Paper 3 Effects of Allocation of Attention On Habituation To Olfactory and Visual Food Stimuli in Children

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

Physiology & Behavior 84 (2005) 313 – 319

Effects of allocation of attention on habituation to olfactory


and visual food stimuli in children
Leonard H. Epstein*, Frances G. Saad, April M. Giacomelli, James N. Roemmich
Department of Pediatrics, School of Medicine and Biomedical Sciences, State University of New York,
Farber Hall; Room G56, 3435 Main Street, Buffalo, NY 14214, United States

Received 9 August 2004; received in revised form 6 December 2004; accepted 15 December 2004

Abstract

Responding to food cues may be disrupted by allocating attention to other tasks. We report two experiments examining the effects of
allocation of attention on salivary habituation to olfactory plus visual food cues in 8–12-year-old children. In Experiment 1, 42 children were
presented with a series of 8 hamburger food stimulus presentations. During each intertrial interval, participants completed a controlled (hard),
or automatic (easy) visual memory task, or no task (control). In Experiment 2, 22 children were presented with 10 presentations of a pizza
food stimulus and either listened to audiobook or no audiobook control. Results of Experiment 1 showed group differences in rate of change
in salivation ( p=0.014). Children in the controlled task did not habituate to repeated food cues, while children in the automatic ( pb0.005) or
no task ( pb0.001) groups decreased responding over time. In Experiment 2, groups differed in the rate of change in salivation ( p=0.004).
Children in the no audiobook group habituated ( pb0.001), while children in the audiobook group did not habituate. Changes in the rate of
habituation when attending to non-food stimuli while eating may be a mechanism for increasing energy intake.
D 2004 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Habituation; Salivation; Attention; Olfaction

A large and consistent body of research has shown that In addition to the influence of novel food cues on
rats [1–5], non-human primates [6,7] and humans [8–13] dishabituation or recovery of responding, non-food stimuli
habituate to repeated food cues, and dishabitutate or recover can serve to dishabituate responding to food cues. For
responding when a new food cue is presented. Research has example, playing a video game in between food presenta-
also shown that habituation to food cues is associated with tions can slow the rate of habituation [16]. Playing video
the termination of eating in non-human primates [6,7] and games can dishabituate responding, and the more arousal
humans [8], and the rate of change for salivary habituation generated by a video game the greater the degree of
and motivated responding for food is very similar in dishabituation [10]. One model that may help understand
children [14]. Presenting a new food after habituation can how food or environmental stimuli can act to influence
result in dishabituation of salivary responding and a responding to repeated food cues is the information
recovery of eating [8]. Similarly, presenting a new food in processing model developed by Wagner [17–19]. Wagner
a motivated behavior paradigm will result in a resumption in argues that habituation is mediated in part by the matching
responding for the new food in children [14]. Dishabituation of new information to information that is stored in short-
of responding to new food cues may in part mediate the term memory, and allocation of attention to new information
influence of food variety on increases in food intake [15]. removes information from short-term memory and disrupts
habituation. The information processing model was tested
by having participants engage in either a controlled search
* Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 716 829 3400; fax: +1 716 829 3993. task (demanding attentional resources), an automatic search
E-mail address: LHENET@buffalo.edu (L.H. Epstein). task (needing fewer attentional resources), or no task in
0031-9384/$ - see front matter D 2004 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.12.009
314 L.H. Epstein et al. / Physiology & Behavior 84 (2005) 313–319

