Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Appetite (2001) 36, 137±145

1
doi:10.1006/appe.2001.0390, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on

Original Article

Mood and carbohydrate cravings


L.Christensen and L. Pettijohn
University of South Alabama

(Received 26 January 2000, finalrevision 25 October 2000, accepted in revised form17 November 2000,
published electronically 9 February 2001)

The relationship between mood and carbohydrate cravings, and the possible role of gender in these associations, was
investigated in a sample of 113 males and 138 female college students. Participants completed a Cravings
Questionnaire and several mood inventories (profile of mood states, Beck Depression Inventory, and the Vitality
Inventory) in groups of 25. Individuals classifying themselves as ``carbohydrate cravers'' reported foods rich in
carbohydrates, and ``protein cravers'' reported protein-rich foods as being the ones they most strongly craved.
Carbohydrate cravers reported feeling distressed prior to their cravings and satisfied, happy/good and relaxed
following carbohydrate consumption. Protein cravers reported feeling anxious or hungry prior to their cravings and
happy, normal, bored, and energetic following protein-rich food consumption. A non-significant correlation existed
between ``protein'' cravers' ratings of craving intensity and mood, but a significant positive correlation existed
between ``carbohydrate'' cravers' ratings of craving intensity and almost all mood scales assessed for both male and
female ``carbohydrate'' cravers. The correlation between craving intensity and mood existed predominately with
individuals who craved sweet carbohydrate-rich foods.
# 2001 Academic Press

Introduction the most attention. This may be because carbohydrate


cravings, originally defined as ``a ravenous appetite for a
The hypothetical construct of cravings, an intense variety of sweet substances including chocolates, cake,
desire or longing for a particular substance pastry, and ice cream'' (Paykel, Mueller & de la Vergne,
(Weingarten & Elston, 1990), has a rather lengthy 1793, p.503), have frequently been reported in women
history as a key explanatory concept in models of both experiencing premenstrual syndrome (e.g. Bancroft,
addiction and alcoholism (Tiffany, 1990). More Cook & Williamson, 1988; Bowen & Grunberg, 1990;
recently, the term has been used to refer to the urge Cohen, Sherwin & Fleming, 1987; Smith & Sauder,
to seek out and consume particular foods. Up to 97% 1969), individuals with seasonal affective disorder
of women and 68% of men report food cravings (e.g. (e.g. Lingjaerde & Reichborn-Kjennerud, 1993;
Weingarten & Elston, 1991). The significance of inves- Rosenthal et al., 1984), and overweight individuals
tigating food cravings is seen in studies (e.g. Bjorvell, (Wurtman & Wurtman, 1986; Wurtman, 1988). While it
Ronnberg & Rossner, 1985) reporting that overweight has been demonstrated (e.g. Drewnowski et al., 1992)
women indicate that carbohydrate cravings are a that foods which have been labeled carbohydrate-rich
precipitant of unwanted eating and an impediment to and associated with carbohydrate cravings are generally
weight loss. sweet carbohydrate/fat-rich foods, it is the sweet com-
Of the different types of food that have been investi- ponent that is salient and the component that seems to
gated, carbohydrate cravings seem to have attracted lead individuals to label the food as a carbohydrate
(e.g. Drewnowski et al., 1992).
Weingarten and Elston (1990) have pointed out that
explanations for cravings have revolved around an
Address correspondence to: Larry Christensen, PhD,
Department of Psychology, University of South Alabama, abstinence model and an expectancy model. The absti-
Mobile, AL36688, U.S.A. nence model suggests that cravings arise from the

