition. OCD patients
Archer, Hay, & Young, 1992; Heimberg, Gur, Erwin, showed a marked deficit in recognition of disgust
Shtasel, & Gur, 1992), and obsessive–compulsive expressions and normal performance on expressions of
disorder (Sprengelmeyer et al., 1997), the subject of anger, fear, happiness, sadness, and surprise. Intrigu-
this investigation. ingly, Tourette’s patients with prominent obsessive–
Even in the case of depression and schizophrenia, compulsive behaviours demonstrated the disgust
which have been more extensively studied, the precise recognition impairment, while those without obses-
nature of the deficits is unclear. Some studies report sive–compulsive behaviours did not. Sprengelmeyer
generally impaired recognition of facial expressions et al. included a control task to rule out reluctance to
among those with major depression (Persad & Polivy, choose the label ‘‘disgust’’ on the part of individuals
1993) or schizophrenia (Lewis & Garver, 1995), which with OCD.
would suggest that the impairments reflect a general The findings presented by Sprengelmeyer et al.
deficit in face processing or overall neurocognitive (1997) have attracted attention not only for their pattern
functioning, rather than expression recognition per se of results but also for their magnitude. Every individual
(Addington & Addington, 1998; Bryson, Bell, & with OCD was impaired in the recognition of disgust,
Lysaker, 1997; Kerr & Neale, 1993). More typically, whereas no participant without clinically significant
however, results point to problematic recognition of obsessive–compulsive behaviours showed this impair-
specific facial expressions in depression (Mikhailova, ment. Moreover, OCD patients showed specific
Vladimirova, Iznak, Tsusulkovskaya, & Sushko, 1996; impairment in the recognition of disgust, rather than
Rubinow & Post, 1992) and schizophrenia (Davis & a general pattern of poor performance on the task. Some
Gibson, 2000; Dougherty, Bartlett, & Izard, 1974; observers have suggested that the effect may occur only
Kucharska-Pietura & Klimkowski, 2002; Muzekari & among a subset of individuals with OCD—perhaps
Bates, 1977; Walker, Marwit, & Emory, 1980). Notably, those with contamination concerns (Power & Dalgleish,
in a study with normal undergraduates, Rozin, Taylor, 1997; Woody & Tolin, 2002). Other researchers have
Ross, Bennette, and Hejmadi (2005) observed wide pointed to brain functioning in OCD, with imaging
variability across participants in general ability to results thus far pointing toward the basal ganglia and
classify emotions depicted in facial expression but no anterior insula (Phillips, Young et al., 1998; Phillips
evidence of individual differences in specific recogni- et al., 1997; Sprengelmeyer, Rausch, Eysel, & Przuntek,
tion deficits. 1998).
Although affect recognition deficits have been Despite the unusually strong effect observed by
observed in both schizophrenia and depression, the Sprengelmeyer and his colleagues, no other research
findings show important differences with potential team has replicated the result. Parker, McNally,
clinical implications. The impairments in the recogni- Nakayama, and Wilhelm (2004) used procedures that
tion of facial expressions are more severe in schizo- were very close to those used by Sprengelmeyer, with
phrenia than in depression (Feinberg, Rifkin, Schaffer, the addition of new models for the facial expressions.
& Walker, 1986; Heimberg et al., 1992). Impairment in They found no overall differences in performance
affect recognition appears to improve upon symptom between the OCD and normal control groups. Buhl-
remission in depression (Mikhailova et al., 1996) but mann, McNally, Etcoff, Tuschen-Caffier, and Wilhelm
not in schizophrenia (Addington & Addington, 1998; (2004) reported similar performance for individuals
Gaebel & Woelwer, 1992), although the deficits predict with OCD and normal controls on a recognition task of
poor treatment outcome in depression (Geerts & prototypical emotional expressions. Although Rozin
Bouhuys, 1998). et al. (2005) used a normal sample, they conducted an
In the case of obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD), analysis examining 26 individuals who scored in the
researchers have speculated that disgust may play a role clinical range on the Obsessive Compulsive Inventory
in some forms of the disorder, particularly contamina- (Foa, Kozak, Salkovskis, Coles, & Amir, 1998). These
tion-based types (Phillips, Senior, Fahy, & David, individuals actually recognized disgust expressions
1998a; Power & Dalgleish, 1997; Woody & Teachman, better than did participants with lower scores.
2000). In 1997, Sprengelmeyer et al. tested 12 The Sprengelmeyer et al. (1997) study was intriguing,
participants with OCD (primarily checking symptoms), but without replication it simply remains mysterious.
12 with Tourette’s Syndrome (five of whom also Several of the replication attempts described above were
showed prominent obsessive–compulsive behaviours), underpowered by ordinary standards, although the initial
58 K.M. Corcoran et al. / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 22 (2008) 56–66

