Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 17

Global Environmental Change 81 (2023) 102698

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Global Environmental Change


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/gloenvcha

Using Protection Motivation Theory to examine information-seeking


behaviors on climate change
Jun Li a, Ping Qin a, Yifei Quan b, Jie-Sheng Tan-Soo c, *
a
School of Applied Economics, Renmin University of China, China
b
Institute of Energy, Environment and Economy, Tsinghua University, China
c
Lee Kuan Yew Sch of Public Policy, National University of Singapore, Singapore

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Many earlier studies concluded that exposure to changes in local weather or extreme weather events prompt
China public interest in climate change, and in turn raise support for mitigation policies. However, these findings do not
Developing countries square with observations of record-breaking temperatures, and decades of failure to reduce greenhouse gas
Climate change
emissions. To address this conundrum, we use Protection Motivation Theory to form hypotheses on the specific
Big data
type of climate change-related information that individuals seek during periods of extreme local weather. Using
daily-level internet search engine data from Chinese cities, we find that residents are purposeful and rational in
seeking information on climate change. Specifically, when faced with high or abnormal temperatures, they are
much more likely to seek information to appraise their susceptibility to climate change threats, and evaluate
coping responses. On the other hand, due to the lack of direct benefits, they do not seek out information on
climate mitigation behaviors. In contrast to earlier studies, our findings suggest that it is unlikely that extreme
weather events will prompt support for climate mitigation actions. Instead, as worldwide weather becomes more
extreme and unpredictable, it is likely that public’s attention will shift in the direction of adaptation measures.

1. Introduction garnering public attention in setting of policy agenda (Birkland, 2017).


In the context of climate change, studies have identified the lack of
Partly due to the slow pace of greenhouse gas (GHG) emissions public awareness and enthusiasm as one of the main reasons for lack­
reduction, there are now growing calls in the scientific and policy luster progress in implementing emissions mitigation policies (Drum­
community to re-imagine research and policies that can lead to stronger mond et al., 2018; Lorenzoni et al., 2007; Rahimi, 2020).
mitigation efforts (Stern, 2021; Stern and Stiglitz, 2022; Stoddard et al., As such, uncovering the factors driving beliefs and interests in
2021). Among the numerous suggestions for advancing climate change climate change will aid policymakers in forming policies and engaging
research, there is increasing recognition that the public’s GHG emissions the public on such topics, and even time release of new policies more
are overlooked, and individuals’ mitigation efforts can play a larger role appropriately (Birkland, 1997, 2017). Toward this end, an enduring
in the climate crisis (Stern, 2021; Stern and Stiglitz, 2022; Stoddard finding across time, space, and research methodology is that personal
et al., 2021). experiences of extreme weather events or large changes to local weather
There are at least two reasons why individuals and the public can is a strong predictor of i) public interest and information seeking in
play a crucial role in climate change. First, the residential sector is the climate change, ii) belief in climate change, and to a lesser extent, iii)
world’s third-largest energy consumer, directly accounting for around support for climate mitigation policies (e.g., Bergquist et al., 2019;
17% of global CO2 emissions (Nejat et al., 2015). On top of this, being Brulle et al., 2012; Cavanagh et al., 2014; Choi et al., 2020; Constantino
the final consumer, the public is also indirectly responsible for emissions et al., 2022; Donner and McDaniels, 2013; Hughes et al., 2020; Kirilenko
in the transportation and industrial sectors. In this regard, effective et al., 2015; Konisky et al., 2016; Lang, 2014b; Li et al., 2011; Osber­
reduction in GHG emissions depends crucially on inducing the public to ghaus and Demski, 2019; Pianta and Sisco, 2020; Ray et al., 2017; Sisco
reduce their carbon footprint (Scannell and Gifford, 2013). et al., 2017a; Sisco and Weber, 2022; Tan-Soo et al., 2022; Taylor et al.,
Second, theories on policy processes recognize the importance of 2014; Zaval et al., 2014).

* Corresponding author.
E-mail address: jiesheng.tan@nus.edu.sg (J.-S. Tan-Soo).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2023.102698
Received 12 September 2022; Received in revised form 7 May 2023; Accepted 15 May 2023
Available online 25 May 2023
0959-3780/© 2023 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
J. Li et al. Global Environmental Change 81 (2023) 102698

