Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 19

Hearing Research 399 (2021) 107992

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Hearing Research
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/heares

Review Article

Do rat auditory event related potentials exhibit human mismatch


negativity attributes related to predictive coding?
Jaishree Jalewa a, Juanita Todd a, b, c, Patricia T. Michie a, b, c, Deborah M. Hodgson a, b, c,
Lauren Harms b, c, d, *
a
School of Psychology, University of Newcastle, Callaghan, New South Wales, Australia
b
Priority Research Centre for Brain and Mental Health Research, University of Newcastle, Callaghan, New South Wales, Australia
c
Hunter Medical Research Institute, New Lambton Heights, New South Wales, Australia
d
School of Biomedical Sciences and Pharmacy, University of Newcastle, Callaghan, New South Wales, Australia

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Rodent models play a significant role in understanding disease mechanisms and the screening of new
Received 12 December 2019 treatments. With regard to psychiatric disorders such as schizophrenia, however, it is difficult to replicate
Received in revised form the human symptoms in rodents because these symptoms are often either ‘uniquely human’ or are only
29 April 2020
conveyed via self-report. There is a growing interest in rodent mismatch responses (MMRs) as a
Accepted 4 May 2020
Available online 28 May 2020
translatable ‘biomarker’ for disorders such as schizophrenia. In this review, we will summarize the at-
tributes of human MMN, and discuss the scope of exploring the attributes of human MMN in rodents.
Here, we examine how reliably MMRs that are measured in rats mimic human attributes, and present
Keywords:
Mismatch negativity
original data examining whether manipulations of stimulus conditions known to modulate human
Schizophrenia MMN, do the same for rat MMRs. Using surgically-implanted epidural electroencephalographic elec-
Mismatch responses trodes and wireless telemetry in freely-moving rats, we observed human-like modulations of MMRs,
Electroencephalogram namely that larger MMRs were elicited to unexpected (deviant) stimuli that a) had a larger change in
Deviance pitch compared to the expected (standard) stimulus, b) were less frequently presented (lower proba-
Probability bility), and c) had no jitter (stable stimulus onset asynchrony) compared to high jitter. Overall, these
Jitter findings contribute to the mounting evidence for rat MMRs as a good analogue of human MMN,
Predictive coding
bolstering the development of a novel approach in future to validate the preclinical models based on a
translatable biomarker, MMN.
© 2020 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

Contents

1. Mismatch negativity (MMN) in humans . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2


1.1. Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2
1.2. MMN and relevance to predictive coding . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3
2. Why are animal models of MMN important? . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4
3. What are the features of human MMN that are important to model in an animal? . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4
3.1. Predictive coding (prediction error vs. adaptation) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5
3.2. Other measures relevant to predictive coding . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5
3.2.1. Sensitivity to deviance difference . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5
3.2.2. Sensitivity to deviant probability . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5
3.2.3. Other factors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 6
4. How well do existing reports of rat MMN show that they model each of these aspects? . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 6
4.1. Adaptation independence . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 6

* Corresponding author. Priority Research Centre for Brain and Mental Health
Research, University of Newcastle, Callaghan, New South Wales, Australia.
E-mail address: lauren.harms@newcastle.edu.au (L. Harms).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heares.2020.107992
0378-5955/© 2020 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
2 J. Jalewa et al. / Hearing Research 399 (2021) 107992

4.2. Sensitivity to deviance difference . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7


4.3. Sensitivity to deviant probability . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7
4.4. Sensitivity to SOA manipulation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7
5. Original research . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 8
5.1. Methods . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 8
5.1.1. Ethics statement . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 8
5.1.2. Animals . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 8
5.1.3. Surgery to implant EEG electrodes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 8
5.1.4. Sound generation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 8
5.1.5. Mismatch response (MMR) testing . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 8
5.1.6. Experiment design and stimuli . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 8
5.1.7. Data extraction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 9
5.1.8. Statistical analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 9
5.2. Results . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 9
5.2.1. Effect of deviance difference on rat MMRs . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 9
5.2.2. Effect of deviant probability on rat MMRs . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 9
5.2.3. Effect of SOA variability (jitter) on rat MMRs . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 11
6. General discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 11
Acknowledgements . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 15
Supplementary data . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 15
References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 15

1. Mismatch negativity (MMN) in humans responses to expected sounds in the environment are relatively
small, whereas unpredictable sounds evoke larger responses, pro-
1.1. Introduction portional to how unexpected the sound is. Such neural adjustment
(gain-control) is an important property that enables us to ignore
Previous studies strongly support the presence of auditory predictable sounds, stay focused on the stimuli of interest, while
sensory dysfunction in schizophrenia and altered responses of the still remaining alert to potentially important changes in the sur-
auditory system in individuals with schizophrenia when discrimi- rounding environment. This gain-control is reflected in the ampli-
nating a deviant sound from standard repetitious tones (Javitt and tude of mismatch negativity (MMN), an auditory component of the
Sweet, 2015; Na €a
€t€
anen and Kahkonen, 2009; Catts et al., 1995; event related potential (ERP) or magnetic field, automatically eli-
Salisbury et al., 2002; Javitt et al., 1995, 1996). Since the first pub- cited in response to a rare, unexpected deviant stimulus among
lication of reduced mismatch negativity (MMN) in schizophrenia expected common standard stimuli in an oddball sequence para-
(Shelley et al., 1991), many subsequent papers have replicated the digm (Garrido et al., 2009; Na €a€ta
€nen and Alho, 1995b) (Fig. 1).
reduced amplitude of MMN in schizophrenia patients (Todd et al., To elicit an MMN response, the deviance must be discriminable.
2012), a phenomenon well-established by meta-analyses of more Considering the simplest paradigm, a deviant sound can differ from
than 100 studies (Bodatsch et al., 2015; Erickson et al., 2016; the standards along any acoustic physical dimension including
Umbricht and Krljes, 2005). Moreover, studies have shown that frequency (pitch), intensity, duration or deviation in stimulus onset
MMN amplitude is reduced in schizophrenia in response to deviant asynchrony (SOA, time between the onset of adjacent stimuli) or
sounds (Umbricht et al., 2003; Magno et al., 2008) across a wide omission of stimuli in a regular progression. If the deviant and
range of stimulus conditions including stimulus deviance type, standard sounds are discriminable, subtraction of the responses
probability and interstimulus/interdeviant interval (Javitt et al., evoked by frequent standard sounds from the responses evoked by
1998; Javitt, 2000; Shelley et al., 1999; Michie et al., 2000; Todd infrequent deviant sounds result in a difference response wave-
et al., 2008). Furthermore, significantly reduced MMN amplitude form, from which the MMN amplitude is observed at approximately
has been found in both first-episode as well as ultra-high risk 100e250 ms post-stimulus in humans (Fig. 1C) (Javitt et al., 1994,
populations in comparison to healthy individuals (Nagai et al., 2008; Na €€
ata€nen et al., 1978, 2007; Na €a
€t€
anen and Alho, 1995b;
2017; Perez et al., 2014), suggesting MMN as a biomarker of dis- Na€€ €nen and Michie, 1979).
ata
ease progression (Carbajal and Malmierca, 2018; Tada et al., 2019; MMN is also generated by deviants that violate complex abstract
Avissar and Javitt, 2018). Overall, due to its quantitative nature and rules. For instance, complex auditory stimuli such as speech sounds
the findings of reduced MMN amplitude in neurodevelopmental (De La Salle et al., 2019, Maiste et al., 1995; Na €a
€t€
anen et al., 1997;
and neuropsychiatric disorders, MMN is of interest to cognitive Alho et al., 1998; Wang et al., 2012) or ‘melodies’ (harmonic tones)
neuroscience. The extent of clinical potential possessed by MMN (Quiroga-Martinez et al., 2019; Tervaniemi et al., 2015; Vuust et al.,
necessitates the investigation of human MMN attributes in rodent 2012; Brattico et al., 2006; Fujioka et al., 2004, 2005) elicit MMN in
models, which are important for preclinical research in order to humans (Nikjeh et al., 2009).
develop novel therapeutic interventions. Over the past decade, the The MMN response has been attributed to neural generators
number of studies on rodent models of MMN has increased sub- within the temporal and frontal lobes (auditory cortex, including
stantially, many of them focusing on how well human MMN attri- primary auditory cortex (A1), located along the superior temporal
butes are recapitulated in rodent responses. In this review, we will gyrus) (Fishman, 2014; Giard et al., 1990; Na €a
€ta
€nen and Alho,
discuss the main attributes of human MMN and how well they are 1995a; Alho, 1995; Kasai et al., 1999), and is dependent on the ac-
recapitulated in studies of rodent mismatch responses (MMRs), tivity of NMDA receptors (glutamatergic neurotransmission) (Javitt
followed by original data that also addresses some of the main et al., 1996; Gunduz-Bruce et al., 2012; Kort et al., 2017). Moreover,
questions. MMN occurs as two components - one that is typically earlier
Optimal auditory sensory processing ensures that neural (generated in the superior temporal gyrus) and another that is
J. Jalewa et al. / Hearing Research 399 (2021) 107992 3

Fig. 1. Illustration of oddball sequence paradigm and mismatch negativity (MMN). A. Descending deviant oddball sequence where unexpected low frequency deviants (LF DEV)
are interspersed among expected high frequency standards (HF STD). B. Ascending deviant oddball sequence where unexpected high frequency deviants (HF DEV) are interspersed
among expected low frequency standards (LF STD). A & B together forms a flip-flop control sequence, which allows for the comparison of the response to a stimulus when it is a
deviant to the same tone when it is a standard, thus this design controls for the differential responses to the physical characteristics of the stimuli. C. MMN (the shaded area), is an
‘auditory surprise signal’: the more surprising the deviant sound, the larger the MMN size. MMN is measured as a subtraction waveform, obtained by subtracting the standard
response from the deviant response. C. Adapted from (Kujala and Na €€ata
€nen, 2001) with permission.

usually slightly later (generated in the inferior frontal gyrus), from the adapted/attenuated N1 response and suggested that there
demonstrated by both EEG and hemodynamic measurements is no separate MMN mechanism (J€ €skel€
aa ainen et al., 2004). Further,
(Doeller et al., 2003; Li et al., 2019). Further, MMN generation in May and Tittinen comprehensively reviewed the possibility of
humans is hypothesised to occur due to activity in large pyramidal MMN generation based on the adaptation hypothesis and proposed
cells in the supragranular layers, based on studies in macaques that MMN is part of a latency and amplitude modulated auditory
(Javitt et al., 1994), as reviewed in (Todd et al., 2012). N1 response (May and Tiitinen, 2010). However, data from studies
investigating the adaptation and memory-based hypotheses of
MMN have supported the notion that MMN is due to other mech-
1.2. MMN and relevance to predictive coding anisms and cannot be solely attributed to adaptation (Jacobsen and
Schro€ ger, 2001; Maess et al., 2007).
There have been two dominant hypotheses proposed to explain Another explanation of the MMN generation is based on the
the underlying neuronal mechanism of MMN generation (Na €€ €nen
ata ‘Sensory memory hypothesis (regularity violation)’, according to
et al., 2005; May and Tiitinen, 2010). According to the ‘Neural which MMN is a novel (non-obligatory) ERP component that re-
Adaptation hypothesis’, repeated presentation of the standard flects a deviation between properties of an incoming sound and
(common) stimuli results in adapted/attenuated activity from the those of a neural ‘memory trace’ established by the preceding
responsive neurons along the auditory pathway, while these neu- standard sounds (Na €a
€t€
anen et al., 2005). The brain forms a model
rons stay responsive to those stimuli (the deviants) that occur less (hypothesis), based on previous sensory input, incorporating the
frequently (Ja €€
askela€inen et al., 2004; Na €€ €nen et al., 2005;
ata rule underlying the regularity when a sequence of sounds are
Ulanovsky et al., 2004, Na €a
€ta
€nen et al., 1978). This effect is called presented. Based on this rule, the brain generates a prediction
stimulus-specific adaptation (SSA) (Duque et al., 2018), and is regarding the next sound in the sequence, and the prediction error
known to occur in the auditory system of cats (Ulanovsky et al., associated with sounds matching the prediction declines. When
2003, 2004), rats (Malmierca et al., 2009; Szymanski et al., 2009; properties of the incoming sound fail to agree with the prediction, a
Von Der Behrens et al., 2009; Antunes et al., 2010; Taaseh et al., much larger prediction error signal is elicited (Naatanen, 2001;
2011), mice (Duque et al., 2018), and non-human primates Naatanen and Winkler, 1999; Paavilainen, 2013). According to the
(Fishman and Steinschneider, 2012). For SSA reviews, see predictive coding model, MMN is therefore a highly informative
(Malmierca et al., 2015; Pe rez-Gonzalez and Malmierca, 2014; Ayala measure of prediction error signalling, which results from a viola-
et al., 2016). In a purely adaptation-based framework of MMN, rare tion of the hypothesised auditory pattern by the unexpected
deviants activate afferents that are less adapted than those stimu- auditory stimulus (Damaso et al., 2015; Duque et al., 2015; Fishman,
lated by the frequent standards and thus, elicit a larger obligatory 2014; Garrido et al., 2009; Durschmid et al., 2016), as hierarchical
N1 response, which yields the MMN. Initial studies on anesthetized models allow the brain to construct prior expectations in a dynamic
cats showed that the neurons in primary auditory cortex (A1) and context-sensitive manner (Friston, 2005). Prediction errors
exhibit strong SSA (Ulanovsky et al., 2003, 2004). Additionally, drive efficient use of cognitive resources to help direct which
€€
Ja askela€inen et al. proposed that MMN in humans is not different
4 J. Jalewa et al. / Hearing Research 399 (2021) 107992

