Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 6

Art Therapy

Journal of the American Art Therapy Association

ISSN: 0742-1656 (Print) 2159-9394 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/uart20

Short-Term Mood Repair Through Art: Effects of


Medium and Strategy

Jennifer E. Drake , Katelyn Coleman & Ellen Winner

To cite this article: Jennifer E. Drake , Katelyn Coleman & Ellen Winner (2011) Short-Term
Mood Repair Through Art: Effects of Medium and Strategy, Art Therapy, 28:1, 26-30, DOI:
10.1080/07421656.2011.557032

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/07421656.2011.557032

Published online: 14 Apr 2011.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 2231

View related articles

Citing articles: 11 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=uart20
Art Therapy: Journal of the American Art Therapy Association, 28(1) pp. 26–30 
C AATA, Inc. 2011

brief reports
Short-Term Mood Repair Through Art: Effects of Medium
and Strategy

Jennifer E. Drake, Katelyn Coleman, and Ellen Winner, Boston, MA

Abstract ing, by which one expresses negative feelings; and distrac-


tion, by which one expresses positive or neutral feelings in
This study examined the effects of expressive media (draw- order to forget negative feelings (Gross, 2007; Gross & John,
ing versus writing) and emotion regulation strategy (coping by 2003; Larsen, 2000; Parkinson & Totterdell, 1999; Thayer,
venting versus coping by distraction) on short-term mood repair. Newman, & McClain, 1994). Artists have described purg-
After inducing a sad mood in 40 participants, the researchers ing themselves of suffering by expressing their pain in their
randomly assigned them to one of two conditions: drawing or art (venting), but they also speak of how creating takes them
writing. Mood valence was assessed before and after the activ- away from ordinary life (distraction), as did Graham Greene
ity, and participants reported whether they used the activity to (1980) when he spoke of “escape.”
vent or to distract themselves. Findings indicated that mood va- Research on the writing process has uncovered the
lence was significantly more positive after drawing than writing, role of venting by showing that writing about emotional
and more positive when individuals reported using distraction and stressful events has long-term health, cognitive, and
rather than venting to regulate their emotions. Drawing in this affective benefits. Documented benefits include improved
study was a more effective means of immediate mood repair than immune function (Pennebaker, Kiecolt-Glaser, & Glaser,
writing; both activities repaired mood more effectively through 1988), elevated academic performance (Pennebaker & Fran-
cis, 1996), and a decrease in the number of visits to a physi-
distraction than through venting.
cian (Pennebaker & Beall, 1986; Pennebaker, Colder, &
Sharp, 1990). In one study, individuals who wrote after los-
ing their job were more likely to find employment 3 months
Introduction later (Spera, Buhrfeind, & Pennebaker, 1994). The bene-
Testimonials on the therapeutic power of creative work fits of writing about stress also extend to persons diagnosed
are commonplace among artists and writers. The author with depression; depressed individuals characterized as being
Graham Greene (1980) wrote that writing is “a form of ther- likely to suppress their emotions had fewer depressive symp-
apy” and wondered how it was that “all those who do not toms 6 months after expressive writing (Gortner, Rude, &
write, compose, or paint can manage to escape the madness, Pennebaker, 2006).
the melancholia, the panic fear which is inherent in the hu- Although writing about a stressful event has long-term
man situation” (p. 285). Artists and writers seem to realize benefits, writing may be less beneficial than drawing for
that creating art has the power to improve mood. short-term mood repair. In a 2004 study, Pizarro assigned
Clearly art making must improve mood by providing participants to one of three conditions: writing about a
an avenue through which to regulate one’s emotions. Re- stressful event, drawing about a stressful event, or draw-
search has demonstrated individual differences in emotion ing a still life from observation. Participants in both draw-
regulation strategies, including the ways in which individu- ing conditions reported lower negative affect after the in-
als repair negative affect (Gross, 2007; Gross & John, 2003; tervention than did those in the writing condition. An-
Larsen, 2000). Among the many strategies proposed to regu- other study showed that writing does improve short-term
late mood, two seem particularly related to art making: vent- mood, but only when the content of the writing is positive
and therefore distracts the person from focusing on negative
Editor’s Note: Doctoral candidate Jennifer E. Drake, MA,
thoughts (Hemenover, Augustine, Shulman, Tran, & Bar-
Katelyn Coleman, BA, and Ellen Winner, PhD, are in the Depart- lett, 2008). Participants who wrote about positive autobio-
ment of Psychology, Boston College. Dr. Winner is also affiliated graphical events reported greater short-term mood improve-
with Project Zero, Harvard Graduate School of Education. Cor- ment than those who wrote about their thoughts.
respondence concerning this report may be addressed to the first Research on the regulation of emotions has shown that
author at drakejc@bc.edu both distraction and venting can be effective strategies.