However, despite these findings, it remains unclear why changes to emissions (Akram et al., 2020), and are much more vulnerable to climate
local weather would prompt higher public interest or information change compared to their developed countries counterparts (Barreca
seeking in climate change. Moreover, given that climate change is a et al., 2016; Guo et al., 2014), the former has so far lagged behind their
multi-faceted topic, there is scientific and policy relevance in dis­ developed counterparts in climate mitigation and adaptation policies
tinguishing how public interest or information-seeking behaviors are (Halsnæs and Verhagen, 2007; Mertz et al., 2009). Similarly, research on
spread across the different aspects of climate change. public sentiments of climate change is mostly focused on developed
On this note, we build on existing studies to examine and generate countries and/or data-rich societies (e.g., Kirilenko et al., 2015; Ray
new insights on how changes to local weather will affect public interest et al., 2017; Sisco et al., 2017a; Zanocco et al., 2019). Given that climate
and information seeking across different types of behavioral responses change is a globally-caused problem, worldwide cooperation is needed
toward climate change in the world’s largest GHG emitter country – to collectively reduce emissions. As such, this study brings focus and
China. much-needed evidence to how the public perceives climate change in a
Our study approach can be briefly described in three steps. large emitter, and developing country.
First, we enlist the Protection Motivation Theory (PMT) to structure Second, even though existing research in this literature mostly
the relationship between local weather and individual behaviors concluded that variation in local weather is likely to increase public
(Rogers, 1975). In essence, the PMT predicts that when one is faced with belief or awareness of climate change, this optimistic outlook fails to
environmental threats, they will seek out information to appraise their account for the possibility that individuals can respond in varied ways to
susceptibility to dangers, and explore cost-effectiveness of various the perceived threats of climate change. For instance, it is intuitive that
coping responses (Bockarjova and Steg, 2014; Rogers, 1975). individuals assessing high probability of continued rising temperature in
Second, individuals in turn require up-to-date information to the future are far more likely to adopt air-conditioners rather than
appraise threats and availability of coping responses. Given that internet electric vehicles since the former will directly help alleviate the adverse
search engines are ubiquitous sources of information, we follow an effects of heat. In this regard, we use Protection Motivation Theory
established strand of literature (e.g., Choi et al., 2020; Lang, 2014b; (PMT) as a starting point to systematically form hypotheses on how local
Osberghaus and Demski, 2019; Qin and Zhu, 2018) by using internet temperature changes would affect information seeking in three main
search volume on China’s largest search engine (Baidu) to proxy for types of behavioral responses toward climate change – knowledge,
information seeking in climate change. adaptation, and mitigation. As such, this study offers a more complete
Third, using the PMT, we form a series of hypotheses on the rela­ picture of the relationship between daily temperature and public in­
tionship between local weather and information-seeking in climate terest in climate change.
change. To test these hypotheses, we amass a daily-level dataset that The third contribution involves dataset. Earlier studies examining
spans across ten years, and covers over 300 Chinese cities, we arrive at climate change sentiments mostly relied on cross-sectional (e.g., Hughes
the following main findings. et al., 2020) or high frequency but short timespan (e.g., Osberghaus and
First, we find that, similar to studies conducted in developed coun­ Demski, 2019) datasets. The drawback of cross-sectional datasets is that
tries, Chinese citizens mentally associate changes to local weather with temperature changes at very high frequency, and there are many un­
climate change as internet search volume on keywords related to the observed factors (e.g., ownership of air-conditioner, acclimatization)
latter grows significantly on days with high or abnormal temperatures. that can moderate the effects between temperature and sentiments. In
For instance, search volume on climate change increases by 50.4% on a contrast, while high-frequency longitudinal data can iron out many of
day with average temperature >30 ◦ C relative to a <5 ◦ C day. Similarly, these issues by introducing statistical fixed effects to control for time-
both “hotter-than-expected” and “cooler-than-expected” days also elicit varying and time-invariant factors, they should ideally be com­
increased searches on climate change. plemented by a long timespan. In this regard, our dataset has the
Second, a key point of departure from existing studies is that we following attributes that allow for statistically rigorous analyses, and yet
recognize climate change is a multi-faceted topic. There could be many yield highly generalizable results: i) daily-level, ii) covers 329 cities in
different behavioral responses even though individuals mentally asso­ China (accounting for around 96% of total population), and iii) 10 years
ciate local weather changes with climate change. On this note, the PMT (from 2011 to 2020).
offers more refined predictions on the types of information needed for
individuals to appraise environmental threats and coping measures. We 2. Background and literature review
find that the earlier results were mostly driven by searches on individual
climate adaptation measures, and to a lesser extent, searches on climate Unlike many environmental problems where the causes and impacts
change knowledge. On the other hand, we detect almost no relationship are largely undisputed (e.g., air and water pollution, deforestation), is­
between daily temperature and public interest in climate mitigation sues surrounding climate change have always been scientifically and
topics. politically controversial (Boykoff and Boykoff, 2004; Corbett and Dur­
Third, an implicit assumption of the PMT framework is that in­ fee, 2004). However, despite debates surrounding the causes, it is
dividuals are seeking information based on local weather conditions. If objectively clear that global temperature has been increasing rapidly in
true, this means that we can further narrow down the keyword searches the past decades. For instance, the year 2020 was recorded as the second
to match with underlying weather and climatic conditions. In this re­ warmest in human history at 1.2 ◦ C warmer than pre-industrial period
gard, we find that Chinese residents are indeed strategic in searching for (1850 to 1900) (NOAA, 2021). On a decadal basis, 2011 to 2020 was
climate change-related information. There are greater interests on key­ also the warmest on record where average temperature is 0.82 ◦ C higher
words related to global warming, and heat adaptation on hot days, compared to the 20th century (NOAA, 2021).
relative to other adaptation information also related to climate change. Due to the saliency of local weather, and connections between daily
Similarly, those residing in predominantly hot regions are less likely to weather and long-term climate trends, researchers have long hypothe­
search for climate change information on a warm day, compared to sized that personal experiences with weather can influence interests and
those from cooler regions. sentiments on climate change (Whitmarsh, 2008). There are two
In all, this study makes three contributions to the broad literature on possible mechanisms behind this relationship.
the relationship between climate change and individuals’ actions. First, from a psychological perspective, noticeable weather abnor­
First, this study is one of the few to investigate the extent to which malities may remind individuals of climate change, due to a mental
daily temperature changes affects public interest in climate change association between these two concepts (Pianta and Sisco, 2020). There
topics in China, and in developing countries in general. Even though is evidence that individuals who perceive climate change as a relatively
developing countries account for approximately 50 % of global CO2 certain and relatable phenomenon (e.g., affecting people and places