aspects of the environment provide important information about et al., 2018; Yuan et al., 2009; Swerdlow et al., 2016). Similar
changes that may need to be incorporated to improve future pre- treatment-resistance is also found for the cognitive impairments of
dictions. As such, the processes underlying prediction and predic- schizophrenia (Kahn and Keefe, 2013; Kahn et al., 2015; Forray and
tion error generation serve a relevance-filtering function. An Buller, 2017; Altamura and Glick, 2010), which have a substantial
important contextual variable to this process is time, which de- negative impact on patient functioning (Greenwood et al., 2005,
termines how the relevance of sound is filtered in a given context Zaragoza Domingo et al., 2015; Bowie et al., 2018). Unfortunately,
(Todd et al., 2018). The amplitude of prediction error indexed by pre-clinical validation of potential novel antipsychotics in animal
MMN can therefore serve as a measure of how relevant a deviant models, often fail in human clinical trials, particularly for cognitive
sound is in the context of the regularities (standards), but this impairments. This is partly attributed to lack of suitable animal
quantification is also impacted by an accumulated estimate of models of disease with associated biomarkers of neural-circuit
‘confidence’ in the goodness-of-fit of the active prediction model. function across species (Young and Light, 2018; Young and Geyer,
Confidence in this sense can be thought of as reflecting precision in 2015; Light and Na €a€ta
€nen, 2013), but also because of use of
the model, which is inversely proportional to variance in the sen- flawed analogues of human cognitive tasks (Young and Geyer,
sory input (Lieder et al., 2013). As the relevance of a deviation in 2015). If rodent MMRs are found to be accurate analogues of hu-
sound is based on the information extracted from the regularities, man MMN, and a biomarker of the integrity of neural circuit
the period of stability of a pattern over time is an important factor function, they could potentially be used as complimentary outcome
that influences the MMN amplitude (Todd et al., 2011, 2018; Lieder measures, alongside behavioural examinations, in preclinical drug
et al., 2013; Cowan, 1984; Bendixen et al., 2007; Cowan et al., 1993; development studies. Further, an effective treatment that amelio-
Winkler et al., 1996; Bader et al., 2017). When the regularities are rates the MMN deficit has the potential to improve remission rates
stable (low variability), precision is higher, which means precision given recent evidence that remission is predicted by the magnitude
is inversely proportional to the environmental variance (Fitzgerald of MMN along with other factors (Kim et al., 2020).
and Todd, 2018). Moreover, each time the sensory input matches Therefore, it is important to investigate and understand
the top-down expectation, precision estimates increase, and when whether rodents exhibit the attributes of human MMN. If the sci-
deviation occurs from the expectation under a high-confidence entific community is able to demonstrate that the rodent brain is
scenario (higher precision), it results in larger MMN. Further, this capable of generating mismatch responses (MMRs) with similar
accumulated confidence results in stronger predictions for the features to human MMN, the door can be opened to further ex-
likely future events. Overall, an exposure to a very predictable amination of the neurocircuitry and pharmacology of these re-
context for a sufficient amount of time results in confident higher- sponses, and their relationship with cognitive impairments,
level predictions and this is referred to as contextual learning on potentially leading to the discovery of novel treatments that can
which the expression of MMN depends (Todd et al., 2013, 2014a, serve as new candidate therapies.
2018; Frost et al., 2016; Tavano et al., 2014; Wacongne et al., 2011; Moreover, successful demonstration of the MMRs in rats, anal-
Lieder et al., 2013). ogous to humans, has the potential to facilitate approaches for
Further, predictive coding, which is a general theory of validation of schizophrenia animal models. The great value of MMN
perceptual inference, has been considered as a general framework as a translational measure is that it is automatically elicited, par-
that attempts to unify both of the above mentioned hypotheses ticipants do not need to receive instruction or pay attention to
(Garrido et al., 2009). According to this framework, the brain is generate an MMN to auditory stimuli. In this way, MMRs can be
considered as a hierarchically organised system where each level elicited in rodents without the need for lengthy training. Mea-
adjusts between the bottom-up sensory inputs and top-down surement of rodent MMRs can complement other investigations of
predictions (Garrido et al., 2009). In summary, the ‘predictive schizophrenia-like behaviour in animal model validation studies.
coding framework’ has been proposed to explain both the ‘repeti- However, to do this, we need to ensure that the rodent MMRs meet
tion suppression’ (or, adaptation) caused by stimulus repetition, certain criteria of human MMN in order to maximize the trans-
and ‘prediction error’ (also called ‘deviance detection’), an lational potential of MMN. This will allow us to comprehend the
enhanced response to a stimulus that violates an expected regu- underlying mechanism of MMN and, will become an important tool
larity (Parras et al., 2017; Garrido et al., 2009; Carbajal and to validate rodent models.
Malmierca, 2018).

2. Why are animal models of MMN important?


3. What are the features of human MMN that are important
The reduction in MMN in schizophrenia patients have been to model in an animal?
associated with poor functional outcome (Rasser et al., 2011; Light
and Braff, 2005; Featherstone et al., 2018), which has led to a strong MMN is a signal of enhanced prediction error that occurs in
interest in MMN as a potential clinically useful biomarker (Erickson response to an unexpected stimulus due to violation of a pattern of
€a
et al., 2016; Light and Na €t€
anen, 2013). The correlation with func- regularity, as opposed to a decrease in response to a repeated
tional impairment is important and more consistent across studies stimulus (adaptation). When seeking to establish a rodent analogue
than correlations with clinical symptoms (particularly positive of human MMN it is important to consider that the responses
symptoms), suggesting that MMN does appear to be a reflection of measured will be unlikely to show the same morphology in a rat
some basic aspect of the integrity of the nervous system that de- brain as it is a differently-sized and -shaped brain compared to the
termines how well the individual functions. In fact, Rasser and human brain. Confidence in a rodent analogue therefore cannot be
colleagues suggest links between MMN and frontotemporal grey based on ‘face’ validity determined by familiarity but should
matter pathology may mediate the link between MMN and func- instead be sought through establishing ‘construct’ validity. An
tional status in patients (Rasser et al., 2011). Although MMN important task in determining the validity of a rodent MMN
impairment in schizophrenia patients is one of the most highly analogue is therefore to establish whether we are measuring
replicated neurophysiological features of the disorder (Na €€
ata€nen something that maps on to the same construct (i.e. predictive
et al., 2016), currently available treatments provide only partial coding) which can be facilitated by exploring whether amplitude
remediation of the MMN reduction (Zhou et al., 2013; Greenwood modulation seen in human MMN occurs in a rodent.
J. Jalewa et al. / Hearing Research 399 (2021) 107992 5

3.1. Predictive coding (prediction error vs. adaptation) memory-based hypothesis. According to some of the in-
terpretations based on the adaptation hypothesis, MMN latency
Although a hotly debated topic, there is accumulating evidence and duration should be similar to the N1 response, an experimental
that the kind of “active” prediction implicated in the mismatch manipulation should affect MMN and N1 similarly, and no MMN
process can indeed be separated from adaptation (Harms et al., should be elicited in the absence of N1. However, N1 responses are
2016; N€ aa€ta
€nen et al., 2005). The first compelling evidence for closely time-locked to the onset of the stimulus and even a change
some degree of adaptation-independent MMN responses in in magnitude of the stimulus does not affect the peak latency. But,
humans was published by Jacobsen and Schro € ger who developed MMN latency is affected by magnitude of the stimulus change,
an experimental protocol to control for the refractoriness effects in which indicates that the latency and duration of N1 and MMN do
order to demonstrate that MMN occurs even when these are not track together (Na €a€t€
anen et al., 2005). Second, an MMN
controlled (Jacobsen and Schro € ger, 2001). The study involved pre- response is elicited even in the absence of N1 response, in a stim-
sentation of 50 ms tones in three blocks (ascending deviant, ulus omission paradigm (Yabe et al., 1997, 1998; Na €a
€t€
anen et al.,
descending deviant and control). The control block comprised of 10 2005). Third, the sensitivity of N1 to various pharmacological ma-
equiprobable tones of varying frequency (pitch) presented in a nipulations differs from that of MMN. For example, MK-801
pseudorandom sequence including the tones presented in the (NMDA-receptor antagonist) administration eliminated the MMN
oddball sequence as deviant and standard (Jacobsen and Schro € ger, response in monkeys but had no effect on the equivalent of N1
2001), at the same probability (10%) as they are in the oddball se- responses (Javitt et al., 1996; Na €€ €nen et al., 2005). Such
ata
quences. This sequence type, known as the ‘many standards’, the adaptation-independence can also be found for auditory ERPs
‘Jacobsen’ or the ‘equiprobable’ control condition, can be used to outside the MMN timescale and in the middle latency response
disentangle the two variables confounded with standard and period, a timescale associated more with late subcortical and early
deviant stimuli presentation: those of probability and expectation. cortical auditory processing (Grimm et al., 2011; Grimm and Escera,
That is, the deviant is both improbable and unexpected, whereas 2012; Escera et al., 2014).
the standard is both probable and expected. If responses to these
stimuli differ, is it due to probability or expectation? If probability 3.2. Other measures relevant to predictive coding
was solely responsible for the MMN, this would be consistent with
the adaptation hypothesis, whereas if expectation were respon- 3.2.1. Sensitivity to deviance difference
sible, the prediction error hypothesis would be supported. Thus, a MMN is generated by an automatic discrimination process
response to the control stimulus, improbable but not violating an (Lyytinen et al., 1992) and human studies have shown modulation
expected pattern (because no pattern is established), can be of MMN amplitude by the degree of stimulus deviance. That is, an
compared to the deviant response to isolate the effect of the increase in the magnitude of the difference between the deviant
violation of the expected regularity in the sequence. Significant and standard sound results in an increased MMN amplitude and a
‘classic MMN’ was observed in the deviant vs. standard comparison decreased peak latency (Pakarinen et al., 2007; Horva th et al., 2008;
but in addition, significant differences were found in the deviant € ger and Winkler, 1995;
Tiitinen et al., 1994; Todd et al., 2012; Schro
compared to the control (Jacobsen and Schro €ger, 2001). This finding Tse and Penney, 2008). As noted earlier, research demonstrates that
suggested that MMN in humans, at least in part, could be attributed MMN is elicited in response to simple changes on a variety of
to a memory-based prediction error process other rather than stimulus dimensions: frequency (Grimm and Schro €ger, 2007;
adaptation alone (Jacobsen and Schro € ger, 2001). Using a similar Na€€
ata€nen et al., 1982; Paavilainen et al., 1993b), duration
control methodology, an additional study also demonstrated (Paavilainen et al., 1993a; Joutsiniemi et al., 1998; Hsu et al., 2010;
similar effects for duration deviants (Jacobsen and Schro €ger, 2003). Suga et al., 2016; Kompus et al., 2015) or intensity (Winkler et al.,
Whole-head magnetoencephalography studies have demonstrated 1990; Althen et al., 2011). For deviants that vary on simple stim-
that adaptation and memory-based mechanisms can be separated ulus dimension, MMN size relates to deviance discrimination as
in time using many standards control (Maess et al., 2007; Hsu et al., MMN gets smaller with smaller deviance difference and eventually
2010), supporting that MMN is a memory-based response. More- disappears when the subject is unable to behaviourally distinguish
over, a neuronal model of predictive coding in MMN supports the the stimulus difference (Daikhin and Ahissar, 2012; Na €a
€t€anen et al.,
idea that MMN results from an active cortical prediction rather than 2007). In regards to predictive coding, this suggests that a predic-
a passive synaptic habituation (Wacongne et al., 2012). tion error response will adjust in size depending on the size of the
Memory-based prediction error responses have also been found prediction violation, which means that the larger the deviance
in studies using a cascade control, which is a regular cascadic difference in a given context, the greater the degree of violation,
sequence where tones are presented in either ascending or which therefore leads to comparatively larger prediction error
descending frequencies (Ruhnau et al., 2012). Unlike the many response. However, the response cannot keep increasing, but
standards control, where tone order is random, the cascade instead normalises to context (divisive normalisation) (Carandini
sequence establishes a pattern of regularity. In other words, the and Heeger, 2011).
cascade control tone is highly comparable to the deviant tone in the
oddball sequence: it is the same physical tone, presented at the 3.2.2. Sensitivity to deviant probability
same rate, and presented within a sequence where a prediction can The probability of occurrence of a deviant stimulus among the
be generated. The control tone only differs from the oddball deviant regular standard stimuli is also a critical determinant of MMN size
in one way: it ‘fits’ with the predictive model, whereas the deviant (Sabri and Campbell, 2001; Shelley et al., 1999). Human studies
violates the prediction. This may suggest that the corrected MMN have found that MMN amplitude increases with decreasing prob-
obtained based on cascade control sequence would give a more ability of occurrence of a deviant (rare deviant or highly unexpected
accurate quantification of prediction error when compared to the change). In other words, larger MMN occurs to more marked
MMN calculated based on many standards control sequence. But, a probability violation (Todd et al., 2012; Sabri and Campbell, 2001;
recent study by Wiens et al. have demonstrated that both the Horva th and Winkler, 2004; Sonnadara et al., 2006; Lopez-
controls are comparable controls for the auditory frequency MMN Caballero et al., 2016). The deviant probability is relevant for pre-
in oddball tasks (Wiens et al., 2019; Parras et al., 2017). dictive coding as the precision associated with a model of regularity
Na€a
€ t€
anen et al. further reviewed the evidence in favour of the will be influenced by the number and size of failures to account for
6 J. Jalewa et al. / Hearing Research 399 (2021) 107992

sensory input e i.e., the probability of occurrence of the deviant brain in response to an unexpected omission of an auditory stim-
stimulus. For instance, having a sequence with a 25% deviant ulus (Joutsiniemi and Hari, 1989; Hughes et al., 2001; Wacongne
probability relative to a sequence with a 10% deviant probability et al., 2012; Yabe et al., 1997; Raij et al., 1997; Horva th et al.,
will reduce the precision associated with the prediction that the 2007; Nordby et al., 1994). In other words, sound omission elicits
standard is the next stimulus. Under such a low-confidence sce- an MMN-like response, time-locked to the expected onset of the
nario, when the prediction is violated, smaller MMN will be sound (Winkler and Czigler, 2012; Oceak et al., 2006). Based on the
generated. assumption that the physically absent stimulus cannot actually
produce a perceptual response, it has been argued that omission
3.2.3. Other factors MMN is evidence of ‘pure’ memory-based, prediction error process
In addition to the magnitude of deviance difference and deviant (May and Tiitinen, 2010; Yabe et al., 1997; Na€€ €nen et al., 2005).
ata
probability, manipulation of the stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA,
the time between the onset of one stimulus to the onset of the next) 4. How well do existing reports of rat MMN show that they
affects the MMN amplitude, as demonstrated in human studies model each of these aspects?
€€
(Ja askela€inen et al., 1999; Sabri and Campbell, 2001; Müller et al.,
2005; Sussman et al., 1999; Weise et al., 2014). As SOAs increase It is not surprising to find that non-primate animals such as
past 1s, the MMN decreases until it disappears at SOAs of 10s, rodents exhibit SSA to auditory stimuli: it is a relatively ‘simple’
which is in agreement with the hypothesis that MMN is reliant on process that can presumably occur at the single synapse level. A
auditory sensory memory (Ja €€ €inen et al., 1999; Sabri and
askela memory-based prediction error mechanism, however, necessitates
Campbell, 2001). Further, the timing of stimulus presentation is far more complexity, such as sensory memory, prediction genera-
an important contextual variable that determines filtering of sound tion, stimulus comparison and prediction error processes, and it is
based on relevance. Moreover, the period of stability or the degree not as readily assumed that the rodent brain is capable of engaging
of regularity i.e., how regularly the sounds occur, influences the in all of these processes to generate a ‘true’ human-like MMN with
MMN amplitude (Todd et al., 2018; Jones, 2002). We describe some degree of prediction error. That said, the shifting and ori-
variability in timing as ‘jitter’, which means variability in the SOA. enting of behavioural responses to unexpected stimuli is certainly
Research findings have shown that larger MMN is produced in observed in many other animal species, even those of less ‘intelli-
response to sequences with a regular SOA, as compared to a vari- gence’ than rodents, so it would not be surprising if the rodent
able SOA (Winkler et al., 2001; Takegata and Morotomi, 1999; Todd brain is capable of generating prediction error responses. Studying
et al., 2018; Tervaniemi et al., 1997; Takegata et al., 2001a, 2001b). MMRs in rodents is a relatively new and growing field of research
Recently, another attribute of human MMN has been estab- (Featherstone et al., 2018; Tikhonravov et al., 2008). Some initial
lished. Although the brain can quickly begin to alter predictions studies conducted as early as the 1990s report the presence of ERPs
based on the introduction of a new ‘standard’ stimulus (as few as and MMN-like activity in rodents (Yamaguchi et al., 1993; Ehlers
2e3 repetitions of a new regularity are enough for adjustment), it et al., 1994). In the rodent brain, MMN-like ERPs have been
has been shown that the stimulus composition presented first can observed to commence less than 100 ms from stimulus onset.
have lasting effects. In other words, what we learn first results in a MMN-like activity elicited in response to the deviant tones have
bias towards processing of a sound (Todd et al., 2011). The first been found in anesthetized rats (Ruusuvirta et al., 1998;
report on this primacy bias or ‘first-impression bias’ used a two- Tikhonravov et al., 2010; Astikainen et al., 2006), awake rats
tone sequence where a short and long duration sound switched (Eriksson and Villa, 2005) and awake mice (Umbricht et al., 2005;
roles as standard and deviant multiple times in different sound Ehrlichman et al., 2008). Moreover, anesthetized rats have
sequences creating different periods of “rule” or regularity stability. demonstrated a comparable ability of change detection in the
One of the main findings of this study was that only the tone that speech sounds (Ahmed et al., 2011; Astikainen et al., 2014; Kurkela
was encountered first as a deviant was impacted by the period of et al., 2016) and complex sounds (Yang et al., 2019).
local stability e that is, it was larger for longer periods of a stable However, as reviewed above, the MMN in humans has specific
rule and smaller for shorter periods of a stable rule (Todd et al., characteristics, and demonstrates unique attributes in response to
2011). The MMN to deviant sounds in the alternate block type did changing stimuli parameters. Apart from adaptation-independence
not show this same modulation by stability. This difference in the and other criteria to determine whether MMRs exist in rodents, (as
pattern of modulation in longer and shorter period of stability in reviewed in (Harms et al., 2016)), there are additional important
these alternating block sequences only occurs when there is a attributes associated with human MMN that remains unexplored in
regularity in the block length (Todd et al., 2018). A series of studies rodents and thus, need to be investigated. These attributes include
on this first impressions bias (Todd et al., 2013, 2014b; Mullens sensitivity to the amount of ‘mismatch’ (i.e., difference in the
et al., 2014, 2016; Frost et al., 2016) have supported the hypothe- physical properties of the standard vs the deviant or-deviance
sis that the different modulation patterns of MMN amplitude in the difference), to differences in deviant probability, to sequence
two blocks are due to systematically higher precision in models of jitter, to the primacy bias, and to omission deviants. To utilize MMN
the first block type versus the second block type that continue so as a biomarker and to bridge the pre-clinical and clinical gap, we
long as the longer term block structure is predictable, but drop as need to overcome the argument that MMN-like responses can be
soon as this longer term pattern is broken (Mullens et al., 2016; explained by adaptation. Additionally, we need a battery of
Fitzgerald and Todd, 2018). Most recently, through the application neurophysiological outcome measures in rodents, exhibiting fea-
of dynamic causal modelling, the hypothesised differences in pre- tures equivalent to that of human MMN. While the properties of
cision for the two blocks were supported by significant differences human MMN in terms of primacy bias and omission are important
in model parameter associated with precision e that is, the degree and to our knowledge, no studies have investigated these yet, it is
of inhibitory gain on the superficial pyramidal cell representing the not our intention to cover them here, and they remain to be
“prediction error” units (Fitzgerald et al., 2019). determined.
The detection of violations in regularity form the basis of pre-
dictive coding and a special case of prediction violation is the 4.1. Adaptation independence
processing of omissions or missing stimuli (Bendixen et al., 2012).
Several studies have shown that MMN can be elicited in the human The generation of a human-like MMN has been demonstrated in
J. Jalewa et al. / Hearing Research 399 (2021) 107992 7