26
DRAKE / COLEMAN / WINNER 27

Pennebaker and Beall (1986) found that individuals who vent their feelings; mood improvement in the venting condi-
wrote about stressful or traumatic events (venting) had fewer tion was no greater than mood improvement in the control
visits to a health center 6 months later than those who wrote condition.
about the facts of the trauma or those who wrote about In our study, we hypothesized that (a) the activity of
everyday events (distraction). However, Lischetzke and Eid drawing would have a more positive effect on short-term
(2003) found venting to be beneficial only when individu- mood valence than the activity of writing, and (b) par-
als perceived themselves as competent in mood regulation. ticipants who use drawing or writing to distract from sad
Geen, Quanty, and Berkowitz (1977) reviewed the literature ideation rather than to vent their sadness would show the
and concluded that venting does not improve mood. Vent- greatest mood valence improvement.
ing seems to be related to rumination; Bushman (2002) has
shown that rumination when angry serves to increase anger. Method
In his study Bushman induced anger and then asked par-
ticipants to either hit a punching bag or sit quietly. Among Participants
those in the punching bag condition, half the participants
were asked to think about the person who had angered them The participants were 40 undergraduates enrolled in
(rumination); the other half were asked to think about some- a college in the U.S. Northeast (23 women and 17 men)
thing positive (distraction). Those who ruminated as they ranging from 18 to 22 years of age. Participants were re-
hit the punching bag grew angrier and more aggressive than cruited from psychology classes and received one research
both the participants who distracted themselves as they hit credit as part of a course requirement. The sample consisted
the bag and the participants who sat quietly. However, this primarily of students from middle- to upper-middle-class,
study did not actually allow a comparison of coping strate- well-educated families. The study was approved by the col-
gies (venting versus distraction). Such a comparison would lege’s Institutional Review Board and all participants pro-
have had to look at the effect of hitting the punching bag vided written informed consent.
(venting) versus sitting quietly thinking about something
positive (distraction). Materials and Procedure
Recent research has shown distraction to be an effective
way of coping with negative affect. Engaging in a cognitively The participants first completed a version of the Rus-
demanding task (e.g., a math problem) decreased negative sell, Weiss, and Mendelsohn (1989) Affect Grid as modi-
affect after a negative mood induction (van Dillen & Koole, fied by Goldstein (2009). The Affect Grid is a one-item self-
2007). This effect has also been demonstrated with the use of report scale that measures two dimensions of mood: (a) va-
humor. Strick, Holland, van Baaren, and van Knippenberg lence, as ranging from pleasantness to unpleasantness; and
(2009) found that individuals’ moods improved to a greater (b) arousal, as ranging from activation to deactivation. This
degree when viewing humorous stimuli that involved high measure has been found to be reliable and valid in assessing
rather than low cognitive demands. These studies demon- mood (Russell et al., 1989). Each participant rated his or her
strate that distraction from a negative mood can occur when mood “as it is right now” (mood rating Time 1) by placing
individuals engage in cognitively demanding tasks. As the- a single mark anywhere on the Affect Grid. In the modified
orized by van Dillen and Koole (2007), such tasks distract version, scores range from -9 to 9 on both axes, allowing
us by loading our working memory and “preventing mood- participants to report a negative valence. Only valence was
congruent processing” (p. 715). examined in this study because there was no theoretical rea-
The goal of the present research study was to compare son to expect arousal to be affected by art making and in
the mood repair effects of drawing versus writing, and to de- two previous studies no effect of art making on arousal was
termine whether mood is more effectively repaired when the demonstrated (Dalebroux et al., 2008; DePetrillo & Win-
medium (either drawing or writing) is used to vent negative ner, 2005).
feelings or to distract oneself from negative feelings. Previous The participants then watched a 5-minute clip from the
research has demonstrated that drawing has positive short- movie The Laramie Project, a documentary about the mur-
term mood repair benefits (Dalebroux, Goldstein, & Win- der of a young man who was tortured and killed for being
ner, 2008; DePetrillo & Winner, 2005), but whether this openly gay. The clip was intended to induce a negative mood
mood repair occurs by venting or by distraction is not yet and in previous research was found to be effective (Gold-
clear. DePetrillo and Winner (2005) reported that drawing stein, 2009). In the video clip, the young man’s father speaks
improved short-term mood more than did copying geomet- at the sentencing of his son’s murderer and describes his
ric shapes (an activity that does not allow for self-expression), son’s death, the publicity that surrounded it, and the fam-
and mood improved equally for those who vented by draw- ily’s opinion of the death penalty. To ensure that the video
ing negative images and those who distracted themselves by clip would induce a sad mood, four pilot participants rated
drawing positive images. Dalebroux et al. (2008) replicated how sad the film clip was on a 7-point scale with 1 indicat-
the finding that art making improves short-term mood to ing a low degree and 7 indicating a high degree of sadness.
a greater degree than a control activity but failed to repli- The pilot participants rated the film clip as sad (M = 5.25,
cate the equivalence of venting versus distraction as cop- SD = .96).
ing strategies. Mood improved most for those who used After viewing the clip, the experimenter asked the par-
drawing as a means to distract themselves rather than to ticipants to rate their mood again using the Affect Grid
28 SHORT-TERM MOOD REPAIR THROUGH ART