2
J. Li et al. Global Environmental Change 81 (2023) 102698

Fig. 1. Schematic framework of Protection Motivation Theory (PMT) as applied to environmental threats posed by local weather conditions.

close to them) are more motivated to engage in climate action than traditional data sources to assess the relationship between monthly
people who are more psychologically distant from climate change local weather and public interest in climate change.
(McDonald et al., 2015). An early attempt by Lang (2014b) used search index on one of the
Second, from the viewpoint of environmental economics and most popular internet search engines in the world – Google – to show that
behavioral psychology, when faced with weather anomalies, rational search volume on “climate change” and “global warming” increases
individuals will seek out defensive and mitigation behaviors related to when local temperature is high across cities in the United States. The
climate change (e.g., Bockarjova and Steg, 2014; Champ et al., 2003). rationale behind this research approach is even though internet searches
For instance, Auffhammer (2014) and Li et al. (2019) both found that do not directly convey support or belief, they signal public interest or
residents in China purchase or use air-conditioners at a higher frequency information seeking in a particular topic. The merit of web search data is
during periods of high temperature. Similarly, Bockarjova and Steg that such data are high-frequency hence allowing for more precise sta­
(2014) found that individuals choosing to reduce their carbon footprint tistical estimations. A further advantage of using internet searches is that
is another plausible behavioral response when threatened with extreme datasets are available for multiple countries. For example, Choi et al.
weather events (Bockarjova and Steg, 2014). (2020) applied the same approach to 74 cities around the world and
Due to these mechanisms, it is widely hypothesized that public in­ came to a similar conclusion that abnormally warm months lead to
terest in climate change spikes during periods of high or abnormal heightened Google search volume on climate change. Other than using
temperature. In turn, researchers have explored this relationship using internet searches, researchers are increasingly using social media posts
various notions of public attention and definitions of local weather. to reflect sentiments toward climate change. Sisco et al. (2017a) and
There is a line of research that assess how direct weather experiences Moore et al. (2019) use geo-coded social media messages from Twitter to
can affect attitudes toward climate change based on field or in-person assess if the American public is more likely to post messages on the
surveys. In one of the earliest studies, Li et al. (2011) used survey data weather during weeks of abnormal temperature.
from the United States and Australia to show that respondents are more In all, regardless of data sources or research approach, existing
likely to believe in climate change if the interview was conducted on a studies are consistent in showing that public interest in climate change is
hotter-than-expected day. Both Donner and McDaniels (2013) and Zaval piqued when there are abnormalities in temperature or occurrences of
et al. (2014) arrived at similar conclusions where they found that extreme weather events. However, despite the relatively broad set of
Americans’ belief in climate change increased with anomalously warm literature, there are still two important knowledge gaps that this study
weather. Other than temperature, researchers also examined if extreme attempts to fill.
weather events have similar effects. Konisky et al. (2016) paired a First, a glaring concern with current research utilizing internet
database of storm events in the United States with public opinion data, searches is that they do not distinguish between different topics in
and found positive associations between extreme weather events and climate change even though theories and frameworks linking environ­
concern for climate change. However, this relationship is most likely mental threats to human behaviors predict specific forms of behavioral
short-lived as the findings are statistically significant only limited to responses (Liu et al., 2018; Semenza et al., 2008). Most existing studies
recent events. Bergquist et al. (2019) focused on one event – Hurricane use search indices provided by Google Trends (e.g., Choi et al., 2020;
Irma – and used a rare longitudinal dataset to assess attitudes toward Lang, 2014a), which covers all internet searches related to climate
climate change before and after the event. They found that respondents’ change. However, as mentioned earlier, individuals and households may
belief in climate change and willingness-to-pay to mitigate carbon not only be broadly interested in climate change topics. Instead, expe­
emissions increased after experiencing the hurricane. Lastly, using non- riencing extreme weather events may cause them to search for specific
weather phenomenon as the trigger, a recent study examined if the ways to protect themselves from or even prevent future events. There is
Covid-19 pandemic would erode public’s concern for climate change. a small, but growing strand of literature that pays attention to such re­
Using a multi-country longitudinal dataset (survey taken before and lationships. Osberghaus and Demski (2019) found that residents who
after the pandemic), they found no evidence to support the hypothesis experienced moderate (as opposed to severe) floods in Germany are
that concern for climate change will be overshadowed by other signifi­ more likely to search for electricity from renewable sources. Similarly,
cant public events (Sisco et al., 2023). Sisco and Weber (2022) showed that Americans are more likely to
Even though survey-based studies mostly concluded that direct search for “renewable energy” on weeks with abnormally high
experience of extreme weather results in heightened interest or beliefs in maximum temperature whereas interests on other climate mitigation
climate change, such studies are limited as they are dependent on policies (e.g., carbon tax and cap-and-trade) remain unaffected. Con­
fortuitous survey timing. Moreover, just as weather, public attitudes and stantino et al. (2022) found using survey data that Americans that
interest change frequently, it is thus difficult for conventional field experienced extreme weather events are more willing to pay for climate
surveys to keep up with and capture the latest sentiments. As such, there mitigation policies.
is a growing strand of literature that makes creative usage of non- We expand upon this line of work as we use searches on keywords