Sprengelmeyer results yielded such large effects that structured clinical interview, clinical participants with a
even very small samples would be expected to replicate primary diagnosis of OCD or panic disorder (without
them. Parker et al. (2004) raised one interesting clue. history of OCD) were invited to participate in the study.
Although they failed to replicate the overall results, Many OCD patients also met criteria for major
Parker et al. described one individual, the most severe depression (32.5%), and some (13.9%) met criteria for
OCD patient in the sample, who showed marked an additional anxiety disorder diagnosis. Some in the
impairment in recognition of disgust, suggesting that panic disorder group also met criteria for major
severity of OCD may be an important element of the depression (5%) or another anxiety disorder (27.7%).
effect. The OCD and panic disorder groups did not differ with
Unfortunately, severity of symptoms was not regard to their DSM-IV Axis V level of functioning
detailed in the Sprengelmeyer et al. (1997) study. (GAF scale means were 56.29  8.04 and 57.93  7.79
The study also provided little information on other for the OCD and panic disorder groups, respectively),
diagnostic issues such as how diagnosis was deter- t(60) = 0.81, p > 0.40. As is typically the case, the main
mined, which comorbid conditions were present, or symptom varied across participants with OCD: check-
types of OCD symptoms beyond checking. The present ing/ordering (33.3%), contamination (22.2%), primary
study aimed to replicate the Sprengelmeyer et al. obsessions (i.e., sexual, blasphemous, or aggressive
finding, using a sample of individuals with OCD who obsessions in the absence of rituals; 22.2%), super-
were seeking treatment in anxiety specialty clinics as stitious rituals (8.3%), and hoarding (5.6%). Some
well as adding methodological controls such as participants (8.3%) presented with symptoms that did
structured clinical interviews for diagnosis and symp- not fit these categories. The mean age of onset of OCD
tom severity assessment. Because individuals with was 18.1 years (8.0).
depression show deficits in the recognition of facial Participants in the normal comparison group (n = 36)
expressions of emotion, we also examined the role of were recruited through advertisements located through-
comorbid depression. out the University of British Columbia campus. They
Although we adhered closely to the method were selected to balance the OCD sample for age and
presented by Sprengelmeyer et al. in many ways, we gender, and they were excluded from participation if
changed the stimuli to test the robustness of the finding. they endorsed a history of anxiety disorder or psychosis
This study used two models (one male and one female) or symptoms consistent with a current mood disorder.
from the Ekman and Friesen (1976) set, neither of Most of the participants were women (63.9%), with a
which was the one used by Sprengelmeyer et al. The similar proportion across groups, x2(2, N = 112) = 2.89,
most common error for Sprengelmeyer’s OCD parti- p > 0.20. Participants’ mean age was 34.0 years
cipants was to select anger in place of disgust when (S.D. = 11.1), with no group differences, F(2,
naming facial expressions, an error also observed in 108) = 2.47, p = 0.09. Of participants who reported
normal samples (Ekman & Friesen, 1976; Rozin et al., their ethnic or racial group, 75.9% identified themselves
2005). The Sprengelmeyer et al. stimuli were prepared as Caucasian, 10.7% identified as Asian, and 10.7%
by morphing each facial expression with two others to identified other racial or ethnic backgrounds. There was
increase the difficulty of the task. In the case of disgust a significant difference in distribution of ethnicity
stimuli, the expression was morphed with anger or across groups, x2(2, N = 109) = 7.32, p < 0.05, due to
sadness, but not fear. Stimuli in the current study the normal comparison group having a higher propor-
included all possible morphed combinations of disgust, tion of non-Caucasian individuals. The two clinical
anger, fear and sadness although only the prototypical groups did not differ on ethnic composition, x2(2,
facial expressions were analyzed. N = 74) = 1.86, p > 0.15.

2. Method 2.2. Measures

2.1. Participants 2.2.1. Diagnostic interviews


Diagnostic technicians on staff at two research-based
Three different samples were tested. Individuals with specialty anxiety clinics conducted the diagnostic
primary OCD (n = 40) and those with primary panic interviews for the clinical groups. Interviewers were
disorder (n = 36) were recruited from anxiety disorder well-trained master’s level technicians with several
specialty clinics in Vancouver, Canada and Hartford, years’ experience conducting diagnostic evaluations for
Connecticut. On the basis of an initial evaluation with a research trials. Because patients were being assessed in
K.M. Corcoran et al. / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 22 (2008) 56–66 59

preparation for participation in various treatment disgust-related behaviour (Rozin, Haidt, McCauley,
research projects, two different structured interviews Dunlop, & Ashmore, 1999).
were used: the Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedule
for DSM-IV (ADIS-IV; Brown, DiNardo, & Barlow, 2.3. Stimuli
1994) or the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV
Axis I Disorders (SCID-I; First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & From a set of black and white photographs shown by
Williams, 1997). The ADIS-IV is a semi-structured Ekman and Friesen (1976) to elicit reliable responses in
interview for diagnosis of anxiety, mood, somatoform, normal individuals, photographs of anger, disgust, fear,
and substance abuse disorders with good interrater and sadness expressions were selected for two models,
reliability for panic disorder and OCD (Brown, Di one male and one female. The model used in the
Nardo, Lehman, & Campbell, 2001). The SCID-I is the Sprengelmeyer et al. (1997) study was intentionally not
most widely used diagnostic interview in North selected for this study. The expressions happiness and
American psychiatric research (Antony & Barlow, surprise, which are easily recognized and rarely
2002). Earlier versions of the measure showed adequate confused with disgust, were not used.
interrater reliability for diagnosis of OCD (Williams Following the Sprengelmeyer procedure, stimuli for
et al., 1992). Screening questions from the SCID-I were this study were prepared using a computer interpolation
used as a diagnostic tool for the normal comparison program. Each facial expression was morphed with
group. every other expression in a procedure that yielded
expressions that blended features of two emotions, thus
2.2.2. Obsessive–compulsive symptoms increasing the difficulty level of the task. These
The Yale-Brown Obsessive–Compulsive Scale, a photographs, as well as the prototypical facial expres-
widely used measure with good reliability and validity sions, were presented to participants using a customized
(Goodman et al., 1989; Woody, Steketee, & Chambless, software program.
1995), was used to assess the severity of OCD
symptoms. The diagnostic technician administered 2.4. Procedure
the YBOCS following the structured diagnostic inter-
view for those who met criteria for OCD. The mean Prospective clinical participants were screened by
YBOCS Total score for OCD participants in this study telephone and subsequently completed their diagnostic
was 22.97 (S.D. = 4.58). Subscale scores were 11.76 interview and questionnaires at the clinic. Participants
(S.D. = 2.26) for Obsessions and 11.47 (S.D. = 3.10) for completed the facial recognition task at their first or
Compulsions. second visit to the clinic. Participants in the normal
comparison group completed the SCID-I screening
2.2.3. Depressive symptoms interview in a quiet non-clinic experimental room
The Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) is a com- before beginning the computer task.
monly used measure to evaluate severity of depressive The facial expression task involved four trials; in
symptoms in clinical and non-clinical samples. It has each trial participants classified the same 60 individual
excellent psychometric properties (Beck, Steer, & facial expressions presented in random order. Partici-
Garbin, 1988). The mean BDI score for the OCD pants indicated whether each expression most
group was 16.59 (S.D. = 10.57) and 11.58 (S.D. = 8.41) resembled anger, disgust, fear or sadness by using
for the panic disorder group, t(53) = 1.93, p < 0.06. the mouse to select one of these four answer choices
located below the photographs. The order of the answer
2.2.4. Disgust sensitivity choices was counterbalanced across blocks of trials.
The Disgust Scale (Haidt, McCauley, & Rozin, Each photograph remained on the computer screen until
1994) is a widely used broad index of sensitivity to a response was made and the participant clicked on a
disgust-eliciting stimuli across eight domains of disgust button labelled ‘‘Next,’’ which appeared below the
elicitors, such as food, body products, or mutilation. response options. The entire task took approximately
This measure elicits ratings of how disgusting particular 20 min.
experiences would be, such as, ‘‘You see maggots on a
piece of meat in an outdoor garbage pail.’’ The scale 3. Results
developers report good internal consistency and
evidence of discriminant validity (Haidt et al., 1994), The primary question of interest in this study was
and the measure relates moderately well (r = 0.51) to whether OCD patients differ from those with panic and
60 K.M. Corcoran et al. / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 22 (2008) 56–66

