Gerard Naring, Mariette Brie & Andre Brouwers

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 14

Work & Stress, October December 2006; 20(4): 303 315


Beyond demand control: Emotional labour and symptoms
of burnout in teachers

GÉRARD NÄRING1,2, MARIETTE BRIËT1, & ANDRÉ BROUWERS1


1
Faculty of Psychology, Open University, The Netherlands, and 2Department of Clinical Psychology,
Radboud University Nijmegen, The Netherlands

Abstract
Teaching is a profession that involves a high level of emotional labour. This includes such behaviours
as surface acting (displaying an emotion that is not actually felt), deep acting (the activity undertaken
to actually feel a required emotion), and suppression of emotion. In many professions, this emotional
labour is thought to be related to high levels of burnout. The aim of our study was to show that
emotional labour has a unique relationship with burnout that is separate from its relationship with the
variables of the Demand Control Support (DCS) model. Emotional labour was studied, together with
the variables of the Karasek Job Demand Control Support model, in a random sample of 365
mathematics teachers in the Netherlands. We used the Dutch Questionnaire on Emotional Labor (D-
QEL) that measures: (1) surface acting, (2) deep acting, (3) suppression, and (4) emotional
consonance. In line with other studies, job characteristics were found to be specifically related to
emotional exhaustion. Surface acting was significantly related to depersonalization, and emotional
consonance (the absence of emotional labour) was related to personal accomplishment. We conclude
that whereas the DCS model has been valuable for understanding emotional exhaustion, emotional
labour provides an additional perspective for understanding work stress.

Keywords: Emotional labour, burnout, teaching, work-related stress, job demands, job control, support,
emotive effort

Introduction
Teaching is a profession that requires almost constant interaction with students. A teacher
has to be enthusiastic and lively in order to catch and hold students’ attention.
Furthermore, teachers may adopt extreme ebullience when praising a student for a good
answer or give an impression of calm confidence when confronted with a disruptive student.
Furthermore, teachers are expected to ensure the orderly conduct of classes throughout the
day, every working day. In order to perform these tasks adequately, teachers have to show or
exaggerate some emotions (Ogbonna & Harris, 2004) and minimize or suppress the
expression of other emotions (Ybema & Smulders, 2002). Teachers consider the faking of
emotions to be stressful (Ogbonna & Harris, 2004), but the exact consequences for their
job-related well-being following strict regulation of their emotions are not fully known.
Teaching is a profession with a high level of stress (Johnson, Cooper, Cartwright, Donald,
Taylor, & Millet, 2005) and a high level of burnout (Brouwers & Tomic, 2000). Because

Correspondence: G. Näring, Faculty of Psychology, Open University, P.O. Box 9103, 6500 HD Nijmegen, The
Netherlands. Tel: 31 24 3612706. E-mail: gerard.naring@ou.nl

ISSN 0267-8373 print/ISSN 1464-5335 online # 2006 Taylor & Francis


DOI: 10.1080/02678370601065182
304 G. Näring, M. Briët & A. Brouwers

emotional exhaustion is a key characteristic of the burnout syndrome (Maslach, Schaufeli,


& Leiter, 2001), an emotional labour perspective may be useful in trying to understand the
development of emotional exhaustion. Hochschild was the first to note that, especially in
service jobs, employees are often required to show certain emotions in order to please the
customer. Having to show such emotions while one is not actually feeling them, or having to
suppress one’s own emotions when their expression does not seem appropriate, were taken
together in devising the concept of emotional labour (Hochschild, 1983). Hochschild
introduced the term surface acting to refer to the display of the characteristics of an emotion
that are regarded as appropriate, but are not actually felt. Deep acting describes the activity
that is undertaken to actually feel an emotion that is thought to be required. Finally,
emotional consonance captures the situation where somebody effortlessly feels the emotion
that is required in a certain situation.
Hochschild already noted that the requirement to express emotions that are incompatible
with experienced emotions would cause repeated stress (Hochschild, 1983). As repeated
stress is thought to lead to reactions of psychological strain and physical illness (van der
Doef & Maes, 1999), an increasing number of studies have explored the relationship
between emotional labour and job stress or burnout. Several studies have reported
significant relationships between emotional labour and the emotional exhaustion dimension
of burnout (Brotheridge & Grandey, 2002; Brotheridge & Lee, 2003; Pugliesi, 1999;
Zammuner & Lotto, 2001) and thus made the theoretical importance of this relationship
clear. These studies did not, however, assess the relevance of this relationship, taking into
consideration the importance of several well-known stressful work characteristics as job
demands, job control, and social support (Karasek, 1979). The aim of the present study is
to assess the relevance of an emotional labour perspective in studying burnout when the
variables of Karasek’s job characteristics model are taken into account.
The relationship between job characteristics and well-being is usually studied with the
Demand Control Support (DCS) model, and this model has also been successfully applied
to the study of job-related well-being such as emotional or work-related exhaustion and
burnout (van der Doef & Maes, 1999). In general, there is now accumulated evidence that
the three now-classical variables from this model (high job demands, low possibilities to
regulate one’s work, little support) each cause strain, and that their effect is cumulative (de
Lange, Taris, Kompier, Houtman, & Bongers, 2003). For burnout, there is also substantial
evidence of an interactive effect of demands and control (the strain hypothesis of the DCS
model) and much less evidence for the iso-strain hypothesis, which specifies a negative
effect of the combination of high demands, low control, and little social support (van der
Doef & Maes, 1999).
The DCS model is thus useful for our understanding of burnout, but there is clear
evidence that emotional labour also has significant relationships with burnout level
(Zammuner & Galli, 2005). The relative importance of both emotional labour and the
DCS model in predicting burnout level is, however, not known exactly. Pugliesi (1999)
found negative effects of emotional labour on job satisfaction that were independent of the
effect of job demands and control. In another study the need to suppress emotions
predicted emotional exhaustion, and lack of support from colleagues or supervisor had both
an independent as well as a buffering effect (Ybema & Smulders, 2002). As the latter study
did not include quantitative demands and control, the DCS model was only partially taken
into account.
Burnout and emotional labour in teachers 305