between presentations of repeated food cues in two experi- 1.1.2. Procedures


ments [12]. Research has suggested that there is a distinction Parents of the participants were screened by telephone
between automatic and controlled modes of information to determine whether they met the above inclusion
processing [20–22]. Automatic processes require fewer criteria, with the exception of liking of the test foods,
attentional resources and are possible when stimuli are which was assessed during the session. Eligible partic-
consistently and reliably mapped to their appropriate ipants were scheduled for a 90-min appointment and the
responses. On the other hand, controlled responses demand parents were instructed to not have the children consume
more attentional resources when the stimulus-response the test food for 24 h and not to eat for at least 3 h prior
mappings are inconsistent and unreliable. Consistent with to coming to the laboratory. Upon arrival to the
the allocation of attention hypothesis, the automatic and no laboratory, participants and parents read and signed
task groups habituated to the repeated presentation of food informed consent and assent forms. Parents then com-
cues in both experiments, but the controlled search group pleted a socioeconomic form [27], while children com-
did not. pleted 5-point Likert scales, which assessed liking of the
The purpose of this study is to extend our research on study foods and hunger, the Dutch Eating Behavior
the influence of allocation of attention on habituation to Questionnaire adapted for children [28] and a same-day
food cues to children. Research suggests that children food recall. Participants’ heights and weights were then
consume a substantial amount of energy while engaged in measured.
other activities, such as watching television [23,24], and
reducing access to behaviors such as television watching 1.1.2.1. Experimental tasks. To adapt participants to the
may reduce energy intake [25]. Consistently engaging in experimental methods, participants received four 1-min
alternative activities while eating may alter the rate of practice sessions of the search task, followed by a 1-min
habituation to repeated food cues, and thus, increase habituation trial using water as a neutral stimulus. They
energy intake. We have completed an initial study in were then provided with eight 1-min habituation trials to
children demonstrating habituation to repeated food cues half of a fresh Burger KingR hamburger sandwich served on
and showed that the habituation model fit both salivation a 7 in. plate and heated in a microwave oven for 25 s. After
as well as motivated responding to obtain food [14]. The each habituation trial, food stimuli were promptly removed
two studies in this report are designed to replicate the from the experimental room. During salivation collection,
influence of tasks that vary in their attentional demands on participants wore headphones and listened to 62 dB of white
habituation to repeated food cues, and to examine the noise to control for auditory stimulation. After completion
influence of common auditory distractors on the rate of of the habituation trials, food liking was reassessed. The
habituation. Experiment 1 compares the influence of search tasks took place in the 1-min period between
memory tasks that require different degrees of attention, habituation trials 1 and 8, presented on a color video
while Experiment 2 evaluates the influence of attending to monitor controlled by a BASIC computer program. The
interesting audiobooks. tasks consisted of a four consonant memory-set presented
for 10 s followed by 20 presentations of 2 character search
frames shown for 2 s. Each search frame remained on the
1. Experiment 1 screen for the same duration to equate total task duration
across groups. Participants responded yes by pressing the
1.1. Method left mouse button if a search frame matched the memory-set
or no by pressing the right button if the search frame did not
1.1.1. Participants match a memory-set.
Participants were 21 male and 21 female non-obese (b95 The controlled search task had target and distractor
BMI percentile) [26] children between the ages of 8 and 12 consonants that varied from trial to trial [20,21]. The
years old recruited from a newspaper advertisement and controlled task provided participants with memory-set that
provided US$20.00 compensation for participation. Poten- included four consonants, and search frames that either
tial participants were excluded if they met any of the included at least one consonant that matched the consonants
following criteria: medications or conditions that could in the memory-set (correct response) or two incorrect
influence olfactory sensory responsiveness or appetite (e.g., consonants (incorrect response). The automatic search task
upper respiratory illness, diabetes, ADHD, methylpheni- required less allocation of attention than the controlled
date); dietary restraint that would alter responding to food search task since the search frames included either a correct
cues; current psychological disorder or developmental consonant and a numeric character (correct response) or two
disability; or not rating test foods at least moderately liked. numeric characters (incorrect response). The no task
Participants were randomly assigned to one of the three condition did not involve attending to a task, and
groups: controlled search task, automatic search task and no participants in the no task condition continued to listen to
task. The sample included 98% non-Hispanic white children white noise during the intertrial interval. The number of
and 2% African American children. errors in each memory-set trial were recorded as a
L.H. Epstein et al. / Physiology & Behavior 84 (2005) 313–319 315