0195±6663/01/020137+09 $35.00/0 # 2001 Academic Press


138 L. Christensen and L. Pettijohn

dysphoric state created from abstaining from a desired Method


or needed substance. The expectancy model sug-
gests that cravings are triggered by exposure to either Research participants
internal or external cues associated with the desired
substance. Most of the literature on carbohydrate The research participants consisted of 113 males
cravings and consumption has focused on the relation- (M age ˆ 2144; SD ˆ 395) and 138 females (M age ˆ
ship with dysphoric mood. The most common assump- 2149; SD ˆ 526) of approximately equal educational
tion is that a dysphoric mood elicits carbohydrate background (males M ˆ 1359 years; SD ˆ 122 and
cravings because of a serotonin deficit. This deficit females M ˆ 1320 years; SD ˆ 111). The mean weight
promotes the consumption of carbohydrates because of the males was 17085 lbs (SD ˆ 3698) and the mean
pure carbohydrate elevates central serotonin synthesis weight of the females was 14042 lbs (SD ˆ 3449).
(Wurtman, 1987), and the increased synthesis of ser- Table 1, which depicts the mean Beck Depression
otonin is assumed to ameliorate the dysphoric mood Inventory (BDI; Beck et al., 1979), and Profile of
which strengthens the connection between dysphoric Mood States (POMS; McNair, Lorr & Droppleman,
mood and carbohydrate consumption. 1992) scale scores for the subject sample, reveals that
The evidence supporting a connection between a the sample represents, based on the BDI, a non-
dysphoric mood and carbohydrate consumption comes depressed group of individuals and exhibits about the
from a variety of sources. Hill, Weaver and Blundell same mood level as the average college student in the
(1991) found a significant correlation between food normative sample (McNair et al., 1992).
cravings and emotional eating. Krauchi, Wirz-Justice
and Graw (1990) revealed that individuals with seasonal Assessment instruments
affective disorder experienced an increase in carbohy- A ``craving questionnaire'', patterned after one used
drate cravings during winter depression. Fernstrom, by Weingarten and Elston (1991), was used to identify
Krowinski & Kupfer (1987), Kazes et al. (1993), and the type of cravings a person experienced, measure the
Christensen & Somers (1994) have revealed that indivi- intensity of participants' cravings, identify the foods
duals not only report increasing their preference for that were craved, and the relationship of the craved
sweet tasting foods as they become depressed, but that foods to the affective state of the participants. This
depressed individuals consume more carbohydrates, questionnaire asked participants for demographic infor-
especially simple carbohydrates, than their non- mation regarding their age, weight, education, if they
depressed counterparts. Other studies have revealed that had any physical or psychological illnesses for which
consumption of carbohydrate-rich foods leads to an they were currently being treated or psychological ill-
amelioration of the dysphoric mood state in individuals nesses for which they had been treated in the past, to
with seasonal affective disorder (Rosenthal et al., identify any medications currently taking, and whether
1989), individuals with severe premenstrual syndrome females were pregnant. Participants were then asked if
(Wurtman et al., 1989), and overweight individuals they ``generally experienced intense desires or urges
(Wurtman, 1988). This connection has led some (e.g. (cravings) to eat specific foods''? This question was used
Leibenluft et al., 1993) to suggest that dysphoric indi- as the participants self identification as a food craver.
viduals may be engaged in a type of self-medication with The craving questionnaire next asked participants to
carbohydrates. describe their food cravings as either carbohydrate,
The data supporting the connection between dys- protein, or fat cravings. Examples were given of each
phoric mood, carbohydrate cravings, and consump-
tion of carbohydrate-/fat rich foods is confined
primarily to individuals with specific disorders, pri- Table 1. Mean BDI and POMS scale scores. BDI,
marily psychiatric disorders. However, Weingarten Beck Depression Inventory; POMS, Profile of Mood
and Elston (1991) have revealed that food cravings States. POMS SD is for the raw scores. POMS T-score
exist among a very large portion of the population, is based on the norms for college students
and that much of this craving is for sweets (Pelchat, Instrument Scale Mean raw score T-Score SD
1997). Given that a dysphoric mood stimulates car-
BDI 809 Ð 699
bohydrate cravings in several psychiatric groups, it POMS Tension 1157 48 666
seems logical that such a relationship would also exist POMS Depression 1130 48 1088
among the general population. The present study POMS Anger 1657 60 892
investigated the relationship between trait mood states POMS Vigor 1641 51 664
and general carbohydrate cravings in a sample of POMS Fatigue 934 48 655
POMS Confusion 857 48 543
college students.
Carbohydrate cravings 139