The aim of this study is to show that emotional labour has a unique relationship with
burnout that is separate from its relationship with the variables from the DCS model. This
line of reasoning results in several hypotheses. We expect to replicate the positive
relationship between the emotional labour concept of surface acting and the burnout
dimensions of emotional exhaustion and depersonalization (Brotheridge & Grandey, 2002;
Grandey, 2003; Zammuner & Lotto, 2001):
. Hypothesis 1a: surface acting will have a significant positive relationship with
emotional exhaustion that is separate from its relationship with the variables of the
DCS model.
. Hypothesis 1b: surface acting will have a significant positive relationship with
depersonalization that is separate from its relationship with the variables of the DCS
model.

A positive relationship between deep acting and emotional exhaustion has been previously
reported (Grandey, 2003), but this is not a consistent finding (Brotheridge & Grandey,
2002; Zammuner & Lotto, 2001). The effects of deep acting may vary, as successful deep
acting will by definition result in emotional consonance. This also explains why a positive
relationship between deep acting and personal accomplishment has been reported
(Brotheridge & Grandey, 2002). In our study, no specific hypotheses for deep acting will
be formulated.
In a comparative study of various professions, one type of emotional labour, the need to
suppress emotions, was studied (Ybema & Smulders, 2002). The jobs of the more than
4,000 participants were first grouped into 40 categories. Police, firemen, and security
officers indicated most strongly that they had to suppress emotions, but teachers
experienced this quite often too. The need to hide emotions had a strong relationship
with emotional exhaustion. This results in our second hypothesis:
. Hypothesis 2a: suppression of emotions will have a significant positive relationship
with emotional exhaustion that is separate from its relationship with the variables of the
DCS model.
. Hypothesis 2b: suppression of emotions will have a significant positive relationship
with depersonalization that is separate from its relationship with the variables of the
DCS model.

Emotional consonance is a dimension indicating that felt emotions do not call for the
activation of regulatory processes (Zammuner & Galli, 2005). A high level of emotional
consonance will indicate that a person effortlessly expresses emotions that are felt and that
these emotions are at the same time required for the job. This should result in a heightened
feeling of personal accomplishment and the absence of emotional exhaustion:
. Hypothesis 3a: emotional consonance will have a significant negative relationship with
depersonalization that is separate from its relationship with the variables of the DCS
model.
. Hypothesis 3b: emotional consonance will have a significant positive relationship with
personal accomplishment that is separate from its relationship with the variables of the
DCS model.

We decided to test these hypotheses in a sample of teachers.


306 G. Näring, M. Briët & A. Brouwers

Method
Participants
The Dutch Association of Mathematics Teachers supported this project and provided
means and assistance to send questionnaires to a sample of 1,000 that was randomly
selected by a computer from their 2,000 members, all teachers in secondary schools. The
total number of teachers of mathematics in the Netherlands is about 8,000. The response
rate was 36.5%, and the final sample consisted of a representative sample of 269 men and
96 women, with a mean age of 48.8 years, SD /8.35. The mean number of years of
experience in the profession is 21.37, SD /9.78, ranging from 1 to 42 years. The mean
number of teaching hours per week is 18.69, with a range from 0 (administration) to 29,
SD /6.77.
We assessed the prevalence of burnout symptoms using cut-off scores for the diagnosis of
burnout for the Dutch population (Schaufeli & van Dierendonck, 1995). According to
these criteria, emotional exhaustion was present in 23% of the sample, scores ]/20;
depersonalization in 23.3%, scores ]/8; and low personal accomplishment in 53.4%,
scores 5/28. Thirty teachers, 8.2% of the sample, met all three criteria, 17.5% met two, and
finally 38.9% met one criterion.

