Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

Energy Policy 93 (2016) 335–344

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Energy Policy
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/enpol

Monetary or environmental appeals for saving electricity? –Potentials


for spillover on low carbon policy acceptability
Julia Steinhorst n, Ellen Matthies
Institute of Psychology I, Department of Environmental Psychology, Otto-von-Guericke University Magdeburg, PO Box 4120, 39016 Magdeburg, Germany

H I G H L I G H T S

 Policy acceptability may be influenced by type of framing and individual factors.


 We examined long-term spillover effects in association with type of framing.
 Framing (environmental vs. monetary) interacts with personal ecological norms.
 For strong personal ecological norms, environmental framing increases acceptability.
 Also, strong personal ecological norms increase low carbon policy acceptability.

art ic l e i nf o a b s t r a c t

Article history: The acceptability of low carbon policies is an important precondition for energy system transitions, such
Received 16 June 2015 as the German Energiewende. This long-term experimental study examines the potential for behavioural
Received in revised form spillover on the acceptability of low carbon policies, caused by a framed intervention to promote elec-
3 February 2016
tricity saving behaviour. Clients of a German energy provider were randomly assigned to continuously
Accepted 16 March 2016
Available online 25 March 2016
receive electricity saving tips with either monetary framing (saving potential in €) or environmental
framing (saving potential in CO2). The control group did not receive any information. In two follow-up
Keywords: surveys, four (N¼ 333) and nine months (N ¼258) later, participants rated the acceptability of several low
Monetary framing carbon policies. A pre-survey assessed the personal ecological norm for saving electricity. Participants
Environmental framing
with strong personal ecological norms reported generally higher policy acceptability. After environ-
Policy acceptability
mental framing they also indicated higher acceptability compared to the monetary framing or control
Personal norms
Low carbon policies group. These results indicate that information campaigns should be designed carefully in order to pro-
mote positive spillover effects. Environmental framing of private-sphere behaviour can increase the
disposition for further pro-environmental behaviour in the public sphere, e.g. policy acceptability. When
appealing to monetary benefits in pro-environmental behaviour, there is a risk of inhibiting positive
spillover effects.
& 2016 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction (compared to 1990) will be reached in 2050 (German Federal


Ministry for Economic Affairs and Energy [BMWi], 2014). As a
Since the mid-1990s the German government has decided to challenging long-range project, the Energiewende incorporates a
introduce changes in the national energy system in order to sig- wide range of low carbon policies that serve two core objectives:
nificantly reduce carbon emissions. In the last couple of years the expansion of renewable energy sources and energy efficiency.
Germany has begun to undergo a considerable transformation of Low carbon policies therefore range from regulatory policies for
its energy infrastructure, including a nuclear phase-out and a reducing the overall energy demand (e.g. by reducing household
carbon emissions) to infrastructural changes, such as expanding
significant extension of its renewable energy supply. This process,
renewable energy power plants, power lines and power storage
the so-called German Energy Transition, or Energiewende, will
facilities (Morris and Pehnt, 2012). Below, we use the term low
bring about challenges in all kinds of sectors until the ambitious
carbon policies for policy instruments that pursue carbon emission
goal of a 80–95% reduction of greenhouse gas emissions reduction.
An essential precondition for a sustainable energy transition
n
Corresponding author. and its policy measures is their acceptance and support by the
E-mail address: Julia.Steinhorst@ovgu.de (J. Steinhorst). German population. Policy support is defined as the “tacit

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.enpol.2016.03.020
0301-4215/& 2016 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
336 J. Steinhorst, E. Matthies / Energy Policy 93 (2016) 335–344

endorsement of or willingness to accept environmental measures knowledge, there is no experimental evidence on this form of
and regulations” (Poortinga et al., 2004, p. 76). Similarly, policy spillover yet.
acceptability is characterised as an “evaluative judgement” towards The present study investigates the spillover effects due to a
a new technology or policy instruments (Huijts et al., 2012, p. 527). framed intervention aimed at household electricity saving on the
Thus, civil society plays a prominent role in the transformation acceptability of low carbon policies. To the best of our knowledge,
process. On the one hand, it became apparent that local resistance the present study is the first approach to address the influence of
can lead to cancellation or delay e.g. of onshore and offshore wind framing on low carbon policy acceptability and the occurrence of
farms (Devine-Wright, 2005; Jones and Eiser, 2009), biogas facil- pro-environmental spillover on this high impact pro-environ-
ities (Upham and Shackley, 2007) and the construction of sus- mental behaviour. By means of an experimental long-term design
tainable energy technologies in general (Schweizer-Ries, 2008; we examine spillover effects of the most commonly used strate-
Wüstenhagen et al., 2007). On the other hand, reducing the energy gies for promoting pro-environmental behaviour: monetary and
consumption of private households is an important means of the environmental framing, while taking into account the influence of
German Energiewende. initial personal ecological norms.
In order to ensure and promote the public acceptability of en-
ergy system transitions, such as the German Energiewende with a 1.1. Spillover effects on policy acceptability related to type of framing
perspective of more than 50 years, it is important to study pro-
moting and inhibiting factors, preferably in the long run. Re- The spillover hypothesis was first introduced by Scott (1977).
peatedly, simple and easy pro-environmental behaviour in the As postulated, it has been shown that the performance of pro-
private-sphere has been suggested as a “wedge” or catalytic be- environmental behaviour (Thøgersen, 1999) or its promotion in
haviour which can promote other environmentally relevant be- light of environmental values or goals (Evans et al., 2013; Spence
haviours (see Thøgersen and Crompton, 2009). There is first evi- et al., 2014) can increase the disposition to show further pro-en-
dence, that this behaviour can positively affect high-impact be- vironmental behaviour in the future. Nevertheless, positive spil-
haviour in the public sphere, such as the acceptability of low lover cannot be taken for granted and may appear under certain
carbon policies (Thøgersen and Noblet, 2012). The term positive circumstances only (for reviews see Thøgersen and Crompton,
spillover describes the phenomenon that an increase in a particular 2009; Truelove et al., 2014). Empirical evidence shows that it is
pro-environmental behaviour may result in the performance of usually of small to modest size (Thøgersen and Ölander, 2003).
other kinds of pro-environmental behaviour. Yet, even among Chances increase with the perceived similarity of behaviours
private-sphere behaviours, only a handful of studies have reported (Thøgersen, 2004), therefore, if the actor believes that both serve a
indications for positive spillover, mostly in similar behavioural pro-environmental goal.
domains (Thøgersen, 2004) and with spillover effects of small to Also, most indications for positive spillover effects have been
modest size (Thøgersen and Ölander, 2003). found among different private-sphere behaviours (e.g. Evans et al.,
In order to trigger positive spillover, it is highly important how 2013; Spence et al., 2014; Steinhorst et al., 2015; Thøgersen, 1999,
the “wedge” or catalytic behaviour is promoted. Environmental 2004; Thøgersen and Ölander, 2003). Stern et al. (1999) introduced
campaigners, businessmen and politicians often choose to en- the term private-sphere behaviour for pro-environmental beha-
courage pro-environmental behaviour by means of monetary ar- viour carried out in the private sphere (e.g. the purchase, use and
guments, thereby hoping for changes in a range of further pro- disposal of personal and household products) in contrast to pro-
environmental behaviour (see Thøgersen and Crompton, 2009). environmental behaviour in the public sphere (such as policy
Current studies, however, have revealed the risk of framing pro- support or acceptability). According to Stern (2000), public-sphere
environmental behaviour with monetary reasons or arguments. behaviours are of higher importance for environmental change
Namely: monetary framing of the source behaviour can limit po- because “although these behaviours affect the environment only
sitive spillover and environmental framing itself can increase the indirectly, by influencing public policies, the effects may be large,
disposition to show further pro-environmental behaviour (Evans because public policies can change the behaviours of many people
et al., 2013; Steinhorst et al., 2015). Monetary appeals for pro-en- and organisations at once” (p. 409). Our study focusses on a form
vironmental behaviour, which is a commonly used strategy in of spillover usually not investigated: from private-sphere beha-
environmental campaigning, can counteract the original purpose viour (here: saving electricity) to public-sphere behaviour (here:
of the campaign (to trigger a wide range of pro-environmental the acceptability of low carbon policies). Up to now, only a few
behaviour) by limiting pro-environmental behavioural change to entirely correlational studies point to this form of behavioural
the targeted behaviour. In this context, Thøgersen and Crompton spillover: In the environmental context, private-sphere behaviour
(2009) point out that “It is crucially important to examine the ef- has been shown to positively correlate with policy support (Stern
fect of public campaigns aimed at encouraging individuals to et al., 1999; Thøgersen and Noblet, 2012; Tobler et al., 2012; Willis
modify their behaviour in simple ways that serve to reduce per- and Schor, 2012). For instance, in a study by Thøgersen and Noblet
sonal environmental impact: to what extent do such campaigns (2012) everyday “green” behaviour significantly contributed to
contribute to building public acceptance of, and demand for, far- predicting the acceptance of wind power when controlling for
reaching government interventions?”(p. 142). environmental concern.
While psychologists mainly focus on pro-environmental be- Yet, the reasons for the occurence of positive spillover have not
haviour in the private sphere (e.g. reducing electricity consump- been completely uncovered. In regard to pro-environmental be-
tion or car use), there is a need for research on how most common haviour, it has been suggested that people who show one pro-
intervention strategies (informational measures) influence high- environmental behaviour will be likely to strive for consistency by
impact pro-environmental behaviour in the public sphere, such as acting pro-environmentally in other areas as well (Thøgersen,
policy acceptability. To date, only few studies have examined this 2004). This recurrent explanation for spillover effects is based on
research question (e.g. Bolderdijk et al., 2013; Boomsma and Steg, the cognitive dissonance theory (Aronson, 1997). According to
2014; Walker et al., 2014). Also, research on factors that trigger Aronson individuals who are confronted with inconsistency be-
positive spillover effects from pro-environmental private-sphere tween their self-concept and a cognition about their behaviour
behaviour to public-sphere behaviour, such as the acceptability of will experience some kind of mental stress or discomfort. Conse-
policy measures is purely correlational (Thøgersen and Noblet, quently they will be motivated to reduce this dissonance, for ex-
2012; Tobler et al., 2012; Willis and Schor, 2012). To the best of our ample by acting in accordance with their self-concept.
J. Steinhorst, E. Matthies / Energy Policy 93 (2016) 335–344 337