rodents by including carefully-designed control sequences. To 2011) and others (Chen et al., 2015; Hamm and Yuste, 2016; Jung
determine whether the rat brain is capable of generating deviance et al., 2013; Shiramatsu et al., 2013; Sivarao et al., 2014), have
detection (a criteria of human MMN) (Harms et al., 2016), Harms demonstrated that MMR in rodents cannot be explained by adap-
and colleagues used three different control sequences - 1. the flip- tation only and there exists a mechanism of prediction error
flop control (Fig. 1A &B) (to control for differential responses to the response, similar to human MMN.
physical characteristics of standards and deviants as in (Jacobsen
and Schro € ger, 2003)); 2. the many standards control (to control 4.2. Sensitivity to deviance difference
for differential adaptation as in (Jacobsen and Schro € ger, 2003;
Jacobsen and Schro €ger, 2001)) and, 3. the cascade control (to con- Although MMN-like responses have been studied and demon-
trol for differential adaptation) (Harms et al., 2014; Ruhnau et al., strated in animal models, few studies have demonstrated that ro-
2012). In the flip-flop oddball sequences, two oddball sequences dent MMRs show the same sensitivity to deviance magnitude as
are presented, such that the identity of the standard and deviant the human MMN. An initial study in the awake, freely moving rats
reverses, which means that the same tone appears as a standard in found MMN-like response to the duration deviant magnitude
one sequence, and a deviant in the other. In a many standards (Roger et al., 2009). Further, Shiramatsu et al. investigated the
control sequence, the deviant tone is present among other equi- sensitivity of putative MMN in the auditory cortex of anesthetized
probable tones and the tones are presented pseudo-randomly so no rats to the deviance magnitude and found that mean amplitude to a
pattern of regularity is established. Similarly, the cascade control small change in frequency was smaller in comparison to the large
sequence presents the same stimuli presented as deviants, at the change in frequency (Shiramatsu et al., 2013). Another study in
same rate when they are deviants, but they are within an estab- anesthetized rats found that auditory cortical responses to the
lished pattern of regularity and therefore, can serve as a control that deviant and the MMR amplitude were larger for increased differ-
represents both a rare and expected stimulus. While the majority of ences between the deviant and the standard (Ruusuvirta et al.,
rodent studies have used many standards control sequence, some 2015). These findings demonstrate that the rat brain certainly has
have used cascade control sequence. Harms et al. observed the capability to generate MMRs that scale with deviance differ-
adaptation-independent prediction error responses using the many ence. However, additional replication using different recording
standards control, but not with the cascade control (Harms et al., systems and methods, and in addition, in awake rats, would greatly
2014). However, subsequent studies using both anesthetized rats consolidate this conclusion. To examine if the manipulations in
and awake mice, improved upon the design of the cascade degree of deviance have an effect on rodents as in humans, we
sequence and were able to observe prediction error responses us- tested the MMR against low, mid and high deviance difference,
ing unitary recordings in localised auditory areas (Parras et al., discussed later in this review.
2017) as well as by recording local field potentials from the dura
above the auditory cortex (Yang et al., 2019). 4.3. Sensitivity to deviant probability
Using a many standards control, memory-based change detec-
tion, analogous to humans has been observed in anesthetized rats Larger MMR amplitudes have been observed in awake rats using
(Astikainen et al., 2011; Ahmed et al., 2011; Ruusuvirta et al., 2015; epidural recordings, in response to frequency deviants with 0.1
Shiramatsu et al., 2013; Parras et al., 2017; Yang et al., 2019), awake probability of occurrence as compared to 0.2, thus providing initial
rats (Harms et al., 2014; Polterovich et al., 2018) and anesthetized support for the hypotheses that the rat brain can generate a MMR
mice (KURKELA et al., 2018). Furthermore, Nakamura et al. con- with probability sensitivity (Jung et al., 2013). On similar lines,
ducted studies in awake as well as anesthetized rats to investigate Sivarao et al. found sensitivity of MMR in freely behaving rats to
whether rat can produce a deviant response to high/low frequency deviant probability and the authors suggest that low deviant
and long/short duration deviant sounds. The authors used a many probability is important to establish a context of regularity (Sivarao
standards control and found that awake rats exhibited MMN-like et al., 2014). To confirm this attribute in rats, we conducted a sys-
activity in response to high frequency and long duration tones tematic study and investigated the effect of low, mid and high
(Nakamura et al., 2011). probability manipulation.
Along similar lines, other studies have employed cascade control
sequences to dissociate SSA from the prediction error response in 4.4. Sensitivity to SOA manipulation
anesthetised rats (Parras et al., 2017; Yang et al., 2019). Control
measures such as many standards and the cascade control are As observed in humans, some rodent studies have demonstrated
useful for understanding and exploring the MMN mechanism. a dependence of MMR amplitude on the sensory memory trace by
However, this requires a careful placement of the deviant and manipulating the inter stimulus interval (ISI) (KURKELA et al., 2018;
standard sounds in the control sequence as studies have found Astikainen et al., 2011). The ISI is roughly equivalent to the SOA, but
differential ERPs in response to when the sounds are assigned in measures the offset-to-onset time, rather than presenting an onset-
the middle (may result in an overestimation of the MMN) vs edge to-onset time, as the SOA does. Larger MMR amplitude have been
(extreme substandard effect, which may result in an underesti- observed in response to deviant sounds with shorter 375 ms ISIs
mation of deviance detection) in the control sequence (Harms et al., compared to longer 600 ms ISI in anesthetized mice (KURKELA
2014, Jacobsen and Schro €ger, 2001; Jacobsen and Schro € ger, 2003). et al., 2018). Earlier, similar effects of ISI manipulation have been
Additionally, different levels of Df (frequency difference between shown by Astikainen and colleagues in anesthetized rats
two consecutive sounds in control sequence) may result in differ- (Astikainen et al., 2011). These findings indicate that the rat brain
ential across-frequency adaptation, resulting in confounded find- does indeed show sensitivity to SOA manipulations, similar to hu-
ings. Furthermore, stimulus type also plays an important role as man MMN, which indicate that rat MMRs may also reflect memory
Yang et al. found genuine MMN in anesthetized rats in response to processes. A thus far unanswered, but relevant question, is to
4000 Hz sound only and not 4600 Hz while using both many determine the limit of the rat brain’s auditory sensory memory in
standard as well as cascade controls (Yang et al., 2019). On the this context, i.e. what is the largest SOA at which rat MMRs can still
contrary, Nakamura et al. demonstrated genuine MMN in response be observed?
to high frequency sound but not low frequency sound (Nakamura In the current study, we will focus on some of the key attributes
et al., 2011). Overall, we (Harms et al., 2014; Nakamura et al., of human MMN that either have not yet been addressed in rodents
8 J. Jalewa et al. / Hearing Research 399 (2021) 107992

at all, or not been addressed in our specific experimental setup: 5.1.5. Mismatch response (MMR) testing
sensitivity to deviance difference, sensitivity to probability MMRs were investigated in awake, freely moving rats. Using a
manipulation and sensitivity to environmental uncertainty (or SOA reusable adhesive, a B-100mAh battery was connected to a wire-
‘jitter’). less-10-channel telemetric headstage (Multi Channel Systems,
Reutlingen, Germany). The wireless headstage was then inserted
5. Original research into the socket on the rat’s head (implanted earlier, as described in
section 5.1.3), and the rat was placed in a 32 cm diameter circular
5.1. Methods plastic bucket (containing bedding) below the speaker through
which acoustic stimuli were presented, in a sound-attenuating
5.1.1. Ethics statement chamber (Med Associates, St. Albans, USA). Multi Channel Sys-
All experiments followed the stringent guidelines of the Na- tems MCRack software was used to record the EEG data. Digitiza-
tional Health and Medical Research Council’s Australian code of tion of each channel of EEG data was performed at 2000 Hz (high
practice for the care and use of animals for scientific purposes. The pass filter 0.1 Hz, low pass filter 5000 Hz, voltage range ± 12.4 mV)
Animal Care and Ethics Committee, University of Newcastle, NSW, (Harms et al., 2014).
Australia approved the experimental protocols (Ethics approval
number: A-2016-610). The surgical procedures were executed un- 5.1.6. Experiment design and stimuli
der maintained anaesthesia and all efforts were made to reduce the
5.1.6.1. Deviance difference experiment. Rats were tested on three
number of animals, and minimize the pain and suffering following
conditions of increasing frequency difference between the deviant
surgery through use of analgesics.
and standard stimuli: 1. low deviance difference (standard 6636 Hz,
deviant 8137 Hz), 2. mid deviance difference (standard 6636 Hz,
5.1.2. Animals
deviant 9977 Hz) and, 3. high deviance difference (standard
Male and female Wistar rats (age: 3e4 months, n ¼ 10e12 per
6636 Hz, deviant 12233 Hz) in a flip-flop design (so that standard
sex) underwent surgeries for the study. All the rats were housed in
and deviant ERPs were elicited by the tones with identical physical
pairs in the standard cages, in the temperature and humidity
characteristics). The roles of standard (87.5%) and deviant (12.5%) in
controlled Behavioural Sciences animal facility at the University of
each condition were reversed resulting in an ascending (low fre-
Newcastle, under normal light 12hr day-night cycle, and had ad
quency standard and high frequency deviant) or a descending (high
libitum access to the food and water.
frequency standard and low frequency deviant) deviant sequence.
The three deviance difference conditions across two flip-flop se-
5.1.3. Surgery to implant EEG electrodes
quences resulted in a total of six sequences presented to the rats.
Surgery was performed to implant stainless steel screw elec-
EEG was recorded for each rat on each of these six sequences in a
trodes, similar to the previous studies from the laboratory (Harms
randomised order for a single session (lasting 1 h 33 min that
et al., 2014). Rats were anesthetised with isoflurane and fixed
included a minimum 1 min silence between each sequences). For
upon a stereotaxic frame (Stoelting, IL, USA), using ear bars. Car-
each sequence type, a total of 200 deviants and 1400 standards
profen (5 mg/kg s.c.) and buprenorphine (0.05 mg/kg s.c.) were
were presented in a pseudorandom order with a SOA of 500 ms,
administered as analgesics prior to the start of the surgery. Six
and no fewer than three standards between each deviant.
0.9 mm burr holes were drilled at locations corresponding to the
four recording sites [dorsal to the left and right frontal cortices (LFC
and RFC, 2.00 mm anterior to Bregma and 2.00 mm lateral to the 5.1.6.2. Probability difference experiment. Rats were exposed to
midline), the left and right auditory cortices (LAC and RAC, 5.00 mm ascending and descending oddball sequences comprising two tones
posterior to Bregma and 4.00 mm lateral to the midline)], the (a low frequency tone of 6636 Hz and a high frequency tone of
ground electrode (left posterior cortex, 2.00 mm anterior to 12233 Hz), in a flip-flop design. Three different deviant probability
Lambda and 2.50 mm left of the midline), and the reference elec- conditions were used: 1/4 (25%, high), 1/8 (12.5%, mid), 1/16
trode dorsal to the cerebellum (1.00 mm posterior to Lambda and (6.125%, low). As above, there were six types of sequences, pre-
1.00 mm to the right of the midline). The complete assembly of the sented to each rat, in a randomised order within a single session.
connector along with six screws and their respective wires were The sessions lasted longer than the deviance difference experiment,
fixed to the head using self-adhesive cement (SmartCem2, Dents- as a sufficient number of deviant stimuli needed to be presented for
ply, Australia) followed by temporary crown and bridge material the low probability condition. Each session lasted 2 h 13 min, and
dental cement (Integrity, Dentsply, Australia). The skin was then out of a total of 2400 stimuli presented for each sequence type at an
glued back onto the dental cement with Vetbond, followed by SOA of 500 ms, there were 150, 300 and 600 deviants presented in
subcutaneous administration of sterile 0.9% saline (10 ml/kg body the low, mid and high probability conditions, respectively.
weight, s.c.) to balance for any fluid loss during the surgery. Testing
was performed after a week of post-surgery recovery. 5.1.6.3. Jitter experiment. Rats were exposed to ascending and
descending oddball sequences comprising two tones (a low fre-
5.1.4. Sound generation quency tone of 6636 Hz and a high frequency tone of 8137 Hz), in a
A custom program was written in Presentation software (Neu- flip-flop design. This difference is equivalent to the low deviance
robehavioral Systems, Inc.) to generate the auditory stimuli (fre- difference of the previously described paradigm (section 5.1.6.A).
quency-modulated broadband tones, oscillating around a central Three different SOA conditions were used: 500 ms SOA (No Jitter),
frequency ± 3%), which were amplified, and delivered through a 450e550 ms variable SOA (Low Jitter), and 250e750 ms highly
speaker mounted on the roof of the experimental chamber (Harms variable SOA (High Jitter). The SOA in variable SOA conditions var-
et al., 2014). A sound meter (Bruel & Kjaer Model 2260) was used to ied pseudorandomly in 10 and 50 ms steps for the Low and High
calibrate the intensity of the free-field sounds to approximately Jitter conditions, respectively. As above, there were six types of
70 dB sounds in the range of 6636Hz and 12233Hz. Stimuli used sequences, presented to each rat, in a randomised order within a
were always broadband frequency-modulated tones of 100 ms in single session. For each sequence type, a total of 200 deviants and
duration, each centred on a particular frequency (e.g. 12233Hz) ± 1400 standards were presented in a pseudorandom order, with no
3%, with 10 ms rise/fall times. fewer than three standards between each deviant.
J. Jalewa et al. / Hearing Research 399 (2021) 107992 9