Procedure
The teachers received our questionnaire at their home address together with a letter of
recommendation from the board of the Association. The questionnaire could be returned
anonymously to our university in a postage prepaid envelope. After 3 weeks, all of the
teachers received a letter reminding them of the survey.

Materials
Dutch Questionnaire for Emotional Labor (D-QEL). We used the penultimate version of the
questionnaire to measure emotional labour, since it has good psychometric properties
(Briët, Näring, Brouwers, & van Droffelaar, 2005). This instrument consists of four scales,
of which three measure the following types of emotional labour: surface acting, deep acting,
and suppression. The fourth scale measures emotional consonance. A high level of
emotional consonance will indicate that a person effortlessly expresses emotions that are
felt, and we regard this as the absence of emotional labour. We translated items for surface
acting (items 1, 2, 3, 4, 5) and deep acting (items 7, 8) from the emotional labour
questionnaire of Grandey (2003), which employed items from the Emotional Labour Scale
(Brotheridge & Lee, 1998, 2003). An example of a surface acting item is: ‘‘I pretend to have
the emotions I need to display for my job.’’ An example of a deep acting item is: ‘‘I make an
effort to actually feel the emotions I need to display toward others.’’ From the same study,
we selected two items that measure emotional consonance (items 10, 11) that were taken
from another study (Grandey, 1998). One of these emotional consonance items is: ‘‘I react
to students’ emotions naturally and easily.’’ A third emotional consonance item was taken
from a study in which it was a reversed item in a scale that measured dissonance: ‘‘The
emotions I show the students match what I truly feel’’ (Kruml & Geddes, 2000). We added
one deep acting item (emotive effort, item 4) from the study by Kruml and Geddes (2000),
item 9 in our questionnaire. Others developed an Emotion Requirement Scale to measure
perceived display rules (Best, Downey, & Jones, 1997). We rephrased the items that were
used to measure the: ‘‘Requirement to hide negative emotions’’ so that they ask about
Burnout and emotional labour in teachers 307

actually hiding anger and disgust and fear, and called the hypothesized separate dimension
suppression (items 18, 19, 20). An example of a suppression item is: ‘‘I hide my anger about
something someone has done.’’ People apparently suppress emotions at work just as often as
they fake them (Mann, 1999). All items were measured on a 5-point scale (1/‘‘never’’; 5 /
‘‘always’’).
The structure of the resulting scale was tested on the scores of the present sample of
teachers with confirmatory factor analysis (CFA). In order to test the proposed factorial
structure of the measure, CFA with maximum likelihood estimation were used utilizing the
AMOS 3.61 computer program (Arbuckle, 1996). As the aim of this study was to test a
hypothesis, CFA was preferred above Principal Component Analysis, which is more
suitable for exploratory purposes. The resulting comparative fit index (CFI), the Bentler-
Bonnet non-normed fit index (NNFI), and the root mean square error of approximation
(RMSEA) are reported. CFI and NNFI values/.90 indicate that the model is acceptable
(Bentler & Bonnet, 1980). Values of RMSEA 5/.05 indicate a good fit of the model (Byrne,
2001).
The CFA of the 4-factor model resulted in indices that varied between .90 and .93 (x2 /
210.20, df /71, p /.001, CFI /.93, NNFI /.90, RMSEA /.08). Item 20 was subse-
quently removed because this improved the internal reliability of suppression significantly
(from a/.64 to a/.85). The Cronbach’s alphas of the resulting scales were adequate to
high: surface acting, five items, a / .83 (n / 362); deep acting, four items, a/ .85 (n /
351); suppression, two items, a/ .85 (n / 365); and emotional consonance, three items,
a / .63 (n / 361). A slightly modified version of the scale was cross validated in a sample of
nurses and the 4-factor structure of the scale was confirmed (Briët et al., 2005).

Burnout. Burnout was measured with the Dutch version of the Maslach Burnout Inventory
for Teachers (MBI-NL-Ed; Schaufeli & van Horn, 1995). The questionnaire consists of
eight items that measure emotional exhaustion, five items that measure depersonalization,
and seven items that measure personal accomplishment. Items can be answered on a 7-
point scale ranging from ‘‘never’’ to ‘‘always/daily.’’ The possible ranges for emotional
exhaustion, depersonalisation, and personal efficacy are 0 48, 0 30, and 0 42, respec-
tively. Indicative for burnout are high scores on emotional exhaustion and depersonalization
and low scores on personal accomplishment. Cronbach’s a was .90 for emotional
exhaustion (n / 364), a/ .64 for depersonalization (n / 360), and a / .85 for personal
accomplishment (n / 358).