3
J. Li et al. Global Environmental Change 81 (2023) 102698

specifically related to three main topics in climate change – knowledge, information that individuals are using to inform their consequent be­
mitigation, and adaptation – to better assess how local weather prompt haviors. On this note, we can form, using Protective Motivation Theory,
public interest in different aspects of climate change. the following hypotheses on how changes to local weather influence the
Second, existing literature examining impacts of climate change types of climate change-related information the Chinese public search
mostly define temperature either as continuous variables (e.g., Sisco for.
et al., 2017b), intervals (e.g., Qin et al., 2022), anomalies (e.g., Marlon Hypothesis 1: Variation in local weather is positively correlated with in­
et al., 2021), or extreme events (e.g., Brulle et al., 2012). However, due formation seeking in climate change
to a lack of guidance from theoretical frameworks, it is unclear which The rationale behind this hypothesis is straightforward. Earlier
measure of temperature is the most appropriate (Newell et al., 2021). In research mostly concluded that personal experiences of large or extreme
this study, we use an array of temperature metrics to uncover the rela­ changes to local weather would cause mental associations with climate
tionship between different measures of temperature and public interest change (Choi et al., 2020; Lang, 2014a). In turn, this association will
in climate change. cause them to want to gather more information on climate change.
Hypothesis 2a: Variation in local weather is positively correlated with
3. Method and material information seeking in climate change knowledge
Hypothesis 2b: Variation in local weather is positively correlated with
3.1. Conceptual framework information seeking in climate change adaptation
Hypothesis 2c: Variation in local weather has little to no effects on in­
We begin our discussion of how weather changes can prompt formation seeking in climate change mitigation
behavioral responses by using the Protection Motivation Theory (PMT). Even though Hypothesis 1 has been investigated in great detail by
The PMT was first postulated by Rogers (1975) to explain how fears of earlier literature (e.g., Lang, 2014a), we can delve deeper into the spe­
adverse outcomes and efficacy of responses work together to prompt cific climate change-related information for which the public searches.
behavioral adaptations. While PMT was initially used to explain in­ According to the PMT, individuals will first assess the severity and their
dividuals’ taking actions (or not) to avoid adverse health outcomes (e.g., susceptibility to threats posed by climate change.
smoking cessation to avoid lung cancer), this framework has been To do that, they are more likely to require general scientific infor­
increasingly applied in the environmental context to explain adoption of mation. Hypothesis 2a relates to general scientific knowledge on climate
pro-environmental behaviors (e.g., Bockarjova and Steg, 2014; Bradley change that allows individuals to assess their susceptibility.
et al., 2020; Chen, 2020; Janmaimool, 2017; Nguyen et al., 2020). Second, individuals will also need information to appraise the cost-
In the context of climate change, the PMT begins with the individual effectiveness of various coping responses toward climate change. In
appraising environmental threats, and ways to cope with these threats this regard, we formulate two additional hypotheses to examine this
(Fig. 1). phenomenon. Hypothesis 2b and 2c respectively relate to climate
First, threats appraisal consists of i) perceived severity of the envi­ adaptation, and climate mitigation measures. However, we deviate in
ronmental threat, ii) perceived susceptibility or vulnerability to the the expected effects where we hypothesize that local weather will only
environmental threat, and iii) utility gains from the environmental trigger information seeking on climate adaptation topics, and not
threat. The intuition behind factors (i) and (ii) is that individuals that climate mitigation. The rationale is as follows. Even though mitigation