This explanation for cognitive dissonance is built on the im- personal norms strongly influence all kinds of behaviours per-
plicit assumption that all people strive to perceive themselves as a formed with pro-environmental intention. Based on the value-
moral (e.g. pro-environmental) person (Thøgersen, 2004) and to belief-norm theory (Stern et al., 1999) several studies demon-
appear as a morally integer person to others (see Truelove et al., strated that the acceptability of political measures is significantly
2014). Another possibility may be that people more likely perceive influenced by personal norms, e.g. in the context of transport
themselves as pro-environmental after performing behaviour in policies (Eriksson et al., 2006, 2008), low carbon policies (De Groot
light of a pro-environmental motivation. The self-perception the- and Steg, 2009; Steg et al., 2005) and pro-environmental policies
ory (Bem, 1972) posits that people infer their attitudes from past in general (Poortinga et al., 2004). Thus, we conclude the following
behaviour. In line with this theory, if past behaviour is introduced assumption.
in the light of pro-environmental motives, a person will reattribute
H2. Individuals with a strong personal ecological norm for redu-
the performance of this behaviour to a pro-environmental attitude
cing electricity consumption also show a higher acceptability of
and thus be motivated to act in line with this attitude in the future.
low carbon policies, such as infrastructural changes and subsidies
As a consequence, chances for positive spillover between pro-en-
for reducing household carbon emissions.
vironmental behaviour may increase with environmental framing
or labelling, demonstrated in regard to private-sphere behaviour
(Cornelissen et al., 2008; Evans et al., 2013; Spence et al., 2014). If 1.3. How do framing effects interact with personal ecological norms?
past behaviour is introduced in light of a monetary motivation,
however, a person will be motivated to act in line with this mo- So far, it has been shown that positive spillover does vary with
tivation as well. Therefore this person will more likely show fur- the strength of a person's pro-environmental norms (Thøgersen,
2004; Thøgersen and Ölander, 2003). Furthermore, there are in-
ther behaviour with monetary benefits, often not inherent in pro-
dications that the type of framing can trigger positive spillover on
environmental behaviour. As a consequence, chances of positive
the acceptability of low carbon policies (see Section 1.1). Both
spillover on further pro-environmental behaviour will be inhibited
spillover mechanisms can be traced back to the fact that pro-en-
by monetary framing, which has been demonstrated in regard to
vironmental goals are more accessible and salient to a person with
private-sphere behaviour already (Evans et al., 2013; Steinhorst
strong or activated pro-environmental norms. Thus, both me-
et al., 2015).
chanisms may possibly interact and result in an even stronger
Hence, the effects of monetary or environmental framing on
spillover effect. In support for this reasoning, a study by Dickerson
pro-environmental behaviour should differ considerably, depend-
et al. (1992) was able to show that an induced salience for a dis-
ing on the motive that was made salient, as outlined above. In the
crepancy between a person's norm for pro-environmental beha-
tradition of Levin, Schneider & Gaeth`s(1998) specification of goal
viour and actual behaviour can lead to pro-environmental beha-
framing, we refer to the term framing as an appeal to one of several
viour change. According to Chong and Druckman (2007, p. 111)
possible motives or reasons for performing a specific behaviour.
“Framing can work on […] three levels, by making new beliefs
Framing is understood to work by priming behaviour with certain
available about an issue, making certain available beliefs acces-
values or motives or making motives or values salient (Evans et al.,
sible, or making beliefs applicable or “strong” in people's
2013; Spence et al., 2014). Recipients of framing will consequently
evaluations”.
be more likely to perform behaviour that is in line with these
The cognitive dissonance theory (Aronson, 1997), as mentioned
motives or goals (Maio et al., 2009; Vansteenkiste et al., 2004).
above, states that an inconsistency appears between a person's
These results may also apply to the acceptability of low carbon
self-concept and a cognition about own behaviour will result in an
policies, substantiated by the fact that pro-environmental values
aspiration to reduce the inconsistency. In reference to framing, this
are positively related to pro-environmental policy support
means that the pro-environmental framing of electricity saving
(Bolderdijk et al., 2013; Poortinga et al., 2004; Steg et al., 2005) and
behaviour can activate a person's cognition of perceiving elec-
renewable energy sources (Steg et al., 2015).
tricity saving behaviour as pro-environmental. After performing
H1. Against the background of the presented literature, we pos- this behaviour, a person will be more willing to act consistently on
tulate that (repeated) environmental framing of electricity saving further pro-environmental behaviour types, such as the accept-
tips increases the acceptability of low carbon policies, such as in- ability of low carbon policies. The cognitive dissonance theory
frastructural changes and subsidies for reducing household carbon (Aronson, 1997) also posits that the desire to reduce the incon-
emissions. This positive influence is not expected for monetary sistency will even be stronger, if it is perceived as important, be-
framing. cause it threatens the self-concept of a person. Therefore, in-
dividuals with strong personal norms will be more receptive to
1.2. The role of personal ecological norms for policy acceptability this inconsistency.
Thus we can conclude: When the initial personal norm for a
Apart from the positive influence of environmental framing on specific pro-environmental behaviour is strong or made salient by
behavioural spillover, studies by Thøgersen and Ölander (2003) framing, chances for positive spillover on further pro-environ-
and Thøgersen (2004) suggest that strong personal norms for pro- mental behaviour, such as low carbon policy acceptability will
environmental behaviour increase the likelihood for positive increase. This effect should be even stronger when both conditions
spillover from one pro-environmental behaviour (e.g. saving apply. Otherwise, if the personal ecological norm is not strong or
electricity) to another (e.g. policy acceptability). Personal norms does not become salient (in the case of monetary framing) this will
can be described as feelings of moral obligation, which are gen- not lead to a feeling of inconsistency, not causing a motivation to
erated by the activation of a person's internalised moral standards act in accordance to one's personal ecological norm.
or values (Schwartz, 1977; Schwartz and Howard, 1981). According Empirical evidence so far supports this reasoning. A study by
to the norm activation theory (Schwartz, 1977; Schwartz and Ho- Parag et al. (2011) found evidence for a moderating effect of atti-
ward, 1981) these feelings of moral obligation can be experienced tude on the influence of carbon emission framing (mixed with
due to a person's self-expectation to show certain behaviour in a monetary arguments) on intentions for the target behaviour (en-
particular situation. More specifically, personal ecological norms ergy saving), when compared to a purely monetary frame. Fur-
result from a person's self-expectation to behave environmental thermore, environmental information congruent with pro-en-
friendly (see Hunecke et al., 2001). According to Stern et al. (1999), vironmental values has been found to interact with environmental
338 J. Steinhorst, E. Matthies / Energy Policy 93 (2016) 335–344