Social support. Social support was measured with two scales, each containing six items from
the Emotional Support Subscale of the Social Support List Discrepancies (SSL-D; van
Sonderen, 1991). Answers to the items are given on a 4-point scale running from ‘‘I miss it’’
to ‘‘it happens too often.’’ A high score indicates much social support. Items from the scales
‘‘emotional support from colleagues’’ and ‘‘emotional support from staff members’’ were
both included and scores added to create a general measure of social support. This resulted
in a Cronbach’s a of .90, n /356.

Control. An assessment of the workers’ autonomy can be regarded as a refined measurement


of the control dimension (de Jonge, Janssen, & van Breukelen, 1996). Autonomy was
measured with six items from the Maastricht Autonomy List (MAL: de Jonge, Landeweerd,
& van Breukelen, 1994). Answers are given on a 5-point scale varying from very few
308 G. Näring, M. Briët & A. Brouwers

possibilities to very many possibilities. A high score indicates a high level of autonomy, a/
.83 (n / 362).

Quantitative demands. Quantitative demands were measured with six items from the
Vragenlijst Organisatiestress Doetinchem [Organizational Stress Questionnaire] (VOS-D;
Bergers, Marcelissen, & de Wolff, 1986). Respondents are asked to indicate how often
they experience difficult quantitative demands on a 5-point Likert scale varying from
‘‘seldom’’ to ‘‘very often.’’ A high score indicates high quantitative demands, a/ .80
(n / 362).

Method of analysis
Correlations between the study variables were calculated. All variables were centred before
entering them into a regression analysis (Aiken & West, 1991). We performed a separate
stepwise hierarchical regression analysis for each of the three burnout symptoms. In the first
step demographic variables were entered as predictors, in the second the three variables
from the DCS model and the interaction of demand and control, in the third step emotional
labour and emotional consonance.

Results
Description of the sample
Table I presents a description of the sample. A comparison with a heterogeneous sample of
teachers from another Dutch study (Brouwers & Tomic, 2000) indicated that our sample
reported much lower levels of emotional exhaustion and depersonalization and similar
levels of personal accomplishment.

Table I. Demographic characteristics, job characteristics, and burnout symptoms of the sample.

Variable Cronbach’s a Range Mean SD n

Demographic variables
Gender - - - - 365
Age - 21 64 48.80 8.34 365
No. of teaching hours - 0 29 18.69 6.77 365
DCS model
Quantitative demands .80 6 30 16.85 4.44 362
Emotional demand .64 5 22 11.55 2.66 362
Control .83 7 30 19.36 4.10 362
Support .90 12 36 29.90 5.28 356
Emotional labour
Surface acting .83 4 24 9.50 3.28 362
Deep acting .85 4 20 7.68 3.31 351
Suppression .85 2 9 4.24 1.55 365
Emotional consonance .63 5 15 10.60 2.05 361
Burnout symptoms
Emotional exhaustion .90 0 36 13.96 8.13 364
Depersonalization .64 0 21 5.56 3.48 360
Personal .85 4 42 27.34 6.36 358
accomplishment
Table II. Correlations between demographic variables, work characteristics and emotional labour.

Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12

1. Gender -
2. Age - -
3. No. of teaching hours /.13* /.06 -
4. Quantitative demands .01 /.04 .02 -
5. Control .09 .02 .05 .28** -
6. Support .04 .02 /.09 /.32** .18** -
7. Emotional exhaustion /.06 .04 .10 .59** /.33** /.37** -

Burnout and emotional labour in teachers 309


8. Depersonalization /.17** .12* .13* .19** /.28** /.28** .47** -
9. Personal accomplishment .07 /.22** /.01 /.07 .23** .15** /.32** /.43** -
10. Surface acting /.13* .07 .16** .19** /.07 /.22** .36** .39** /.16** -
11. Deep acting /.07 .12* .08 .06 /.07 /.19** .10 .05 .06 .24** -
12. Suppression .02 .04 /.04 .01 /.01 /.07 .10 .20** /.19** .20** -.05 -
13. Emotional consonance .19** /.15** /.02 .08 .16** /.01 /.14* /.39** .54** /.26** .16** /.19**

Significance levels indicated with asterisks: * pB/ .05; ** pB/ .01


310
G. Näring, M. Briët & A. Brouwers
Table III. Hierarchical regression analysis of predictors of emotional exhaustion, depersonalization, and personal accomplishment (n /345).