type of craving to maximize the probability that the psychological disorders at the time of completion of
participants would associate the appropriate food (e.g. the assessment instruments. Two females reported
candy) with the appropriate type of craving (e.g. car- being pregnant and their data was excluded from the
bohydrate). After specifying the type of food craving study to eliminate any confound of pregnancy on crave
experienced, participants rated the ``general intensity'' of ratings or mood assessment. Ninety-one percent, 228
these cravings on a 6-point scale from ``very weak'' to of the participants, responded positively to the
``very strong''. A rating of ``intensity'' was used as the question asking them if they ``generally experienced
measure of craving rather than a measure such as fre- intense desires or urges (cravings) to eat specific
quency because self-report measures using rating scales foods''. This is consistent with the data obtained by
(i) are common indices of the degree of craving or desire Weingarten and Elston (1991) who revealed that 88%
for some object (Weingarten & Elston, 1990); (ii) have of college students reported food cravings. Participants
been demonstrated to be useful for quantifying and were then classified as being carbohydrate, protein, or
measuring subjective states such as attitudes and per- fat cravers based on their response to the question
sonality (Robinson, Shaver & Wrightsman, 1991); and which asked them to identify the type of food
(iii) would seem to capture the ``intensity'' or ``longing'' (carbohydrate, protein or fat) that they most craved.
component included in the general definition of All participants, including the 9% who indicated that
craving, ``an intense desire or longing for a substance'' they did not experience ``intense desires or urges to eat
(Weingarten & Elston, 1990, p.231), more effectively specific foods'', were asked to make such a designation
than a frequency measure would. Participants next to permit categorization of all individuals into a
specified the food they craved the most or ``strongest'' as specific type of craver to maximize the range of
well as their mood, and how they felt just before craving intensity of craving from those who stated that they
this food and after consuming the food. experienced no intense urge or desire to eat a specific
Mood state was assessed with the POMS and BDI. food too those who experienced varying degrees of
The POMS is a factor analytically derived self-report desire. Of the 251 participants, 72% described them-
inventory consisting of 65 adjectives responded to on a selves as carbohydrate cravers, 26% as protein cravers,
5-point rating scale and providing a measure of six mood and 2%, six participants, as fat cravers. Fat cravers
states: tension-anxiety, depression-dejection; anger- were eliminated from further analysis because of their
hostility; vigor-activity; fatigue-inertia; and confusion- small number. Chi-square analysis revealed that
bewilderment. Participants are instructed to respond to significantly, 2(1) ˆ 784, p < 001, more females
each adjective in terms of how they have been feeling identified themselves as carbohydrate cravers than as
``during the past week including today''. The BDI is a protein cravers. Males did not differ significantly,
21-item self-report inventory responded to on a 3-point p > 005, in terms of the number of self identified
scale and designed to measure the severity of depression. carbohydrate and protein cravers. The foods most
The Vitality Inventory (VI; Christensen & Ouellette, strongly craved by carbohydrate cravers were choco-
1995) is a 22-item factor analytically derived scale late (25%), pasta (13%), desserts (12%), candy (7%),
designed to measure the degree of vigor and exhaustion potatoes (7%), ice cream (6%) and bread (6%). The
experienced by the participants. Participants respond to remaining 24% of the participants identified pizza,
each question in terms of how they have been feeling potato chips, rice, crackers/pretzels, cookies, vegeta-
during the past month including today. bles and soda as the carbohydrates they craved most.
When carbohydrate cravers were asked how they felt
Procedure just before they experienced their food craving, the four
The research participants read and signed a ``consent most frequently stated responses, as revealed in Table 2,
to participate'' form after all questions asked had been were anxiety, fatigued, hunger and depressed. Anxiety,
answered. They then completed the assessment battery, fatigue, and depressed accounted for 67% of the
consisting of the Craving Questionnaire and the psy- responses and hunger account for 14% of the responses.
chological inventories, in groups of 25. The inventories The remaining 19% of responses consisted of happy/
and the craving questionnaire comprising the assess- good, angry, bored, and relaxed. After consuming the
ment battery were counterbalanced to control for an craved food 79% of the participants stated that they felt
order effect. satisfied, happy/good, relaxed, or energetic. The other
21% of responses consisted of tired/sleepy, guilty,
Results normal, same or no difference, and wanting more of the
craved food.
Inspection of the Craving Questionnaire revealed that Protein cravers reported that the food most frequently
none of the participants reported any physical or craved was steak/beef (48% of responders), followed by
140 L. Christensen and L. Pettijohn

chicken (20% of responders) with seafood, cheese, pizza, relaxed, with tired, energetic, and normal comprising
vegetables, and milk comprising the remaining 32% of the remaining 20% of responses.
responders. Table 2 reveals that the affective state most To determine if intensity of participants cravings were
frequently reported as occurring prior to the craving related to their mood state, correlations were computed
was anxious (36% of responders), and hungry (34% between participants' rating of craving intensity and
of responders) with happy, normal, bored, and energetic their mood state as assessed by the BDI, POMS, and VI.
comprising the remaining 30% of responders. Fol- These correlations, which were computed separately for
lowing consumption of the craved protein, 80% of carbohydrate cravers and protein cravers in addition to
the responders reported being satisfied, good, or a combined analysis, appear in Table 3. When con-
sidering all participants combined, the results reveal that
all mood scales were significantly correlated, p < 005,
Table 2. Affective states reported by carbohydrate with craving intensity except the confusion and vigor
and protein cravers before and after consumption of scales of the POMS, and the vitality scale of the VI. Half
the food most strongly craved of the significant correlations were of small effect size
Before consumption After consumption
and half were of medium effect size (Cohen, 1988).
Similar results were obtained when the cravings ratings
Affective state %reporting Affective state %reporting were correlated with the mood scales for carbohydrate
A. Carbohydrate cravers cravers alone. All mood scales were significantly
Anxious 27 Satisfied 25 p < 005, correlated with ratings of craving intensity
Fatigued 26 Happy/Good 21 with the exception of the vigor scale of the POMS, and
Hungry 14 Relaxed 20 the vitality scale of the VI, and 71% of the correlations
Depressed 14 Energetic 13
Happy/Good 11 Tired/Sleepy 9 were of medium effect size. For protein cravers, only
Angry 3 Guilty 5 one mood scale, the anger scale of the POMS, was sig-
Bored 3 Normal 3 nificantly, p < 005, correlated with ratings of craving
Relaxed 2 Same 2 intensity.
Wanting more 2 Correlations were then computed separately for
B. Protein cravers
Anxious 36 Satisfied 40 female and male ratings to identify a gender difference
Hungry 34 Good 21 which may exist in the relationship between type of
Happy 11 Relaxed 20 cravings and mood. Correlations were first computed
Normal 9 Tired 9 between female carbohydrate and protein cravers'
Bored 8 Energetic 7 ratings of craving intensity and the various measures of
Energetic 2 Normal 3
mood. As revealed in Table 4, mood was significantly