5.1.7. Data extraction Difference/Probability/Jitter (depending on the experiment), Site


Data were processed off-line using EEGDisplay (version 6.4.1) as (AC and FC) and Frequency (low and high if both were included). If
described in Harms et al. (2014). An initial step involved exclusion interactions with site were observed, appropriate post-hoc com-
of gross artefact intervals in the continuous EEG record using an parisons at each site were conducted.
automated algorithm that rejected signals exceeding ±1400 mV. In the final analysis of each experiment, significant effects of
Thereafter, epochs were extracted from the continuous EEG con- independent variables on MMRs were followed up to determine
sisting of 100 ms pre-stimulus baseline and a 300 ms post-stimulus whether DEV responses, STD responses, or both, underpinned
interval. At epoch extraction, ERPs were baseline corrected over a changes in the MMR. Repeated measures ANOVA were used with
100 ms pre-stimulus interval. Epochs were averaged for each rat either the deviant or standard mean amplitude response as the
and separately for each stimulus type. After the pre-processing dependent variable, and with Deviance Difference/Probability/Jitter
steps of bad channel rejection and artefact rejection prior to seg- (depending on the experiment), Site (AC and FC) and Frequency
menting into epochs and averaging, the latency ranges of individual (low and high if both were included) as within-subjects factors.
ERP components were determined based on the morphologies of
the ERPs, taken from grand averages and thereafter, their mean 5.2. Results
amplitude extracted for each rat. The ERPs showed distinct com-
ponents characterised by negative and positive peaks (extracted 5.2.1. Effect of deviance difference on rat MMRs
over latency windows). For the deviance difference experiment, For all components, significant MMRs (Deviant > Standard)
mean amplitude measures were extracted from four peaks at the were only found for high frequency stimuli as demonstrated by
following time windows: N18: 14e22.5 ms (AC), 11.5e21 ms (FC); significant Frequency  Stimulus interactions (N18: F(1,20) ¼ 30.836,
P32: 24.5e39.5 ms (AC), 22e42 ms (FC); N54: 40e67.5 ms (AC), p < 0.001; P32: F(1,20) ¼ 41.679, p < 0.001; N54: F(1,20) ¼ 19.582,
42.5e67.5 (FC); N86: 67.5e105 ms (both sites) (Supplementary p < 0.001; N86: F(1,20) ¼ 6.622, p ¼ 0.018), followed by significant
Figs. 4A and B). Likewise, mean amplitudes were extracted for the (p < 0.05) posthoc pairwise effects demonstrating DEV > STD for
probability dataset using the following time windows: N18: responses to high, but not low frequency stimuli. Therefore, further
14e22.5 ms (AC), 12e20.5 ms (FC); P32: 23.5e40 ms (AC), analyses to study the deviance difference effect was performed on
21.5e44 ms (FC); N54: 42e67 ms (AC), 46e67 (FC); N86: mean amplitudes in response to high frequency stimuli only.
67e105 ms (both sites) (Supplementary Figs. 4C and D). Similarly, The degree of difference between the deviant and standard
mean amplitudes were extracted for the jitter experiment dataset affected the difference MMR measure for each component, as
using the following time windows: N18: 13e22 ms (AC), 13e22 ms demonstrated by main effects of Deviance (N18: F(2,40) ¼ 5.346,
(FC); P32: 22.5e42.5 ms (AC), 20.5e42.5 ms (FC); N54: 43e70 ms p ¼ 0.009; P32: F(2,40) ¼ 6.049, p ¼ 0.005; N54: F(2,40) ¼ 10.713,
(AC), 46e67 (FC); N86: 70e105 ms (both sites) (Supplementary p < 0.001; N86: F(2,40) ¼ 5.848, p ¼ 0.006). The evoked potentials to
Figs. 4E and F). While EEG was recorded at left and right AC as the three levels of ascending frequency are presented in Fig. 2 for
well as left and right FC, MMRs were averaged across left and right the MMR difference waves (Fig. 2A), deviant ERP (Fig. 2C) and
sites. regular standard ERP (Fig. 2E). The post hoc comparisons indicated a
significant effect of the deviance difference on the mean ampli-
5.1.8. Statistical analysis tudes (±standard error, SE) of all the MMR components [N18: ((high
The sample size for the experiments was n ¼ 10e12 per sex. All vs. mid, p ¼ 0.017) (high vs. low, p ¼ 0.018),); P32: ((high vs. low,
the analyses were performed using SPSS 25 and analysis of variance p ¼ 0.008), (mid vs. low, p ¼ 0.007); N54: ((high vs. mid, p ¼ 0.002)
(ANOVA) with repeated measures was used to analyse the mean (high vs. low, p ¼ 0.001),); N86: ((high vs. low, p ¼ 0.007))] (Fig. 2B).
amplitudes of the ERP components and the effect of Deviance dif- There were no Site  Deviance interaction effects on the mean
ference, Probability or Jitter. For each of the three experiments, an amplitudes of N18, P32 and N54 MMR components, however, N86
initial repeated measures ANOVA was performed on mean ampli- (F(2,40) ¼ 3.427, p ¼ 0.042) component exhibited significant
tude responses with within-subjects factors of Stimulus (DEV, STD), Site  Deviance interaction effect. Further post hoc comparisons
Frequency (High, Low), Site (AC, FC), and the experimental revealed a significant Deviance effect on the N86 mean amplitudes
manipulation of either Deviant difference (High, Medium, Low), in auditory cortex only [(low vs. mid, p ¼ 0.003) (low vs. high,
Probability (Low, Mid, High) or Jitter (No Jitter, Low Jitter, High p ¼ 0.001),] and not in the frontal cortex [(low vs. mid, p ¼ 0.803),
Jitter). The primary purpose of this initial analysis was to determine (low vs. high, p ¼ 0.133) (mid vs. high, p ¼ 0.101),].
which frequency produced significant MMRs, given that for each The MMR is a difference measure, and therefore is influenced by
experiment there was both a high frequency deviant and a low two factors: the response to the deviant and that to the standard.
frequency deviant. In these analyses, we examined the effect of Therefore, we performed additional analyses to examine what
Stimulus (DEV vs. STD) on the mean amplitude response for each underlies the sensitivity to the MMR to changes in deviance dif-
component of the ERP, and whether the effect of Stimulus was ference: does the response to the deviant, the standard, or both,
present for both frequencies or one only (for example, high fre- change? Significant Deviance difference effects were found for
quency, as demonstrated previously (Harms et al., 2014)). Such an deviant response for all components (N18: F(2,40) ¼ 9.709, p < 0.001;
effect would be demonstrated by a significant P32: F(2,40) ¼ 3.697, p ¼ 0.034; N54: F(2,40) ¼ 19.591, p < 0.001; N86:
Stimulus  Frequency interaction followed by relevant Stimulus F(2,40) ¼ 7.656, p ¼ 0.002), and similar to the MMR, all pairwise post
contrasts at each frequency. If there was evidence that only high- hoc comparisons revealed that the deviant response in the high
frequency stimuli produced an MMR (i.e. DEV > STD), we limited deviance difference condition was consistently higher than that in
further analyses to just the high frequency stimuli. the low deviance difference condition (Fig. 2D). For the Standard
For subsequent analyses, the MMR was computed for each stimulus (Fig. 2F), there were no significant main effects of
condition: DEV e STD. In this computation, and in analyses Deviance.
comparing deviant and standard stimuli, the flip-flop control was
employed and only stimuli of the same frequency were compared, 5.2.2. Effect of deviant probability on rat MMRs
e.g., 8137Hz deviant vs. 8137Hz standard. The second major anal- Frequency  Stimulus interactions were observed on mean
ysis used a repeated measures ANOVA with the MMR as the amplitude responses (N18: F(1,23) ¼ 9.376, p ¼ 0.006; P32:
dependent variable, and within-subjects factors of Deviance F(1,23) ¼ 19.833, p < 0.001; N54: F(1,23) ¼ 41.000, p < 0.001; N86:
10 J. Jalewa et al. / Hearing Research 399 (2021) 107992

Fig. 2. Effect of deviance difference on event related potentials averaged over auditory and frontal cortex of the control male and female rats, in the ascending oddball
paradigm. A & B. MMR (Deviant e Standard). A. MMR waveform. Difference wave obtained by subtracting the responses to standard stimulus from the responses to the deviant
stimulus for low (blue), mid (grey), high (red) deviance differences. B. MMR mean amplitude. Mean amplitudes (±standard error, SE) of N18, P32, N54 and N86 components of rat
MMRs. There is a significantly larger MMR amplitude in response to the high deviance difference (in red) as compared to the low deviance difference (in blue) or mid deviance
difference (in grey). C & D. ERP responses to Deviant stimuli. C. Grand averaged ERP response (waveform) to deviant stimuli. D. Deviant mean amplitude. Mean amplitudes (±standard
J. Jalewa et al. / Hearing Research 399 (2021) 107992 11

F(1,23) ¼ 17.622, p < 0.001). We found that Deviant Auditory Cortex [(low vs. high, p ¼ 0.002), (mid vs. high, p ¼ 0.003),
response > Standard response for all the components (Stimulus Supplementary Fig. 2C], but were also present at the Frontal Cortex
effect: p < 0.05 for all). Further, pairwise comparisons between (low vs. high, p ¼ 0.032; Supplementary Fig. 2D). There were no
Deviant and Standard responses, split for Frequency, revealed that significant Probability effects on the mean amplitudes of any of the
deviant responses were significantly larger than those for stan- components in response to Standard stimuli (Fig. 3E and F).
dards for all the components at high frequencies (p < 0.001 for all
components). In addition, such effects held for two components
5.2.3. Effect of SOA variability (jitter) on rat MMRs
(P32 and N86) for the low frequency stimuli (p < 0.001), but not for
For all components, significant MMRs (Deviant > Standard)
N18 (p ¼ 0.071) or N54 (p ¼ 0.126). Therefore, in the further ana-
were found for high frequency stimuli as demonstrated by signifi-
lyses to examine the probability effect on mean amplitudes, both
cant Frequency  Stimulus interactions (N18: F(1,22) ¼ 10.415,
high and low frequency stimuli responses were included.
p ¼ 0.004; P32: F(1,22) ¼ 21.526, p < 0.001; N54: F(1,22) ¼ 10.482,
Next, we examined the effect of Probability on MMRs. There was
p ¼ 0,004; N86: F(1,22) ¼ 18.080, p < 0.001), but were also found for
a main effect of Probability on MMR mean amplitude for P32
low frequency stimuli at P32 (p ¼ 0.012). Therefore, further ana-
(F(2,46) ¼ 5.614, p ¼ 0.007) and N54 (F(2,46) ¼ 5.244, p ¼ 0.009)
lyses to study the jitter effect was performed on mean amplitudes
components. The post hoc comparisons indicated a significant
in response to both high and low frequency stimuli.
probability effect on the mean amplitudes of P32 (low vs. high,
No significant effects of Jitter were observed for any of the
p ¼ 0.001) and N54 [(low vs. high, p ¼ 0.003) (mid vs. high,
components examined (N18: F(2,44) ¼ 0.388, p ¼ 0.680; P32:
p ¼ 0.027),] MMR components (Fig. 3B). There were significant
F(2,44) ¼ 0.452, p ¼ 0.639; N54: F(2,44) ¼ 2.067, p ¼ 0.139; N86:
main effects of Frequency on MMR mean amplitude for all the
F(2,44) ¼ 0.258, p ¼ 0.774). However, there were significant main
components (N18: F(1,23) ¼ 9.376, p ¼ 0.006; P32: F(1,23) ¼ 19.833,
effects of Frequency on MMR mean amplitude for all the compo-
p < 0.001; N54: F(1,23) ¼ 41.000, p < 0.001; N86: F(1,23) ¼ 17.622,
nents (N18: F(1,22) ¼ 10.415, p ¼ 0.004; P32: F(1,22) ¼ 21.526,
p < 0.001). The post hoc pairwise comparisons revealed a signifi-
p < 0.001; N54: F(1,22) ¼ 10.482, p ¼ 0.004; N86: F(1,22) ¼ 18.080,
cant frequency effect on the mean amplitudes of all the compo-
p < 0.001). The post hoc pairwise comparisons revealed a signifi-
nents (N18 (low frequency vs. high frequency, p ¼ 0.006); P32 (low
cant frequency effect on the mean amplitudes of all the compo-
frequency vs. high frequency, p < 0.001); N54 (low frequency vs.
nents (N18 (low frequency vs. high frequency, p ¼ 0.004); P32 (low
high frequency, p < 0.001); N86 (low frequency vs. high frequency,
frequency vs. high frequency, p < 0.001); N54 (low frequency vs.
p < 0.001) (Supplementary Fig. 1A). In every case, the mean
high frequency, p ¼ 0.004); N86 (low frequency vs. high frequency,
amplitude of a component was larger in response to a high fre-
p < 0.001). Further, there were no significant Frequency  Jitter
quency stimulus. However, there were no significant
interaction effects on the mean amplitudes of any of the compo-
Frequency  Probability interaction effects on the mean amplitudes
nents (N18: F(2,44) ¼ 0.905, p ¼ 0.412; P32: F(2,44) ¼ 1.309, p ¼ 0.280;
of any of the components (N18: F(2,46) ¼ 0.152, p ¼ 0.860; P32:
N54: F(2,44) ¼ 0.176, p ¼ 0.839; N86: F(2,44) ¼ 0.296, p ¼ 0.746)
F(2,46) ¼ 0.854, p ¼ 0.433; N54: F(2,46) ¼ 1.651, p ¼ 0.203; N86:
(Supplementary Fig. 3).
F(2,46) ¼ 0.769, p ¼ 0.469) (Supplementary Fig. 1B).
Although Jitter effects were not significant in the ANOVA, we
Additionally, there were Site  Probability interaction effects on
examined the post-hoc pairwise tests for P32 and N54, given re-
the mean amplitudes of three components (N18: F(2,46) ¼ 4.195,
sults from the Probability experiments suggest these two compo-
p ¼ 0.021; P32: F(2,46) ¼ 4.830, p ¼ 0.012; N54: F(2,46) ¼ 3.944,
nents have MMN-like attributes. There were no pairwise effects of
p ¼ 0.026). Further post hoc comparisons revealed a significant
Jitter on P32, but the MMR response at N54 in the No Jitter con-
Probability effect on the N54 mean amplitudes in auditory cortex
dition was significantly larger than that in the High Jitter condition
only [(low vs. high, p ¼ 0.001) (mid vs. high, p ¼ 0.004),] and not in
(p ¼ 0.042) (Fig. 4B). Further, we investigated whether the deviant
the frontal cortex [(low vs. mid, p ¼ 0.540), (low vs. high, p ¼ 0.080)
response, the standard response, or both was responsible for the
(mid vs. high, p ¼ 0.303),] (Supplementary Fig. 2A), while, a Prob-
jitter effects. Pairwise comparisons indicated a marginal effect of
ability effect was observed on the P32 mean amplitudes in frontal
Jitter for the deviant stimulus for N54 component with larger MMR
cortex only [(low vs. high, p < 0.001) (mid vs. high, p ¼ 0.046),] and
amplitude for ‘no jitter’ as compared to ‘high jitter’ (p ¼ 0.066)
not in auditory cortex [(low vs. mid, p ¼ 0.229), (low vs. high,
(Fig. 4D). There were no significant Jitter effects on the mean am-
p ¼ 0.155) (mid vs. high, p ¼ 0.973),] (Supplementary Fig. 2B).
plitudes of any of the components in response to Standard stimuli
Significant pairwise Site x Probability effects were not observed in
(Fig. 4E and F).
posthoc comparisons for N18.
Further, we investigated whether the deviant response, the
standard response, or both was responsible for the probability ef- 6. General discussion
fects. Probability effects for the deviant stimulus were found for
N54 (F(2,46) ¼ 5.706, p ¼ 0.006), with post-hoc comparisons indi- Using a wireless telemetry system, we examined the effects of
cating that low vs high (p ¼ 0.004) and mid vs high probability three manipulations in freely moving, unrestrained rats: the
(p ¼ 0.008) effects were significant (Fig. 3D). The effect of Proba- physical difference between the expected and unexpected stimuli,
bility on P32 Deviant response was not significant (F(2,46) ¼ 2.496, the probability of the unexpected stimulus and the variability in
p ¼ 0.094). Similar to the MMR, Site x Probability effects were SOAs (jitter). We hypothesised that larger MMRs would be elicited
observed for the Deviant response at P32 (F(2,46) ¼ 3.832, p ¼ 0.029) to unexpected (deviant) stimuli that a) have a larger change in pitch
and N54 (F(2,46) ¼ 3.312, p ¼ 0.045), with Probability effects limited compared to the expected (standard) stimulus, b) are less
to the Frontal Cortex site for P32 (low vs. high, p ¼ 0.002, frequently presented (lower probability), and c) are presented in a
Supplementary Fig. 2D) and were not significant for Auditory Cor- fixed SOA (no jitter) environment. MMRs were elicited and the
tex sites. Probability effects for N54 were significant for the morphology of the components resembled those previously
observed in our laboratory (Harms et al., 2014; Nakamura et al.,