Dimensions of burnout

Emotional exhaustion Depersonalization Personal accomplishment

Predictor Beta F R2 (change) Beta F R2 (change) Beta F R2 (change)

Step 1
Demographic variables .92 (.05) 5.90*** (.03) 9.99*** (.06)
Age .05 .05 /.20***
Gender .01 /.05 /.10*
Step 2
Work characteristics (DCS) 58.51*** (.41) 13.18*** (.13) 6.05*** (.06)
Quantitative demands .47*** .04 /.06
Control /.15** /.17*** .11*
Social support /.15** /.19*** .12*
Quantitative demands x Control /.07 /.09* /.04
Step 3
Emotional labour 6.59*** (.04) 19.73*** (.16) 30.60*** (.24)
Surface acting .17*** .21*** .06
Deep acting /.01 /.02 .02
Suppression .03 .09* /.08
Emotional consonance /.08 /.27*** .50***

Note : Standardized beta coefficients are given from the final regression model with all variables included.
Significance levels : *p 5/.05; **p 5/.01; ***p 5/.001.
Burnout and emotional labour in teachers 311

Correlations of symptoms of burnout with job characteristics and emotional labour


Correlations between the dimensions of burnout and the job characteristics and emotional
labour are given in Table II. Both emotional exhaustion and depersonalization were related
to more quantitative demands, less control, and less social support. Furthermore, both
emotional exhaustion and depersonalization were related to more surface acting. More
depersonalization was also related to more suppression. Finally, personal accomplishment
was positively correlated with control and support, and also positively correlated with
emotional consonance. Deep acting was not correlated with any of the burnout symptoms.

Regression of symptoms of burnout on job characteristics and emotional labour


The strength of the relationship between emotional labour and burnout symptoms, after
taking into account the relationship with the variables from the DCS model, was assessed in
a hierarchical regression analysis. The results of the separate analyses for each burnout
dimension are given in Table III. In the first step, the demographic variables age and gender
were entered in the analysis. Age was found to be negatively related to personal
accomplishment and was not related to emotional exhaustion or depersonalization.
In the second step, the separate variables of the DCS model were entered and the
interaction of demand with control. More demands, less control, and less support were all
related to more emotional exhaustion. More control and more social support were related
to less depersonalization; the interaction of more control and more demands was also
separately related to less depersonalization. More control and more support were positively
related to personal accomplishment
In the third step the emotional labour variables were entered. Hypothesis 1a and 1b were
confirmed, as more surface acting was significantly related to more emotional exhaustion
and to more depersonalization. Hypothesis 2a was not supported, as suppression was not
related to emotional exhaustion. Hypothesis 2b was supported as more suppression was
related to more depersonalization. Hypothesis 3a was supported as emotional consonance
had a negative relationship to depersonalization. Hypothesis 3b was also confirmed, as
emotional consonance had a positive relationship to personal accomplishment.
Because others have reported a positive relationship between deep acting and personal
accomplishment (Brotheridge & Grandey, 2002), we examined this relationship in more
detail. We performed an additional regression analysis with personal accomplishment as
criterion in which emotional consonance was entered in a separate fourth step. This analysis
revealed that deep acting was a significant predictor of personal accomplishment in step 3,
b /.15, p /.006, but after entering emotional consonance in the regression model in step 4,
this relationship disappeared.

Discussion
In line with a multitude of earlier studies that have used the DCS model, our study indicates
that quantitative demands, control, and support are significantly related to emotional
exhaustion. In addition to these relationships, the emotional labour dimension of surface
acting was, as hypothesized, also found to be related to emotional exhaustion. The
relationship between surface acting and emotional exhaustion is a replication of earlier
findings (Brotheridge & Grandey, 2002; Brotheridge & Lee, 1998; Grandey, 2003;
Zammuner & Galli, 2005; Zammuner & Lotto, 2001) and supports the idea that having
to pretend certain emotions may come at a personal cost in terms of feeling emotionally
312 G. Näring, M. Briët & A. Brouwers