Table 3. Correlations and their effect size between mood measures and craving intensity in carbohydrate and
protein cravers, separately and combined. BDI, Beck Depression Inventory; POMS, Profile of Mood States; VI,
Vitality Inventory. d, the effect size index. The d corresponding to a given r was determined by rounding the r in
the table down to the nearest r depicted in Table 221 of Cohen (1988). For example, an r of 018 was rounded
down to an r of 0148 and then given the effect size of 03 rather than rounding up to the nearest r of 0196 which
would correspond to an effect size of 04. **p < 001; *p < 005
Instrument Scale Carb cravers Protein cravers Combined
N ˆ 175 N ˆ 64 N ˆ 239
r d r d r d
BDI Depression 028** 06b ÿ009 Ð 023** 04a
POMS Depression 029** 06b 015 Ð 026** 05b
POMS Tension/Anxiety 028** 05b 016 Ð 026** 05b
POMS Anger 022** 04a 026* 05b 022** 04a
POMS Confusion 029** 06b 0002 Ð 010 Ð
POMS Fatigue 028** 05b 017 Ð 026** 05b
POMS Vigor ÿ013 Ð 002 Ð 010 Ð
VI Exhaustion 023** 04a 012 Ð 021** 03a
VI Vitality ÿ005 Ð ÿ008 Ð ÿ006 Ð
a
Small.
b
Medium.
c
Large effect size.
Carbohydrate cravings 141

Table 4. Correlations and their effect size between mood measures and craving intensity in female carbohydrate
and protein cravers. BDI, Beck Depression Inventory; POMS, Profile of Mood States; VI, Vitality Inventory. d,
the effect size index. The d corresponding to a given r was determined by rounding the r in the table down to the
nearest r depicted in Table 221 of Cohen (1988). For example, an r of 018 was rounded down to an r of 0148
and then given the effect size of 03 rather than rounding up to the nearest r of 0196 which would correspond to
an effect size of 04. **p < 00l; *p < 005
Instrument Scale Carb cravers Protein cravers
N ˆ 118 N ˆ 14
r d r d
BDI Depression 026** 05b ÿ002 Ð
POMS Depression 022* 04a ÿ042 Ð
POMS Tension/Anxiety 021* 04a ÿ025 Ð
POMS Anger 019* 04a ÿ052 Ð
POMS Confusion 027** 05b ÿ028 Ð
POMS Fatigue 024** 05b ÿ010 Ð
POMS Vigor ÿ017 Ð ÿ017 Ð
VI Exhaustion 018** 03a ÿ006 Ð
VI Vitality ÿ005 Ð 009 Ð
a
Small.
b
Medium.

Table 5. Correlations and their effect size between mood measures and craving intensity in male carbohydrate
and protein cravers. BDI, Beck Depression Inventory; POMS, Profile of Mood States; VI, Vitality Inventory. d,
the effect size index. The d corresponding to a given r was determined by rounding the r in the table down to the
nearest r depicted in Table 2.2.1 of Cohen (1988). For example, an r of 018 was rounded down to an r of 0148
and then given the effect size of 03 rather than rounding up to the nearest r of 0196 which would correspond to
an effect size of 04. **p < 001; *p < 005
Instrument Scale Carb cravers Protein cravers
N ˆ 57 N ˆ 50
r d r d
* b
BDI Depression 032 06 ÿ010 Ð
POMS Depression 042** 09c 023 Ð
POMS Tension/Anxiety 044** 09c 023 Ð
POMS Anger 031* 06b 032* 06b
POMS Confusion 033* 06b 001 Ð
POMS Fatigue 034* 06b 023 Ð
POMS Vigor ÿ004 Ð ÿ002 Ð
VI Exhaustion 031* 06b 016 Ð
VI Vitality ÿ005 Ð ÿ014 Ð
a
Small.
b
Medium.
c
Large effect size.