from normal controls in their ability to recognize facial Table 1


expressions of disgust. Frequency of correct facial Mean (and standard deviation) frequency of correct identification of
facial expressions of emotions by diagnostic group
expression identifications was tabulated for each
participant, across trials for each of the four pure facial Expression Participant group
expressions. Hit rate was the dependent variable in OCD Panic Normal
3  4 repeated measures ANOVA, with one within- Anger 22.14 (2.67) 21.69 (2.36) 21.61 (2.97)
subjects factor (stimulus expression: anger, disgust, Disgust 19.36 (4.89) 21.39 (4.03) 21.86 (3.21)
fear, and sadness) and one between-groups factor Fear 23.58 (1.00) 23.56 (1.13) 23.50 (1.16)
(diagnostic group: OCD, panic, and normal control).1 Sadness 21.81 (3.40) 22.81 (1.77) 22.36 (2.64)
This analysis was accompanied by planned nonortho- Note: These stimuli were taken directly from the Ekman and Friesen
gonal contrasts, described below. If OCD patients are set of emotional expressions; participants rated 24 of these photo-
specifically impaired in their recognition of the graphs.
expression of disgust, the analysis should show an
interaction of facial expression with diagnostic group. ing disgust was not different from their recognition
The Greenhouse-Geisser adjustment corrected for a of sadness (q = 2.72, p > 0.10) or anger (q = 0.59,
violation of the assumption of sphericity. In addition, p > 0.10). There was no main effect for diagnostic group,
due to the violation of sphericity, four participants from F(2, 105) = 1.65, p > 0.10, h2p ¼ 0:03.
the OCD group were randomly dropped from the
analyses of variance to form equal size groups 3.2. Planned comparisons
(Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001).2
Planned nonorthogonal contrasts were conducted in
order to test specific hypotheses not tested directly by
3.1. Analysis of variance the omnibus ANOVAs. Whereas the overall F-test
yields information about whether there are any
Table 1 presents the frequency of correct identifica- differences among the groups, planned contrasts permit
tion of the prototypical facial expressions for each specific predictions to be tested with relatively strong
diagnostic group; the maximum number of correct statistical power. The number of contrasts was limited to
responses was 24. Results of the ANOVA revealed a two in order to maintain control over Type I error
significant main effect of stimulus expression, F(2, (Rosenthal & Rosnow, 1985). The first contrast
226) = 19.16, p < 0.0001, h2p ¼ 0:15, which was qua- compared the three groups specifically on disgust
lified by a significant interaction, F(2, 226) = 2.70, recognition. The OCD group performed this task
p < 0.05, h2p ¼ 0:05. Simple main effects analyses significantly less accurately than the other two groups,
revealed a significant effect of stimulus expression for the F(1, 105) = 10.78, p < 0.01, r = 0.31. The second
OCD group, F(3, 226) = 11.32, p < 0.0001, h2p ¼ 0:13, contrast compared recognition of disgust facial expres-
and for the panic group, F(3, 226) = 3.70, p < 0.05, sions in the OCD group against all other emotional
h2p ¼ 0:05. Tukey’s method of post hoc comparisons expressions and other groups. The OCD group
revealed that the OCD group was significantly less recognized disgust expressions less accurately than
accurate at the recognition of disgust relative to all other all other groups and all other emotional expressions,
expressions (fear: q = 8.10, p < 0.001; anger: q = 5.33, F(1, 331) = 29.45, p < 0.00001, r = 0.28.
p < 0.01; sadness: q = 4.69, p < 0.01). In addition,
Tukey’s method revealed that panic patients recognized 3.3. Predictors of poor performance
fear significantly more accurately than they recognized
disgust (q = 4.16, p < 0.05). Their accuracy at recogniz- Fig. 1 shows the distribution of hit rates for
recognition of pure disgust expressions across the three
1
Analyses were also conducted using Wagner’s unbiased hit rate, groups. Most individuals in the panic and normal
HU. As the results were essentially unchanged, and hit rate is con- control groups showed excellent ability to recognize
ceptually easier to understand, only the analyses of hit rate are disgust, with the mean coming close to 100% accuracy
described. despite the presence of several individuals who
2
The analysis was additionally performed with the intact sample performed quite poorly. The distribution for the OCD
(i.e., without random removal of participants). Identical results were
found for the main effects and interactions, but some post hoc tests
group, however, was much more variable. In an effort to
could not be calculated accurately with unequal cell sizes, so the equal examine potential predictors of poor performance, OCD
cell size results are presented. participants were divided into two groups: those who
K.M. Corcoran et al. / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 22 (2008) 56–66 61