(mood rating Time 2). The experimenter then randomly as-


signed the participants to one of two conditions: writing or
drawing. There were 20 participants in each condition with
no difference in sex distribution across conditions (χ 2 =
.102, p = .749). In the writing condition, participants were
given paper and pen and were told: “Use the next 10 min-
utes to write about anything you would like. You will be able
to take it with you when you leave.” In the drawing condi-
tion, participants were given a white sheet of paper and a
fine point black marker. Due to our desire to control for the
use of color, participants in both the writing and drawing
conditions received a writing instrument of a single color.
Participants in the drawing condition were told: “Use the
next 10 minutes to draw anything you would like. You will
be able to take it with you when you leave.” Participants Figure 1 Mean Valence at Times 1, 2, and 3 for the Writ-
were then given the Affect Grid again and asked to rate their ing and Drawing Conditions
mood (mood rating Time 3).
Finally, to determine the strategy that participants used,
at the end of the session we handed out a questionnaire that Comparison of Writing and Drawing
asked participants: “Which of the following functions did
the task serve for you?” They were given three choices: “It To compare the effectiveness of writing versus drawing
helped me vent my feelings” (venting), “It helped me to in improving short-term mood, we performed a two-way re-
think about things other than the movie clip” (distraction), peated measures ANOVA, with condition as the between
and “Other—specify.” Nine participants checked “Other” subjects factor, and time as the repeated measures. We then
and thus were excluded from the strategy analyses. We used performed post-hoc paired sample t tests to clarify the re-
the remaining responses (n = 31) to classify individuals as sults. There was both a main effect of condition, F (1, 38)
having used the drawing or writing activity to discharge neg- = 4.002, p = .05, and a main effect of time, F (2, 76) =
ative feelings through venting or to distract themselves from 35.729, MSE = 8.396, p <.001. Paired sample t tests re-
negative feelings. vealed that, as expected, valence was higher at Time 3 (after
drawing or writing) than at Time 2 (p <.001). There was no
difference in valence at Time 1 and Time 3 (p = .174).
Results As shown in Figure 1, there was an interaction between
condition and time, F (1, 38) = 11.362, p = .002. This
Preliminary Results interaction occurred due to differences at Time 3 between
conditions: valence was significantly higher at Time 3 for
Table 1 presents the mean valence scores for Time 1, drawing than for writing. In the drawing condition, valence
Time 2, and Time 3 by condition. A one-way ANOVA by at Time 3 was restored to Time 1 levels: there was no dif-
condition revealed no effect of condition at Time 1, F (1, 38) ference in valence at Time 1 and Time 3 (p = .34). In the
= 0.000, p = 1.000. Thus, the drawing and writing groups writing condition, however, there was a significant difference
had equivalent valence scores prior to the mood induction. in valence between Time 1 and Time 3 (p = .01) because va-
To determine the effectiveness of the film clip in inducing a lence after the writing task did not rise to the level of Time
negative mood, a paired sample t test was performed with va- 1. Thus, our first hypothesis was supported in that drawing
lence at Time 1 and Time 2. As expected, valence was lower was found to be a more effective tool for short-term mood
at Time 2 than at Time 1, t(39) = 7.077, p <.001, indi- repair than writing.
cating that the film clip had significantly decreased mood
valence. A one-way ANOVA by condition also revealed that
there were no condition differences between conditions at Effect of Venting Versus Distraction on Mood
Time 2, F (1, 38) = 0.683, p = .414. Thus there were no Valence
differences in mood between the groups at the start of the
study, or after they viewed the film clip. Of the 40 participants, 13 reported that they used the
drawing or writing conditions to vent their sad feelings and
18 reported that they used the activity to distract them-
Table 1 Mean Valence (and Standard Deviations) for selves from feeling sad. A chi-square test showed no differ-
Each Condition at Time 1, Time 2, and Time 3 ence in strategy distribution across conditions (χ 2 = .267,
p = .605). Figure 2 shows the mean valence by condition for
Condition n Time 1 Time 2 Time 3 the individuals using venting versus distraction. To compare
Writing 20 2.58 (3.76) −2.93 (2.78) −.08 (3.99) whether strategy used (venting vs. distraction) differed by
Drawing 20 2.58 (3.48) −2.20 (2.77) 3.38 (2.24) condition (writing vs. drawing), a strategy by condition uni-
variate ANOVA was performed on valence scores at Time 3.
DRAKE / COLEMAN / WINNER 29