error, SE) of N18, P32, N54 and N86 components in response to the deviant stimulus. E & F. ERP responses to Standard stimuli. E. Grand averaged ERP response (waveform) to
standard stimuli. F. Standard mean amplitude. Mean amplitudes (±standard error, SE) of N18, P32, N54 and N86 components in response to the standard stimulus. (For interpretation
of the references to colour in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the Web version of this article.)
12 J. Jalewa et al. / Hearing Research 399 (2021) 107992

Fig. 3. Effect of deviant probability on event related potentials averaged over auditory and frontal cortex of the control male and female rats, averaged for the frequency. A & B. MMR
(Deviant e Standard). A. MMR waveform. Difference wave obtained by subtracting the responses to standard stimulus from the responses to the deviant stimulus for low (red), mid
(grey), high (blue) probability. B. MMR mean amplitude. Mean amplitudes (±standard error, SE) of N18, P32, N54 and N86 components of rat mismatch responses (MMRs). N54
component has a significantly larger MMR amplitude in response to the low deviant probability (in red) as compared to the high deviant probability (in blue) or mid deviant
probability (in grey). P32 component has a significantly larger MMR amplitude in response to the low deviant probability (in red) as compared to the high deviant probability (in
blue). C & D. ERP responses to Deviant stimuli. C. Grand averaged ERP response (waveform) to deviant stimuli. D. Deviant mean amplitude. Mean amplitudes (±standard error, SE) of
J. Jalewa et al. / Hearing Research 399 (2021) 107992 13

2011), with increased responses to the deviant compared to the of jitter. Overall, the pattern of findings in all the manipulations
standard, at a range of latencies, potentially representing unique suggest that the rat brain extracts the auditory information from
neuronal populations recruited in the response. the environment and processes information in a similar fashion to
The purpose of the first study was to test the hypothesis that the humans. Importantly, our results support the notion that the rat
‘amount of mismatch’ between the standard and deviant tones brain is capable of generating MMRs similar to MMN and that the
modulate the MMN amplitude in rodents, similar to the findings in results conform to what might be expected if a predictive coding
humans. We found that the MMR amplitude was largest for the process occurs in rats.
high deviance difference, in all the components, as compared to the A limitation of the current work is that no controls for deviant
low or mid deviance difference, which is in agreement with the probability, such as the many standards or cascade control, have
hypothesis. Moreover, our findings are in accordance with the been employed, so the MMRs measured in the current study may
previous studies in rodents. Astikainen et al. observed MMRs in be due to either or both adaptation and prediction error. We have
urethane anesthetized rats in response to a frequency deviance previously used a many standards control in the same strain of rats,
difference of 200Hz (5%) (Standard: 4000Hz, Deviant: 3800Hz or using the same methodology as the current study, and have found
4200Hz) and 500Hz (12.5%) (Standard: 4000Hz, Deviant: 3500Hz evidence of adaptation-independent prediction error (Harms et al.,
or 4500Hz) (Astikainen et al., 2011). Another study from the same 2014, 2018). However, because such a sequence was not run in the
group found responses to ascending and descending deviant tones current experiments, we do not know the relative contributions of
across different deviance magnitudes in anesthetized rats. Larger adaptation and prediction error to the observed MMR effects of
MMRs were observed for the higher deviance difference (Deviant: deviance difference, deviant probability or jitter. Moreover, recently
5800Hz, Standard: 4000Hz) when compared to the low difference both adaptation, known as repetition suppression and the
(Deviant: 5300Hz, Standard: 4000Hz) (Ruusuvirta et al., 2015). adaptation-independent response known as prediction error, have
Earlier, Shiramatsu et al. had observed similar effects on MMR been brought together under the umbrella concept of predictive
amplitude (Shiramatsu et al., 2013). Moreover, an effect of the coding (Parras et al., 2017; Garrido et al., 2009; Carbajal and
frequency deviance difference have also been demonstrated in Malmierca, 2018). Therefore, despite not knowing whether it is
awake mice, suggesting that MMR is related to the magnitude of repetition suppression or prediction error response that is manip-
the deviant and standard difference (Umbricht et al., 2005). ulated with changing deviant probability, sequence jitter etc., these
The second experiment examining probability effects found that effects still contribute to the overall process of predictive coding.
MMR amplitude was largest for low probability conditions when Future studies should employ the use of the many standards con-
compared to mid and high probability, supporting the hypothesis trol or cascade control sequences to determine the relative con-
that the rarer (‘surprising’) the deviant is, the larger the response. tributions of adaptation-dependent and -independent processes.
Our results support the findings from previous studies (Jung et al., Smaller MMN amplitude in individuals with schizophrenia is a
2013; Sivarao et al., 2014). Interestingly, we found that there were potential index of abnormal auditory sensory memory processing
Site  Probability interaction effects on mean amplitudes. A sig- (Catts et al., 1995), a feature of schizophrenia psychopathology
nificant Probability effect was found on the N54 mean amplitudes (Javitt et al., 1993), demonstrated in multiple studies (Javitt et al.,
for the auditory cortex site only, while an effect on the P32 mean 1995, 1996; Salisbury et al., 2002). Javitt and colleagues’ research
amplitudes was found in the frontal cortex. Although we cannot findings show that MMN amplitude is reduced in schizophrenia
precisely determine the location of the generators based on these across a wide range of stimulus conditions including manipulations
site differences, due to ambiguity attributable to volume conduc- of stimulus deviance, probability and interstimulus/interdeviant
tion, these findings suggest that different brain regions are interval (Javitt et al., 1998; Javitt, 2000). Shelley et al. found that
responsible for the generation of P32 and N54. schizophrenia-related MMN amplitude reductions become more
Our results from the third experiment, offers preliminary sup- extreme as the deviant probability is reduced in schizophrenia
port towards jitter effects on N54 component in rat. The MMR patients (Shelley et al., 1999). Similarly, Todd et al. found significant
amplitude was largest in ‘no jitter’ condition as compared to ‘low’ main effects of probability (low: 6% and high: 20%) on MMN
and ‘high jitter’ conditions. These results are in line with the human amplitude in healthy individuals. Interestingly, they found a sig-
findings that have shown a decrease in MMN amplitude when an nificant reduction in MMN amplitude in schizophrenia patients in
environmental variance (jitter) is added to the sound sequence response to low probability condition but not the high probability
(Todd et al., 2018): when sounds are presented with variable SOA, condition (Todd et al., 2006). Moreover, Umbricht and colleagues
there is more uncertainty in the timing of the sequence. Although found that temporal uncertainty, which usually reduces MMN in
the characteristics of the deviant and the standard remain the same healthy controls, had no significant effect on the MMN amplitude in
and the deviant is just as improbable as it is in a stable sequence, schizophrenia patients (Umbricht et al., 2005; Todd et al., 2018). In
the uncertainty in the regular timing of the sequence leads to more addition, as covered above, there is an overall trend for individuals
overall uncertainty in modelling the auditory environment, with schizophrenia to not only have overall reduced MMN, but to
reducing the confidence in the predictive model, and thus reducing also generate MMNs that are not responsive to manipulations that
the prediction error response. However, we note the effect of jitter modulate MMN in healthy controls (Todd et al., 2012). While not a
was quite small and only significant when the analysis was diagnostic biomarker, such effects have been hypothesised to be
confined to the most MMN-like components of the rat ERP. This associated with attentional impairments in the disorder, for
may be because the frequency separation between high and low example, if every prediction-violating stimulus, no matter how
frequency sounds was the lowest of the three deviance differences extreme, elicits the same low error response, the system has lost a
used in experiment 1, which was found to produce the smallest mechanism for weighing salience and directing appropriate
MMR. Perhaps a more robust MMR might have resulted if high attentional resources (Todd et al., 2012). Research findings suggest
deviance difference was used, resulting in a more substantial effect that the brain in schizophrenia fails to represent different

N18, P32, N54 and N86 components in response to the deviant stimulus. E & F. ERP responses to Standard stimuli. E. Grand averaged ERP response (waveform) to standard stimuli.
F. Standard mean amplitude. Mean amplitudes (±standard error, SE) of N18, P32, N54 and N86 components in response to the standard stimulus. (For interpretation of the references
to colour in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the Web version of this article.)
14 J. Jalewa et al. / Hearing Research 399 (2021) 107992

Fig. 4. Effect of Jitter manipulation on event related potentials obtained from auditory and frontal cortex of the control male and female rats. A & B. MMR (Deviant e
Standard). A. MMR waveform. Difference wave obtained by subtracting the responses to standard stimulus from the responses to the deviant stimulus for high (red), low (grey), no
(blue) jitter. B. MMR mean amplitude. Mean amplitudes (±standard error, SE) of N18, P32, N54 and N86 components of rat mismatch responses (MMRs). N54 component has a
significantly larger MMR amplitude in response to the no jitter (in blue) as compared to the high jitter (in red) or low jitter (in grey). C & D. ERP responses to Deviant stimuli.
C. Grand averaged ERP response (waveform) to deviant stimuli. D. Deviant mean amplitude. Mean amplitudes (±standard error, SE) of N18, P32, N54 and N86 components in response to
the deviant stimulus. E & F. ERP responses to Standard stimuli. E. Grand averaged ERP response (waveform) to standard stimuli. F. Standard mean amplitude. Mean amplitudes
(±standard error, SE) of N18, P32, N54 and N86 components in response to the standard stimulus. (For interpretation of the references to colour in this figure legend, the reader is
referred to the Web version of this article.)
J. Jalewa et al. / Hearing Research 399 (2021) 107992 15