p < 0.01. Similar numbers of participants (<5) in each


group showed impaired recognition of facial expres-
sions other than disgust, but the distribution of these
deficits was equivalent across groups.
Using a series of t-tests, OCD patients who
performed normally on the pure expression task were
compared to those who showed a deficit in an effort to
discover descriptive or illness-related factors that
correlate with impaired recognition of disgust expres-
sions. Sample sizes differ for each t-test due to missing
data. Severity of symptoms was of particular interest,
given the results reported by Parker et al. (2004).
Symptom severity indicators used in these analyses
were: (a) Global Assessment of Functioning (GAF; the
Fig. 1. Hit rate for recognition of disgust facial expressions across Axis V indicator of general functioning and illness
diagnostic groups. severity gleaned from the diagnostic interview and rated
by the interviewer), (b) YBOCS Total Score, (c)
recognized pure (prototypical) facial expressions of YBOCS Compulsions subscale score, (d) YBOCS
disgust within normal limits and those who were clearly Obsessions subscale score, (e) BDI score, and (f)
impaired at recognizing disgust. Performing within duration of OCD in years. Participants’ current age was
normal limits was arbitrarily defined as demonstrating a also explored as a possible predictor, as age has been
hit rate within 1.5 standard deviations of the mean of the found to be related to performance on facial expression
normal comparison group. Twenty seven individuals judgment tasks (McDowell, Harrison, & Demaree,
fell into this group, with a mean hit rate of 22.56 1994). One-tailed tests were used for t-tests due to the a
(S.D. = 1.69). 13 individuals were clearly impaired, priori hypotheses that performance would be negatively
falling more than 1.5 standard deviations below the affected by greater severity of symptoms and higher
mean of the normal comparison group in the recognition age.
of prototypical facial expressions of disgust (hit rate Table 2 shows the results of this analysis. OCD
M = 12.92, S.D. = 3.43). patients with normal disgust recognition were sig-
Using this definition for impaired recognition nificantly more functional and less symptomatic than
performance described above, in addition to the 13 those who were impaired in their recognition of disgust
participants with impaired performance in the OCD facial expressions. OCD patients who were impaired on
group, three participants in the normal comparison the disgust recognition task had significantly higher
group and four participants in the panic group scores on the YBOCS (both subscales as well as the
demonstrated impaired functioning on this task. Their total score) and significantly lower GAF ratings. Results
observations can be seen as within-group outliers in of the t-tests on age of onset, duration of OCD, current
Fig. 1. This difference in distribution of impaired age, and disgust sensitivity did not reveal significant
performance was significant, x2(2, N = 112) = 9.19, differences.

Table 2
Comparison of individuals with OCD who showed normal versus impaired recognition of disgust facial expressions
Variable Normal mean (S.D.) Impaired mean (S.D.) t d.f. r
y
GAF 57.08 (7.66) 53.00 (8.54) 1.47 36 0.21
YBOCS total 21.96 (4.44) 25.08 (4.29) 2.02* 35 0.30
YBOCS obsessions 11.30 (2.27) 12.73 (2.00) 1.78* 32 0.25
YBOCS compulsions 10.74 (3.11) 13.00 (2.57) 2.09* 32 0.34
BDI 15.6 (8.97) 18.78 (13.87) 0.74 27 0.19
Duration of OCD 15.3 (11.33) 17.36 (12.56) 0.48 32 0.00
Age 34.12 (11.71) 36.62 (13.82) 0.59 37 0.02
Disgust sensitivity 16.51 (6.47) 15.79 (5.96) 0.27 24 0.12
Note: yp < 0.10, *p < 0.05 (one-tailed); GAF: Global Assessment of Functioning (SCID-I); YBOCS: Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale;
BDI: Beck Depression Inventory. Correlations refer to hit rates; all are in the expected direction.
62 K.M. Corcoran et al. / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 22 (2008) 56–66