to clinical populations. Future research would also benefit


from examining the role that color plays in the use of visual
art making to enhance mood.
Our results were also consistent with previous findings
that distraction improves mood valence to a greater extent
than does venting, which is a form of rumination. It is pos-
sible that creating images that were unrelated to the film in-
creased cognitive demand and decreased mood-congruent
memories. This explanation would be consistent with the
working memory model developed by van Dillen and Koole
(2007) but requires further investigation. Future research
should investigate whether distraction is effective only when
the distracting task leads to a positive emotion (as in the
present study) or whether it is also effective when the dis-
tracting task leads to a neutral emotion.
Figure 2 Mean Valence at Time 3 for Writing and Draw-
ing Conditions by Strategy Future research should examine not only the actual ben-
efits of art making but also participant predictions about
the effects of art making on mood and cognition. Previ-
ous research on affective forecasting has demonstrated that
This analysis revealed no effect of strategy, F (1, 27) = 2.467,
individuals overestimate their emotional reactions to future
p = .128. There was no interaction between condition and
events (Wilson & Gilbert, 2005). Studies on how art making
strategy, F (1, 27) = 0.025, p = .875. Thus, the greater effec-
affects people that also examine participants’ own theories
tiveness of distraction occurred for both drawing and writ-
about the effects of art making would help us to understand
ing. Due to the small cell size when strategy and condition
the role of art making in human well-being.
were crossed, we also performed a one-way ANOVA by strat-
Testimonials by artists about the therapeutic power of
egy on valence at Time 3 collapsed across condition. Valence
creating are supported by this study. However, the conven-
at Time 3 was marginally higher for those who reported us-
tional wisdom that creativity is healing because it allows
ing distraction as a coping strategy (2.30 vs. 0.393), F (1, 29)
self-expression may apply only in the case of long-term ther-
= 2.802, p = .10. Thus, the results provide modest (near-
apeutic effects. For art making to improve mood immedi-
significant) support for our second hypothesis: Drawing and
ately, it is better to use art to distract oneself rather than to
writing are more effective at repairing mood when they are
express one’s pain. Perhaps this explains why Graham Greene
used to distract than when they are used to vent.
(1980) described the act of creation as a means of escape.