drained or exhausted (Hochschild, 1983). This finding adds to the evidence that surface
acting in particular should be regarded as stressful (Mann & Cowburn, 2005).
With the exception of quantitative demands, the variables from the DCS model were also
found to be associated with depersonalization. Of the three emotional labour strategies that
we measured, surface acting and suppression were also found to be significantly related to
depersonalization. This result replicates but also extends findings on surface acting in
various professions and students (Brotheridge & Grandey, 2002; Brotheridge & Lee, 2002).
As our study takes the DCS model into account, it is possible to give an estimate of the
relative importance of emotional labour and the DCS model to our understanding of
depersonalization. A comparison of the amount of variance explained in steps 2 and 3
indicated that the DCS model was more important than emotional labour in explaining
emotional exhaustion in our sample of teachers. Emotional labour and the DCS model
seem, however, to be equally important when trying to understand depersonalization.
A new finding in our study is that suppression is significantly related to depersonalization.
Most studies of emotional labour have focused on the production of emotions or emotional
expressions and have not measured suppression separately. Some have used a measure of
‘‘self-focused behaviour’’ in which suppression is assumed but, taken together with surface
acting, Pugliesi (1999) and others have used a measure of ‘‘emotional dissonance’’ that also
explicitly comprises both suppression and surface acting (Zapf, Seifert, Schmutte, Mertini,
& Holz, 2001). Brotheridge and Lee (2003) saw suppression as an activity that is an
inherent part of surface acting. As suppression measured with a separate scale was only
weakly correlated with surface acting in their study (Brotheridge & Lee, 2003), this
assumption does not hold. Apparently people do not necessarily have to suppress an
emotion before they act another one. Because several studies report that suppression is
used as a separate strategy in various professions (Mann, 1999; Ybema & Smulders, 2002),
we also measured it separately in our questionnaire. Other researchers also recently added a
subscale to measure suppression to the Emotional Labor Scale (Lee & Brotheridge, 2006),
the instrument that is most often used in the USA.
Although it is tempting to conclude that surface acting and suppression contribute to a
feeling of depersonalization, our study design does not allow any conclusions about
causality. It is just as plausible to assume that teachers who are exhausted or who feel
detached from their students do not respond to certain situations in a way that feels natural.
In such a state, teachers might have to use surface acting and suppression more.
The third dimension of burnout, personal accomplishment, was strongly related to
emotional consonance, with a beta weight of .50. Emotional labour measures the effort to
deal with the experience or the expression of emotions and is therefore thought to
contribute to stress or strain. Emotional consonance measures the absence of such effort and
is a useful extension of the nomological network or constructs related to emotional labour
(Diefendorff, Croyle, & Gosserand, 2005). Emotional consonance can be useful in gaining
an understanding of personal accomplishment.
Deep acting was not found to be related to personal accomplishment in our study, which
seems to be in contrast with findings in another study (Brotheridge & Grandey, 2002). After
entering emotional consonance in the regression model for personal accomplishment, the
relationship of deep acting with personal accomplishment disappeared. Brotheridge and
Lee re-analysed data from a previous study (Brotheridge & Grandey, 2002) and presented a
path model in which authenticity of emotional expression was a stronger predictor of
personal accomplishment than deep acting (Brotheridge & Lee, 2002). These findings on
authenticity parallel our findings on emotional consonance. It therefore seems that
Burnout and emotional labour in teachers 313

emotional consonance, the absence of emotional labour, is more strongly associated with
personal accomplishment than deep acting.
The varying results that have been obtained on the relationship between deep acting and
emotional exhaustion and personal accomplishment might result from samples that differ in
the skilfulness of the individual to apply deep acting. Studies that assess emotional labour
repeatedly during the training and supervision of teachers or nurses might give us an
understanding of how people change in their use of emotional labour. Over time, emotional
labour may vary in amount, in type, and, especially for deep acting, in the degree to which
the desired emotional result is obtained. By studying trainees we will get a better
understanding of what constitutes adequate professional behaviour and how it develops.
Future studies may also point out which levels of deep acting, faking, and suppression can
be regarded as adequate or healthy and which levels are harmful.
An indication that some of our findings can be generalized to other European countries
can be derived from a comparison from the beta weights in the regression of emotional
exhaustion on quantitative demand. This beta weight is .44 in our study, which is almost
identical to that of an Italian sample, .46 (Pisanti, Gagliardi, Razzino, & Bertini, 2003) and
a Greek sample, .45, (Pomaki & Anagnostopoulou, 2003) of secondary school teachers.
The DCS model has been very influential. The three concepts from this model are still
important for burnout researchers, but the scope is getting broader. Job demands now are
sometimes subsumed under the header workload; support can be taken in a broader sense
to comprise the community (Maslach et al., 2001). Reward in financial or other ways,
(un)fairness or inequity of workload or pay, and finally personal values all seem to play a
role in the aetiology of burnout symptoms, but workload is most directly related to
exhaustion (Maslach et al., 2001).
Some limitations of our study need to be discussed. The emotional labour instrument
that we developed and used in this study (Briët et al., 2005) does not measure the acting of
negative emotions such as anger and aggression. A scale to measure the faking of negative
emotions has been developed by others (Glomb & Tews, 2004). For studies in professions
in which such emotions are also expressed (Fitness, 2000), a measure of the (non)expres-
sion of negative emotions in the workplace will have to be included.
It should also be noted that we reported on one specific type of teacher (mathematics) in
one specific setting (secondary schools). It is not known whether such teachers have special
personality characteristics or whether teaching mathematics entails different social
interactions from teaching other subjects. What is known from other studies is that the
emotional ‘‘geography’’ in primary school is one of emotional intensity, whereas teachers in
secondary school seem to behave in more distant ways than their primary school colleagues
(Hargreaves, 2000). The situation in secondary school may, however, become very
demanding, for example when pupils refuse to do assignments, or worse, when pupils
like to act ‘‘bad’’ because the reaction of the peer group makes them feel ‘‘good’’ (Paulle,
2005). Clearly, some schools may suffer more from such problems than others, which is a
factor that should be taken into account in further studies.
An important finding in our study is that emotional consonance gives a clue towards
understanding personal accomplishment. While it is important to investigate what makes
people feel exhausted, there is an increasing interest in what makes people enthusiastic
about their work and what makes them feel competent. In theoretical models that focus on
engagement (Schaufeli & Bakker, 2004) and resources (de Jonge, Dormann, & van Vegchel,
2004), emotional consonance may play an important role.
314 G. Näring, M. Briët & A. Brouwers

