Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 6

Art Therapy

Journal of the American Art Therapy Association

ISSN: 0742-1656 (Print) 2159-9394 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/uart20

Reducing Negative Mood Through Drawing:


Comparing Venting, Positive Expression, and
Tracing

Kayla Smolarski, Kristy Leone & Steven J. Robbins

To cite this article: Kayla Smolarski, Kristy Leone & Steven J. Robbins (2015) Reducing Negative
Mood Through Drawing: Comparing Venting, Positive Expression, and Tracing, Art Therapy, 32:4,
197-201, DOI: 10.1080/07421656.2015.1092697

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/07421656.2015.1092697

Published online: 13 Nov 2015.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 1487

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Citing articles: 8 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=uart20
Art Therapy: Journal of the American Art Therapy Association, 32(4) pp. 197–201, © AATA, Inc. 2015

brief report
Reducing Negative Mood Through Drawing: Comparing
Venting, Positive Expression, and Tracing

Kayla Smolarski, Kristy Leone, and Steven J. Robbins

Abstract Winner, 2008; Wilkinson & Chilton, 2013), the viewing


of aesthetically pleasing images (Abbott et al., 2013; Bell &
The authors examined whether instructions to express Robbins, 2007), the distracting effects of manual manipula-
emotional states represent an active ingredient in the mood- tion and focused attention (Abbott et al., 2013), the mean-
enhancing properties of drawing. Forty-five undergraduates ingful act of creation (Abbott et al., 2013), or the
were asked to make a list of recent stressful events (negative distraction produced through expression of either positive
mood induction) before being randomly assigned to one of three or neutral emotions (Drake, Coleman, & Winner, 2011;
conditions: drawing to express happiness (positive expression), Drake & Winner, 2012).
drawing to express current stress (venting), or tracing and A number of studies have begun exploring this ques-
coloring a simple line drawing (distraction control). Mood tion. For example, De Petrillo and Winner (2005) found
improved in all three conditions, but the positive expression that drawing enhanced mood more than copying geomet-
condition showed significantly greater improvement than either ric shapes or completing word puzzles. Thus, the effect of
the tracing or venting groups. Venting and tracing produced drawing results from more than simple motor movements
similar levels of mood elevation. Positive emotional expression or focused attention. Bell and Robbins (2007) examined
appears to be an active ingredient in the beneficial effects of whether art production represents part of the active ingre-
drawing on mood. dient in mood enhancement by comparing the effects of
drawing with the viewing of art prints. The drawing con-
dition showed significantly greater reductions in negative
mood. Unfortunately, neither Boothby and Robbins
(2011) nor Abbott et al. (2013) were able to replicate this
Introduction difference between producing and viewing art.
Numerous randomized controlled trials have docu- More recently, the role of different forms of mood
mented the efficacy of drawing in improving mood (see expression in drawing has received attention. Curl
Slayton, D’Archer, & Kaplan, 2010, for a review). The evi- (2008) compared groups asked to express either positive
dence supporting drawing as an intervention for negative or negative emotions through drawing or collage mak-
mood motivates a follow-up question: What are the “active ing. The form of art making was unrelated to outcome,
ingredients” underlying these effects? As Kapitan (2012) but individuals asked to express positive emotions
discussed, art therapy research is ready to move beyond sim- showed reductions in stress, whereas those assigned to
ple demonstrations of efficacy to consideration of specific negative expression showed increased stress levels. In a
causal mechanisms. A number of different active ingre- similar study, Dalebroux et al. (2008) found that an
dients have been proposed for the effects of drawing. His- induced negative mood was better remediated by draw-
torically, the expression of negative emotions through art ing to express happiness compared to drawing to express
creation has been the most popular explanation—a “vent- sad feelings or crossing out symbols on a page. Drake
ing” or “catharsis” account (Chambala, 2008; Curl, 2008; and Winner (2012) showed that a purportedly neutral
Kramer, 2000). However, the act of drawing may elevate drawing task (“draw a house”) produced more mood ele-
mood for a host of other reasons including the expression vation than either venting (“draw something from the
of positive emotions to counteract negative ones (Abbott, film”) or the simple passage of time. They intended the
Shanahan, & Neufeld, 2013; Dalebroux, Goldstein, & draw-a-house task to represent a “distraction” condition,
but as they noted in their discussion, drawing a house
Editor’s Note: Kayla Smolarski and Kristy Leone are under- could involve positive emotional expression as well.
graduate psychology majors and Steven J. Robbins is professor of Several other studies suggest that distraction is no bet-
psychology at Arcadia University, Glenside, PA. Correspondence ter than venting in reducing negative mood. Pizarro (2004)
concerning this article may be addressed to the third author at compared undergraduates assigned to draw a picture repre-
robbinss@arcadia.edu senting a traumatic experience with those asked to draw a
197
198 DRAWING AND NEGATIVE MOOD