framing: For instance, Boomsma and Steg (2014) observed an in- 2.1. Procedure
creased acceptability of lower street lighting levels for people with
strong biospheric values when provided with value-congruent Online clients of a local energy provider in a German city in the
pro-environmental information compared to no information. Thus, Ruhr-area were invited to participate in an electricity saving pro-
the kind of information did not affect acceptability ratings in ject. For participation they registered in a web portal. As incentives
people with weak biospheric values (Boomsma and Steg, 2014). for taking part in the project we offered tailored electricity saving
Similarly, in a study by Bolderdijk et al. (2013) environmental in- tips and a lottery for vouchers from an online retailer.
formation (a movie on bottled water) showed no effect compared Prior to the intervention, participants were randomly assigned
to neutral information, but positively interacted with biospheric to three different groups: two experimental groups which received
values and strengthened behavioural intentions and policy ac- electricity saving tips accompanied either by monetary framing or
environmental framing and a control group which received no
ceptability if congruent values were strong. If congruent values
information. The intervention consisted of a short text paragraph
were weak, however, there was no significant interaction effect
introducing a problem situation and five to ten electricity saving
(Bolderdijk et al., 2013).
tips accompanied by the annual saving potential in Euro (mone-
Given this empirical evidence, we may infer that, similarly,
tary framing) or CO2 (environmental framing). The monetary
personal norms will interact with environmental framing effects.
framing text explained the rise of energy prices in Germany and
As we mentioned earlier, activated values or moral standards are
how saving electricity may help to reduce the financial burden for
understood as a precondition for feelings of moral obligation (or
households. The environmental framing group read a short para-
personal norms), in the tradition of the norm activation theory graph on the threat of climate change and how saving electricity
(Schwartz, 1977; Schwartz and Howard, 1981). Environmental can help to minimise negative effects (for details see Appendix A).
framing has been argued to take effect upon the activation of One and three months after the initial framing and intervention,
environmental values and norms (Evans et al., 2013; Steinhorst participants received email reminders with their electricity saving
et al., 2015). Several studies have proven that the strength of tips attached, again framed in terms of monetary or environmental
personal norms increases chances of pro-environmental spillover benefits (example in Appendix B).
in the private sphere (Thøgersen, 2004; Thøgersen and Ölander, Table 1 presents the study design with one pre-assessment
2003). In regard to public-sphere behaviour, as mentioned earlier, prior to the intervention and two follow-up assessments, four and
personal norms have been demonstrated to be important pre- nine months later. The pre-assessment was carried out in June
dictors of the acceptability of transport policies (Eriksson et al., 2013 in order to identify the most relevant electricity saving do-
2006, 2008), low carbon policies (De Groot and Steg, 2009; Steg mains for the sample. The intervention took place in November
et al., 2005) and pro-environmental policies in general (Poortinga 2013 and was repeated in December 2013 and February 2014. A
et al., 2004). Thus, we presume that findings for private-sphere list of five to ten framed electricity saving tips could be down-
behaviour are also applicable to public-sphere behaviour, such as loaded in a web portal until the end of the study and was sent to
low carbon policy acceptability. participants as an attachment in two email-reminders (repeated
intervention). Participants evaluated the acceptability of low car-
H3. The strength of personal ecological norms moderates the in- bon policies in two follow-up assessments after the initial inter-
fluence of the type of framing on policy acceptability. More spe- vention, starting in March and August 2014.
cifically: (Repeated) environmental framing has a stronger positive
influence on the acceptability of low carbon policies for individuals 2.2. Sample
with a strong personal ecological norm for saving electricity,
compared to individuals with a weak personal ecological norm. The initial sample consisted of 360 participants who took part
However, the influence of (repeated) monetary framing on the in the pre-assessment, the intervention and Follow-up assessment
acceptability of low carbon policies is not moderated by personal 1. In Follow-up assessment 2 the number of participants dropped
ecological norms, since personal ecological norms are not con- to 329. For both follow-up assessments, manipulation control
gruent with this type of framing. questions were applied in order to confide participants who had
not processed the framing carefully. Only participants who in-
dicated that they had read, downloaded, displayed or discussed
the electricity saving tips were included in the analysis. Therefore,
2. Methods for the first follow-up assessment 333 participants (92.5%), and for
the second follow-up assessment 258 (78.4%) participants were
This study was carried out as part of a larger intervention study included in the final sample. The manipulation control filtering
aimed at encouraging households to reduce their electricity con- process did not distinctly alter the socio-demographic character-
sumption. The purpose of the present paper was to investigate how istics or the perceived seriousness of environmental problems in
potential spillover effects on the acceptability of low carbon policies the samples. In Follow-up assessment 1 the average age of parti-
are influenced by the way the source behaviour, electricity saving, is cipants was 48.38 (SD ¼15.00; Min ¼ 20, Max ¼82, 0.6% missing)
promoted (in terms of monetary or environmental framing). and 36.6% were female (0.6% missing). The educational level of

Table 1
Study design.

Group Pre-assessment Intervention Repeated intervention Follow-up assessment 1 Follow-up assessment 2

6/2013 11/2013–01/2014 12/2013 & 2/2014 3–5/2014 8–10/2014


Monetary framing O1 X1 X1 O2 O3
R Environm. framing O1 X2 X2 O2 O3
Control group O1 – – O2 O3

Dependent variables: O1: personal ecological norms for saving electricity


O2 and O3: acceptability of low carbon policies
J. Steinhorst, E. Matthies / Energy Policy 93 (2016) 335–344 339

29.9% of the participants was a university or college degree, 30.5% environmental framing monetary framing control group
finished high school and 39.6% obtained less than a high school

acceptability for infrastructural


5
degree (3.6% missing). Socio-demographic characteristics of the

changes - Follow-up 1
samples in Follow-up assessment 1 (N ¼333) and Follow-up as-
4
sessment 2 (N ¼258) barely differed from each other. In compar-
ison to the German population, the study sample contained
a bigger proportion of male and slightly older participants 3
(Bundeszentrale für politische Bildung, 2013). The perceived ser-
iousness of environmental problems was slightly higher in com- 2
parison to the answers of a representative German survey
(N ¼ 2000) conducted by the German Federal Environment Agency n = 42 n = 45 n = 47 n = 69 n = 60 n = 70
(Umweltbundesamt, 2012). On the question “When evaluating the 1
weak personal ecological norms strong personal ecological norms
policy of the federal government for environmental protection,
should the government, in your opinion…” participants in Follow- framing condition
up assessment 1 chose the answer options “…do more” with 72.4%, Fig. 1. Mean acceptability ratings for infrastructural changes in Follow-up assess-
“…do less” with 3.0% and “…or is it right as it currently is?” with ment 1 for subjects with weak (N ¼134) and strong personal ecological norms
24.6%, compared to 64%, 6%, and 30% in the representative German (N ¼ 199).
Note. aError bars indicate 95% confidence interval.
survey (Umweltbundesamt, 2012).