3.4. Role of depression did not show the stark demarcation reported by
Sprengelmeyer et al. (1997). Recall that all of the
Examining the hit rates for pure disgust expressions, Sprengelmeyer et al. OCD patients showed a marked
which showed a larger effect size than the morphed deficit in recognizing disgust, and no participant
expressions, comorbid depression does not account without obsessive–compulsive symptoms showed the
for the poor recognition rates exhibited by the OCD deficit. In our sample, about a third of the participants
group. The correlation between recognition rate of pure with OCD were impaired in their recognition of disgust
disgust expressions and the BDI was r = 0.19. expressions.
Individuals who were impaired in their recognition What distinguishes OCD patients with impaired
of disgust were not more likely to be diagnosed recognition of disgust from those who perform the task
with current or partially remitted depression, x2(1, normally? Depression, frequently comorbid with OCD,
N = 40) = 1.05, p > 0.30. is associated with facial expression recognition deficits,
but recognition of disgust in this study was not
3.5. Role of symptom profile associated with depression symptoms or diagnosis.
Some researchers have proposed that individuals who
Individuals with OCD were classified according to are highly sensitive to contamination would be either
their primary symptom in an effort to discover whether hypersensitive or impervious to others’ expressions of
contamination concerns (which some researchers disgust, but these ideas were not supported. Patients
hypothesize would be more related to disgust percep- with primary contamination symptoms were no more
tion) or checking rituals (which was the primary likely than other patients to show the effect, and
symptom experienced by individuals in the Sprengel- sensitivity to experiencing disgust was unrelated to task
meyer et al. study) would be related to impaired performance. Nor was age of onset a factor in the effect,
recognition of disgust expressions. Among participants which would argue against this kind of learning
with primary contamination concerns, 22% showed explanation.
impaired performance on the task. In comparison, 18% Participants with OCD who showed poor recognition
of participants with primary checking rituals showed of disgust were more functionally impaired and
impaired performance. A larger percentage of indivi- reported more severe OCD symptoms than did those
duals with primary obsessions (45%) showed impaired who performed normally on the task. Severity of OCD
performance, but with such a small sample, this may be a critical factor behind previous failures to
difference was not statistically significant, x2(2, replicate the finding. Parker et al. (2004) reported one
N = 31) = 2.27, p > 0.30. Other types of OCD symp- patient who showed the disgust recognition deficit; this
toms were not analyzed because of low frequency. patient had the most severe symptoms in their sample.
Participants in the three primary symptom groups did The mean YBOCS score for our sample was a half
not differ with regard to severity of OCD symptoms, standard deviation higher than for the Parker et al.
F(2, 29) = 1.38, or global functioning, F(2, 30) = 1.77. sample. Power may also be a factor; our sample was
substantially larger than either the Parker et al. or
4. Discussion Buhlmann et al. (2004) samples.
An explanation is not immediately apparent for why
OCD patients were significantly less accurate at severity of OCD would be associated with a deficit in
recognizing facial expressions of disgust as compared to recognition of the specific facial expression of disgust.
the normal and panic disorder comparison groups. OCD Severity of symptoms has been associated with deficits
patients were also significantly less accurate in in facial information processing in schizophrenia
recognizing disgust in comparison to their recognition (Kohler, Bilker, Hagendoorn, Gur, & Gur, 2000;
of three other negative emotions. These results Martin, Baudouin, Tiberghien, & Franck, 2005). The
supported the initial hypotheses but are contrary to results are less consistent for depression, as facial
two published failures to replicate these deficits among expression recognition deficits have been correlated
patients with OCD (Buhlmann et al., 2004; Parker et al., with symptom severity in some studies (Mikhailova
2004). Given the difficulty of publishing negative et al., 1996) but not others (Leppänen, Milders, Bell,
results, the actual number of failed attempts to replicate Terriere, & Hietanen, 2004). Within OCD, severity is an
this result is probably higher. unreliable predictor of performance on other neuro-
Although the present study did find statistically cognitive tasks. In a recent review, about half of 22
significant differences between groups, the results studies examining the issue found performance on
K.M. Corcoran et al. / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 22 (2008) 56–66 63

neuropsychological tests to be related to severity the four used in the Parker et al. study (H. Parker,
(Kuelz, Hohagen, & Voderholzer, 2004). personal communication, November 1, 2005). Although