correlated with carbohydrate cravers' ratings of crav- significant correlations were found between ratings of
ing intensity on all scales except POMS vigor and the craving intensity and all mood scales except the POMS
VI vitality scale. About half of these correlations were of Vigor and the VI vitality scale. All of these correla-
small effect size and half were of medium effect size. tions were of either large or medium effect size. For
However, none of the mood scales correlated sig- male protein cravers only the POMS anger scale was
nificantly with the ratings of craving intensity of the significantly, p < 005 correlated with ratings of craving
female protein cravers. Correlations between the ratings intensity.
of craving intensity and mood for male carbohydrate When the correlations between mood scales and
and protein cravers revealed a slightly different pattern craving intensity are compared for male and female
of results. For male carbohydrate cravers (see Table 5), carbohydrate cravers (see Tables 4 and 5) it is apparent
142 L. Christensen and L. Pettijohn

Table 6. Correlations and their effect size for carbohydrate cravers who crave sweet and non-sweet foods.
*
p < 005
Instrument Scale Cravers of sweet carb foods Cravers of non-sweet foods
N ˆ 53 N ˆ 42
r d r d
BDI Depression 020 Ð 014 Ð
POMS Tension 027* 05b 033* 07b
POMS Depression 024* 05b 013 Ð
POMS Anger 031* 06b 013 Ð
POMS Fatigue 025* 05b 019 Ð
POMS Vigor 007 Ð ÿ003 Ð
POMS Confusion 024* 05b 030* 06b
VI Exhaustion 026* 05b 010 Ð
VI Vitality 018 Ð ÿ003 Ð
a
Small.
b
Medium.
c
Large effect size.

that significant correlations exist on the same mood hydrate rich foods (e.g. chocolate, pasta and desserts)
scales. However, the effect size is greater in males on the as the foods which they most strongly craved lending
POMS depression, anxiety, and anger and VI exhaus- validity to their designation as carbohydrate cravers.
tion scales. The difference in the size of the correlations These foods, however, are not pure carbohydrates or
is equivalent to a small effect size on all of these scales. even predominately carbohydrates in some instances.
The carbohydrate cravers were then divided into Rather, they are foods that are sweet carbohydrate/
those who stated that the food they craved most was a fat rich foods. However, the sweetness is the more
sweet carbohydrate-rich food (e.g. a dessert) or a salient sensory attribute (Drewnowski et al., 1992) and
non-sweet carbohydrate-rich food (e.g. pasta).1 Table 6, the attribute that lends the food to be considered a
which depicts the correlation between the ratings of carbohydrate. Because many of the foods which car-
craving intensity and mood for each of these two groups bohydrate cravers identified as the craved food are also
of individuals, reveals that mood was correlated with fat-rich, there is some speculation (Drewnowski et al.,
craving intensity on all scales except the BDI, the POMS 1992) that the desired macronutrient may be the fat
vigor scale, and the VI vitality scale in individuals who and not the carbohydrate. The bulk of the literature
craved sweet carbohydrate-rich foods. The magnitude seems to suggest that the carbohydrate cravings are
of the correlations was of a medium effect size. For associated with snack foods (e.g. Schlundt et al., 1992).
individuals who craved non-sweet carbohydrate-rich The dominant selection is for sweet snacks (Bowen &
foods, mood was correlated with craving intensity only Grunberg, 1990; Krauchi et al., 1990) rather than non-
on the POMS tension and confusion scales. sweet foods suggesting that it is the sweet carbohydrate
motivating the selection process and not the fat. This
conclusion is also supported by the current data
Discussion indicating that mood is correlated with craving
intensity ratings primarily in individuals who crave
The purpose of the present study was to determine if foods that are sweet. Protein cravers listed foods such
a relationship existed between the intensity of general as steak and chicken as the foods which they most
carbohydrate cravings and trait measures of mood strongly craved again lending validity to their classi-
among a group of nondistressed individuals. Carbo- fication as protein cravers.
hydrate cravers and protein cravers were self identified The largest percentage of individuals (72%) identified
based on their self report of being a carbohydrate, or themselves as carbohydrate cravers which is consistent
protein craver. Carbohydrate cravers identified carbo- with prior research (Pelchat, 1997, Weingarten Elston,
1991) revealing that most cravings are carbohydrate
1
This comparison was suggested by one of the reviewers. cravings. These finding also add to the developing lit-
The N for the participants craving sweet and non-sweet erature indicating that food cravings, and particularly
carbohydrates does not equal the total N for carbohydrate
cravers because the original crave questionnaire had to be con- carbohydrate cravings, are not confined to psychiatric
sulted to classify these participants and some of these ques- populations, but are a rather widespread phenomenon.
tionnaires had been misplaced or thrown away inadvertently. The interesting component is that these cravings are
Carbohydrate cravings 143