still life. There were no mood differences between condi- Forty-five undergraduate participants were asked to make a
tions. Henderson, Rosen, and Mascaro (2007) similarly short list of personal stressors before being randomly
found that individuals screened for trauma symptoms and assigned to the venting condition (“draw your current feel-
asked to draw a specific object showed equivalent changes ings”), the positive expression condition (“draw something
in mood compared to those instructed to express negative that makes you happy”), or the distraction condition (trac-
mood states. In a study comparing participants assigned ing and coloring in a simple line drawing of a sailboat). We
to color in a square or circular outline, Babouchkina and hypothesized that those in the positive expression condition
Robbins (2015) found greater mood enhancement in the would experience greater mood improvement compared to
circular (mandala) condition, but no significant interaction venting or distraction.
with instructions (express negative feelings versus no
instructions).
Taken as a whole, the studies just reviewed point to
positive emotional expression as an important ingredient in Method
the mood enhancement produced by drawing. Recently,
Participants
much attention has been drawn to the importance of repli-
cation in establishing robust phenomena in psychology Forty-five undergraduate students at Arcadia Univer-
(Klein et al., 2014; Open Science Collaboration, 2012). sity in Glenside, Pennsylvania, between the ages of 18 and
Consequently, the present study was designed first with the 22 served as participants (17 men, 28 women). Recruit-
goal of replicating the findings of Curl (2008) and Dale- ment took place through the Sona Systems web-based
broux et al. (2008) showing that positive mood expression scheduling system (www.sona-systems.com) or through
is superior to venting. We wished to see if we could produce word-of-mouth recruitment by the experimenters. Partici-
the same outcome in our specific population of undergradu- pants received course credit when appropriate; no other
ates, using different methods of negative mood induction incentives were offered. We were not able to screen partici-
and assessment. pants for diagnosed emotional disorders because no licensed
We also wished to expand on these initial findings by clinicians were available. However, no individual voluntar-
providing a more direct comparison of positive expression ily revealed to us that they suffered from a mental health
and distraction as potential “active ingredients” in mood condition.
enhancement. As Drake et al. (2011) and Drake and Winner
(2012) discussed, expression of positive emotions might
enhance mood through simple distraction rather than
through the replacement or canceling out of one mood state Materials
by another. Dalebroux et al. (2008) attempted to separate
these two possibilities by comparing a positive expression Mood assessment occurred through administration of
drawing condition with a group asked to identify and cross the Profile of Mood States (POMS) questionnaire (McNair,
out specific symbols printed on a page. However, the symbol Lorr, & Droppleman, 1971). The POMS consists of 65
identification task appears to differ from drawing in multiple mood-related adjectives; participants indicate their current
ways, including the lack of an actual drawing or coloring level of each state on a 5-point Likert scale. Individual items
component and the possible induction of stress through a can be combined into six subscales termed Anger, Fatigue,
test-like procedure (participants could worry about possible Confusion, Tension, Depression, and Vigor. Adding the
errors in their performance, misses or false alarms). Thus, the first five scores and subtracting the Vigor score produces an
conclusion that drawing for positive expression is superior to overall negative mood score. That score served as the out-
drawing for distraction is not yet warranted. come measure in this study.
We attempted in the present study to create a more
drawing-like distraction condition by asking one group
of participants to trace a simple line drawing and then Procedure
color in their traced outline. We chose tracing and col-
oring rather than drawing under “neutral” instructions, All individuals enrolled in the study participated in a
such as the “draw a house” instructions used by Drake single laboratory session lasting approximately 30 minutes.
and Winner (2012), because we thought that any free Participants were seen individually; each session took place
drawing task could involve positive or negative mood in a small research room containing a table and two chairs.
expression. Tracing and coloring appeared to be a task Participants provided informed, written consent before tak-
that involved the motor demands of free drawing and ing the first POMS questionnaire (baseline). They were
the same exposure to a potentially pleasing outcome informed that the study was intended to explore the impact
drawing, but with less potential for the induction or dis- of drawing on mood; however, they were not informed as
charge of personal emotional states. to the specific conditions manipulated across groups. Thus,
More specifically, we compared the effects of three participants were kept blind to their group assignment.
drawing conditions following negative mood induction: Next, individuals were asked to make a list of three stressful
expression of negative feeling (venting), expression of hap- events currently on their mind (we did not collect the lists).
piness (positive expression), and a distraction task (tracing). We intended to create an elevated level of negative mood
SMOLARSKI / LEONE / ROBBINS 199