Imaging studies have highlighted specific neural variations in disgust expressions are reliably identifiable
substrates for perception of others’ disgust (Phillips (Rozin, Lowery, & Ebert, 1994), different facial muscle
et al., 1997; Phillips, Young et al., 1998b; Sprengel- patterns appear to characterize different levels of
meyer et al., 1998). Perception of facial expressions of intensity of disgust affect (Wolf et al., 2005). Among
disgust involve activation of the orbitofrontal cortex individuals with major depression, testing with varying
(Sprengelmeyer et al., 1998) and the anterior insula intensities of emotional expression shows subtle
(Phillips et al., 1997). These regions also appear to be impairments in discrimination accuracy (Surguladze
involved in the response to disgusting visual stimuli et al., 2004), something that has not been specifically
(Schienle, Stark, & Walter, 2002). The putamen, which tested within OCD. Quite possibly, the impairment in
is part of the basal ganglia, also seems to be involved in OCD is only observable when the facial stimulus has
perception of disgust expressions (Phillips et al., 1997; certain features that have yet to be defined. One recent
Sprengelmeyer et al., 1998). Researchers investigating innovation that may be worth exploring is the use of
the biological aspects of OCD have identified some of moving images of facial expressions (Kan, Mimura,
these same regions as of interest. Imaging studies have Kamijima, & Kawamura, 2004).
shown reliable differences between normal controls and A fruitful direction for research may be to explore
OCD patients in activity in the orbitofrontal cortex, similarities between Huntington’s disease and OCD.
especially the orbital gyrus, and in the basal ganglia, Huntington’s disease is characterized by a specific
especially the head of the caudate nucleus (Whiteside, deficit for recognition of facial expressions of disgust
Port, & Abramowitz, 2004). (Gray, Young, Barker, Curtis, & Gibson, 1997;
Nevertheless, many mysteries remain. The brain Sprengelmeyer et al., 1996). Huntington’s patients also
regions identified as being involved in perception of show deficits in perception of disgust in other
emotion do not appear to be specific to disgust. The modalities, such as vocal intonation or gustatory and
orbitofrontal cortex is also activated during perception olfactory perception (Mitchell, Heims, Neville, &
of facial expressions of fear and anger (Sprengelmeyer Rickards, 2005; Sprengelmeyer et al., 1996), but no
et al., 1998). The anterior insula, a part of the limbic extant studies have examined these areas of impair-
system that is believed to be involved emotional aspects ments in OCD. Furthermore, obsessions or compulsions
of sensory experience, is activated during response to often occur as a part of Huntington’s disease (Anderson,
pain and fear-inducing stimuli as well as revulsion Louis, Stern, & Marder, 2001; Naarding & Janzing,
(Schienle et al., 2002). Furthermore, there is no obvious 2003), as they do with Tourette’s syndrome.
explanation for the role of disorder severity. Neuroima- Disgust has only recently appeared as a topic of
ging studies differ with regard to whether OCD interest to anxiety disorders researchers, and research-
symptom severity correlates with brain activity. For ers have begun to examine various ways that disgust
example, Lacerda et al. (2003) reported correlations may be involved in OCD, including emotional
between symptom severity and regional cerebral blood experience in the disorder, motivators of avoidance
flow in bilateral inferior frontal lobes and the right basal and compulsions, and distinctions between OCD
ganglia. On the other hand, in a similarly powered study, subtypes. To use the phenomenon as an opportunity
van den Heuvel et al. (2005) reported that observed to understand neuroscience or perceptual aspects of
frontal–striatal dysfunction was independent of OCD psychopathology of OCD requires being able to
symptom severity. replicate the effect as well as understanding the
Although the results of this study do replicate the conditions under which it occurs. Although several
intriguing finding from the Sprengelmeyer et al. (1997), recent efforts to replicate the phenomenon have failed,
it is clear that the effect is not as robust as it originally the present study documents a partial replication and
appeared. The facial expression stimuli may play an some ideas for understanding the inconsistencies across
important role in the elusiveness of the effect. We studies.
specifically selected different models than the one
Sprengelmeyer’s group used. At the time we designed Acknowledgements
and collected data for the present study, we were
unaware of the parallel work being conducted by the This research was supported in part by a grant to the
two groups at Harvard (Buhlmann et al., 2004; Parker second author from the Social Sciences and Humanities
et al., 2004). By chance, our two models were among Research Council of Canada. We would like to thank
64 K.M. Corcoran et al. / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 22 (2008) 56–66