4 ¼“fairly in favour”, 5 ¼“very much in favour”) with an additional


2.3. Variables
answer option (“do not know”), treated as missing in the
analysis.
For measuring the acceptability of low carbon policies we fo-
Originally, another two items assessed the acceptability of price
cussed on the two most essential political strategies of the German
increases for reducing household carbon emissions: “Increase
Energiewende: i.e. the expansion of renewable energy sources and
prices of household appliances that are not energy efficient by
political measures for energy efficiency. In order to screen for the
10%“ and “Increase electricity price by 10%”. A principal component
effects of framing on policy acceptability, we assessed acceptability
analysis conducted with the complete set of 10 acceptability items
ratings for examples of both types of political measures: the ac-
revealed that all items loaded very high (.77) on one of three
ceptability of infrastructural changes for renewable energy sources
(oblimin) rotated factors in Follow-up assessment 1 and 2.
and subsidies for reducing household carbon emissions. The ac-
Therefore, the mean score of the acceptability ratings was calcu-
ceptability for low carbon policies was assessed with eight items
lated for the factors: infrastructural changes (6 items, Cronbach's
divided into two scales. Items were introduced with: “In the fol-
α ¼.92 for the first and α ¼.94 for the second follow-up assess-
lowing you find a list of climate protection policy measures in
ment), subsidies for reducing household carbon emissions (2
Germany that are already implemented or would be conceivable in
items, Spearman Brown coefficient of .71 and .65; Cronbach's
the future. Please indicate for every measure, if and how strongly
α ¼.70 and α ¼ .65)2 and price increase for electricity and non-ef-
you support them”.
ficient appliances (2 items, Spearman Brown coefficient of .47 and
The scale for the acceptability of infrastructural changes con-
.57; Cronbach's α ¼ .47 and α ¼.54; e.g. Eisinga et al., 2013). Due to
tained six items referring to different domains: the extension of
insufficient (Kline, 1999) reliability values (Cronbach's α o.60) the
renewable energy plants (2 items: “Extension of renewable energy
items on the acceptability of price increases were excluded from
plants [e.g. wind wheels, photovoltaic] in my region” and “Exten-
the subsequent analysis.3
sion of renewable energy plants [e.g. wind wheels, photovoltaic] in
The personal ecological norm for saving electricity was assessed
Germany”), the extension of the electricity grid (2 items: “Exten-
with two items.: “Due to my personal values/ principles for en-
sion of power lines in my region for the distribution of electricity
vironmental protection I feel obliged to save electricity during my
from renewable energy” and “Extension of power lines in Germany
everyday life” and “No matter what other people think or do, due
for the distribution of electricity from renewable energy”, as well
to my personal values/ principles for environmental protection, I
as the extension of electricity storage (2 items: “Extension of sto-
feel personally obliged to save electricity during my everyday life”,
rage options for renewable energy [e.g. underground pumped
measured on a 5-point Likert-scale (1 ¼ “do not agree”, 2 ¼“agree
storage power stations] in my region” “Extension of storage op-
slightly”, 3¼ “agree moderately”, 4 ¼“agree fairly”, 5¼ “agree very
tions for renewable energy [e.g. pumped storage power stations]
much”). This operationalisation is in accordance with previous
in Germany”)..1
measures of personal ecological norms (Matthies et al., 2006).
The scale for the acceptability of subsidies for reducing house-
Internal consistency for the personal ecological norms scale
hold carbon emissions consisted of two items: “Subsidise energy-
was very good (Spearman Brown coefficient and Cronbach's
efficient household appliances so that the price is reduced by 10%”
α ¼.89). Based on a median split (Median ¼4.00) a categorical
and “Subsidise electricity from renewable energy/ green electricity
variable was computed representing a strong and weak personal
so that the price is reduced by 10%”.
ecological norm for saving electricity. The resulting groups were
All items were answered on a 5-point Likert-scale (1 ¼ “not in
similar in gender composition (strong personal ecological norms:
favour”, 2 ¼“slightly in favour”, 3 ¼“moderately in favour”,
35.9% females; weak personal ecological norms: 34.3% males), χ2
(1, N ¼ 332) ¼0.082, p ¼.775, but differed in age (strong personal
1
Items for policy acceptability were phrased in reference to local or national ecological norms: M ¼50.75, SD ¼ 14.83, n¼ 199; weak personal
dimensions due to an implicit research assumption. Researchers and practitioners
have come across a “gap” between the high level of public support for renewable
2
energy technologies and the opposition to specific projects at the local level, called In line with the recommendation of Eisinga et al. (2013) for two-item scales
the NIMBY syndrome (not-in-my-backyard). Therefore, we expected local energy we also calculated the Spearman-Brown coefficient additionally to Cronbach's
plants to be less acceptable due to local disadvantages (e.g. changes in landscape) alpha.
3
but global advantages (reduced carbon emissions). This assumption, however, was We did not use a standardised questionnaire and these items did not load on
not supported by the factor structure and therefore not pursued any further in our one factor. Consequently, we did not proceed with single item testing, since these
study. results would not be trustworthy.
340 J. Steinhorst, E. Matthies / Energy Policy 93 (2016) 335–344

ecological norms: M¼46.00, SD ¼14.96, n ¼ 134), t (330) ¼  2.86, environmental framing monetary framing control group
p ¼0.005, d ¼0.32. 5