especially apparent among females. In the present study ture. The food craving literature has focused most atten-
90% of the females classified themselves as carbo- tion on a self-medication hypothesis (Leibenluft et al.,
hydrate cravers whereas only 53% of males did so. This 1991) which states that cravings for carbohydrate-rich
finding is consistent with other studies (see Ganley, food substances exist because consumption of these
1989) demonstrating a greater incidence of carbohy- foods results in relief of the dysphoric symptoms they
drate cravings in females. However, these cravings do are experiencing. The relief in dysphoric symptoms is
seem to abate with age (Pelchat, 1997). There is little assumed to occur because carbohydrate-rich foods
evidence to suggest why more females would experience increase the plasma ratio of tryptophan to the other
carbohydrate cravings other than the fact that females large neutral amino acid residues. This permits more
are more likely to have a mood disorder such as tryptophan to cross the blood-brain barrier resulting in
depression (Robins et al., 1984). an increased synthesis of central serotonin (Wurtman,
The primary purpose of the present study was to 1987) which in turn is assumed to ameliorate the dys-
determine if a relationship existed between trait mea- phoric mood.
sures of mood and general carbohydrate cravings. Two The self-medication hypothesis makes sense
sources of data indicate that carbohydrate cravings because of the positive effect which consumption of
increase with negative moods. Over two-thirds of the carbohydrate-rich foods has been demonstrated to have
carbohydrate cravers indicated that they experience on mood (e.g. Rosenthal et al., 1989; Wurtman et al.,
mood states such as anxiety, fatigue, and depression 1989). Therefore, a dysphoric mood would be associated
prior to their cravings. The only negative mood state with a desire to consume carbohydrates because of
reported by protein cravers was anxiety and this their negative reinforcing effect of producing a decline
was reported by only 34% of the responders. The other in the dysphoric state, and the positive reinforcing effect
66% reported states such as hunger, happy, bored, or of mood enhancement. However, it has repeatedly been
energetic as the state preceding their cravings. Carbo- pointed out (e.g., Christensen, 1997; Young, 1991) that
hydrate cravers, therefore, are much more likely to the mood alteration produced by consumption of a
identify a negative mood state as preceding their craving. carbohydrate-rich snack is probably not due to an
The correlation between craving intensity and mood increased synthesis of central serotonin because the rise
confirms even further that negative moods are related in the plasma tryptophan ratio which results from the
to carbohydrate cravings. Ratings of carbohydrate carbohydrate consumption may be too small to influ-
cravings were significantly correlated with all negative ence central serotonin significantly (Ashley, Liardon &
mood states. The only ones not correlated with the crave Leathwood, 1985). Also, as little as 4% protein can
ratings were vigor and vitality. These are positive mood block the rise in the plasma tryptophan ratio (Teff,
states. The only mood state that correlated with protein Young & Blundell, 1989) and there are few foods or
cravers ratings of craving intensity was anger. Because meals that contain less than 4% protein. In addition,
nine correlations were computed, it is likely that this the present data reveals that the relationship between
is a chance correlation. This relationship between mood and craving intensity existed primarily for indi-
craving intensity and negative mood is consistent not viduals who craved sweet carbohydrate-rich foods and
only with other studies (e.g. Wurtman et al., 1989; the carbohydrate-serotonin connection is not specific
Wurtman & Wurtman, 1986; Willner et al., 1998) of to such foods.
food cravings using both state and trait mood measures, An abstinence model does not seem to be useful in
but also with studies demonstrating a relationship explaining the relationship between dysphoric mood
between state measures of negative mood and desire to and carbohydrate craving because this model requires
smoke (Tiffany & Drobes, 1990) or use drugs (Childress the withdrawal or abstinence of the target substance
et al., 1994). It is apparent that a negative mood state which, in this case, would be carbohydrates. Most foods,
contributes to a variety of cravings and it makes no meals, and snacks include carbohydrates, and even if
difference whether it is a state or trait mood state. one snack contains no carbohydrate, the subsequent
If negative mood states contribute to cravings, the meal in all probability does. An expectancy model also
question is why? The drug, alcohol, and tobacco litera- does not seem to be very useful in explaining the rela-
ture focuses on issues such as cravings being triggered tionship between dysphoric mood and carbohydrate
by the dysphoric state associated with abstinence of the cravings unless the hedonic pleasure derived from eating
desired or needed substance, and on exposure to stimuli a sweet carbohydrate/fat-rich food temporarily enhan-
associated with the desired substance triggering cravings ces mood. However, this seems like only a remote
(Weingarten & Elston, 1990). While such factors are possibility. At the present time the self-medication
supported in drug, alcohol, and tobacco literature, they hypothesis seems to be the best explanation to account
have received little attention in the food craving litera- for the craving-negative mood relationship.
144 L. Christensen and L. Pettijohn