Wolfgang Linden and Mark Schaller for their helpful First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. W. (1997).
comments on an earlier version of this manuscript, Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV Axis I disorders.
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
which served as the first author’s master’s thesis. Foa, E. B., Kozak, M. J., Salkovskis, P. M., Coles, M. E., & Amir, N.
Finally, we gratefully acknowledge the assistance of the (1998). The validation of a new obsessive–compulsive disorder
late James Grossman, who prepared the stimuli. scale: the Obsessive Compulsive Inventory. Psychological Assess-
ment, 10, 206–214.
Gaebel, W., & Woelwer, W. (1992). Facial expression and emotional
References face recognition in schizophrenia and depression. European
Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 242, 46–52.
Addington, J., & Addington, D. (1998). Facial affect recognition and Geerts, E., & Bouhuys, N. (1998). Multi-level prediction of short-term
information processing in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. outcome of depression: non-verbal interpersonal processes, cog-
Schizophrenia Research, 32, 171–181. nitions and personality traits. Psychiatry Research, 79, 59–72.
Anderson, K. E., Louis, E. D., Stern, Y., & Marder, K. S. (2001). Goodman, W. K., Price, L. H., Rasmussen, S. A., Mazure, C.,
Cognitive correlates of obsessive and compulsive symptoms in Fleischmann, R. L., Hill, C. L., et al. (1989). Yale-Brown
Huntington’s disease. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 799– Obsessive Compulsive Scale: I. Development, use, and reliability.
801. Archives of General Psychiatry, 46, 1006–1011.
Antony, M. M., & Barlow, D. H. (Eds.). (2002). Handbook of Gray, J. M., Young, A. W., Barker, W. A., Curtis, A., & Gibson, D.
assessment and treatment planning for psychological disorders. (1997). Impaired recognition of disgust in Huntington’s disease
New York: Guilford Press. gene carriers. Brain, 120, 2029–2038.
Archer, J., Hay, D. C., & Young, A. W. (1992). Face processing in Haidt, J., McCauley, C., & Rozin, P. (1994). Individual differences in
psychiatric conditions. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 31, sensitivity to disgust: a scale sampling seven domains of disgust
45–61. elicitors. Personality and Individual Differences, 16, 701–713.
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Garbin, M. G. (1988). Psychometric Hargrave, R., Maddock, R. J., & Stone, V. (2002). Impaired recogni-
properties of the Beck depression inventory: twenty-five years of tion of facial expressions of emotion in Alzheimer’s disease.
evaluation. Clinical Psychology Review, 8, 77–100. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 14,
Boucher, J. D., & Carlson, G. E. (1980). Recognition of facial 64–71.
expression in three cultures. Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology, Heimberg, C., Gur, R. E., Erwin, R. J., Shtasel, D. L., & Gur, R. C.
11, 263–280. (1992). Facial emotion discrimination: III. Behavioral findings in
Brown, T. A., DiNardo, P., & Barlow, D. (1994). Anxiety disorders schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 42, 253–265.
interview schedule-IV (ADIS-IV). San Antonio, TX: The Psycho- Izard, C. E. (1971). The face of emotion. New York, NY: Appleton-
logical Corporation. Century-Crofts.
Brown, T. A., Di Nardo, P. A., Lehman, C. L., & Campbell, L. A. Kan, Y., Mimura, M., Kamijima, K., & Kawamura, M. (2004). Journal
(2001). Reliability of DSM-IV anxiety and mood disorders: of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry. Recognition of emo-
implications for the classification of emotional disorders. Journal tion from moving facial and prosodic stimuli in depressed patients,
of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 49–58. 75, 1667–1671.
Bryson, G., Bell, M., & Lysaker, P. (1997). Affect recognition in Kerr, S. L., & Neale, J. M. (1993). Emotion perception in schizo-
schizophrenia: a function of global impairment or a specific phrenia: specific deficit or further evidence of generalized poor
cognitive deficit. Psychiatry Research, 71, 105–113. performance? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 312–318.
Buhlmann, U., McNally, R. J., Etcoff, N. L., Tuschen-Caffier, B., & Ketter, T. A., & Lembke, A. (2002). Impaired recognition of facial
Wilhelm, S. (2004). Emotion recognition deficits in body emotion in mania. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 302–304.
dysmorphic disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 38, Kohler, C. G., Bilker, W., Hagendoorn, M., Gur, R. E., & Gur, R. C.
201–206. (2000). Emotion recognition deficit in schizophrenia: association
Davis, P. J., & Gibson, M. G. (2000). Recognition of posed and with symptomatology and cognition. Biological Psychiatry, 48,
genuine facial expressions of emotion in paranoid and nonpar- 127–136.
anoid schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 445– Kornreich, C., Blairy, S., Philippot, P., Dan, B., Foisy, M., Hess, U.,
450. et al. (2001a). Impaired emotional facial expression recognition in
Dougherty, F. E., Bartlett, E. S., & Izard, C. E. (1974). Responses of alcoholism compared with obsessive–compulsive disorder and
schizophrenics to expressions of the fundamental emotions. Jour- normal controls. Psychiatry Research, 102, 235–248.
nal of Clinical Psychology, 30, 243–246. Kornreich, C., Blairy, S., Philippot, P., Hess, U., Noel, X., Streel, E.,
Ekman, P. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. Cognition and et al. (2001b). Deficits in recognition of emotional facial expres-
Emotion, 6, 169–200. sion are still present in alcoholics after mid- to long-term absti-
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1976). Pictures of facial affect. Palo Alto, nence. Journal on Studies of Alcohol, 62, 533–542.
CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Kosson, D. S., Suchy, Y., Mayer, A. R., & Libby, J. (2002). Facial
Ekman, P., Levenson, R. W., & Friesen, W. V. (1983). Autonomic affect recognition in criminal psychopaths. Emotion, 2, 398–
nervous system activity distinguishes among emotions. Science, 411.
221, 1208–1210. Kucharska-Pietura, K., & Klimkowski, M. (2002). Perception of facial
Ekman, P., Sorenson, E. R., & Friesen, W. V. (1969). Pan-cultural affect in chronic schizophrenia and right brain damage. Acta
elements in facial displays of emotion. Science, 164, 86–88. Neurobiologia, 62, 33–43.
Feinberg, T. E., Rifkin, A., Schaffer, C., & Walker, E. (1986). Facial Kuelz, A. K., Hohagen, F., & Voderholzer, U. (2004). Neuropsycho-
discrimination and emotional recognition in schizophrenia and logical performance in obsessive–compulsive disorder: a critical
affective disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 43, 276–279. review. Biological Psychology, 65, 185–236.
K.M. Corcoran et al. / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 22 (2008) 56–66 65