manipulation check for the controlled search and automatic to the test appointment, to ensure that the participants did
search groups. not consume the test foods earlier in the day and to ensure
that children did not eat 3 h prior to the test appointment.
1.1.2.2. The laboratory environment. The laboratory used Energy intake was calculated using Nutritionist V nutrient
for these experiments was specially constructed for eating analysis software [31].
and olfactory experiments. To isolate the olfactory stimuli,
and eliminate any possibility that stimuli from earlier trials 1.1.3.3. Dietary restraint. Dietary restraint was measured
could influence current responding, the food stimulus was using the dietary restraint scale of the child version of the
removed after each trial and brought into another room. Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire that has been used for
The laboratory is equipped with an air delivery system that both males and females in previous studies [28]. A total
continually cycles fresh air into each room. The rooms are score greater than 7 for questions 1 through 6 was
in negative pressure, so that the exhaust has greater cubic considered dietary restrained.
feet per minute than the supply, and the air in the
laboratory rooms is circulated approximately 10 times/h, 1.1.3.4. Demographics. A demographic questionnaire [27]
or new air is circulated once every 6 min. In addition, the was utilized to obtain information about socioeconomic
experimental room (1385 ft3) includes a HEPA (high status, class, ethnicity and race.
efficiency particle arresting) filter that circulates air from
150 to 175 ft3/min. The combination of the air control 1.1.3.5. Anthropometrics. Height (cm) was assessed by
system and the HEPA filter reduce the possibility of smells using a SECA (SECA, Hanover, MD) stadiometer, and
lingering in the room beyond an individual stimulus weight (kg) was measured by the use of a balance beam
presentation. scale calibrated daily. Height and weight were converted to
BMI (kg/m2).
1.1.3. Measures
1.1.4. Analytical plan
1.1.3.1. Salivation. Whole mouth parotid salivary flow was Characteristics of the participants were compared
measured using the Strongin-Hinsie Peck method [29]. between groups using a one-way analysis of variance
Participants were seated in a comfortable chair and instructed (ANOVA). Changes in salivation from baseline were
on proper dental roll placement (cylindrical, 7 mm diameter, assessed using a mixed analysis of covariance (ANCOVA),
38 mm length, Richmond Dental, Charlotte, NC). They with group (controlled search, automatic search, no task
placed cotton rolls under the tongue and on both the left and control) as the between-subjects variable and trials (1–8) as
right sides of mouth between the cheek and lower gum. After the within-subjects factor and baseline salivation as the
the experimenter ensured that placement of the cotton rolls covariate. Baseline salivation was used as the covariate
was correct, participants were instructed not to swallow any because of the relationship between initial levels of
saliva, not to move their tongue, talk or make any chewing salivation and change in salivation over trials, which was
motions. Olfactory and visual stimuli were presented and significant for seven of the eight trials in Experiment 1.
saliva collected for 1-min periods, followed by 1-min Linear contrasts were used to examine the rate of change by
intertrial intervals. The food stimulus involved having group over the eight salivation trials. Accuracy on the
participants smell and look at food stimuli held approx- memory tasks were assessed using a two-factor mixed
imately 3 in. from their nose, and to attend to the aroma of the repeated measures ANOVA with group (controlled search,
food while imagining its taste. The food stimuli were automatic search) as the between-subjects variable and
microwaved prior to presentation to enhance olfactory intertrial intervals (1–7), as the within-subjects variables.
stimulation. After the 1-min collection period, subjects Statistical analyses were completed using SYSTAT version
removed the dental rolls and returned them to the plastic 10 [32].
bag. Salivation was measured by pre- and post-weights of
cotton rolls to 0.01 g on an Ohaus Precision Standard Scale 1.2. Results
(Florham Park, NJ). The difference from baseline served as
the dependent measure. The average participant was 10.9F1.4 (MFS.D.) years
of age, had a body mass index (BMI=kg/m2) of 18.0F2.5, a
1.1.3.2. Subjective ratings. Liking of study foods, Burger BMI percentile of 53.3F27.7 and a dietary restraint score of
KingR hamburgers in Experiment 1 and Domino’sR cheese 2.2F1.9. Participants rated the test food as moderately to
pizzas in Experiment 2, were rated on 5-point Likert-type highly liked on a 5-point Likert scale of liking (hamburger
scales [30] with 1 anchored by bdo not likeQ and 5 anchored sandwich=4.1F0.8). Average baseline salivation was
by blike very muchQ. Hunger was assessed by a 5-point 1.18F0.71 g. No significant differences were observed in
Likert scale, anchored by 1 as bnot hungryQ to 5 anchored as any baseline measures between groups (Table 1).
bvery hungryQ. Food consumed the day of the experiment The salivation results are presented for the three groups in
was assessed to evaluate total energy intake consumed prior the left graph of Fig. 1. A significant interaction of grouptrial
316 L.H. Epstein et al. / Physiology & Behavior 84 (2005) 313–319