While the self-medication hypothesis is supported by Cohen, I.T., Sherwin, B.B. & Fleming, A. (1987). Food
the results of the present study as well as others, the cravings, mood and the menstrual cycle. Hormones and
Behavior 21, 457±470.
correlation between dysphoric mood and carbohydrate Drewnowski, A., Kurth, C., Holden-Wiltse, J. & Sasari, J.
craving is not strong and accounts for only a small (1992). Food preferences in human obesity: carbohy-
portion of the variance in carbohydrate cravings. This drates versus fats. Appetite 18, 207±221.
means that other factors also contribute to carbohydrate Fernstrom, M.H., Krowinski, R.I. & Kupfer, D.J. (1987).
cravings. If the relationship between dysphoric mood Appetite and food preference in depression: effects of
imipramine treatment. Biological Psychiatry 22, 529±
and carbohydrate cravings is due to a combined posi- 539.
tive and negative reinforcement phenomenon, then a Ganley, R.M. (1989). Emotion and eating in obesity: a
whole host of environmental factors, such as the sight review of the literature. International Journal of Eating
and smell of various food items, as well as thoughts or Disorders 8, 343±361.
imagery, should also be related to carbohydrates. Hill, A.J., Weaver, C.F.L. & Blundell, J.E. (1991). Food crav-
ing, dietary restraint and mood. Appetite 17, 187±197.
These are variables that have been demonstrated (e.g. Kazes, M., Danion, J.M., Grange, D., Pradignac, A.,
Weinstein et al., 1998; for a review, see Tiffany, 1990) to Simon, C.H., Burrus-Mehl, F. et al. (1994). Eating
contribute to drug and alcohol cravings and probably behavior and depression before and after antidepressant
also contribute to carbohydrate cravings. However, treatment: a prospective, naturalistic study. Journal of
virtually no attention has been given to these variables Affective Disorder 30, 193±207.
Krauchi, K., Wirz-Justice, A. & Graw, P. (1990). The rela-
as possible contributors to carbohydrate cravings. tionship of affective state to dietary preference: winter
Investigation of such factors deserves the attention of depression and light therapy as a model. Journal of
individuals interested in the phenomenon of carbo- Affective Disorders 20, 43±53.
hydrate cravings or food cravings if we are to have a Leibenluft, E., Fiero, P.L., Bartko, J.J., Moul, D.E. &
complete understanding of this phenomenon. Rosenthal, N.E. (1993). Depressive symptoms and the
self-reported use of alcohol, caffeine, and carbohydrates
in normal volunteers and four groups of psychiatric out-
patients. American Journal of Psychiatry 150, 294±301.
References Lingjaerde, O. & Reichborn-Kjennerud, T. (1993). Charac-
teristics of winter depression in the Oslo area (60 N).
Ashley, D.V.M., Liardon, R. & Leathwood, P.D. (1985). Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 88, 111±120.
Breakfast meal composition influences plasma trypto- McNair, D.M., Lorr, M. & Droppleman, L. (1984). Profile of
phan to large neutral amino acid ratios of healthy lean mood states manual. San Diego: Educational and Indus-
young men. Journal of Neural Transmission 63, 271±283. trial Testing Service.
Bancroft, J., Cook, A. & Williamson, L. (1988). Food Paykel, E.S., Mueller, P.S. & de la Vergne, P.M. (1973).
craving, mood, and the menstrual cycle. Psychological Amitryptyline, weight gain and carbohydrate craving: a
Medicine 18, 855±860. side effect. British Journal of Psychiatry 123, 501±507.
Beck, A.T., Rush, A.J., Shaw, B.F. & Emory, G. (1979). Pelchat, M.L. (1997). Food cravings in young and elderly
Cognitive therapy of depression. New York: Guilford adults. Appetite 28, 103±113.
Press. Robins, L.N., Helzer, J.E., Weissman, M.M., Orvaschel, H.,
Bjorvell, H., Ronnberg, S. & Rossner, S. (1985). Eating pat- Gruenberg, E., Burke, J.D., Jr & Regier, D.A. (1984).
terns described by a group of treatment seeking over- Lifetime prevalence of specific psychiatric disorders in
weight women and normal weight women. Scandinavian three sites. Archives of General Psychiatry 41, 949±958.
Journal of Behavior Therapy 14, 147±156. Robinson, J.P., Shaver, P.R. & Wrightsman, L.S. (1991).
Bowen, D.J. & Grunberg, N.E. (1990). Variations in food Measures of personality and social psychological attitudes.
preference and consumption across the menstrual cycle. San Diego: Academic Press, Inc.
Physiology and Behavior 47, 287±291. Rosenthal, N.E., Sack, D.A., Gillin, J.C., Lewy, A.J.,
Childress, A.R., Ehrman, R., McLennan, A.T., MacRae, R., Goodwin, F.K., Davenport, Y. et al. (1984). Seasonal
Natale, M. & O'Brien, C.P. (1994). Can induced moods affective disorder: A description of the syndrome and
trigger drug-related responses in opiate abuse patients? preliminary findings with light therapy. Archives of
Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment 11, 17±23. General Psychiatry 41, 72±80.
Christensen, L. (1997). The effect of carbohydrates on affect. Rosenthal, N.E., Genhart, M.J., Caballero, B., Jacobsen, F.M.,
Nutrition: The International Journal of Applied and Basic Skwerer, R.G., Coursey, R.D. et al. (1989). Psychobiolo-
Nutritional Sciences 13, 503±514. gical effects of carbohydrate and protein-rich means in
Christensen, L. & Ouellette, J.A. (1995). Development and patients with seasonal affective disorder and normal con-
validation of a scale to measure vigor and exhaustion. trols. Biological Psychiatry 25, 1029±1040.
Poster session presented at the annual meeting of South- Schlundt, D.G., Virts, K., Sbrocco, T., Pope-Cordle, J. &
eastern Psychological Association, Savannah, GA. Hill, J.O. (1993). A sequential behavioral analysis of
Christensen, L. & Somers, S. (1996). Comparison of nutrient craving sweets in obese women. Addictive Behaviors 18,
intake among depressed and nondepressed individuals. 67±80.
International Journal of Eating Disorders 20, 105±109. Smith, S.L. & Sauder, C. (1969). Food cravings, depression,
Cohen, J. (1988) Statistical power analysis for the behavioral and premenstrual problems. Psychosomatic Medicine 31,
sciences. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 281±287.
Carbohydrate cravings 145