magnitudes of change and one interpretation is that the dynamic model studies (for instance, preclinical schizophrenia models) to
range with which the brain can represent the environment is investigate the underlying mechanisms and pharmacology of
reduced, suggesting that it is the lower ceiling (earlier asymptote), MMN. In sum, our results further support the potential of rodent
responsible for reducing the dynamic range in schizophrenia in- models in investigating MMN, whether it be to probe the neuro-
dividuals (Todd et al., 2012). While MMN reductions are not a circuitry of the response, or to examine how disease-related per-
diagnostic biomarker of schizophrenia (Lavoie et al., 2019), they are turbations of MMN are generated and how they can be ameliorated.
a biomarker of disease progression (Lavoie et al., 2018, 2019).
Overall, there exists a diminished modulation of MMN by factors Acknowledgements
like the deviance difference/probability/jitter in schizophrenia and
being able to demonstrate these attributes in control rats, as JJ is supported by a University of Newcastle Postgraduate
demonstrated here, is highly valuable in advancing research, as Research Scholarship, funded by the Australian government’s
such methodologies can be employed in future studies to validate Research Training Program. The research was funded by the
potential animal models of schizophrenia and to screen candidate Australian National Health and Medical Research Council through a
pharmacotherapies against them. However, to realise the full po- project grant (APP1109283).
tential of rodent MMRs as a pre-clinical tool, it will be important to
understand whether the factors deviance difference/probability/ Appendix A. Supplementary data
jitter affect adaptation-dependent or -independent processes since
recent research has shown that it is the adaptation-independent (or Supplementary data to this article can be found online at
deviance detection) process that is impaired in schizophrenia https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heares.2020.107992.
(Koshiyama et al., 2020).
With an increasing knowledge of the alterations occurring in References
schizophrenia brain and evidences indicating cognitive impairment
as a prominent feature of schizophrenia, there has been a shift in Ahmed, M., Mallo, T., Leppanen, P.H., Hamalainen, J., Ayravainen, L., Ruusuvirta, T.,
research towards the functional recovery in schizophrenia patients Astikainen, P., 2011. Mismatch brain response to speech sound changes in rats.
Front. Psychol. 2, 283.
(Schulz and Murray, 2016). Several reports have demonstrated that Alho, K., 1995. Cerebral generators of mismatch negativity (MMN) and its magnetic
attention is impaired in schizophrenia and these deficits have been counterpart (MMNm) elicited by sound changes. Ear Hear. 16, 38e51.
considered as a primary issue in the disease neuropathology Alho, K., Connolly, J.F., Cheour, M., Lehtokoski, A., Huotilainen, M., Virtanen, J.,
Aulanko, R., Ilmoniemi, R.J., 1998. Hemispheric lateralization in preattentive
(Orellana et al., 2012; Shen et al., 2014; Salgado-Pineda et al., 2003).
processing of speech sounds. Neurosci. Lett. 258, 9e12.
Furthermore, schizophrenia patients are unable to respond Altamura, A.C., Glick, I.D., 2010. Designing outcome studies to determine efficacy
appropriately to an incoming stimulus due to lack of selection and and safety of antipsychotics for ’real world’ treatment of schizophrenia. Int. J.
interpretation as well as their inability to shift and sustain attention Neuropsychopharmacol. 13, 971e973.
Althen, H., Grimm, S., Escera, C., 2011. Fast detection of unexpected sound intensity
(Chudasama and Robbins, 2004). In order to develop new treat- decrements as revealed by human evoked potentials. PloS One 6, e28522.
ments, the behavioural tasks need to hold cross-species validity so Antunes, F.M., Nelken, I., Covey, E., Malmierca, M.S., 2010. Stimulus-specific adap-
that the potential for predictive validity of animal model studies tation in the auditory thalamus of the anesthetized rat. PloS One 5, e14071.
Astikainen, P., Mallo, T., Ruusuvirta, T., Naatanen, R., 2014. Electrophysiological ev-
can be maximised. Therefore, another direction for future research idence for change detection in speech sound patterns by anesthetized rats.
is to investigate the functional significance of MMRs in rats as found Front. Neurosci. 8, 374.
in humans. Studies have found that task irrelevant auditory stim- Astikainen, P., Ruusuvirta, T., Wikgren, J., Penttonen, M., 2006. Memory-based
detection of rare sound feature combinations in anesthetized rats. Neuroreport
ulation results in behavioural distraction, demonstrated by 17, 1561e1564.
increased response times and decreased accuracy in task perfor- Astikainen, P., Stefanics, G., Nokia, M., Lipponen, A., Cong, F., Penttonen, M.,
mance. Using an auditory-visual cross modal distraction paradigm, Ruusuvirta, T., 2011. Memory-based mismatch response to frequency changes in
rats. PloS One 6, e24208.
in which task relevant visual stimuli are preceded by task irrelevant
Avissar, M., Javitt, D., 2018. Mismatch negativity: a simple and useful biomarker of
auditory standard or deviant tones, it has been suggested that N-methyl-d-aspartate receptor (NMDAR)-type glutamate dysfunction in
MMN reflects a potential mechanism capable of distracting ongoing schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 191, 1e4.
Ayala, Y.A., Perez-Gonza lez, D., Malmierca, M.S., 2016. Stimulus-specific adaptation
task performance (Berti, 2013). Previously, it has been suggested
in the inferior colliculus: the role of excitatory, inhibitory and modulatory in-
that a genuine change detection mechanism is involved in trig- puts. Biol. Psychol. 116, 10e22.
gering attention switching (Escera et al., 2002). Moreover, distrac- Bader, M., Schro € ger, E., Grimm, S., 2017. How regularity representations of short
tion by a deviant sound is an important mechanism that enables sound patterns that are based on relative or absolute pitch information
establish over time: an EEG study. PloS One 12, e0176981.
fast switches of attention to a potentially relevant stimulus and this Bendixen, A., Roeber, U., Schro €ger, E., 2007. Regularity extraction and application in
is considered as a basic mechanism of adaptation to environmental dynamic auditory stimulus sequences. J. Cognit. Neurosci. 19, 1664e1677.
changes (Berti, 2013). Bendixen, A., Sanmiguel, I., Schro €ger, E., 2012. Early electrophysiological indicators
for predictive processing in audition: a review. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 83,
In conclusion, we conducted systematic experiments to reveal 120e131.
whether MMR in rats ‘maps on’ to human MMN with respect to the Berti, S., 2013. The role of auditory transient and deviance processing in distraction
response to changing stimulus parameters. It was found that rat of task performance: a combined behavioral and event-related brain potential
study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7, 352.
responses at two latencies (P32 and N54) showed the hypothesised Bodatsch, M., Brockhaus-Dumke, A., Klosterkotter, J., Ruhrmann, S., 2015. Fore-
modulation in response to shifts in the ‘unexpectedness’ of the casting psychosis by event-related potentials-systematic review and specific
deviant stimulus, how different it was from the standard, and how meta-analysis. Biol. Psychiatr. 77, 951e958.
Bowie, C.R., Best, M.W., Depp, C., Mausbach, B.T., Patterson, T.L., Pulver, A.E.,
often it was presented. Further, the jitter effect was found on N54 Harvey, P.D., 2018. Cognitive and functional deficits in bipolar disorder and
component only. Moreover, previous studies from the laboratory schizophrenia as a function of the presence and history of psychosis. Bipolar
have shown that between P32 and N54, N54 is the only one that Disord. 20, 604e613.
Brattico, E., Tervaniemi, M., Na €a€ta
€nen, R., Peretz, I., 2006. Musical scale properties
shows strong adaptation-independent deviance detection (Harms
are automatically processed in the human auditory cortex. Brain Res. 1117,
et al., 2014) and is reduced by an NMDAR antagonist (another 162e174.
attribute of human MMN) (Harms et al., 2018). These previous Carandini, M., Heeger, D.J., 2011. Normalization as a canonical neural computation.
findings from our group and the present results suggest that the Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 51e62.
Carbajal, G.V., Malmierca, M.S., 2018. The neuronal basis of predictive coding along
rodent MMR, particularly the N54 component measured in our the auditory pathway: from the subcortical roots to cortical deviance detection.
preparation, is highly ‘MMN-like’, and can be used in future animal Trends Hear 22, 2331216518784822.
16 J. Jalewa et al. / Hearing Research 399 (2021) 107992

Catts, S.V., Shelley, A.M., Ward, P.B., Liebert, B., Mcconaghy, N., Andrews, S., Johnston, P., Solowij, N., Kulkarni, J., Croft, R.J., 2018. The effects of glycine on
Michie, P.T., 1995. Brain potential evidence for an auditory sensory memory auditory mismatch negativity in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 191, 61e69.
deficit in schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatr. 152, 213e219. Grimm, S., Escera, C., 2012. Auditory deviance detection revisited: evidence for a
Chen, I.W., Helmchen, F., Lutcke, H., 2015. Specific early and late oddball-evoked hierarchical novelty system. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 85, 88e92.
responses in excitatory and inhibitory neurons of mouse auditory cortex. Grimm, S., Escera, C., Slabu, L., Costa-Faidella, J., 2011. Electrophysiological evidence
J. Neurosci. 35, 12560e12573. for the hierarchical organization of auditory change detection in the human
Chudasama, Y., Robbins, T.W., 2004. Psychopharmacological approaches to modu- brain. Psychophysiology 48, 377e384.
lating attention in the five-choice serial reaction time task: implications for Grimm, S., Schro € ger, E., 2007. The processing of frequency deviations within
schizophrenia. Psychopharmacology 174, 86e98. sounds: evidence for the predictive nature of the Mismatch Negativity (MMN)
Cowan, N., 1984. On short and long auditory stores. Psychol. Bull. 96, 341e370. system. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 25, 241e249.
Cowan, N., Winkler, I., Teder, W., N€ aa€t€
anen, R., 1993. Memory prerequisites of Gunduz-Bruce, H., Reinhart, R.M., Roach, B.J., Gueorguieva, R., Oliver, S.,
mismatch negativity in the auditory event-related potential (ERP). J. Exp. Psy- D’souza, D.C., Ford, J.M., Krystal, J.H., Mathalon, D.H., 2012. Glutamatergic
chol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 19, 909e921. modulation of auditory information processing in the human brain. Biol. Psy-
Daikhin, L., Ahissar, M., 2012. Responses to deviants are modulated by subthreshold chiatr. 71, 969e977.
variability of the standard. Psychophysiology 49, 31e42. Hamm, J.P., Yuste, R., 2016. Somatostatin interneurons control a key component of
Damaso, K.A., Michie, P.T., Todd, J., 2015. Paying attention to MMN in schizophrenia. mismatch negativity in mouse visual cortex. Cell Rep. 16, 597e604.
Brain Res. 1626, 267e279. Harms, L., Fulham, W.R., Todd, J., Budd, T.W., Hunter, M., Meehan, C., Penttonen, M.,
De La Salle, S., Shah, D., Choueiry, J., Bowers, H., Mcintosh, J., Ilivitsky, V., Knott, V., Schall, U., Zavitsanou, K., Hodgson, D.M., Michie, P.T., 2014. Mismatch negativity
2019. NMDA receptor antagonist effects on speech-related mismatch negativity (MMN) in freely-moving rats with several experimental controls. PloS One 9,
and its underlying oscillatory and source activity in healthy humans. Front. e110892.
Pharmacol. 10, 455. Harms, L., Fulham, W.R., Todd, J., Meehan, C., Schall, U., Hodgson, D.M., Michie, P.T.,
Doeller, C.F., Opitz, B., Mecklinger, A., Krick, C., Reith, W., Schro €ger, E., 2003. Pre- 2018. Late deviance detection in rats is reduced, while early deviance detection
frontal cortex involvement in preattentive auditory deviance detection: neu- is augmented by the NMDA receptor antagonist MK-801. Schizophr. Res. 191,
roimaging and electrophysiological evidence. Neuroimage 20, 1270e1282. 43e50.
Duque, D., Ayala, Y.A., Malmierca, M.S., 2015. Deviance detection in auditory Harms, L., Michie, P.T., N€ €t€
aa anen, R., 2016. Criteria for determining whether
subcortical structures: what can we learn from neurochemistry and neural mismatch responses exist in animal models: focus on rodents. Biol. Psychol. 116,
connectivity? Cell Tissue Res. 361, 215e232. 28e35.
Duque, D., Pais, R., Malmierca, M.S., 2018. Stimulus-specific adaptation in the Horv ath, J., Czigler, I., Jacobsen, T., Maess, B., Schro€ger, E., Winkler, I., 2008. MMN or
anesthetized mouse revealed by brainstem auditory evoked potentials. Hear. no MMN: no magnitude of deviance effect on the MMN amplitude. Psycho-
Res. 370, 294e301. physiology 45, 60e69.
Durschmid, S., Edwards, E., Reichert, C., Dewar, C., Hinrichs, H., Heinze, H.J., Horv ath, J., Czigler, I., Winkler, I., Teder-Salejarvi, W.A., 2007. The temporal window
Kirsch, H.E., Dalal, S.S., Deouell, L.Y., Knight, R.T., 2016. Hierarchy of prediction of integration in elderly and young adults. Neurobiol. Aging 28, 964e975.
errors for auditory events in human temporal and frontal cortex. Proc. Natl. Horv ath, J., Winkler, I., 2004. How the human auditory system treats repetition
Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 113, 6755e6760. amongst change. Neurosci. Lett. 368, 157e161.
Ehlers, C.L., Kaneko, W.M., Robledo, P., Lopez, A.L., 1994. Long-latency event-related Hsu, W.Y., Cheng, C.H., Lin, H.C., Liao, K.K., Wu, Z.A., Ho, L.T., Lin, Y.Y., 2010. Memory-
potentials in rats: effects of task and stimulus parameters. Neuroscience 62, based mismatch response to changes in duration of auditory stimuli: an MEG
759e769. study. Clin. Neurophysiol. 121, 1744e1750.
Ehrlichman, R.S., Maxwell, C.R., Majumdar, S., Siegel, S.J., 2008. Deviance-elicited Hughes, H.C., Darcey, T.M., Barkan, H.I., Williamson, P.D., Roberts, D.W., Aslin, C.H.,
changes in event-related potentials are attenuated by ketamine in mice. 2001. Responses of human auditory association cortex to the omission of an
J. Cognit. Neurosci. 20, 1403e1414. expected acoustic event. Neuroimage 13, 1073e1089.
Erickson, M.A., Ruffle, A., Gold, J.M., 2016. A meta-analysis of mismatch negativity in J€
a€askel€
ainen, I.P., Ahveninen, J., Bonmassar, G., Dale, A.M., Ilmoniemi, R.J.,
schizophrenia: from clinical risk to disease specificity and progression. Biol. Levanen, S., Lin, F.H., May, P., Melcher, J., Stufflebeam, S., Tiitinen, H.,
Psychiatr. 79, 980e987. Belliveau, J.W., 2004. Human posterior auditory cortex gates novel sounds to
Eriksson, J., Villa, A.E., 2005. Event-related potentials in an auditory oddball situa- consciousness. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 101, 6809e6814.
tion in the rat. Biosystems 79, 207e212. J€
a€askel€
ainen, I.P., Hautama €ki, M., N€ €t€
aa anen, R., Ilmoniemi, R.J., 1999. Temporal span
Escera, C., Corral, M.J., Yago, E., 2002. An electrophysiological and behavioral of human echoic memory and mismatch negativity: revisited. Neuroreport 10,
investigation of involuntary attention towards auditory frequency, duration and 1305e1308.
intensity changes. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 14, 325e332. Jacobsen, T., Schro €ger, E., 2001. Is there pre-attentive memory-based comparison of
Escera, C., Leung, S., Grimm, S., 2014. Deviance detection based on regularity pitch? Psychophysiology 38, 723e727.
encoding along the auditory hierarchy: electrophysiological evidence in Jacobsen, T., Schro €ger, E., 2003. Measuring duration mismatch negativity. Clin.
humans. Brain Topogr. 27, 527e538. Neurophysiol. 114, 1133e1143.
Featherstone, R.E., Melnychenko, O., Siegel, S.J., 2018. Mismatch negativity in pre- Javitt, D.C., 2000. Intracortical mechanisms of mismatch negativity dysfunction in
clinical models of schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 191, 35e42. schizophrenia. Audiol. Neuro. Otol. 5, 207e215.
Fishman, Y.I., 2014. The mechanisms and meaning of the mismatch negativity. Brain Javitt, D.C., Doneshka, P., Grochowski, S., Ritter, W., 1995. Impaired mismatch
Topogr. 27, 500e526. negativity generation reflects widespread dysfunction of working memory in
Fishman, Y.I., Steinschneider, M., 2012. Searching for the mismatch negativity in schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatr. 52, 550e558.
primary auditory cortex of the awake monkey: deviance detection or stimulus Javitt, D.C., Doneshka, P., Zylberman, I., Ritter, W., Vaughan JR., H.G., 1993. Impair-
specific adaptation? J. Neurosci. 32, 15747e15758. ment of early cortical processing in schizophrenia: an event-related potential
Fitzgerald, K., Auksztulewicz, R., Provost, A., Paton, B., Howard, Z., Todd, J., 2019. confirmation study. Biol. Psychiatr. 33, 513e519.
Hierarchical Learning of Statistical Regularities over Multiple Timescales of Javitt, D.C., Grochowski, S., Shelley, A.M., Ritter, W., 1998. Impaired mismatch
Sound Sequence Processing: A Dynamic Causal Modelling Study. bioRxiv. negativity (MMN) generation in schizophrenia as a function of stimulus devi-
Fitzgerald, K., Todd, J., 2018. Hierarchical timescales of statistical learning revealed ance, probability, and interstimulus/interdeviant interval. Electroencephalogr.
by mismatch negativity to auditory pattern deviations. Neuropsychologia 120, Clin. Neurophysiol. 108, 143e153.
25e34. Javitt, D.C., Spencer, K.M., Thaker, G.K., Winterer, G., Hajos, M., 2008. Neurophysi-
Forray, C., Buller, R., 2017. Challenges and opportunities for the development of new ological biomarkers for drug development in schizophrenia. Nat. Rev. Drug
antipsychotic drugs. Biochem. Pharmacol. 143, 10e24. Discov. 7, 68e83.
Friston, K., 2005. A theory of cortical responses. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Javitt, D.C., Steinschneider, M., Schroeder, C.E., Arezzo, J.C., 1996. Role of cortical N-
Sci. 360, 815e836. methyl-D-aspartate receptors in auditory sensory memory and mismatch
Frost, J.D., Winkler, I., Provost, A., Todd, J., 2016. Surprising sequential effects on negativity generation: implications for schizophrenia. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S.
MMN. Biol. Psychol. 116, 47e56. A. 93, 11962e11967.
Fujioka, T., Trainor, L.J., Ross, B., Kakigi, R., Pantev, C., 2004. Musical training en- Javitt, D.C., Steinschneider, M., Schroeder, C.E., Vaughan JR., H.G., Arezzo, J.C., 1994.
hances automatic encoding of melodic contour and interval structure. J. Cognit. Detection of stimulus deviance within primate primary auditory cortex: intra-
Neurosci. 16, 1010e1021. cortical mechanisms of mismatch negativity (MMN) generation. Brain Res. 667,
Fujioka, T., Trainor, L.J., Ross, B., Kakigi, R., Pantev, C., 2005. Automatic encoding of 192e200.
polyphonic melodies in musicians and nonmusicians. J. Cognit. Neurosci. 17, Javitt, D.C., Sweet, R.A., 2015. Auditory dysfunction in schizophrenia: integrating
1578e1592. clinical and basic features. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 16, 535e550.
Garrido, M.I., Kilner, J.M., Stephan, K.E., Friston, K.J., 2009. The mismatch negativity: Jones, S.J., 2002. The internal auditory clock: what can evoked potentials reveal
a review of underlying mechanisms. Clin. Neurophysiol. 120, 453e463. about the analysis of temporal sound patterns, and abnormal states of con-
Giard, M.H., Perrin, F., Pernier, J., Bouchet, P., 1990. Brain generators implicated in sciousness? Neurophysiol. Clin. 32, 241e253.
the processing of auditory stimulus deviance: a topographic event-related po- Joutsiniemi, S.L., Hari, R., 1989. Omissions of auditory stimuli may activate frontal
tential study. Psychophysiology 27, 627e640. cortex. Eur. J. Neurosci. 1, 524e528.
Greenwood, K.E., Landau, S., Wykes, T., 2005. Negative symptoms and specific Joutsiniemi, S.L., Ilvonen, T., Sinkkonen, J., Huotilainen, M., Tervaniemi, M.,
cognitive impairments as combined targets for improved functional outcome Lehtokoski, A., Rinne, T., Na €a
€ta
€nen, R., 1998. The mismatch negativity for
within cognitive remediation therapy. Schizophr. Bull. 31, 910e921. duration decrement of auditory stimuli in healthy subjects. Electroencephalogr.
Greenwood, L.M., Leung, S., Michie, P.T., Green, A., Nathan, P.J., Fitzgerald, P., Clin. Neurophysiol. 108, 154e159.
J. Jalewa et al. / Hearing Research 399 (2021) 107992 17