Table 1
Characteristics of participants in the controlled search, automatic search and the no task groups for Experiment 1
Group
Control task Automatic task No task
M S.D. M S.D. M S.D.
Age (years) 11.3 1.2 10.9 1.3 10.6 1.5
Body mass index (BMI) 17.9 2.4 18.4 2.8 17.6 2.4
BMI percentile 50.5 26.8 57.2 30.2 52.3 27.6
Restraint scores 2.5 1.7 2.2 2.4 2.0 1.8
Hamburger liking 4.0 0.7 4.0 1.0 4.3 0.8
Subjective hunger 4.5 0.5 4.1 0.9 4.7 0.6
Hamburger olfactory hedonics 4.4 0.6 3.9 1.1 3.9 1.0
Self-reported calories (kcal) 753 299 697 272 777 311
Socioeconomic status 43.1 12.1 42.5 8.9 46.3 10.4
Baseline salivation (g) 1.27 0.71 1.09 0.72 1.18 0.75

was found for salivary habituation ( F(14,266)=2.07, change pattern of errors over trials are presented in the right
p=0.014), and there was a significant change over trials graph of Fig. 1.
( F(7,266)=2.55, p=0.015). Contrasts showed the automatic
search group ( F(1,38)=9.04, p=0.005) and the no task group
( F(1,38)=21.67, pb0.001) displayed habituation over the 2. Experiment 2
trials as their salivation gradually decreased, and there was no
significant difference in their rate of change. The controlled 2.1. Methods
task showed no significant change over trials. Contrasts
revealed a significant difference in the rate of change between 2.1.1. Participants
the controlled task versus the two groups that did habituate Participants were 12 male and 10 female 9–12-year-old
(automatic search and no-task groups) ( F(1,38)=6.25, non-obese (b95 BMI percentile) [26] children. Participants
p=0.017). Children showed a non-significant increase in were randomly assigned to one of two groups: audiobook
liking ratings from 4.1 to 4.4, with no differences in the and no audiobook. The sample included 59% non-Hispanic
change by groups. white children, 9% Hispanic children, 18% African Amer-
The analysis of the accuracy on trials 1–7 showed a ican and 14% were mixed race of African American and
significant interaction of grouptrial ( F(6,156)=4.53, American Indian.
pb0.001), with a main effect of total errors ( F(1,26)=21.82,
pb0.001), as well as a main effect of trials ( F(6,156)=3.29, 2.1.2. Procedures
p=0.004). Contrasts showed a significant decrease in errors General procedures were identical to those of Experiment
over trials for the controlled search group ( F(1,26)=23.96, 1. Before the habituation trials began, the participants rated
pb0.001). The differences between groups in errors and the how much they liked the study food stimulus, Domino’sR

Fig. 1. Changes (meanFS.E.M.) in salivation (left panel) and errors (right panel) for participants in the controlled search, automatic search and no task groups
for Experiment 1.
L.H. Epstein et al. / Physiology & Behavior 84 (2005) 313–319 317

Table 2 multiple-choice questions about the story which were


Characteristics of participants in the audio book group and the no audio written at the third grade level and the liking scales were
book group for Experiment 2
administered. Participants in the no audiobook group
Group
continued to listen to 62 dB of white noise during the
Audiobook No audiobook intertrial intervals.
M S.D. M S.D.
Age (years) 10.7 1.1 11.4 1.2 2.1.3. Analytical plan
Body mass index (BMI) 18.4 1.5 19.3 2.8 A one-way ANOVA compared characteristics of the
BMI percentile 64.9 19.5 64.6 24.7 participants between groups. Changes in salivation from
Restraint scores 4.1 3.3 3.2 2.2
Pizza liking 4.7 0.5 4.8 0.4
baseline were assessed using a mixed analysis of covariance
Subjective hunger 4.4 0.8 4.0 0.9 (ANCOVA), with group (audiobook, no audiobook) as the
Pizza olfactory hedonics 4.5 0.8 4.3 1.2 between-subjects variable and trials (1–10) as the within-
Self-reported calories (kcal) 545 378 817 452 subjects factor and baseline salivation as the covariate.
Socioeconomic status 38.2 13.1 45.0 10.9 Baseline salivation was used as the covariate, as baseline
Baseline salivation (g) 1.15 0.66 1.23 0.50
levels were related to change for 7 of the 10 trials in
Experiment 2. Contrasts were used to examine the rate of
cheese pizza and each of the three audiobooks using a 5- change over the 10 salivation trials for each group.
point Likert scale, with 1 indicating bdo not like at allQ and 5
indicating blike very muchQ. The participants then ranked 2.2. Results
the audiobooks, placing the number 1 next to their most
favorite and the number 3 next to their least favorite. The The average participant was 11.0F1.1 years of age,
three audiobooks were from a series of Bunnicula children’s had a BMI of 18.8F2.2, had a BMI percentile of
audiobooks [33–35]. Children were provided with ten 1-min 64.7F21.7 and a restraint score of 3.6F2.8. Participants
habituation trials with a fresh slice of a Domino’sR cheese rated the habituating food stimulus pizza and audiobooks
pizza served on a 9 in. plate and heated in the microwave as moderately to highly liked on a 5-point Likert scale of
for 10 s. liking (pizza=4.8F0.4, audiobooks=3.7F0.6). Average
Participants in the audiobook group were presented baseline salivation was 1.19F0.57 g. No significant
sequential presentations from their most preferred audio- between group differences were observed in any of the
book stimuli using a Sony DiscmanR (Sony, Tokyo, Japan) baseline measures (Table 2). All children in the audio-
during the 1-min intertrial intervals that preceded each of the book condition answered two questions about the story
10 salivary habituation trials. Participants were told to correctly.
attend to the audiobook and they would be asked questions Salivation results presented in Fig. 2 showed a between-
at the conclusion of the experimental session. When the group difference ( F(1,19)=10.54, p=0.004) and a signifi-
habituation trials were completed, children were asked two cant interaction of grouptrial ( F(9,171)=2.34, p=0.016).