Teff K.L., Young, S.N & Blundell, J.E. (1989). The effect of of depression and craving. Psychopharmacology 136,
protein or carbohydrate breakfast on subsequent plasma 272±283.
amino acid levels, satiety and nutrient selection in normal Wurtman, J.J. (1988). Carbohydrate craving, mood changes,
males. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 34, and obesity. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 49 (suppl),
829±837. 37±39.
Tiffany, S.T. (1990). A cognitive model of drug urges and Wurtman, J.J., Brzezinski, A., Wurtman, R.J. & Laferrere, B.
drug-use behavior: role of automatic and non-automatic (1989). Effect of nutrient intake on premenstrual depres-
processes. Psychological Review 97, 147±168. sion. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology 161,
Tiffany, S.T. & Drobes, D.J. (1990). Imagery and smoking 1228±1234.
urges: the manipulation of affective content. Addictive Wurtman, R.J. (1987). Dietary treatments that affect brain
Behavior 15, 531±539. neurotransmitters: effects on calorie and nutrient intake.
Weingarten, H.P. & Elston, D. (1990). The phenomenology Annals of the New York Academy of Science 161,
of food cravings. Appetite 15, 231±246. 179±190.
Weingarten, H.P. & Elston, D. (1991). Food cravings in a Wurtman, R.J. & Wurtman, J.J. (1986). Carbohydrate crav-
college population. Appetite 17, 167±175. ing, obesity and brain serotonin. Appetite 7 (suppl),
Weinstein, A., Feldtkeller, B., Malizia, A., Wilson, S., 99±103.
Bailey, J. & Nutt, D.J. (1998). Integrating the cognitive Young, S.N. (1989). The 1989 Borden Award lecture. Some
and physiological aspects of craving. Journal of Psycho- effects of dietary components (amino acids, carbo-
pharmacology 12, 31±38. hydrate, folic acid) on brain serotonin synthesis, mood
Willner, P., Benton, D., Brown, E., Cheeta, S., Davies, G., and behavior. Canadian Journal of Physiology and
Morgan, J. & Morgan, M. (1998). ``Depression'' increases Pharmacology 69, 893±903.
``craving'' for sweet rewards in animal and human models

You might also like