Jung, F., Stephan, K.E., Backes, H., Moran, R., Gramer, M., Kumagai, T., Graf, R., Mullens, D., Woodley, J., Whitson, L., Provost, A., Heathcote, A., Winkler, I., Todd, J.,
Endepols, H., Tittgemeyer, M., 2013. Mismatch responses in the awake rat: 2014. Altering the primacy bias–how does a prior task affect mismatch nega-
evidence from epidural recordings of auditory cortical fields. PloS One 8, tivity? Psychophysiology 51, 437e445.
e63203. Müller, D., Widmann, A., Schro €ger, E., 2005. Deviance-repetition effects as a func-
Kahn, R.S., Keefe, R.S., 2013. Schizophrenia is a cognitive illness: time for a change in tion of stimulus feature, feature value variation, and timing: a mismatch
focus. JAMA Psychiatry 70, 1107e1112. negativity study. Biol. Psychol. 68, 1e14.
Kahn, R.S., Sommer, I.E., Murray, R.M., Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Weinberger, D.R., Naatanen, R., 2001. The perception of speech sounds by the human brain as re-
Cannon, T.D., O’donovan, M., Correll, C.U., Kane, J.M., Van Os, J., Insel, T.R., 2015. flected by the mismatch negativity (MMN) and its magnetic equivalent
Schizophrenia. Nat Rev Dis Primers 1, 15067. (MMNm). Psychophysiology 38, 1e21.
Kasai, K., Nakagome, K., Itoh, K., Koshida, I., Hata, A., Iwanami, A., Fukuda, M., €€
Na at€anen, R., Alho, K., 1995a. Generators of electrical and magnetic mismatch re-
Hiramatsu, K.I., Kato, N., 1999. Multiple generators in the auditory automatic sponses in humans. Brain Topogr. 7, 315e320.
discrimination process in humans. Neuroreport 10, 2267e2271. €€
Na at€anen, R., Alho, K., 1995b. Mismatch negativity–a unique measure of sensory
Kim, J.S., Kwon, Y.J., Lee, H.Y., Lee, H.S., Kim, S., Shim, S.H., 2020. Mismatch nega- processing in audition. Int. J. Neurosci. 80, 317e337.
tivity indices as a prognostic factor for remission in schizophrenia. Clin Psy- €€
Na at€anen, R., Gaillard, A.W., Mantysalo, S., 1978. Early selective-attention effect on
chopharmacol Neurosci 18, 127e135. evoked potential reinterpreted. Acta Psychol. 42, 313e329.
Kompus, K., Westerhausen, R., Craven, A.R., Kreegipuu, K., Poldver, N., Passow, S., €€
Na at€anen, R., Jacobsen, T., Winkler, I., 2005. Memory-based or afferent processes in
Specht, K., Hugdahl, K., N€ €t€
aa anen, R., 2015. Resting-state glutamatergic neuro- mismatch negativity (MMN): a review of the evidence. Psychophysiology 42,
transmission is related to the peak latency of the auditory mismatch negativity 25e32.
(MMN) for duration deviants: an (1)H-MRS-EEG study. Psychophysiology 52, €€
Na at€anen, R., Kahkonen, S., 2009. Central auditory dysfunction in schizophrenia as
1131e1139. revealed by the mismatch negativity (MMN) and its magnetic equivalent
Kort, N.S., Ford, J.M., Roach, B.J., Gunduz-Bruce, H., Krystal, J.H., Jaeger, J., MMNm: a review. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 12, 125e135.
Reinhart, R.M., Mathalon, D.H., 2017. Role of N-Methyl-D-Aspartate receptors in €€
Na at€anen, R., Lehtokoski, A., Lennes, M., Cheour, M., Huotilainen, M., Iivonen, A.,
action-based predictive coding deficits in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatr. 81, Vainio, M., Alku, P., Ilmoniemi, R.J., Luuk, A., Allik, J., Sinkkonen, J., Alho, K., 1997.
514e524. Language-specific phoneme representations revealed by electric and magnetic
Koshiyama, D., Kirihara, K., Tada, M., Nagai, T., Fujioka, M., Usui, K., Araki, T., brain responses. Nature 385, 432e434.
Kasai, K., 2020. Reduced auditory mismatch negativity reflects impaired devi- €€
Na at€anen, R., Michie, P.T., 1979. Early selective-attention effects on the evoked
ance detection in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/ potential: a critical review and reinterpretation. Biol. Psychol. 8, 81e136.
sbaa006(2020). €€
Na at€anen, R., Paavilainen, P., Rinne, T., Alho, K., 2007. The mismatch negativity
Kujala, T., Na €a€ta
€nen, R., 2001. The mismatch negativity in evaluating central audi- (MMN) in basic research of central auditory processing: a review. Clin. Neu-
tory dysfunction in dyslexia. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 25, 535e543. rophysiol. 118, 2544e2590.
Kurkela, J.L., Lipponen, A., Hamalainen, J.A., Naatanen, R., Astikainen, P., 2016. Pas- €€
Na at€anen, R., Simpson, M., Loveless, N.E., 1982. Stimulus deviance and evoked po-
sive exposure to speech sounds induces long-term memory representations in tentials. Biol. Psychol. 14, 53e98.
the auditory cortex of adult rats. Sci. Rep. 6, 38904. €€
Na at€anen, R., Todd, J., Schall, U., 2016. Mismatch negativity (MMN) as biomarker
Kurkela, J.L.O., Lipponen, A., Kylaheiko, I., Astikainen, P., 2018. Electrophysiological predicting psychosis in clinically at-risk individuals. Biol. Psychol. 116, 36e40.
evidence of memory-based detection of auditory regularity violations in Naatanen, R., Winkler, I., 1999. The concept of auditory stimulus representation in
anesthetized mice. Sci. Rep. 8, 3027. cognitive neuroscience. Psychol. Bull. 125, 826e859.
Lavoie, S., Jack, B.N., Griffiths, O., Ando, A., Amminger, P., Couroupis, A., Jago, A., Nagai, T., Kirihara, K., Tada, M., Koshiyama, D., Koike, S., Suga, M., Araki, T.,
Markulev, C., Mcgorry, P.D., Nelson, B., Polari, A., Yuen, H.P., Whitford, T.J., 2018. Hashimoto, K., Kasai, K., 2017. Reduced mismatch negativity is associated with
Impaired mismatch negativity to frequency deviants in individuals at ultra-high increased plasma level of glutamate in first-episode psychosis. Sci. Rep. 7, 2258.
risk for psychosis, and preliminary evidence for further impairment with Nakamura, T., Michie, P.T., Fulham, W.R., Todd, J., Budd, T.W., Schall, U., Hunter, M.,
transition to psychosis. Schizophr. Res. 191, 95e100. Hodgson, D.M., 2011. Epidural auditory event-related potentials in the rat to
Lavoie, S., Polari, A.R., Goldstone, S., Nelson, B., Mcgorry, P.D., 2019. Staging model in frequency and duration deviants: evidence of mismatch negativity? Front.
psychiatry: review of the evolution of electroencephalography abnormalities in Psychol. 2, 367.
major psychiatric disorders. Early Interv Psychiatry 13, 1319e1328. Nikjeh, D.A., Lister, J.J., Frisch, S.A., 2009. Preattentive cortical-evoked responses to
Li, Q., Liu, G., Yuan, G., Wang, G., Wu, Z., Zhao, X., 2019. Single-trial EEG-fMRI reveals pure tones, harmonic tones, and speech: influence of music training. Ear Hear.
the generation process of the mismatch negativity. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 13, 30, 432e446.
168. Nordby, H., Hammerborg, D., Roth, W.T., Hugdahl, K., 1994. ERPs for infrequent
Lieder, F., Stephan, K.E., Daunizeau, J., Garrido, M.I., Friston, K.J., 2013. omissions and inclusions of stimulus elements. Psychophysiology 31, 544e552.
A neurocomputational model of the mismatch negativity. PLoS Comput. Biol. 9, Oceak, A., Winkler, I., Sussman, E., Alho, K., 2006. Loudness summation and the
e1003288. mismatch negativity event-related brain potential in humans. Psychophysiol-
Light, G.A., Braff, D.L., 2005. Mismatch negativity deficits are associated with poor ogy 43, 13e20.
functioning in schizophrenia patients. Arch. Gen. Psychiatr. 62, 127e136. Orellana, G., Slachevsky, A., Pena, M., 2012. Executive attention impairment in first-
Light, G.A., Na €a€ta
€nen, R., 2013. Mismatch negativity is a breakthrough biomarker for episode schizophrenia. BMC Psychiatr. 12, 154.
understanding and treating psychotic disorders. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. Paavilainen, P., 2013. The mismatch-negativity (MMN) component of the auditory
110, 15175e15176. event-related potential to violations of abstract regularities: a review. Int. J.
Lopez-Caballero, F., Zarnowiec, K., Escera, C., 2016. Differential deviant probability Psychophysiol. 88, 109e123.
effects on two hierarchical levels of the auditory novelty system. Biol. Psychol. Paavilainen, P., Jiang, D., Lavikainen, J., Na €a
€ta
€nen, R., 1993a. Stimulus duration and
120, 1e9. the sensory memory trace: an event-related potential study. Biol. Psychol. 35,
Lyytinen, H., Blomberg, A.P., N€ a€
at€anen, R., 1992. Event-related potentials and 139e152.
autonomic responses to a change in unattended auditory stimuli. Psycho- Paavilainen, P., Tiitinen, H., Alho, K., Na €a
€ta
€nen, R., 1993b. Mismatch negativity to
physiology 29, 523e534. slight pitch changes outside strong attentional focus. Biol. Psychol. 37, 23e41.
Maess, B., Jacobsen, T., Schro € ger, E., Friederici, A.D., 2007. Localizing pre-attentive Pakarinen, S., Takegata, R., Rinne, T., Huotilainen, M., Na €€ €nen, R., 2007. Mea-
ata
auditory memory-based comparison: magnetic mismatch negativity to pitch surement of extensive auditory discrimination profiles using the mismatch
change. Neuroimage 37, 561e571. negativity (MMN) of the auditory event-related potential (ERP). Clin. Neuro-
Magno, E., Yeap, S., Thakore, J.H., Garavan, H., De Sanctis, P., Foxe, J.J., 2008. Are physiol. 118, 177e185.
auditory-evoked frequency and duration mismatch negativity deficits endo- Parras, G.G., Nieto-Diego, J., Carbajal, G.V., Valdes-Baizabal, C., Escera, C.,
phenotypic for schizophrenia? High-density electrical mapping in clinically Malmierca, M.S., 2017. Neurons along the auditory pathway exhibit a hierar-
unaffected first-degree relatives and first-episode and chronic schizophrenia. chical organization of prediction error. Nat. Commun. 8, 2148.
Biol. Psychiatr. 64, 385e391. rez-Gonza
Pe lez, D., Malmierca, M.S., 2014. Adaptation in the auditory system: an
Maiste, A.C., Wiens, A.S., Hunt, M.J., Scherg, M., Picton, T.W., 1995. Event-related overview. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 8, 19.
potentials and the categorical perception of speech sounds. Ear Hear. 16, 68e90. Perez, V.B., Woods, S.W., Roach, B.J., Ford, J.M., Mcglashan, T.H., Srihari, V.H.,
Malmierca, M.S., Anderson, L.A., Antunes, F.M., 2015. The cortical modulation of Mathalon, D.H., 2014. Automatic auditory processing deficits in schizophrenia
stimulus-specific adaptation in the auditory midbrain and thalamus: a potential and clinical high-risk patients: forecasting psychosis risk with mismatch
neuronal correlate for predictive coding. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 9, 19. negativity. Biol. Psychiatr. 75, 459e469.
Malmierca, M.S., Cristaudo, S., Pe rez-Gonza lez, D., Covey, E., 2009. Stimulus-specific Polterovich, A., Jankowski, M.M., Nelken, I., 2018. Deviance sensitivity in the audi-
adaptation in the inferior colliculus of the anesthetized rat. J. Neurosci. 29, tory cortex of freely moving rats. PloS One 13, e0197678.
5483e5493. Quiroga-Martinez, D.R., Hansen, N.C., Hojlund, A., Pearce, M.T., Brattico, E., Vuust, P.,
May, P.J., Tiitinen, H., 2010. Mismatch negativity (MMN), the deviance-elicited 2019. Reduced prediction error responses in high-as compared to low-
auditory deflection, explained. Psychophysiology 47, 66e122. uncertainty musical contexts. Cortex 120, 181e200.
Michie, P.T., Budd, T.W., Todd, J., Rock, D., Wichmann, H., Box, J., Jablensky, A.V., Raij, T., Mcevoy, L., Makela, J.P., Hari, R., 1997. Human auditory cortex is activated by
2000. Duration and frequency mismatch negativity in schizophrenia. Clin. omissions of auditory stimuli. Brain Res. 745, 134e143.
Neurophysiol. 111, 1054e1065. Rasser, P.E., Schall, U., Todd, J., Michie, P.T., Ward, P.B., Johnston, P., Helmbold, K.,
Mullens, D., Winkler, I., Damaso, K., Heathcote, A., Whitson, L., Provost, A., Todd, J., Case, V., Soyland, A., Tooney, P.A., Thompson, P.M., 2011. Gray matter deficits,
2016. Biased relevance filtering in the auditory system: a test of confidence- mismatch negativity, and outcomes in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 37,
weighted first-impressions. Biol. Psychol. 115, 101e111. 131e140.
18 J. Jalewa et al. / Hearing Research 399 (2021) 107992