against which the impact of the drawing conditions could Results


be assessed. A second POMS questionnaire (pre-treatment)
was then administered to document that groups showed Overall negative mood scores on the POMS at the
comparable increases in negative mood prior to the treat- three measurement points (baseline, pre-treatment, and
ment phase of the study. post-treatment) for the three treatment groups can be found
Based on a preconstructed blocked randomization in Table 1.
table, participants were then given their drawing instruc-
tions in a sealed envelope. Fifteen participants were assigned Negative Mood Induction
to each of the three conditions. The experimenter handed
an envelope to each participant and instructed the person In order to evaluate the success of the initial mood
to (a) read the instructions on the first page, (b) copy the induction, we compared changes in POMS scores from
instructions onto one side of an included blank sheet of baseline to pre-treatment across groups. A 2-factor, repeated
paper (as a manipulation check to show that the instruc- measures ANOVA with group (venting, positive expression,
tions were read), and (c) spend 10 minutes drawing accord- tracing) as a between-factor and time (baseline, pre-treat-
ing to the instructions. Participants were asked to return all ment) as a within-factor revealed a significant main effect of
materials to their envelope and notify the experimenter time, F(1, 42) D 25.8, p < .01, hp2 D 0.38. However,
when a timer signaled that 10 minutes had passed. In this there was neither a significant main effect of group nor a
way, the experimenter was kept blind to each participant’s significant Group £ Time interaction. As can be seen in
group assignment. In addition to the sealed envelope, each Table 1, all three groups showed substantial increases in
participant received 20 to 30 assorted colored markers. Sim- negative mood following the stressful event list. Across
ilar sets of markers were used across participants; due to an groups, overall negative mood on the POMS rose from a
oversight, the specific set of colors in each set was not mean score of 27.8 to 42.8, an increase of 54%. The lack
recorded. However, because the sets were similar and the of group effects documents that the three groups showed
experimenters blinded to group assignment, any small varia- comparable increases in negative mood and were well
tions in the marker sets were distributed randomly across matched for mood states prior to the drawing
participants. manipulation.
Participants in the expression of negative feeling condi-
tion (Group Venting) received an instruction page asking Drawing Intervention Outcome
them to create a drawing to express current feelings of
stress. Participants in the expression of happiness condition In order to test the impact of the three drawing condi-
(Group Positive Expression) were asked to draw something tions, we conducted a 2-factor, repeated measures ANOVA
that made them happy. Participants in the distraction con- looking at group and time (pre-treatment, post-treatment).
dition (Group Tracing) were asked to trace a coloring book A treatment effect would be revealed as a significant interac-
line drawing of a sailboat provided in their envelope. They tion, meaning that the groups showed different levels of
were also asked to color in their traced image. All three mood change following the drawing manipulation. Overall,
groups received a blank 8.500 £ 1100 sheet of white copy there was a main effect of time, F(1, 42) D 25.3, p < .01,
paper on which to make their drawings. hp2 D 0.38; mood generally improved from pre- to post-
When the experimenter returned following the draw- treatment across all three conditions. Of greater interest, we
ing intervention, she administered the third and final found a significant Group £ Time interaction, F(2, 42) D
POMS questionnaire to each participant (post-treatment). 4.0, p < .05, hp2 D 0.16. As Table 1 documents, Group
Participants were asked to make a list of three things that Positive Expression showed 3.4 times more improvement
were currently making them happy (to counteract the initial than Group Venting (pre-post change score of 36.4 versus
negative mood induction) and were then debriefed as to the 10.7) and 2.6 times more improvement than Group Trac-
nature of the study. They were instructed to keep both the ing (pre-post change score of 36.4 versus 13.9). To examine
positive and negative mood induction sheets upon exiting these differences further, we ran post-hoc analyses on pre-
the lab. post change scores using Fisher’s least significant difference

Table 1. POMS Overall Negative Mood Scores at Baseline, Pre-Treatment, and Post-Treatment