assess higher levels of threats are more likely to take actions to protect or and adaptation are the two most commonly-used responses toward
insulate themselves. On the other hand, factor (iii) refers to the net climate change (IPCC, 2014), their effects are highly distinct from each
utility or ‘happiness’ gains derived from climate change or the envi­ other. There is abundant evidence showing that climate mitigation (e.g.,
ronmental threat. While climate change is largely considered detri­ adoption of electric vehicles, reduced usage of electricity) delivers
mental to human well-being, Rode et al. (2021) showed that there are benefits only in the long-term, and even so in highly contingent on
some instances where it is welfare-enhancing. For example, those others’ actions. On the other hand, adaptation-type coping responses (e.
residing in cold climates are likely to benefit from temperature increases g., adoption of air-conditioners) are much more direct and rapid in
during winter months. alleviating the adverse effects of climate change (Biesbroek et al., 2009;
Second, appraisal of coping responses is a function of i) perceived Landauer et al., 2019; Sharifi, 2020). As such, we predict that Chinese
effectiveness of response, ii) self-assessed ability to implement response residents are much more likely to seek information on climate adapta­
effectively, and iii) cost of responding. Toward this end, factors (i) and tion due to their higher coping appraisal.
(iii) are tied to a large degree to the specific weather changes that the Hypothesis 3a: The climate change-related information public seeks out is
individual is facing. For instance, air-conditioners (a climate adaptation strongly related to the types of local weather changes they are facing
strategy) are obviously much more effective compared to say electric An implicit assumption of the PMT is that individuals only need
vehicles (a predominantly climate mitigation strategy) for individuals useful information that helps to appraise both threats and coping re­
facing heatwaves or abnormally high temperatures. Factor (ii) can be sponses (Beck and Feldman, 1983; Rippetoe and Rogers, 1987). For
construed as non-pecuniary “learning costs” that one needs to incur from instance, when faced with local weather changes, it is less relevant for an
adoption of the particular behavior. For example, it has been noticed individual from China to search for information on “glaciers retreat”
that individuals are adopting relatively trivial and ineffective carbon even though this phenomenon is related to global climate change.
mitigation behaviors (such as turning off lights) as more impactful be­ Likewise, one would also expect individuals to search for adaptation
haviors are more costly from individuals’ standpoint (Huddart Kennedy responses that are relevant to the local weather changes they are
et al., 2015; Stoddard et al., 2021; Thøgersen and Crompton, 2009). experiencing. As such, in this hypothesis, we group keywords in each
It should also be noted that the PMT states these two appraisals are climate change topic into sub-categories to examine how closely infor­
executed sequentially, where the individual will only consider coping mation searches are related to the local weather experienced by
responses after a threshold level of threat appraisal is reached (Rogers, individuals.
1983). If the evaluations result in high appraisals of both threats and Hypothesis 3b: The climate change-related information public seeks out is
viability of coping mechanisms to deal with the threat, the individuals strongly related to the city’s underlying climatic conditions
will then channel these assessments into intentions, and subsequently Another dimension in which information-seeking behavior is related
behaviors (Rogers, 1983). to local conditions is via the underlying climatic conditions. Similar to
In this regard, we argue that individuals require specific and up-to- Hypothesis 3a, residents from locations where say, hot weather is
date information to appraise both environmental threats and coping regularly encountered are less likely to search for information on future
responses, and that internet search engines reveal the types of susceptibility and/or coping responses on a warm day relative to