acceptability for infrastructural


changes - Follow-up 2
4
3. Results

Data analysis was originally planned in the repeated measures 3


design or multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA), most ap-
propriate for longitudinal data and interaction hypothesis. How- 2
ever, data screening revealed missing preconditions of normally
distributed data. Hence, the most robust procedure in order to test n = 35 n = 32 n = 36 n = 53 n = 46 n = 56
1
for main and interaction effects were single analyses of variance
weak personal ecological norms strong personal ecological norms
(ANOVAs), combined with non-parametric tests in order to con-
firm the main effects. For each of the two identified acceptability framing condition
types (acceptability of infrastructural changes and acceptability of Fig. 2. Mean acceptability ratings for infrastructural changes in Follow-up assess-
subsidies), the effects of type of framing and personal ecological ment 2 for subjects with weak (N¼ 103) and strong personal ecological norms
norms were examined. A two way 2  3 ANOVA included the (N ¼ 155).
Note. aError bars indicate 95% confidence interval.
factors personal ecological norms (weak, strong) and the framing
condition (monetary framing, environmental framing, control
carbon emissions in Follow-up assessment 1, there was no sig-
group).4 Additional information on significant interaction effects
nificant main effect of framing condition (p ¼.251, N ¼ 333), but a
was obtained from a simple effect analysis for pairwise comparisons.
significant main effect of personal ecological norms (F(1, 327) ¼
4.06, p ¼.045, ηp2 ¼0.01) and a significant interaction effect of
3.1. Effects on the acceptability of infrastructural changes
framing condition with personal ecological norms (F(2, 327) ¼3.85,
p¼ .022, ηp2 ¼ 0.02).
In Follow-up assessment 1 the ANOVA revealed no significant
Pairwise comparisons for subjects with strong personal ecolo-
main effect of framing condition on the acceptability of infra-
gical norms (see Fig. 3) showed that acceptability ratings in the
structural changes (p ¼ .965, N ¼333). There was a significant main
environmental framing group were 0.58 points higher than in the
effect of personal ecological norms (F(1, 327) ¼ 4.47, p ¼ .035,
monetary framing group (p ¼.005, 95% CI of the difference ¼0.18
ηp2 ¼0.01) and the interaction effect for framing condition and
to 0.97) and 0.48 points higher than in the control group (p ¼.015,
personal ecological norms was marginally significant (F(2, 327) ¼
95% CI of the difference ¼0.09 to 0.86). The monetary framing
4.01, p¼ .068, ηp2 ¼0.03).5
group and the control group did not differ in their acceptability
Pairwise comparisons for subjects with strong personal ecolo-
ratings (p ¼.620). For subjects with weak personal ecological
gical norms (see Fig. 1) showed that the acceptability in the en-
norms, no significant differences in acceptability ratings were
vironmental framing group was marginally higher than in the
found under the three framing conditions.
monetary framing group (p¼ .075). The other groups did not differ.
For the acceptability of subsidies for reducing household car-
For subjects with weak personal ecological norms however, no
bon emissions in Follow-up assessment 2, there was no significant
significant differences in acceptability ratings were found under
main effect of framing condition (p¼ .795, N ¼258), no main effect
the three framing conditions.
of personal ecological norms (p ¼ .400), but a significant small to
In Follow-up assessment 2, again there was no significant main
medium interaction effect for framing condition and personal
effect of framing condition on the acceptability of infrastructural
ecological norms (F(2, 252)¼6.12, p o.01, ηp2 ¼ 0.05).
changes (p¼.552, N¼258) and the main effect for personal ecolo-
For the subjects with strong personal ecological norms (see
gical norms in Follow-up assessment 1 had disappeared (p¼ .300),
Fig. 4), the environmental framing group showed 0.46 points
but there was a significant interaction effect of framing condition and
higher acceptability ratings than the control group (p¼ .033, 95% CI
personal ecological norms (F(2, 252)¼ 3.73, p¼ .025, ηp2 ¼ 0.03).
of the difference ¼ 0.04 to 0.88). There were no differences be-
Pairwise comparisons for subjects with strong personal ecolo-
tween the monetary framing group and the control group re-
gical norms (see Fig. 2) revealed a 0.43 points higher acceptability in
gardless of the strength of personal ecological norms. Pairwise
the environmental framing group than in the control group
comparisons for subjects with weak personal ecological norms
(p¼ .033, 95% CI of the difference ¼0.03 to 0.82) and a marginally
(see Fig. 4) revealed 0.68 points lower acceptability ratings in the
significant increase in acceptability ratings compared to the mone-
environmental framing group compared to the control group
tary framing group (p¼.067). The monetary framing group and the
(p ¼.010, 95% CI of the difference ¼ 1.20 to  0.17) and margin-
control group did not differ in their acceptability ratings (p¼.844).
ally lower ratings compared to the monetary framing group
For subjects with weak personal ecological norms, no differences on
(p ¼.063).
acceptability ratings were found between framing conditions.

3.2. Effects on the acceptability of subsidies for reducing household


4. Discussion
carbon emissions
The purpose of this study was to explore the effects of com-
For the acceptability of subsidies for reducing household
monly used strategies for promoting pro-environmental beha-
viour, therefore emphasising monetary rather than environmental
4
The precondition of normally distributed data for the ANOVA was not met. benefits. More specifically, we examined how the framing
On this account we conducted additional Kruskal-Wallis-Tests for the main effects (monetary vs. environmental) of an intervention to promote a
of personal ecological norms and condition framing on acceptability ratings and private-sphere pro-environmental behaviour (electricity saving
found the same pattern of significant results and effects sizes. Consequently, the
interaction effect in the ANOVA can be interpreted as valuable.
behaviour) affects further pro-environmental behaviour in the
5
Benchmarks for ηp2 are 0.01 for a small but not trivial effect, 0.06 for a public sphere (the acceptability of low carbon energy policies).
medium and 0.14 for a large effect. Furthermore, we aimed at taking into account the role of personal
J. Steinhorst, E. Matthies / Energy Policy 93 (2016) 335–344 341

environmental framing monetary framing control group The monetary framing of electricity saving behaviour in our
5 study presented no positive effect on the acceptability of infra-
structural changes and subsidies for reducing household carbon
acceptability for subsidies

emissions. This is in line with research demonstrating that, when


4
promoting pro-environmental behaviour, appeals to monetary
-Follow-up 1

benefits may discourage positive influences on further pro-en-


3 vironmental behaviour (Evans et al., 2013; Steinhorst et al., 2015).
In this context, a review on spillover effects (Thøgersen and
Crompton, 2009) similarly concludes that the motivation to sup-
2
port sustainable policies seems more likely to be sustained if it
n = 42 n = 45 n = 47 n = 69 n = 60 n = 70 was adopted due to intrinsic motives (such as environmental
1 protection), rather than extrinsic motives, such as monetary
weak personal ecological norms strong personal ecological norms
benefits.
framing condition
4.2. The influence of personal ecological norms on policy
Fig. 3. Mean acceptability ratings for subsidies in Follow-up assessment 1 for
subjects with weak (N ¼ 134) and strong personal ecological norms (N ¼ 199). acceptability
Note. aError bars indicate 95% confidence interval.
As expected, the results of the present study revealed that
personal ecological norms for pro-environmental behaviour are
environmental framing monetary framing control group positively related to the acceptability of low carbon policies,
5 namely infrastructural changes as well as subsidies (Hypothesis 2).
Our results are in line with other studies that support the corre-
acceptability for subsidies

lation of personal ecological norms and public-sphere pro-en-


4
vironmental behaviours such as policy acceptability (De Groot and
-Follow-up 2

Steg, 2009; Eriksson et al., 2006, 2008; Steg et al., 2005; Steg et al.,
3 2006; Stern et al., 1999).

2
4.3. The moderating role of personal ecological norms on spillover
effects in association with framing
n = 35 n = 32 n = 36 n = 53 n = 46 n = 56
1 Hypothesis 3 predicted that personal ecological norms play a
weak personal ecological norms strong personal ecological norms
moderating role on the influence of the framing condition on ac-
framing condition ceptability ratings. We expected the acceptability of low carbon
Fig. 4. Mean acceptability ratings for subsidies in Follow-up assessment 2 for
policies to be higher for individuals with strong personal ecolo-
subjects with weak (N ¼ 103) and strong personal ecological norms (N ¼ 155). gical norms. We did not expect an effect of framing conditions for
Note. aError bars indicate 95% confidence interval. subjects with weak personal ecological norms or an effect of
personal ecological norms under monetary framing. Overall, re-
norms as a predictor as well as a moderator for potential spillover sults confirmed these assumptions, which is in line with findings
effects. that framing or labelling effects can be moderated by pro-en-
In the following, results will be discussed for each hypothesis. vironmental values (Bolderdijk et al., 2013; Boomsma and Steg,
2014) or attitudes (Parag et al., 2011).
4.1. Spillover effects on policy acceptability caused by framing For individuals with strong personal ecological norms, en-
vironmental framing had a significant positive effect on accept-
Contrary to our expectations of a positive spillover effect, en- ability ratings. For subjects with a weak personal ecological norm
vironmental framing had no influence on either one of the scales for saving electricity, however, the framing condition had no effect
for policy acceptability (Hypothesis 1). There are two possible on acceptability ratings. This positive spillover effect caused by the
reasons for these results. On the one hand our manipulation may framing condition in case of strong personal norms appeared in
not have been strong enough to uncover a rather small increase in spite of the absent main effect of the framing condition itself (see
the public-sphere behaviour. Indeed, studies on private-sphere Section 4.1). A possible explanation might be that the similarity
behaviour have found a spillover effect of small size (Thøgersen between saving electricity and low carbon policies is more ap-
and Ölander, 2003), apparent in only one third of possible beha- parent if it is important for a person to behave environmental
viours (Evans et al., 2013). On the other hand, the connection of friendly. This is the case when personal ecological norms are
electricity saving behaviour with low carbon policies may not have strong (Thøgersen, 2004).
become salient. Studies on spillover effects provide support for the One exception to the consistent moderating role of personal
latter explanation, showing that positive spillover usually appears ecological norms was the negative effect of environmental framing
in similar behavioural domains (Stern et al., 1999; Thøgersen, for participants with weak personal ecological norms, only present
2004). Private-sphere behaviours among themselves are much on the acceptability of subsidisation measures and only in the
more similar than private-sphere and public-sphere behaviour. second follow-up assessment after nine months. This unexpected
Nevertheless, both behaviours targeted in our study (electricity effect may be due to a defensive response, resulting in a seemingly
saving behaviour and the acceptability of low carbon policies) paradox reaction. Schwartz and Howard (1981) describe a similar
share the same behavioural domain of energy relevant behaviour effect that occurred in studies on altruistic behaviour, when an
and indications for positive spillover from a private-sphere beha- appeal was presented in a highly pressuring manner. The en-
viour to a public-sphere behaviour has been proven possible be- vironmental framing itself may not have had such a strong influ-
fore for the acceptance of wind power (Thøgersen and Noblet, ence, but pro-environmental arguments often used in the media
2012) as well as political action and support for policy measures may have primed the climate debate in society, which was then
(Tobler et al., 2012; Willis and Schor, 2012). perceived as heavily pressuring. Schwartz and Howard's (1981)
342 J. Steinhorst, E. Matthies / Energy Policy 93 (2016) 335–344