Group N Baseline Pre Post Pre–Post


Positive Expression 15 26.7 (36.2) 46.0 (46.9) 9.6 (29.8) 36.4 (33.6)
Venting 15 24.3 (25.1) 41.9 (32.6) 31.3 (30.9) 10.7 (27.8)
Tracing 15 32.4 (42.7) 40.5 (44.5) 26.6 (34.2) 13.9 (17.4)
Note. All values are mean (SD). For the pre - post difference scores, positive values indicate improvements in mood
(reductions in POMS overall negative mood ratings).
200 DRAWING AND NEGATIVE MOOD

test. Group Positive Expression showed significantly more prompted personal emotional states less conducive to mood
mood improvement than either Group Venting (p D .01) improvement than those produced in the positive condi-
or Group Tracing (p D .03). Venting and Tracing Groups tion. Replications of the present results should continue to
did not differ in magnitude of mood change (p D .74). employ novel control conditions to conclusively isolate pos-
itive expression through drawing as the active ingredient in
Discussion mood enhancement.
As in previous studies related to art therapy in our lab
The results of this study are easily summarized: Follow- (Babouchkina & Robbins, 2015; Bell & Robbins, 2007;
ing induction of a negative mood state, drawing in order to Boothby & Robbins, 2011; Kimport & Robbins, 2012),
express happiness (positive expression) produced substan- both the primary researchers and participants in this study
tially more mood enhancement than drawing to express were undergraduate college students. Consequently, the
current feelings of stress (venting) or simple tracing and col- present results also require replication with clinical popula-
oring (distraction control). The tracing and venting condi- tions treated by trained art therapists. Furthermore, our
tions did not differ from one another. These results results came about following a single, 30-minute session.
replicate earlier findings showing that drawing to express The long-term impact of drawing under different instruc-
positive feelings is most effective in mood improvement tional conditions remains to be assessed. As the third author
(e.g., Curl, 2008; Dalebroux et al., 2008). has argued previously, studies such as the present one
The present experiment expands on these earlier stud- should be viewed as “proof-of-concept”—in the absence of
ies through inclusion of the tracing and coloring control any of the other characteristics of typical art therapy
condition (distraction). Individuals drawing to express hap- (trained therapists, clients with diagnosable disorders, mul-
piness showed almost three times more mood improvement tiple sessions), we still demonstrated that drawing to express
compared to the tracing group. Thus, the impact of making happiness produced substantially more mood enhancement
a drawing of something that evoked happiness exceeds the than either venting or tracing. These findings add to a
results produced by a focused, attention-demanding draw- growing body of literature suggesting that art therapy inter-
ing task producing a pleasing, colored image. This result ventions should direct clients toward positive emotional
further supports the claim that a drawing about something expression, and away from venting or simple distraction, to
positive is effective by counteracting or replacing an initial achieve maximum treatment efficacy.
negative mood state with a positive one (Abbott et al.,
2013; Dalebroux et al., 2008; Wilkinson & Chilton,
2013). Acknowledgments
The present study has several specific design virtues
that are worth noting. First, random assignment reduces The authors would like to thank Grace Hiegl and Sabryna
the likelihood of many alternative explanations for group Hunt for their help in running participants in the study.
differences including selection bias, maturation, history, The first two authors contributed equally to this study;
testing, and regression to the mean. Second, experimenter order of authorship was determined by coin toss.
blinding precludes the possibility that observer expectations
could have contributed to the results. Finally, participant
blinding rules out the possibility of demand effects contam- References
inating group differences in outcome.
Art therapy studies face unique challenges in attempt- Abbott, K. A., Shanahan, M. J., & Neufeld, R.W. J. (2013).
ing to isolate the active ingredients underlying efficacy find- Artistic tasks outperform nonartistic tasks for stress reduction.
ings. As we discussed earlier, the creation of an artwork Art Therapy: Journal of the American Art Therapy Association,
30, 71–78. doi:10.1080/07421656.2013.787214
contains many components such as creativity, expression of
personal memories or feelings, expression of general mood Babouchkina, A., & Robbins, S. J. (2015). Reducing negative
states such as depression or happiness, specific motor behav- mood through mandala creation: A randomized controlled trial.
iors, focused attention, a sense of accomplishment or com- Art Therapy: Journal of the American Art Therapy Association,
pletion, and the ultimate viewing of a potentially 32, 34–39. doi:10.1080/07421656.2015.994428
aesthetically pleasing end product. No single comparison
group can reasonably be expected to isolate one and only Bell, C. E., & Robbins, S. J. (2007). Effect of art production on
one of these components. The present distraction control negative mood: A randomized, controlled trial. Art Therapy:
condition is no exception. Although intended to isolate dis- Journal of the American Art Therapy Association, 24, 71–75.
traction versus positive expression as the active ingredient doi:10.1080/07421656.2007.10129589
differentiating the groups, other differences between these
Boothby, D. M., & Robbins, S. J. (2011). The effects of music
conditions remain. For example, participants in our tracing listening and art production on negative mood: A randomized,
and coloring condition may not have found their end prod- controlled trial. The Arts in Psychotherapy, 38, 204–208.
uct as pleasing or soothing as those allowed to freely draw a doi:10.1016/j.aip.2011.06.002
happy event. Tracing may still contain a stressful task-like
quality absent in free drawing. And the possibility exists Chambala, A. (2008). Anxiety and art therapy: Treatment in the
that the sailboat image used for tracing could have public eye. Art Therapy: Journal of the American Art Therapy
SMOLARSKI / LEONE / ROBBINS 201