4
J. Li et al. Global Environmental Change 81 (2023) 102698

residents from a cooler climate. The reasons for such behaviors are − 8◦ C to above 8 ◦ C, in steps of 2 ◦ C.2
twofold. First, as residents are already accustomed to the underlying Fourth, heatwaves and cold-spells are examples of extreme events
climatic conditions of their locations, it is less likely for those from warm caused by prolonged abnormal temperatures, and they may cause cu­
(cold) places to mentally associate hot(cool) days with climate change. mulative interest in climate change. As such, we define a heatwave event
In turn, they are also less likely to search for information related to as at least three consecutive days where average daily temperature ex­
future susceptibility to climate change. Second and similarly, if residents ceeds the 90th percentile value of average temperature in the same city
are conditioned to local climates, there is less need for them to search for and date for the reference period of 1981–2010. Under this construction,
coping responses when faced with weather that they are familiar with the first day of a heatwave event will take a value of one, the second day
(Paton et al., 2017). will take a value of two, and as follows. Similarly, a cold-spell is defined
as at least three consecutive days where average temperature is below
the 10th percentile value of the average level in the reference period.
3.2. Empirical approach
Days without heatwave or cold-wave are assigned a value of zero.
Other than temperature, we also include a host of covariates to
We use the following fixed-effects regression model to test the hy­
control for other factors that may affect internet search volume or public
potheses described in Section 3.1:
interest in climate change. First, Wit is a vector of daily weather controls
ln(SI it ) = β0 + f (temit )β + Wit + λt + δit + εit (1) which are included up to quadratic terms. They are total precipitation,
relative humidity, windspeed, hours of sunlight, and atmospheric pres­
The dependent variable in E. (1) is log-transformed SIit which refers sure. Moreover, we include a highly flexible series of calendar-date fixed
to the Baidu search index in city i on calendar date t for keywords related effects, λt , where 1st January 2011 is a different fixed effect from 1st
to climate change. Similar to earlier studies that use the same approach January 2012. These highly granular fixed-effects encompass all time-
(e.g., Choi et al., 2020; Lang, 2014b; Osberghaus and Demski, 2019; Qin related factors including year, month, day-of-week, and public holi­
and Zhu, 2018), we are assuming that internet searches are indicative of days, which serve to control for any national-level temporal confound­
public interest or information seeking in the corresponding topic or ing factors, e.g., public search behavior may vary on weekends, summer
behaviors. holidays, or festivals. Additionally, we also include city-by-year-month
The main explanatory variable is f(temit ), which we construct in four fixed effects denoted by δit . These fixed effects serve two purposes.
different ways to represent ambient temperature. First, they control for any factors fixed at the city level, e.g., size, loca­
First, we include daily average temperature up until quadratic form. tion, and climatic zones. Second, as we interact city with year, they also
This expression allows temperature to have a non-linear relationship control for any city factors that statistically vary at an annual level, e.g.,
with search volumes and has been utilized in many studies empirically regional GDP, population, and the proportion of internet users. In this
examining the impact of climate change on human outcomes (e.g., regard, city-level time-varying fixed effects allow us to flexibly control
Dasgupta et al., 2021). for any city-specific trends.
Second, we categorize daily ambient temperature into one of seven
mutually exclusive temperature intervals. These intervals are in steps of
5 ◦ C and range from below 5 ◦ C to above 30 ◦ C. If city i on calendar date t 3.3. Dataset
has a daily average temperature of 28 ◦ C, the interval of 25 ◦ C to 30 ◦ C
will record a value of one, while the other temperature intervals will The dataset used in this analysis is assembled from two sources.
take value of zero.1 This setup allows us to flexibly estimate a non- First, to gauge public interest in various climate change topics and
parametric relationship between daily temperature and public’s inter­ behaviors, we use daily search volume from the most popular internet
est in climate change. search engine in China – Baidu. Throughout the timespan of our dataset
A third way to define daily ambient temperature is by using tem­ from 2011 to 2020, Baidu is the most-used search engine in China with a
perature anomalies (e.g., Moore et al., 2019; Obradovich et al., 2017; market share ranging from around 60% in the earlier part of the decade
Wang et al., 2020). There is abundant evidence showing that humans to its current 80%.3 Similarly, due to the widespread usage of Baidu, our
can adapt to the underlying climatic conditions at their location (Howe dataset covers 329 cities accounting for around 96% of the country’s
et al., 2013; Lam et al., 2019). As such, it is plausible that daily tem­ population.
perature will only cause increased public attention if it deviates from the Specifically, we use Baidu Index – the equivalent of its international
historical norm. In this regard, we define temperature anomalies as counterpart of Google Trends – to measure the daily total search volume
for a city. Just as Google Trends where its exact computation is pro­
Tanomaliesit = Tit − Tit(1981− (2)
prietary information, Baidu has not released how its indices are
2010)