explanations for subsequent defensive reactions may also apply to technological changes and steering policy instruments that go
our results: First, when an appeal is presented in a highly pres- beyond the extension of renewable energy sources and subsidies
suring manner, the recipient could become suspicious of the mo- for household carbon emission reduction. Results for the latter
tives of the sender, resulting in denial of need. Second, the per- may apply to other energy-efficiency enhancing policies, e.g. price-
ception of being manipulated may result in reactance, resulting in incentives for energy efficient refurbishment.
refusal to show the behaviour. It is puzzling, however, that we
observed this defensive reaction for the acceptability of subsidies 4.5. Implications for future research
only. This finding may be due to the fact that participants with an
initially weak personal ecological norm for saving electricity per- To the best of our knowledge there is hardly any investigation
ceived subsidies as possible financial disadvantages, because they of spillover effects on the acceptability of pro-environmental pol-
were not granted financial benefits. When rating the acceptability icy measures related to framing. Our study contributed to filling
of subsidies, those participants might have sensed it unfair, if other this research gap on policy acceptability and the relevance of dif-
people investigate less for behaving pro-environmentally, some- ferent types of framing. As one of the first studies we investigated
thing they, in their opinion, have already acquired by saving how the framing (monetary vs. environmental) of rather low-im-
electricity. Environmental framing was designed to make partici- pact pro-environmental behaviour (saving electricity) influences
pants perceive their electricity saving behaviour as something that further high-impact pro-environmental behaviour (the accept-
protects the climate. The fact that most participants had already ability of low carbon policies and energy system changes). Key for
contributed to climate protection by their electricity saving be- the value of our study is that it addresses an area not well re-
haviour without anybody else co-financing it, may finally have led searched yet. It contributes to the development of spillover the-
to a defensive reaction. On the contrary, the policy measure in- ories and possible moderating variables. Furthermore, as an ex-
frastructural changes presents inherent monetary advantages that perimental field study, our research findings possess higher eco-
are profitable for all of Germany and the German economy, re- logical validity than lab studies, which is typical for most studies
spectively, which indirectly benefits the individual. on spillover effects (Truelove et al., 2014) and provides means for
Furthermore, another cause might have been that participants causality, contrary to purely correlational approaches so far
with low personal ecological norms were concerned about ap- (Thøgersen and Noblet, 2012; Tobler et al., 2012; Willis and Schor,
pearing pro-environmentally contrary to their desired self-re- 2012).
presentation. Gromet et al. (2013) found that politically con- However, it should be kept in mind that the type of framing
servative participants were reluctant to act in line with pro-en- and personal ecological norms are just part of a range of factors
vironmental appeals. However, this explanation would call for a influencing policy acceptability (for an overview see Steg and Vlek,
systematic effect, which is unlikely, since individuals with low 2009). Hence, subsequent research should further investigate
personal ecological norms did show defensive reactions only on spillover effects in association with environmental framing and
one out of four possible acceptability ratings. further influential factors. Our field experiment is highly relevant
from a practical point of view, also for drawing conclusions based
4.4. Transferability of the present findings on causal effects. Nevertheless, this study can only be a start to
explore spillover effects in association with environmental and
On the one hand, it should be noted that in the study sample monetary framing of pro-environmental interventions on the ac-
(older) men were overrepresented and showed a slightly higher ceptability of pro-environmental policy measures. Future studies
perceived seriousness of environmental problems compared to the should investigate spillover effects on the acceptability of further
German population. This may be due to the fact that participants policy measures.
were “household representatives”, since each household was asked
to select the person responsible for the energy topic in the
household. Consequently, the present results may not be gen- 5. Conclusion and policy implications
eralizable to the German population in total. On the other hand,
the transferability of this German sample to other countries might Within the German Energiewende, a long-range transformation
be impaired. German citizens generally possess strong environ- process, it is not the rather short-lived impact of political measures
mental values. Additionally, due to frequent media coverage and which determines the overall success, but the long-term effects
ongoing infrastructural changes for renewable energy across the and potentials of those political measures. Framing information
country, the energy system transition is highly salient already. arguing in terms of consequences and advantages, accompanies
Nevertheless, our study found high acceptability ratings for in- almost every intervention in the course of the energy transition.
frastructural changes, which are implemented political measures For example, community-owned wind farms can be advertised in
as well as for currently hypothetical subsidies for household car- terms of monetary advantages for potential shareholders and the
bon emission reduction in Germany. In a range of countries, per- community, or in terms of carbon emission reduction. Another
sonal norms (the Netherlands: De Groot and Steg, 2009; Sweden: example is the encouragement of demand side flexibility, which
Eriksson et al., 2006; Eriksson et al., 2008; Germany: Hunecke aims at shifting electricity usage at peak times to times of less
et al., 2001) and environmental framing (the Netherlands: Bol- usage. The purpose of this procedure is to allow a reliable overall
derdijk et al., 2013; Boomsma and Steg, 2014; UK: Walker et al., power supply with less basic energy demand. Flexible time-of-use
2014) have been detected as influencing factors for the pro-en- tariffs provide monetary incentives for private households to shift
vironmental policy support. Thus, our results should nonetheless the time of their energy usage. Simultaneously households con-
be transferable beyond Germany, especially to countries were no tribute to carbon emission reduction by reducing the basic energy
such measures have been implemented yet. demand. In both examples monetary as well as environmental
In terms of transferability, our results may also apply to other advantages are inherent, but it is a matter of the communication
low carbon policy measures that are present in the lives of citizens, process, which one will be highlighted.
especially those that are known and discussed in the media. Ex- The results of the present study shed light on the potential for
amples might be any kind of energy policies currently applied or long-term spillover effects that monetary or environmental
discussed, such as personal carbon allowances. Results may also be framing strategies yield on further behaviours with higher impact.
informative for policies similar to those investigated, therefore Our study demonstrated that a rather small intervention
J. Steinhorst, E. Matthies / Energy Policy 93 (2016) 335–344 343