Fig. 2. Changes (meanFS.E.M.) in salivation for participants in the continuous audio group and no audio group for Experiment 2.
318 L.H. Epstein et al. / Physiology & Behavior 84 (2005) 313–319

The no audiobook group showed habituation over trials the failure of bulimics to experience satiation to increased
( F(1,19)=28.08, pb0.001), while the audiobook group energy and food volume. Consistent with this hypothesis,
showed no significant changes over trials. Children showed bulimic participants increase responding or sensitize to
a non-significant increase in liking ratings from 4.4 to 4.7, repeated food cues [41] which is consistent with binge
with no differences in the change by groups. eating meals that are part of bulimic psychopathology. It is
not known whether these differences in the rate of
habituation precede the development of these disorders,
3. Discussion and may be etiological, or emerge after obesity or bulimia
are stable.
These experiments replicate the findings that children The design of Experiment 1 contrasted tasks that differed
habituate to repeated food cues [14], and Experiment 1 in their requirement for allocation of attentional resources,
extends the observation that allocation of attention disrupts but required a similar response, suggesting that only tasks
the development of habituation that has been shown in that require controlled attention will disrupt the develop-
adults [12] to children. Children who were in the automatic ment of habituation. Experiment 2 compared groups that
search group habituated, as shown by a significant differ- differed in attending to an auditory story or doing no task,
ence in salivation over the eight trials, while children in the which provides an initial attempt to assess generalization of
controlled task group did not show a significant change over controlled laboratory tasks to behaviors similar to those that
time. Both tasks require a matching response, but differ in children may engage in while eating. However, it is not
the degree of allocation of attention, which controls for the possible to know from this study whether listening to any
influence of the overt behavioral response on habituation. sounds would have the same effect on habituation, even if
Not every environmental stimulus will disrupt habituation. they were familiar or not stimulating. An interesting
The degree of allocation of attention to an external stimulus experiment would be to vary the familiarity of the audio
is related to the disruption of the process of habituation stories, and have children listen to familiar or unfamiliar
[17,19]. stories. Since children may have to allocate less attention to
Experiment 2 shows that listening to an audiobook can familiar versus unfamiliar stories, it could be predicted that
disrupt the development of habituation to food cues. children assigned to the unfamiliar story condition would
Listening to audiobooks represents a situation that requires not habituate whereas children assigned to the familiar
attending to auditory stimuli. Watching television or videos condition would habituate.
requires attending to both auditory and visual cues, and thus One limitation of the present studies is that food intake
would be expected to be more distracting than auditory was not measured. Research in adults has consistently
stimulation alone. An alternative paradigm would be to shown a relationship between the development of habitu-
investigate the influences of reading, an activity that ation and the termination of eating [6–8], and that the
involves only visual cues and is often paired with food introduction of a novel food will recover habituated
consumption. responding and a recovery of consumatory behaviors [8].
The influence of attending to external stimuli on However, research on habituation and eating in children is
habituation to food cues may provide insight into in the beginning stages, and the limits of the generalization
mechanisms by which attending to external stimuli while from habituation to food cues to eating in children is not
eating may increase the amount of energy consumed known. Additional research is needed to examine how
during a meal. This may be relevant to understanding why relevant findings obtained using the habituation paradigm is
television watching and obesity are related in children for understanding factors that influence meal termination
[36,37], as well as providing a mechanism for under- and energy intake.
standing why people consume more energy when eating This study focused on the attentional properties of
with others than when alone, often called social facilitation environmental stimuli, but previous research has shown
of eating [38,39]. that the degree of arousal can influence the strength of
The present study focused on how attentional processes dishabituating stimuli [10]. It is possible that the controlled
may influence habituation in normal children. Habituation attentional task would also increase arousal relative to the
may also be a model for disordered eating. For example, automatic task, and arousal may independently influence
both obese [40] and bulimic [41] participants show differ- habituation, as well as interacting with the degree of
ences in the rate of habituation to repeated food cues when allocation of attention. Future research examining the
compared to participants without these disorders. Compared influence of other environmental stimuli such as television
to non-obese participants, obese participants habituate at a watching on the development of habituation to food is
slower rate [40], and habituating at a slower rate may be needed. Studying the relationship between factors that
related in part to the slower development of satiety and disrupt the development of habituation and energy intake
greater energy intake in obese compared to non-obese in children is another area that needs to be investigated. It
persons. Bulimia represents a disorder that is characterized would also be interesting to examine whether there are
by repeated episodes of binge eating, which may be due to individual differences in how children attend to external
L.H. Epstein et al. / Physiology & Behavior 84 (2005) 313–319 319