computed. However, it is public knowledge that the search indices are


where Tit is the daily average temperature for city i on date t, and
representative of daily search volume in a city, and are comparable over
Tit(1981− 2010) is the mean value of daily average temperature for dates t
time and location.4 Currently, Baidu Index has been used in numerous
from 1981 to 2010 in city i (in comparison, the time period of the search
research studies across different disciplines to proxy for Chinese public
index is from 2011 to 2020). The reason behind this construction is that
interest and attention toward specific topics, such as public health (He
residents of city i use historical averages to form expectations about
et al., 2018; Zhong et al., 2019), and tourism (Huang et al., 2017; Yang
temperature. As such, positive anomalies are “hotter-than-expected”
et al., 2015). In addition, on environmental topics, many studies used
days, and negative anomalies are “cooler-than-expected” days. The main
Baidu Index to assess how Chinese residents protect themselves against
difference between using temperature anomalies and temperature levels
as explanatory variables is that the latter do not presume information
seeking or public interest in climate change are elevated only at certain 2
As each day in any city is certain to fall into one of the seven temperature
temperature levels (e.g., above 30 ◦ C). Similar to temperature intervals, anomalies intervals, we need to exclude an interval for statistical identification.
we also define temperature anomalies as binary bins ranging from below Toward this end, we exclude the range of − 2 ◦ C to 0 ◦ C, and all temperature
anomalies coefficients are interpreted with respect to this category.
3
“Search Engine Market Share in China” https://gs.statcounter.com/search
1
As each day in any city is certain to fall into one of the seven temperature -engine-market-share/all/china/. Last accessed: Aug 24, 2022.
4
intervals, we need to exclude an interval for statistical identification. Toward According to Qin and Zhu (2018), the correlation between Baidu Index and
this end, we exclude the range of below 5 ◦ C, and all temperature coefficients actual search volume is most likely linear. We have also corresponded with
are interpreted with respect to this category. Baidu to confirm this assertion (correspondence available upon request).

5
J. Li et al. Global Environmental Change 81 (2023) 102698

Table 1 Table 2
Climate change topics keywords. Summary statistics.
Climate Keywords Sub-topics Variable Mean SD Min Max
change topics
Climate knowledge search index 99.16 180.30 0 3,000
Knowledge Global warming, Greenhouse effect, Direct causes of climate Climate adaptation daily search index 143.01 208.08 0 6,138
Global climate warming, Climate change, and temperature- Climate mitigation daily search index 154.70 302.30 0 63,075
change, Greenhouse gases. related impacts Daily average temperature (◦ C) 14.22 11.20 − 38.58 35.85
El Nino, Glacier melting, Sea level Secondary impacts of
rise climate change
Binary temperature <5 ◦ C 0.21 0.41 0 1
Acid rain, Ozone hole Other atmospheric
intervals 5 ◦ C to 0.12 0.32 0 1
pollution
10 ◦ C
10 ◦ C 0.14 0.34 0 1
Adaptation Air-conditioners, Electric fan, Direct heat-related to15 ◦ C
Heatstroke prevention, Heat- adaptation 15 ◦ C to 0.16 0.37 0 1
avoidance travels 20 ◦ C
Dehumidifier, Clothing index, More general adaptation 20 ◦ C to 0.19 0.40 0 1
Weather warning, Real-time 25 ◦ C
weather 25 ◦ C to 0.15 0.36 0 1
Weather insurance, Emergency Extreme events adaptation 30 ◦ C
bags, Emergency supplies >30 ◦ C 0.02 0.15 0 1

Mitigation Emissions reduction through N.A. Binary temperature <− 8 ◦ C 0.01 0.08 0 1
energy savings, Reduced anomalies intervals − 6 ◦ C to 0.02 0.13 0 1
consumption through energy − 8 ◦C
savings, Energy conservation, − 4 ◦ C to 0.05 0.22 0 1
Green transportation, Low- − 6 ◦C
emissions vehicles, Low-carbon − 2 ◦ C to 0.12 0.33 0 1
lifestyle, Recycling, Vegetarian − 4 ◦C
diet, Water conservation, Waste − 2 ◦ C to 0.24 0.42 0 1
sorting 0 ◦C
0 ◦ C to 2 0.28 0.45 0 1
Notes: The keywords are the top-10 most commonly searched terms for each ◦
C
climate change topic in Baidu. 2 ◦ C to 4 0.18 0.38 0 1