La Barbera, J. D., Izard, C. E., Vietze, P., & Parisi, S. A. (1976). Four- Rozin, P., Lowery, L., & Ebert, R. (1994). Varieties of disgust faces
and six-month-old infants’ visual responses to joy, anger, and and the structure of disgust. Journal of Personality and Social
neutral expressions. Child Development, 47, 535–538. Psychology, 66, 870–881.
Lacerda, A. L. T., Dalgalarrondo, P., Caetano, D., Camargo, E. E., Rozin, P., Taylor, C., Ross, L., Bennette, G., & Hejmadi, A. (2005).
Etchebehere, E. C. S. C. , & Soares, J. C. (2003). Elevated thalamic General and specific abilities to recognise negative emotions,
and prefrontal regional cerebral blood flow in obsessive–compul- especially disgust, as portrayed in the face and the body. Cognition
sive disorder: a SPECT study. Psychiatry Research: Neuroima- and Emotion, 19, 397–412.
ging, 123, 125–134. Rubinow, D. R., & Post, R. M. (1992). Impaired recognition of affect
Leppänen, J. M., Milders, M., Bell, J. S., Terriere, E., & Hietanen, J. K. in facial expression in depressed patients. Biological Psychiatry,
(2004). Depression biases the recognition of emotionally neutral 31, 947–953.
faces. Psychiatry Research, 128, 123–133. Schienle, A., Stark, R., & Walter, B. (2002). The insula is not
Lewis, S. F., & Garver, D. L. (1995). Treatment and diagnostic subtype specifically involved in disgust processing: an fMRI study. Neu-
in facial affect recognition in schizophrenia. Journal of Psychia- roreport: For Rapid Communication of Neuroscience Research,
tric Research, 29, 5–11. 13, 2023–2036.
Martin, F., Baudouin, J.-Y., Tiberghien, G., & Franck, N. (2005). Simonian, S. J., Beidel, D. C., Turner, S. M., Berkes, J. L., & Long, J.
Processing emotional expression and facial identity in schizo- H. (2001). Recognition of facial affect by children and adolescents
phrenia. Psychiatry Research, 134, 43–53. diagnosed with social phobia. Child Psychiatry and Human
McDowell, C. L., Harrison, D. W., & Demaree, H. A. (1994). Is right Development, 32, 137–145.
hemisphere decline in the perception of emotion a function of Sorce, J. F., Emde, R. N., Campos, J. J., & Klinnert, M. D. (1985).
aging? International Journal of Neuroscience, 79, 1–11. Maternal emotional signaling: its effect on the visual cliff
Mikhailova, E. S., Vladimirova, T. V., Iznak, A. F., Tsusulkovskaya, E. behavior of 1-year-olds. Developmental Psychology, 21, 195–
J., & Sushko, N. V. (1996). Abnormal recognition of facial 200.
expression of emotions in depressed patients with major depres- Sprengelmeyer, R., Rausch, M., Eysel, U. T., & Przuntek, H. (1998).
sion disorder and schizotypal personality disorder. Biological Neural structures associated with recognition of facial expressions
Psychiatry, 40, 697–705. of basic emotions. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London,
Mitchell, I. J., Heims, H., Neville, E. A., & Rickards, H. (2005). Series B, 265, 1927–1931.
Huntington’s disease patients show impaired perception of disgust Sprengelmeyer, R., Young, A. W., Calder, A. J., Karnat, A., Lange, H.,
in the gustatory and olfactory modalities. The Journal of Neu- Homberg, V., et al. (1996). Loss of disgust—perception of faces
ropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 17, 119–121. and emotions in Huntington’s disease. Brain, 119(Part 5), 1647–
Muzekari, L. H., & Bates, M. E. (1977). Judgment of emotion among 1665.
chronic schizophrenics. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 33, 662– Sprengelmeyer, R., Young, A. W., Pundt, I., Sprengelmeyer, A.,
666. Calder, A. J., Berrios, G., et al. (1997). Disgust implicated in
Naarding, P., & Janzing, J. G. E. (2003). The neuropsychiatric obsessive–compulsive disorder. Proceedings of the Royal Society
manifestations of Huntington’s disease. Current Opinion in Psy- of London, 264, 1767–1773.
chiatry, 16, 337–340. Surguladze, S. A., Young, A. W., Senior, C., Brébion, G., Travis, M. J.,
Parker, H. A., McNally, R. J., Nakayama, K., & Wilhelm, S. (2004). & Phillips, M. L. (2004). Recognition accuracy and response bias
No disgust recognition deficit in obsessive–compulsive disorder. to happy and sad facial expressions in patients with major depres-
Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 35, sion. Neuropsychology, 18, 212–218.
183–192. Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2001). Using multivariate statistics
Persad, S. M., & Polivy, J. (1993). Differences between depressed and (4th ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon.
nondepressed individuals in the recognition of and response to van den Heuvel, O. A., Veltman, D. J., Groenewegen, H. J., Cath,
facial emotional cues. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 358– D. C., van Balkom, A. J. L. M. , van Hartskamp, J., et al.
368. (2005). Frontal-striatal dysfunction during planning in obses-
Phillips, M. L., Senior, C., Fahy, T., & David, A. S. (1998a). Disgust— sive–compulsive disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 6,
the forgotten emotion of psychiatry. British Journal of Psychiatry, 301–310.
172, 373–375. Walker, E., Marwit, S., & Emory, E. (1980). A cross-sectional study of
Phillips, M. L., Young, A. W., Scott, S. K., Calder, A. J., Andrew, C., emotion recognition in schizophrenics. Journal of Abnormal
Giampietro, V., et al. (1998b). Neural responses to facial and vocal Psychology, 89, 428–436.
expressions of fear and disgust. Proceedings of the Royal Society Whiteside, S. P., Port, J. D., & Abramowitz, J. S. (2004). A meta-
of London, Series B, 265, 1809–1817. analysis of functional neuroimaging in obsessive–compulsive
Phillips, M. L., Young, A. W., Senior, C., Brammer, M., Andrew, C., disorder. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 132, 69–79.
Calder, A. J., et al. (1997). A specific neural substrate for Williams, J. B., Gibbon, M., First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Davies, M.,
perceiving facial expressions of disgust. Nature, 389, 495–498. Borus, J., et al. (1992). The Structured Clinical Interview for
Power, M., & Dalgleish, T. (1997). Cognition and emotion: from order DSM-III-R (SCID): II. Multisite test-retest reliability. Archives of
to disorder. East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press. General Psychiatry, 49, 630–636.
Rosenthal, R., & Rosnow, R. L. (1985). Contrast analysis: focused Wolf, K., Mass, R., Ingenbleek, T., Kiefer, F., Naber, D., & Wiede-
comparisons in the analysis of variance. New York, NY: Cam- mann, K. (2005). The facial pattern of disgust, appetence, excited
bridge University Press. joy and relaxed joy: an improved facial EMG study. Scandinavian
Rozin, P., Haidt, J., McCauley, C., Dunlop, L., & Ashmore, M. (1999). Journal of Psychology, 46, 403–409.
Individual differences in disgust sensitivity: comparisons and Woody, S. R., Steketee, G., & Chambless, D. L. (1995). Reliability and
evaluations of paper-and-pencil versus behavioral measures. Jour- validity of the Yale-Brown Obsessive–Compulsive Scale. Beha-
nal of Research in Personality, 33, 330–351. viour Research and Therapy, 33, 597–605.
66 K.M. Corcoran et al. / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 22 (2008) 56–66

Woody, S. R., & Teachman, B. (2000). Intersection of disgust and fear: Young-Browne, G., Rosenfeld, H. M., & Horowitz, F. D. (1977).
normal and pathological views. Clinical Psychology: Science and Infant discrimination of facial expressions. Child Development,
Practice, 7, 291–311. 48, 555–562.
Woody, S. R., & Tolin, D. F. (2002). The relationship between Zonnevijlle-Bendek, M. J. S., van Goozen, S. H. M., Cohen-Kettenis,
disgust sensitivity and avoidant behavior: studies of clinical P. T., van Elburg, A., & van Engeland, H. (2002). Do adolescent
and nonclinical samples. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 16, anorexia nervosa patients have deficits in emotional functioning?
543–559. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 11, 38–42.

You might also like