stimuli and how these individual differences may influence [18] Wagner AR. Expectancies and the priming of STM. In: Hulse SH,
the process of habituation and food intake. Children who Fowler H, Honig WK, editors. Cognitive Process in Animal Behavior.
Hillsdale, NJ7 Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1978. p. 177 – 209.
experience a greater disruption of habituation may be those [19] Wagner AR. Habituation and memory. In: Dickinson A, Boakes RA,
children who consume more food while attending to other editors. Mechanisms of Learning and Motivation. Hillsdale (NJ)7
environmental stimuli, such as watching television. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1979. p. 53 – 82.
[20] Schneider W, Shiffrin RM. Controlled and automatic human
information processing: I. Detection, search, and attention. Psychol
Rev 1977;84:1 – 66.
Acknowledgements [21] Shiffrin RM, Schneider W. Controlled and automatic human
information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending,
This research was supported by grant HD 44725 awarded and a general theory. Psychol Rev 1977;84:127 – 90.
to Dr. Epstein. [22] Shiffrin RM, Schneider W. Automatic and controlled processing
revisited. Psychol Rev 1984;91:269 – 76.
[23] Matheson DM, Killen JD, Wang Y, Varady A, Robinson TN.
Children’s food consumption during television viewing. Am J Clin
References Nutr 2004;79:1088 – 94.
[24] Epstein LH, Paluch RA, Consalvi A, Riordan K, Scholl T. Effects of
[1] Swithers SE, Hall WG. Does oral experience terminate ingestion? manipulating sedentary behavior on physical activity and food intake.
Appetite 1994;23:113 – 38. J Pediatr 2002;140:334 – 9.
[2] Swithers SE. Effects of physiological state on oral habituation in [25] Epstein LH, Roemmich JN, Paluch RA, Raynor HA. The influence of
developing rats: cellular and extracellular dehydration. Dev Psycho- changes in sedentary behavior on energy and macronutrient intake in
biol 1995;28:131 – 45. youth; 2004. Manuscript submitted for publication.
[3] Swithers-Mulvey SE, Miller GL, Hall WG. Habituation of oromotor [26] Kuczmarski RJ, Ogden CL, Grummer-Strawn LM, Flegal KM, Guo SS,
responding to oral infusions in rat pups. Appetite 1991;17:55 – 67. Wei R, et al. CDC Growth Charts: United States, vol. 314.
[4] Swithers-Mulvey SE, Hall WG. Control of ingestion by oral Hyattsville (MD)7 National Center for Health Statistics; 2000.
habituation in rats. Behav Neurosci 1992;106:710 – 7. p. 24 – 5.
[5] Swithers-Mulvey SE, Mishu KR, Hall WG. Oral habituation in rat [27] Hollingshead AB. Four Factor Index of Social Status. New Haven
pups is in the brainstem. Physiol Behav 1992;51:639 – 42. (CT)7 Yale University; 1975.
[6] Rolls ET, Sienkiewicz ZJ, Yaxley S. Hunger modulates the responses [28] Hill AJ, Pallin V. Dieting awareness and low self-worth: related issues
to gustatory stimuli of single neurons in the caudolateral orbitofrontal in 8-year-old girls. Int J Eat Disord 1998;24:405 – 13.
cortex of the macaque monkey. Eur J Neurosci 1989;1:53 – 60. [29] Peck RE. The SHP test—an aid in the detection and measurement of
[7] Rolls ET, Yaxley S, Sienkiewicz ZJ. Gustatory responses of single depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1959;1:35 – 40.