(environmental framing of private-sphere behaviour), under cer- run this might help to reduce defensive reactions such as found in
tain circumstances, may have positive long-term consequences our study. The communication of monetary aspects might even be
(the promotion of high-impact public-sphere behaviour). We endangering the development of stronger pro-environmental
therefore conclude that environmental framing of pro-environ- norms or attitudes in our society, since it leads to discussing pro-
mental behavioural change can increase the disposition to show environmental aspects less often.
further pro-environmental behaviour in the public sphere. Another policy implication of our findings is to target values or
As our results for interaction effects showed, this is true, at norms that underlie acceptability ratings, as argued by Steg et al.
least, for a subpopulation with strong personal ecological norms. If (2015). When introducing a new policy, politicians and stake-
personal ecological norms are weak, however, environmental holders should carefully consider the perceptions and responses of
framing may have a delayed negative effect on the acceptability of parts of the target population which might oppose. At best, a na-
certain policy measures, due to defensive reactions. Furthermore, tional or regional survey should precede in order to identify the
from the present study it can be concluded that appeals to specific motivations of specific target groups. Tailored information
monetary benefits may discourage positive intentions for further schemes or campaigns that market the policy in a way that im-
pro-environmental behaviour. Generally, low carbon policy ac- prove public acceptance could be the next steps. Also, individuals
ceptability increases with the strength of personal ecological with initially high ecological norms may need different types of
norms, even if these norms are specific for the target behaviour. framing of private-sphere behaviours - like energy saving tips –
Based on the findings of our study we draw conclusions on the than individuals with less developed concerns and norms. This
design of policy measures or intervention approaches. In principle, once again points to the necessity of target group tailored inter-
all kinds of measures for pro-environmental behaviour change ventions (e.g. Abrahamse et al., 2007).
should be designed thoughtfully. If behavioural change in the tar- In general, our study has indicated that the success of inter-
geted behaviour is desired to spill over on further pro-environ- ventions depends on psychological influences. Above and beyond
mental behaviour, then communication strategies should focus on characteristics of the policy, individual factors have been identified
environmental advantages, in particular if the target group pos- to be an important influence on acceptability judgments in many
sesses strong ecological norms. Possible inherent financial ad- psychological studies, e.g. in the context of transport policies
vantages may speak for themselves. Monetary incentives are often (Eriksson et al., 2006, 2008), low carbon policies (De Groot and
perceived as efficient measures for changing a target behaviour Steg, 2009; Steg et al., 2005) and pro-environmental policies in
(see Thøgersen and Crompton, 2009). This, however, has been general (Poortinga et al., 2004). In matters like these, it has proven
found to depend on the perceived worthiness of monetary gains, helpful to consult psychological expertise. Psychologists can pro-
whereas environmental ains are often perceived as worthy, in- vide valuable input for designing informational strategies in a way
dependent of their height (Dogan et al., 2014). Furthermore, Parag that triggers the strongest desirable spillover effect on long-term
et al. (2011) argue that price signals in the energy context may lead motivation and further pro-environmental behaviours with higher
to a relatively weak behavioural impact, since energy costs account impact.
for a relatively small proportion of the total household budget in
many countries. Sheldon et al. (2011) suggest that it is better to
focus information on how sustainability policies benefit present Acknowledgements
and future generations rather than how they help the economy
grow. Thus we can conclude, when there is a choice between This research was conducted as part of a project within the
promoting monetary or environmental advantages - in cases Helmholtz-Alliance ENERGYTRANS in cooperation with the utility
where pro-environmental behaviour serves both - politicians, company DEW21 (http://www.dew21.de), founded by the Helm-
businessmen and environmental campaigners should prefer en- holtz-Association and the state of Saxony-Anhalt. We would like to
vironmental arguments over monetary to promote pro-environ- thank colleagues who supported us in conducting this study,
mental behaviour, especially in the presence of strong ecological especially we thank Heidi Mohnert from DEW21. Our gratitude
norms. also belongs to Paul Stern and two anonymous reviewers that
In general, policies that increase the occurrence of strong pro- commented on this paper.
environmental values and norms in society will render environ-
mental reasoning more effective. If strong pro-environmental va-
lues or norms of the addressee prevail, interventions that increase Appendix A. Supporting information
their salience can trigger positive spillover more effectively due to
underlining the importance of environmental causes. In addition, Supplementary data associated with this article can be found in
strengthening personal ecological norms or the general pro-en- the online version at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.enpol.2016.03.020.
vironmental motivation in society may represent a means to
prevent negative effects, such as defensive reactions. Conse-
quently, it is important to promote the development of pro-en- References
vironmental norms in our society. Not only informational cam-
paigns but also educational curricula may strengthen the inter- Abrahamse, W., Steg, L., Vlek, C., Rothengatter, T., 2007. The effect of tailored in-
nalization of pro-environmental values as norms. In order to fa- formation, goal setting, and tailored feedback on household energy use, en-
cilitate ecological norm development, education should address ergy-related behaviors, and behavioral antecedents. J. Environ. Psychol. 27 (4),
265–276.
environmental issues as early as possible and governments should Aronson, E., 1997. The theory of cognitive dissonance: The evolution and vicissi-
signal high commitment to environmental topics (Thøgersen, tudes of an idea. In: McGarty, C., Haslam, S.A. (Eds.), The Message of Social
2004). Psychology. Blackwell, Oxford, England, pp. 20–36.
Bem, D.J., 1972. Self-perception theory. Adv. Exp. Social. Psychol. 6, 1–62.
Besides education, nudging can represent an influential mea-
Bolderdijk, J.W., Gorsira, M., Keizer, K., Steg, L., 2013. Values determine the (In)ef-
sure to increase norm development. By setting the default to a fectiveness of informational interventions in promoting pro-environmental
pro-environmental option, this option is more likely perceived as behavior. Plos. One 8 (12), e83911.
an indication to a societal standard (Sunstein and Reisch, 2014), Boomsma, C., Steg, L., 2014. The effect of information and values on acceptability of
reduced street lighting. J. Environ. Psychol. 39, 22–31.
which triggers subsequent pro-environmental behaviour. Thus, by Bundeszentrale für politische Bildung [German Federal Agency for Civic Education],
strengthening the general pro-environmental motivation, in a long 2013. Datenreport 2013. Ein Sozialbericht für die Bundesrepublik Deutschland
344 J. Steinhorst, E. Matthies / Energy Policy 93 (2016) 335–344