Association, 25, 187–189. doi:10.1080/07421656.2008. Kimport, E. R., & Robbins, S. J. (2012). Efficacy of creative clay
10129540 work for reducing negative mood: A randomized controlled
trial. Art Therapy: Journal of the American Art Therapy Associa-
Curl, K. (2008). Assessing stress reduction as a function of artistic tion, 29, 74–79. doi:10.1080/07421656.2012.680048
creation and cognitive focus. Art Therapy: Journal of the Ameri-
can Art Therapy Association, 25, 164–169. doi:10.1080/ Klein, R. A., Ratliff, K. A., Vianello, M., Adams, R. B., Jr., Bahnik,
07421656.2008.10129550 S., Bernstein, M. J., . . . Nosek, B. A. (2014). Investigating varia-
tion in replicability: A “many labs” replication project. Social
Dalebroux, A., Goldstein, T. R., & Winner, E. (2008). Short- Psychology, 45, 142–152. doi:10.1027/1864-9335/a000178
term mood repair through art-making: Positive emotion is
more effective than venting. Motivation and Emotion, 32, Kramer, E. (2000). Art as therapy. London, England: Jessica
288–295. doi:10.1007/s11031-008-9105-1 Kingsley.

De Petrillo, L., & Winner, E. (2005). Does art improve mood? A McNair, D. M., Lorr, M., & Droppleman, L. F. (1971). The Pro-
test of a key assumption underlying art therapy. Art Therapy: file of Mood States. San Diego, CA: Educational and Industrial
Journal of the American Art Therapy Association, 22, 205–212. Testing Service.
doi:10.1080/07421656.2005.10129521
Open Science Collaboration. (2012). An open, large-scale, collab-
Drake, J. E., Coleman, K., & Winner, E. (2011). Short-term orative effort to estimate the reproducibility of psychological
mood repair through art: Effects of medium and strategy. Art science. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 657–660.
Therapy: Journal of the American Art Therapy Association, 28, doi:10.1177/1745691612462588
26–30. doi:10.1080/07421656.2011.557032
Pizarro, J. (2004). The efficacy of art and writing therapy: Increas-
Drake, J. E., & Winner, E. (2012). Confronting sadness through ing positive mental health outcomes and participant retention
art making: Distraction is more beneficial than venting. Psychol- after exposure to traumatic experience. Art Therapy: Journal of
ogy of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6, 255–261. the American Art Therapy Association, 21, 5–12. doi:10.1080/
doi:10.1037/a0026909 07421656.2004.10129327

Henderson, P., Rosen, D., & Mascaro, N. (2007). Empirical Slayton, S. C., D’Archer, J., & Kaplan, F. (2010). Outcome
study on the healing nature of mandalas. Psychology of Aesthetics, studies on the efficacy of art therapy: A review of findings. Art
Creativity, and the Arts, 1, 148–154. doi:10.1037/1931- Therapy: Journal of the American Art Therapy Association, 27,
3896.1.3.148 108–119. doi:10.1080/07421656.2010.10129660

Kapitan, L. (2012). Does art therapy work? Identifying the active Wilkinson, R. A., & Chilton, G. (2013). Positive art therapy: Linking
ingredients of art therapy efficacy. Art Therapy: Journal of the positive psychology to art therapy theory, practice, and research.
American Art Therapy Association, 29, 48–49. doi:10.1080/ Art Therapy: Journal of the American Art Therapy Association, 30,
07421656.2012.684292 4–11. doi:10.1080/07421656.2013.757513

You might also like