neurons in the caudolateral orbitofrontal cortex of the macaque [30] van Laerhoven H, van der Zaag-Loonen HJ, Derkx BH. A comparison
monkey. J Neurophysiol 1990;64:1055 – 66. of Likert scale and visual analogue scales as response options in
[8] Wisniewski L, Epstein LH, Caggiula AR. Effect of food change children’s questionnaires. Acta Paediatr 2004;93:830 – 5.
on consumption, hedonics, and salivation. Physiol Behav 1992;52: [31] First DataBank I. Nutritionist V. San Bruno (CA)7 First Databank;
21 – 6. 2000.
[9] Epstein LH, Caggiula AR, Perkins KA, Mitchell SL, Rodefer JS. [32] Systat Software. Systat 10.2. Richmond (CA)7 SYSTAT Software;
Abstinence from smoking influences habituation to food cues. Physiol 2002.
Behav 1992;52:641 – 6. [33] Howe J, Garber V. Nighty-nightmare. New York (NY)7 Random
[10] Epstein LH, Mitchell SL, Caggiula AR. The effect of subjective and House Audio Publishing Group; 1987.
physiological arousal on dishabituation of salivation. Physiol Behav [34] Howe J, Garber V. Howliday Inn. New York (NY)7 Random House
1993;53:593 – 7. Audio Publishing Group; 1982.
[11] Epstein LH, Caggiula AR, Rodefer JS, Wisniewski L, Mitchell SL. [35] Howe J, Garber V. Return to Howliday Inn. New York (NY)7 Random
The effects of calories and taste on habituation of the human salivary House Audio Publishing Group; 1992.
response. Addict Behav 1993;18:179 – 85. [36] Dietz WH, Strasburger VC. Children, adolescents, and television.
[12] Epstein LH, Paluch R, Smith JD, Sayette M. Allocation of attentional Curr Prob Pediatr 1991;21:8 – 31.
resources during habituation to food cues. Psychophysiology [37] Gortmaker SL, Must A, Sobol AM, Peterson K, Colditz GA, Dietz
1997;34:59 – 64. WH. Television watching as a cause of increasing obesity among
[13] Epstein LH, Paluch RA. Habituation of facial muscle responses to children in the United States, 1986–1990. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med
repeated food stimuli. Appetite 1997;29:213 – 24. 1996;150:356 – 62.
[14] Epstein LH, Saad FG, Handley EA, Roemmich JN, Hawk LW, [38] de Castro JM, de Castro ES. Spontaneous meal patterns of humans:
McSweeney FK. Habituation of salivation and motivated responding influence of the presence of other people. Am J Clin Nutr 1989;50:
for food in children. Appetite 2003;41:283 – 9. 237 – 47.
[15] Raynor HA, Epstein LH. Dietary variety, energy regulation and [39] de Castro JM. Social facilitation of duration and size but not rate of
obesity. Psychol Bull 2001;127:325 – 41. the spontaneous meal intake of humans. Physiol Behav 1990;47:
[16] Epstein LH, Rodefer JS, Wisniewski L, Caggiula AR. Habituation and 1129 – 35.
dishabituation of human salivary response. Physiol Behav 1992;51: [40] Epstein LH, Paluch R, Coleman KJ. Differences in salivation to
945 – 50. repeated food cues in obese and nonobese women. Psychosom Med
[17] Wagner AR. Priming in STM: an information-processing mechanism 1996;58:160 – 4.
for self-generated or retrieval-generated depression in performance. [41] Wisniewski L, Epstein LH, Marcus MD, Kaye W. Differences in
In: Tighe TJ, Leaton RN, editors. Habituation. Hillsdale, NJ7 salivary habituation to palatable foods in bulimia nervosa patients and
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1976. p. 95 – 128. controls. Psychosom Med 1997;59:427 – 33.

You might also like