Roger, C., Hasbroucq, T., Rabat, A., Vidal, F., Burle, B., 2009. Neurophysics of temporal Tikhonravov, D., Neuvonen, T., Pertovaara, A., Savioja, K., Ruusuvirta, T.,
discrimination in the rat: a mismatch negativity study. Psychophysiology 46, N€ €t€
aa anen, R., Carlson, S., 2010. Dose-related effects of memantine on a
1028e1032. mismatch negativity-like response in anesthetized rats. Neuroscience 167,
Ruhnau, P., Herrmann, B., Schro €ger, E., 2012. Finding the right control: the mismatch 1175e1182.
negativity under investigation. Clin. Neurophysiol. 123, 507e512. Todd, J., Heathcote, A., Mullens, D., Whitson, L.R., Provost, A., Winkler, I., 2014a.
Ruusuvirta, T., Lipponen, A., Pellinen, E.K., Penttonen, M., Astikainen, P., 2015. What controls gain in gain control? Mismatch negativity (MMN), priors and
Auditory cortical and hippocampal local-field potentials to frequency deviant system biases. Brain Topogr. 27, 578e589.
tones in urethane-anesthetized rats: an unexpected role of the sound fre- Todd, J., Heathcote, A., Whitson, L.R., Mullens, D., Provost, A., Winkler, I., 2014b.
quencies themselves. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 96, 134e140. Mismatch negativity (MMN) to pitch change is susceptible to order-dependent
Ruusuvirta, T., Penttonen, M., Korhonen, T., 1998. Auditory cortical event-related bias. Front. Neurosci. 8, 180.
potentials to pitch deviances in rats. Neurosci. Lett. 248, 45e48. Todd, J., Michie, P.T., Schall, U., Karayanidis, F., 2006. Mismatch negativity in
Sabri, M., Campbell, K.B., 2001. Effects of sequential and temporal probability of schizophrenia: effect of probability, deviant type and duration of illness. Acta
deviant occurrence on mismatch negativity. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 12, Neuropsychiatr. 18, 309e310.
171e180. Todd, J., Michie, P.T., Schall, U., Karayanidis, F., Yabe, H., Na€a
€ta
€nen, R., 2008. Deviant
Salgado-Pineda, P., Baeza, I., Perez-Gomez, M., Vendrell, P., Junque, C., Bargallo, N., matters: duration, frequency, and intensity deviants reveal different patterns of
Bernardo, M., 2003. Sustained attention impairment correlates to gray matter mismatch negativity reduction in early and late schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatr.
decreases in first episode neuroleptic-naive schizophrenic patients. Neuro- 63, 58e64.
image 19, 365e375. Todd, J., Michie, P.T., Schall, U., Ward, P.B., Catts, S.V., 2012. Mismatch negativity
Salisbury, D.F., Shenton, M.E., Griggs, C.B., Bonner-Jackson, A., Mccarley, R.W., 2002. (MMN) reduction in schizophrenia-impaired prediction–error generation,
Mismatch negativity in chronic schizophrenia and first-episode schizophrenia. estimation or salience? Int. J. Psychophysiol. 83, 222e231.
Arch. Gen. Psychiatr. 59, 686e694. Todd, J., Petherbridge, A., Speirs, B., Provost, A., Paton, B., 2018. Time as context: the
Schro € ger, E., Winkler, I., 1995. Presentation rate and magnitude of stimulus deviance influence of hierarchical patterning on sensory inference. Schizophr. Res. 191,
effects on human pre-attentive change detection. Neurosci. Lett. 193, 185e188. 123e131.
Schulz, S.C., Murray, A., 2016. Assessing cognitive impairment in patients with Todd, J., Provost, A., Cooper, G., 2011. Lasting first impressions: a conservative bias in
schizophrenia. J. Clin. Psychiatr. 77 (Suppl. 2), 3e7. automatic filters of the acoustic environment. Neuropsychologia 49,
Shelley, A.M., Silipo, G., Javitt, D.C., 1999. Diminished responsiveness of ERPs in 3399e3405.
schizophrenic subjects to changes in auditory stimulation parameters: impli- Todd, J., Provost, A., Whitson, L.R., Cooper, G., Heathcote, A., 2013. Not so primitive:
cations for theories of cortical dysfunction. Schizophr. Res. 37, 65e79. context-sensitive meta-learning about unattended sound sequences.
Shelley, A.M., Ward, P.B., Catts, S.V., Michie, P.T., Andrews, S., Mcconaghy, N., 1991. J. Neurophysiol. 109, 99e105.
Mismatch negativity: an index of a preattentive processing deficit in schizo- Tse, C.Y., Penney, T.B., 2008. On the functional role of temporal and frontal cortex
phrenia. Biol. Psychiatr. 30, 1059e1062. activation in passive detection of auditory deviance. Neuroimage 41,
Shen, C., Popescu, F.C., Hahn, E., Ta, T.T., Dettling, M., Neuhaus, A.H., 2014. Neuro- 1462e1470.
cognitive pattern analysis reveals classificatory hierarchy of attention deficits in Ulanovsky, N., Las, L., Farkas, D., Nelken, I., 2004. Multiple time scales of adaptation
schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 40, 878e885. in auditory cortex neurons. J. Neurosci. 24, 10440e10453.
Shiramatsu, T.I., Kanzaki, R., Takahashi, H., 2013. Cortical mapping of mismatch Ulanovsky, N., Las, L., Nelken, I., 2003. Processing of low-probability sounds by
negativity with deviance detection property in rat. PloS One 8, e82663. cortical neurons. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 391e398.
Sivarao, D.V., Chen, P., Yang, Y., Li, Y.W., Pieschl, R., Ahlijanian, M.K., 2014. NR2B Umbricht, D., Koller, R., Schmid, L., Skrabo, A., Grubel, C., Huber, T., Stassen, H., 2003.
antagonist CP-101,606 abolishes pitch-mediated deviance detection in awake How specific are deficits in mismatch negativity generation to schizophrenia?
rats. Front. Psychiatr. 5, 96. Biol. Psychiatr. 53, 1120e1131.
Sonnadara, R.R., Alain, C., Trainor, L.J., 2006. Effects of spatial separation and Umbricht, D., Krljes, S., 2005. Mismatch negativity in schizophrenia: a meta-anal-
stimulus probability on the event-related potentials elicited by occasional ysis. Schizophr. Res. 76, 1e23.
changes in sound location. Brain Res. 1071, 175e185. Umbricht, D., Vyssotki, D., Latanov, A., Nitsch, R., Lipp, H.P., 2005. Deviance-related
Suga, M., Nishimura, Y., Kawakubo, Y., Yumoto, M., Kasai, K., 2016. Magneto- electrophysiological activity in mice: is there mismatch negativity in mice? Clin.
encephalographic recording of auditory mismatch negativity in response to Neurophysiol. 116, 353e363.
duration and frequency deviants in a single session in patients with schizo- Von Der Behrens, W., Bauerle, P., Kossl, M., Gaese, B.H., 2009. Correlating stimulus-
phrenia. Psychiatr. Clin. Neurosci. 70, 295e302. specific adaptation of cortical neurons and local field potentials in the awake
Sussman, E., Ritter, W., Vaughan JR., H.G., 1999. An investigation of the auditory rat. J. Neurosci. 29, 13837e13849.
streaming effect using event-related brain potentials. Psychophysiology 36, Vuust, P., Brattico, E., Seppanen, M., N€ €t€
aa anen, R., Tervaniemi, M., 2012. The sound
22e34. of music: differentiating musicians using a fast, musical multi-feature
Swerdlow, N.R., Bhakta, S., Chou, H.H., Talledo, J.A., Balvaneda, B., Light, G.A., 2016. mismatch negativity paradigm. Neuropsychologia 50, 1432e1443.
Memantine effects on sensorimotor gating and mismatch negativity in patients Wacongne, C., Changeux, J.P., Dehaene, S., 2012. A neuronal model of predictive
with chronic psychosis. Neuropsychopharmacology 41, 419e430. coding accounting for the mismatch negativity. J. Neurosci. 32, 3665e3678.
Szymanski, F.D., Garcia-Lazaro, J.A., Schnupp, J.W., 2009. Current source density Wacongne, C., Labyt, E., Van Wassenhove, V., Bekinschtein, T., Naccache, L.,
profiles of stimulus-specific adaptation in rat auditory cortex. J. Neurophysiol. Dehaene, S., 2011. Evidence for a hierarchy of predictions and prediction errors
102, 1483e1490. in human cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 108, 20754e20759.
Taaseh, N., Yaron, A., Nelken, I., 2011. Stimulus-specific adaptation and deviance Wang, X.D., Gu, F., He, K., Chen, L.H., Chen, L., 2012. Preattentive extraction of ab-
detection in the rat auditory cortex. PloS One 6, e23369. stract auditory rules in speech sound stream: a mismatch negativity study
Tada, M., Kirihara, K., Mizutani, S., Uka, T., Kunii, N., Koshiyama, D., Fujioka, M., using lexical tones. PloS One 7, e30027.
Usui, K., Nagai, T., Araki, T., Kasai, K., 2019 Nov. Mismatch negativity (MMN) as a Weise, A., Grimm, S., Trujillo-Barreto, N.J., Schro €ger, E., 2014. Timing matters: the
tool for translational investigations into early psychosis: a review. Int J Psy- processing of pitch relations. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8, 387.
chophysiol. 145, 5e14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2019.02.009. Wiens, S., Szychowska, M., Eklund, R., Van Berlekom, E., 2019. Cascade and no-
Takegata, R., Morotomi, T., 1999. Integrated neural representation of sound and repetition rules are comparable controls for the auditory frequency mismatch
temporal features in human auditory sensory memory: an event-related po- negativity in oddball tasks. Psychophysiology 56, e13280.
tential study. Neurosci. Lett. 274, 207e210. Winkler, I., Cowan, N., Csepe, V., Czigler, I., Na€a
€ta€nen, R., 1996. Interactions between
Takegata, R., Paavilainen, P., Na €a
€ta
€nen, R., Winkler, I., 2001a. Preattentive processing transient and long-term auditory memory as reflected by the mismatch nega-
of spectral, temporal, and structural characteristics of acoustic regularities: a tivity. J. Cognit. Neurosci. 8, 403e415.
mismatch negativity study. Psychophysiology 38, 92e98. Winkler, I., Czigler, I., 2012. Evidence from auditory and visual event-related po-
Takegata, R., Syssoeva, O., Winkler, I., Paavilainen, P., N€ aa€ta
€nen, R., 2001b. Common tential (ERP) studies of deviance detection (MMN and vMMN) linking predictive
neural mechanism for processing onset-to-onset intervals and silent gaps in coding theories and perceptual object representations. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 83,
sound sequences. Neuroreport 12, 1783e1787. 132e143.
Tavano, A., Widmann, A., Bendixen, A., Trujillo-Barreto, N., Schro €ger, E., 2014. Winkler, I., Paavilainen, P., Alho, K., Reinikainen, K., Sams, M., Na €a
€ta
€nen, R., 1990.
Temporal regularity facilitates higher-order sensory predictions in fast auditory The effect of small variation of the frequent auditory stimulus on the event-
sequences. Eur. J. Neurosci. 39, 308e318. related brain potential to the infrequent stimulus. Psychophysiology 27,
Tervaniemi, M., Janhunen, L., Kruck, S., Putkinen, V., Huotilainen, M., 2015. Auditory 228e235.
profiles of classical, jazz, and rock musicians: genre-specific sensitivity to Winkler, I., Schro€ger, E., Cowan, N., 2001. The role of large-scale memory organi-
musical sound features. Front. Psychol. 6, 1900. zation in the mismatch negativity event-related brain potential. J. Cognit.
Tervaniemi, M., Schro € ger, E., Na€€ €nen, R., 1997. Pre-attentive processing of spec-
ata Neurosci. 13, 59e71.
trally complex sounds with asynchronous onsets: an event-related potential Yabe, H., Tervaniemi, M., Reinikainen, K., Na €a€ta
€nen, R., 1997. Temporal window of
study with human subjects. Neurosci. Lett. 227, 197e200. integration revealed by MMN to sound omission. Neuroreport 8, 1971e1974.
Tiitinen, H., May, P., Reinikainen, K., Na €€ €nen, R., 1994. Attentive novelty detection
ata Yabe, H., Tervaniemi, M., Sinkkonen, J., Huotilainen, M., Ilmoniemi, R.J., Na €€
at€
anen, R.,
in humans is governed by pre-attentive sensory memory. Nature 372, 90e92. 1998. Temporal window of integration of auditory information in the human
Tikhonravov, D., Neuvonen, T., Pertovaara, A., Savioja, K., Ruusuvirta, T., brain. Psychophysiology 35, 615e619.
Na€€ €nen, R., Carlson, S., 2008. Effects of an NMDA-receptor antagonist MK-801
ata Yamaguchi, S., Globus, H., Knight, R.T., 1993. P3-like potential in rats. Electro-
on an MMN-like response recorded in anesthetized rats. Brain Res. 1203, encephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 88, 151e154.
97e102. Yang, T., Hamalainen, J.A., Lohvansuu, K., Lipponen, A., Penttonen, M., Astikainen, P.,
J. Jalewa et al. / Hearing Research 399 (2021) 107992 19

2019. Deviance detection in sound frequency in simple and complex sounds in schizophrenia: a mismatch negativity potentials study. Acta Neuropsychiatr. 21,
urethane-anesthetized rats. Hear. Res. 107814. 26e33.
Young, J.W., Geyer, M.A., 2015. Developing treatments for cognitive deficits in Zaragoza Domingo, S., Bobes, J., Garcia-Portilla, M.P., Morralla, C., Group, E.-S.S.,
schizophrenia: the challenge of translation. J. Psychopharmacol. 29, 178e196. 2015. Cognitive Performance associated to functional outcomes in stable out-
Young, J.W., Light, G.A., 2018. Cross-species neurophysiological biomarkers of patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res Cogn 2, 146e158.
attentional dysfunction in schizophrenia: bridging the translational gap. Neu- Zhou, Z., Zhu, H., Chen, L., 2013. Effect of aripiprazole on mismatch negativity
ropsychopharmacology 43, 230e231. (MMN) in schizophrenia. PloS One 8, e52186.
Yuan, G.Z., Zhou, Z.H., Yao, J.J., 2009. Effect of quetiapine on cognitive function in

You might also like