In conclusion, the DCS model has been very valuable for our understanding of emotional
exhaustion. The concept of emotional labour seems to be an additional useful perspective.
The study of surface acting, especially, might give us a better understanding of emotional
exhaustion and depersonalization.

Acknowledgements
The authors appreciate the cooperation of the board and members of the Dutch Association
of Teachers of Mathematics. The first author would like to thank the University of Lisbon
for their hospitality during the preparation of this manuscript.

References
Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions . London: Sage.
Arbuckle, J. L. (1996). AMOS (Version 3.6). [Computer software]. Chicago, IL: SmallWaters Corporation.
Bentler, P. M., & Bonnet, D. G. (1980). Significance tests and goodness of fit in the analysis of covariance
structures. Psychological Bulletin , 88 , 588 606.
Bergers, G. P. A., Marcelissen, F. H. G., & de Wolff, C. J. (1986). Vragenlijst organisatie stress-Doetinchem:
Handleiding [Organizational stress questionaire: Manual] (No. 36). Nijmegen, the Netherlands: Stress Groep,
University of Nijmegen.
Best, R. G., Downey, R. G., & Jones, R. G. (1997, April). Incumbent perceptions of emotional work requirements . Paper
presented at the 12th annual conference of the Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology, St. Louis,
MO.
Briët, M., Näring, G. W. B., Brouwers, A., & van Droffelaar, A. (2005). Emotional labor: Ontwikkeling en
validering van de Dutch questionnaire on emotional labor (D-QEL) [Emotional labour: Development and
validation of the Dutch questionnaire on emotional labour]. Gedrag en Gezondheid , 33 , 318 330.
Brotheridge, C., & Grandey, A. (2002). Emotional labor and burnout: Comparing two perspectives on "people
work. Journal of Vocational Behavior , 60 , 17 39.
Brotheridge, C., & Lee, R. T. (1998, August). On the dimensionality of emotional labour: Development and validation of
the emotional labour scale . Paper presented at the First Conference on Emotions in Organizational Life, San
Diego.
Brotheridge, C., & Lee, R. T. (2002). Testing a conservation of resources model of the dynamics of emotional
labor. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 7 , 57 67.
Brotheridge, C., & Lee, R. T. (2003). Development and validation of the emotional labour scale. Journal of
Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 76 , 365 379.
Brouwers, A., & Tomic, W. (2000). A longitudinal study of teacher burnout and perceived self-efficacy in
classroom management. Teaching and Teacher Education , 16 , 239 253.
Byrne, B. M. (2001). Structural equation modelling with AMOS: Basic concepts, applications, and programming .
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
de Jonge, J., Dormann, C., & van Vegchel, N. (2004). Taakeisen, hulpbronnen en psychische gezondheid: Het
demand-induced strain compensation (DISC)-model [Job demands, job resources and mental health: The
demand-induced strain compensation (DISC) model]. Gedrag en Organisatie , 17 , 59 79.
de Jonge, J., Janssen, P. M., & van Breukelen, C. J. P. (1996). Testing the demand control support model among
health-care professionals: A structural equation model. Work and Stress , 10 , 209 224.
de Jonge, J., Landeweerd, J. A., & van Breukelen, C. J. P. (1994). De Maastrichtse autonomielijst: Achtergrond,
constructie en validering [The Maastricht autonomy questionnaire: Background, construction, and validation].
Gedrag en Organisatie , 7 , 27 41.
de Lange, A. H., Taris, T. W., Kompier, M. A. J., Houtman, I. L. D., & Bongers, P. M. (2003). ‘‘The very best of
the millennium": Longitudinal research and the demand control (support) model. Journal of Occupational
Health Psychology, 8 , 282 305.
Diefendorff, J. M., Croyle, M. H., & Gosserand, R. H. (2005). The dimensionality and antecedents of emotional
labor strategies. Journal of Vocational Behavior , 66 , 339 359.
Fitness, J. (2000). Anger in the workplace: An emotion script approach to anger episodes between workers and
their superiors, co-workers and subordinates. Journal of Organizational Behavior , 21 , 147 162.
Glomb, T. M., & Tews, M. J. (2004). Emotional labor: A conceptualization and scale development. Journal of
Vocational Behavior , 64 , 1 24.
Burnout and emotional labour in teachers 315