Discussion
This study compared the effectiveness of two forms of References
expression—drawing and writing—on short-term mood re- Bushman, B. J. (2002). Does venting anger feed or extinguish
pair and also compared the relative effectiveness in terms of the flame? Catharsis, rumination, distraction, anger, and aggres-
mood elevation of using drawing or writing to vent negative sive responding. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28,
feelings versus to distract oneself from thinking about nega- 724–731.
tive feelings. We did this by inducing a negatively valenced
mood and then assessing mood change after drawing or af- Dalebroux, A., Goldstein, T. R., & Winner, E. (2008). Short-
ter writing. There were no systematic group differences in term mood repair through art-making: Attention redeployment
mood valence either at baseline or after viewing a sad film is more effective than venting. Motivation and Emotion, 32,
288–295.
clip, although of course there likely were individual differ-
ences in mood valence at all three time points. DePetrillo, L., & Winner, E. (2005). Does art improve mood? A
Consistent with Pizarro’s (2004) study, we found that test of a key assumption underlying art therapy. Art Therapy:
art making repairs short-term mood to a greater extent than Journal of the American Art Therapy Association, 22(4), 205–212.
does writing. One possible reason for this is that drawing
may invite people to play with line and form, and hence Geen, R. G., Quanty, M. B., & Berkowitz, L. (1977). The catharsis
may serve as a more effective form of distraction. In addi- of aggression: An evaluation of a hypothesis. Advances in Exper-
tion, drawing could be a more enjoyable activity than writ- imental Social Psychology, 10, 1–37.
ing because it is a more novel activity for most adults than is
Goldstein, T. R. (2009). The pleasure of unadulterated sadness:
writing. But we admit that at present these suggestions are
Experiencing sorrow in fiction, nonfiction, and “in person.” Psy-
conjectures that remain to be tested. chology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 3, 232–237.
Although this study is based on a relative small sample,
it does suggest that art making used as a strategy of distrac- Gortner, E., Rude, S. S., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2006). Benefits of
tion is a more effective means of short-term mood repair. expressive writing in lowering rumination and depressive symp-
Future research would benefit from extending these findings toms. Behavior Therapy, 37, 292–303.
30 SHORT-TERM MOOD REPAIR THROUGH ART

Greene, G. (1980). Ways of escape. New York, NY: Simon and Pennebaker, J. W., Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., & Glaser, R. (1988). Dis-
Schuster. closure of traumas and immune function: Health implications
for psychotherapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
Gross, J. J. (Ed.). (2007). Handbook of emotion regulation. New 56, 239–245.
York, NY: Guilford Press.
Pizarro, J. (2004). The efficacy of art and writing therapy: In-
Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two creasing positive mental health outcomes and participant re-
emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relation- tention after exposure to traumatic experience. Art Therapy:
ships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Journal of the American Art Therapy Association, 21(1), 5–
85, 348–362. 12.

Hemenover, S. H., Augustine, A. A., Shulman, T. E., Tran, T. Q., Russell, J. A., Weiss, A., & Mendelsohn, G. A. (1989). Affect Grid:
& Barlett, C. (2008). Individual differences in the ability to re- A single-item scale of pleasure and arousal. Journal of Personality
pair negative affect. Emotion, 8, 468–478. and Social Psychology, 57, 493–502.

Larsen, R. J. (2000). Toward a science of mood regulation. Psycho- Spera, S., Buhrfeind, E., & Pennebaker, J. W. (1994). Expressive
logical Inquiry, 11, 129–141. writing and coping with job loss. Academy of Management Jour-
nal, 37, 722–733.
Lischetzke, T., & Eid, M. (2003). Is attention to feelings benefi-
cial or detrimental to affective well-being? Mood regulation as a Strick, M., Holland, R. W., van Baaren, R. B., & van Knippen-
moderator variable. Emotion, 3, 361–377. berg, A. (2009). Finding comfort in a joke: Consolatory ef-
fects of humor through cognitive distraction. Emotion, 9, 574–
Parkinson, B., & Totterdell, P. (1999). Classifying affect-regulation 578.
strategies. Cognition and Emotion, 13, 277–303.
Thayer, R. E., Newman, R., & McClain, T. M. (1994). Self-
Pennebaker, J. W., & Beall, S. (1986). Confronting a traumatic regulation of mood: Strategies for changing a bad mood, raising
event: Toward an understanding of inhibition and disease. Jour- energy, and reducing tension. Journal of Personality and Social
nal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 274–281. Psychology, 67, 910–925.

Pennebaker, J. W., Colder, M., & Sharp, L. K. (1990). Accelerat- van Dillen, L. F., & Koole, S. L. (2007). Clearing the mind: A
ing the coping process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- working memory model of distraction from negative mood.
ogy, 58, 528–537. Emotion, 7, 715–723.

Pennebaker, J. W., & Francis, M. E. (1996). Cognitive, emotional, Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2005). Affective forecasting:
and language processes in disclosure. Cognition and Emotion, 10, Knowing what to want. Current Directions in Psychological Sci-
601–626. ence, 14, 131–134.

You might also like