[Data report 2013. A social report for Germany. Retrieved from 〈http://www. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, pp. 189–211.
bpb.de/173887/datenreport-2013〉 [2015/08/31]. Schweizer-Ries, P., 2008. Energy sustainable communities: environmental psy-
Chong, D., Druckman, J.N., 2007. Framing theory. Annu. Rev. Polit. Sci. 10, 103–126. chological investigations. Energy Policy 36 (11), 4126–4135.
Cornelissen, G., Pandelaere, M., Warlop, L., Dewitte, S., 2008. Positive cueing: pro- Scott, C.A., 1977. Modifying socially-conscious behavior: The foot-in-the-door
moting sustainable consumer behavior by cueing common environmental be- technique. J. Consum. Res. 4 (3), 156–164.
haviors as environmental. Int. J. Res. Mark. 25 (1), 46–55. Sheldon, K.M., Nichols, C.P., Kasser, T., 2011. Americans recommend smaller eco-
De Groot, J.I., Steg, L., 2009. Morality and prosocial behavior: The role of awareness, logical footprints when reminded of intrinsic American values of self-expres-
responsibility, and norms in the norm activation model. J. Soc. Psychol. 149 (4), sion, family, and generosity. Ecopsychology 3 (2), 97–104.
425–449. Spence, A., Leygue, C., Bedwell, B., O'Malley, C., 2014. Engaging with energy re-
Devine-Wright, P., 2005. Beyond NIMBYism: towards an integrated framework for duction: does a climate change frame have the potential for achieving broader
understanding public perceptions of wind energy. Wind Energy 8 (2), 125–139. sustainable behaviour? J. Environ. Psychol. 38, 17–28.
Dickerson, C.A., Thibodeau, R., Aronson, E., Miller, D., 1992. Using cognitive dis- Steg, L., Dreijerink, L., Abrahamse, W., 2005. Factors influencing the acceptability of
sonance to encourage water conservation. J. Appl. Social. Psychol. 22 (11), energy policies: a test of VBN theory. J. Environ. Psychol. 25 (4), 415–425.
841–854. Steg, L., Dreijerink, L., Abrahamse, W., 2006. Why are energy policies acceptable and
Dogan, E., Bolderdijk, J.W., Steg, L., 2014. Making Small Numbers Count: Environ- effective? Environ. Behav. 38 (1), 92–111.
mental and Financial Feedback in Promoting Eco-driving Behaviours. J. Con- Steg, L., Perlaviciute, G., van der Werff, E., 2015. Understanding the human di-
sum. Policy 37 (3), 413–422. mensions of a sustainable energy transition. Name: Front. Psychol. 6, 805.
Eisinga, R., Te Grotenhuis, M., Pelzer, B., 2013. The reliability of a two-item scale: Steg, L., Vlek, C., 2009. Encouraging pro-environmental behvaiour: an integrative
Pearson, Cronbach, or Spearman-Brown? Int. J. Public Health 58 (4), 637–642. review and research agenda. J. Environ. Psychol. 29, 309–317.
Eriksson, L., Garvill, J., Norlund, A.M., 2006. Acceptability of travel demand man- Steinhorst, J., Klöckner, C.A., Matthies, E., 2015. Saving electricity – for the money or
agement measures: The importance of problem awareness, personal norm, the environment? Risks of limiting pro-environmental spillover when using
freedom, and fairness. J. Environ. Psychol. 26, 15–26. monetary framing. J. Environ. Psychol. 43, 125–135.
Eriksson, L., Garvill, J., Norlund, A.M., 2008. Acceptability of single and combined Stern, P.C., 2000. Toward a coherent theory of environmentally significant behavior.
transport policy measures: The importance of environmental and policy spe- J. Soc. Issues 56 (3), 407–424.
cific beliefs. Transp. Res. Part A 42, 1117–1128. Stern, P.C., Dietz, T., Abel, T., Guagnano, G.A., Kalof, L., 1999. A value-belief-norm
Evans, L., Maio, G.R., Corner, A., Hodgetts, C.J., Ahmed, S., Hahn, U., 2013. Self-in- theory of support for social movements: the case of environmentalism. Hum.
terest and pro-environmental behaviour. Nat. Clim. Chang. 3 (2), 122–125. Ecol. Rev. 6 (2), 81–97.
German Federal Ministry for Economic Affairs and Energy [BMWi], 2014. A good Sunstein, C.R., Reisch, L.A., 2014. Automatically green: behavioral economics and
piece of work. The Energy of the Future. First “Energy Transition” Progress environmental protection. Harv. Environ. Law Rev. 38, 127.
Report – Summary. Retrieved from 〈http://www.bmwi.de/English/Redaktion/ Thøgersen, J., 1999. Spillover processes in the development of a sustainable con-
Pdf/fortschrittsbericht-kurzfassung-en,property ¼ pdf,bereich¼ bmwi2012,spra sumption pattern. J. Econ. Psychol. 20 (1), 53–81.
che¼ en,rwb ¼ true.pdf〉 [27/04/2015]. Thøgersen, J., 2004. A cognitive dissonance interpretation of consistencies and in-
Gromet, D.M., Kunreuther, H., Larrick, R.P., 2013. Political ideology affects energy- consistencies in environmentally responsible behavior. J. Environ. Psychol. 24
efficiency attitudes and choices. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 110 (23), 9314–9319. (1), 93–103.
Huijts, N.M.A., Molin, E.J.E., Steg, L., 2012. Psychological factors influencing sus- Thøgersen, J., Crompton, T., 2009. Simple and painless? The limitations of spillover
tainable energy technology acceptance: A review-based comprehensive fra- in environmental campaigning. J. Consum. Policy 32 (2), 141–163.
mework. Renew. Sustain. Energy Rev. 16 (1), 525–531. Thøgersen, J., Noblet, C., 2012. Does green consumerism increase the acceptance of
Hunecke, M., Blobaum, A., Matthies, E., Hoger, R., 2001. Responsibility and en- wind power? Energy Policy 51, 854–862.
vironment - Ecological norm orientation and external factors in the domain of Thøgersen, J., Ölander, F., 2003. Spillover of environment-friendly consumer be-
travel mode choice behavior. Environ. Behav. 33 (6), 830–852. haviour. J. Environ. Psychol. 23 (3), 225–236.
Jones, C.R., Eiser, J.R., 2009. Identifying predictors of attitudes towards local onshore Tobler, C., Visschers, V.H.M., Siegrist, M., 2012. Addressing climate change: De-
wind development with reference to an English case study. Energy Policy 37 terminants of consumers’ willingness to act and to support policy measures. J.
(11), 4604–4614. Environ. Psychol. 32 (3), 197–207.
Kline, P., 1999. The Handbook of Psychological Testing 2. Routledge, London. Truelove, H.B., Carrico, A.R., Weber, E.U., Raimi, K.T., Vandenbergh, M.P., 2014. Po-
Levin, I.P., Schneider, S.L., Gaeth, G.J., 1998. All frames are not created equal: a ty- sitive and negative spillover of pro-environmental behavior: an integrative
pology and critical analysis of framing effects. Organ. Behav. Human. Decis. review and theoretical framework. Glob. Environ. Chang. 29, 127–138.
Process. 76 (2), 149–188. Umweltbundesamt [German Federal Environment Agency], 2012. Umweltbe-
Maio, G.R., Pakizeh, A., Cheung, W.-Y., Rees, K.J., 2009. Changing, priming, and wusstsein in Deutschland 2012: Ergebnisse einer repräsentativen Bevölk-
acting on values: effects via motivational relations in a circular model. J. Per- erungsumfrage [Environmental consciousness in Germany 2012]. Retrieved
sonal. Soc. Psychol. 97 (4), 699–715. from 〈http://www.umweltbundesamt.de/sites/default/files/medien/publika
Matthies, E., Klöckner, C.A., Preißner, C.L., 2006. Applying a modified moral decision tion/long/4396.pdf〉 [2013/08/05].
making model to change habitual car use: how can commitment be effective? Upham, P., Shackley, S., 2007. Local public opinion of a proposed 21.5 MW
Appl. Psychol. 55 (1), 91–106. (e) biomass gasifier in Devon: Questionnaire survey results. Biomass. Bioenergy
Morris, C., Pehnt, M., 2012. Energy transition: the German Energiewende. Retrieved 31 (6), 433–441.
from 〈http://energytransition.de/wp-content/themes/boell/pdf/en/German-En Vansteenkiste, M., Simons, J., Lens, W., Sheldon, K.M., Deci, E.L., 2004. Motivating
ergy-Transition_en.pdf〉 [27/02/2015]. learning, performance, and persistencce: the synergistic effects of intrinsic goal
Parag, Y., Capstick, S., Poortinga, W., 2011. Policy attribute framing: a comparison contents and autonomy-supportive contexts. J. Person. Soc. Psychol. 87 (2),
between three policy instruments for personal emissions reduction. J. Policy 246–260.
Anal. Manag. 30 (4), 889–905. Walker, B.J.A., Wiersma, B., Bailey, E., 2014. Community benefits, framing and the
Poortinga, W., Steg, L., Vlek, C., 2004. Values, environmental concern, and en- social acceptance of offshore wind farms: an experimental study in England.
vironmental behavior: a study into household energy use. Environ. Behav. 36 Energy Res. Social. Sci. 3, 46–54.
(1), 70–93. Willis, M.M., Schor, J.B., 2012. Does changing a light bulb lead to changing the
Schwartz, S.H., 1977. Normative influences on altruism. In: Berkowitz, L. (Ed.), world? Political action and the conscious consumer. Ann. Am. Acad. Polit. So-
Advances in Experimental Social Psychology Vol. 10. Academic Press, New York, cial. Sci. 644 (1), 160–190.
pp. 221–279. Wüstenhagen, R., Wolsink, M., Bürer, M.J., 2007. Social acceptance of renewable
Schwartz, S.H., Howard, J.A., 1981. A normative decision-making model of altruism. energy innovation: an introduction to the concept. Energy Policy 35 (5),
In: Rushton, J.P., Sorrentino, R.M. (Eds.), Altruism and Helping Behavior. 2683–2691.

You might also like