Grandey, A. (1998, August). Emotional labor: A concept and its correlates . Paper presented at the First Conference on
Emotions in Organizational Life, San Diego, CA.
Grandey, A. (2003). When "the show must go on": Surface acting and deep acting as determinants of emotional
exhaustion and peer-rated service delivery. Academy of Management Journal , 46 , 86 96.
Hargreaves, A. (2000). Mixed emotions: Teachers’ perceptions of their interactions with students. Teaching and
Teacher Education , 16 , 811 826.
Hochschild, A. R. (1983). The managed heart . Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
Johnson, S., Cooper, C., Cartwright, S., Donald, I., Taylor, P., & Millet, C. (2005). The experience of work-
related stress across occupations. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 20 , 178 187.
Karasek, R. A. (1979). Job demands, job decision latitude, and mental strain: Implications for job redesign.
Administrative Science Quarterly, 24 , 285 308.
Kruml, S. M., & Geddes, D. (2000). Exploring the dimensions of emotional labor. Management Communication
Quarterly, 14 , 8 49.
Lee, R. T., & Brotheridge, C. M. (2006, August). Validation and extension of the Emotional Labor Scale: Evidence from
day-care workers . Paper presented at the Fifth International Conference on Emotions and Worklife, Atlanta, GA.
Mann, S. (1999). Emotion at work: To what extent are we expressing, suppressing, or faking it? European Journal of
Work and Organizational Psychology, 8 , 347 369.
Mann, S., & Cowburn, J. (2005). Emotional labour and stress within mental health nursing. Journal of Psychiatric
and Mental Health Nursing , 12 , 154 162.
Maslach, C., Schaufeli, W. B., & Leiter, M. P. (2001). Job burnout. Annual Review of Psychology, 52 , 397 422.
Ogbonna, E., & Harris, L. C. (2004). Work intensification and emotional labour among UK university lecturers:
An exploratory study. Organization Studies , 25 , 1185 1203.
Paulle, B. (2005). Anxiety and intimidation in the Bronx and the Bijlmer: An ethnographic comparison of two schools .
Amsterdam: Dutch University Press.
Pisanti, R., Gagliardi, M. P., Razzino, S., & Bertini, M. (2003). Occupational stress and wellness among Italian
secondary school teachers. Psychology and Health , 18 , 523 536.
Pomaki, G., & Anagnostopoulou, T. (2003). A test and extension of the demand/control/social support model:
Prediction of wellness/health outcomes in Greek teachers. Psychology and Health , 18 , 537 550.
Pugliesi, K. (1999). The consequences of emotional labor: Effects on work stress, job satisfaction, and well-being.
Motivation and Emotion , 23 , 125 154.
Schaufeli, W. B., & Bakker, A. B. (2004). Job demands, job resources, and their relationship with burnout and
engagement: A multi-sample study. Journal of Organizational Behavior , 25 , 293 315.
Schaufeli, W. B., & van Dierendonck, D. (1995). A cautionary note about the cross-national and clinical validity of
cut-off points for the Maslach burnout inventory. Psychological Reports , 76 , 1083 1090.
Schaufeli, W. B., & van Horn, J. E. (1995). Maslach burnout inventory voor leraren (MBI-NL-LE). Voorlopige
handleiding [Maslach Burnout Inventory for teachers (MBI-NL-LE). Preliminary manual]. Utrecht: University
of Utrecht, PAGO.
van der Doef, M., & Maes, S. (1999). The job demand control( support) model and psychological well-being: A
review of 20 years of empirical research. Work and Stress , 13 , 87 114.
van Sonderen, E. (1991). Social support list discrepancies (SSL-D) . Groningen: University of Groningen,
Noordelijk Centrum voor Gezondheidsvraagstukken.
Ybema, J., & Smulders, P. (2002). Emotionele belasting en de noodzaak tot het verbergen van emoties op het werk
[Emotional demand and the need to hide emotions at work]. Gedrag en Organisatie , 15 , 129 146.
Zammuner, V. L., & Galli, C. (2005). Wellbeing: Causes and consequences of emotion regulation in work settings.
International Review of Psychiatry, 17 , 355 364.
Zammuner, V. L., & Lotto, L. (2001). The regulation of emotions in the helping professions: Its nature, and its antecedents
and consequences . Paper presented at Feelings and Emotions, The Amsterdam Symposium, Amsterdam.
Zapf, D., Seifert, C., Schmutte, B., Mertini, H., & Holz, M. (2001). Emotion work and job stressors and their
effects on burnout. Psychology and Health , 16 , 527 545.

You might also like