C
4 ◦ C to 6 0.07 0.26 0 1
air pollution. For example, Qin and Zhu (2018) found that searches on ◦
C
“emigration” increase on days with poor air quality. Similarly, Dong 6 ◦ C to 8 0.02 0.15 0 1
et al. (2019) and Li et al. (2021) found that searches on “air quality” ◦
C
increase on days when air quality index rose. >8 ◦ C 0.01 0.10 0 1
As a key departure of this study from earlier efforts is that instead of
treating “climate change” as a singular topic, we define and measure Heatwaves (days) 1.62 3.98 0 41
search volumes for different aspects of climate change. To do so, we first Cold-spells (days) 1.16 5.61 0 201
determine to focus on the knowledge (K), mitigation (M), and adapta­ Relative humidity (%) 67.99 18.03 4.22 100
Daily precipitation (mm) 47.96 1,624.83 0 90,699
tion (A) aspects of climate change. The reason is that each of these Daily hours of sunlight 5.57 4 0 15.48
climate change topics is distinct from the others, and they correspond to Sea pressure (kpa) 95 8.65 57.45 104.34
the various types of information needed to assess threats and coping Wind speed (m/second) 2.11 1.04 0 18.99
responses as theorized in the PMT framework. Second, as there are Notes: Number of obs. = 1,155,870. Search index data obtained from Baidu
multiple phrases and expressions to represent the same concept, it is Index. Daily weather data obtained from China Meteorological Data Service
conceivable that different users may use different search terms even if Center. Temperature anomalies is defined as the difference between current
they are looking for the same set of information. In this regard, we utilize temperature on date t and the average temperature for date t across the past 30
a feature from Baidu Index that yields the 10 most closely related key­ years for each city. Heatwave is included as number of days, and is defined as at
words to a search phrase to help select and narrow down to a feasible set least three consecutive days where daily temperature is above 90th percentile of
of keywords. For instance, to construct the set of keywords for K, we historical temperatures. Cold-spell is included as number of days, and is defined
begin with the phrase “climate change”, obtain the 10 most relevant as at least three consecutive days where daily temperature is below 10th
percentile of historical temperatures.
phrases, and repeat this process ten times. In all, we generated a list of
110 keywords, from which we selected the 10 most frequently repeated
search phrases. The specific keywords used to represent each of the three volume as the coefficient of variation (i.e., ratio of standard deviation to
topics are shown in Table 1, and their corresponding topical Baidu Index mean) is between 1.5 and 2.
is the average search volume for their associated keywords. In view of the wide dispersion in search volume, it is thus useful to
The second source of data is daily weather information obtained gain an overall view of how searches on each of these topics have
from 337 ground weather stations managed by China Meteorological changed over time. To do so, we obtain the de-meaned time trend
Data Service Center. Consistent with previous studies, we use inverse through regression analyses. From the search volume time trends plotted
distance weighting to attribute weather for each city in the dataset in Fig. 2, we see that search volume on each of the three topics started off
(Currie and Neidell, 2005; Deschenes and Greenstone, 2007; Schlenker all around the same level in 2011 but experienced diverse trends after­
and Walker, 2015). ward. Public interest in climate knowledge and mitigation steadily
The summary statistics are presented in Table 2. On average, the decline up until around 2015 when it resumes an upward trend. In
daily Baidu Index for knowledge, adaptation, and mitigation are 99.2, contrast, information seeking in climate adaptation topics started on an
142.6, and 154.3 respectively. This means that on any average day, upward trend around early 2013. However, the interest peaked around
there are more searches for climate mitigation than for climate knowl­ 2017 and remains on a downward trend since then.
edge or adaptation. We also observe wide dispersion in daily search In all, this simple graphical depiction shows that public interest in
different types of climate change topics does not share similar trends or

6
J. Li et al. Global Environmental Change 81 (2023) 102698

Fig. 2. Search volume of different climate change topics over time. Notes: This graph shows the de-meaned daily search volume for each topic. The faint dots
represent observations for each day and topic, and the trend lines are fitted for each topic.

Table 3
Climate change information seeking.
Dep. var. log-transformed climate change search index

(1) (2) (3) (4)

Main explanatory var. Avg temperature Temperature intervals Temperature anomalies Heatwaves & cold-spells

Average temperature 0.001


Temperature-square 0.001***

Binary temperature intervals 5 ◦ C to 10 ◦ C − 0.054***


10 ◦ C to15◦ C − 0.096***
15 ◦ C to 20 ◦ C − 0.101***
20 ◦ C to 25 ◦ C − 0.020
25 ◦ C to 30 ◦ C 0.168***
>30 ◦ C 0.408***

Binary temperature anomalies intervals ≤-8◦ C 0.105***


− 6◦ C to − 8◦ C 0.062***
− 4◦ C to − 6◦ C 0.023***
− 2◦ C to − 4◦ C − 0.008*
0 ◦ C to 2 ◦ C 0.023***
2 ◦ C to 4 ◦ C 0.073***
4 ◦ C to 6 ◦ C 0.111***
6 ◦ C to 8 ◦ C 0.146***
>8◦ C 0.192***

Heatwave 0.029***
Cold-spell 0.0003**
Constant 5.971** 9.096*** 3.015 6.173***
Adj. R-squared 0.751 0.751 0.751 0.751

Notes: Number of obs. = 1,155,870. All regressions are estimated with calendar date fixed effects, city-year-month fixed effects, and weather controls (precipitation,
sea level pressure, relative humidity, wind speed, hours of sunlight, and all of their quadratics). All coefficients in Column (2) should be interpreted with respect to the
excluded temperature interval of below 5 ◦ C. Temperature anomalies is defined as the difference between current temperature on date t and the average temperature
for date t across the past 30 years for each city. All coefficients in Column (3) should be interpreted with respect to the excluded temperature anomalies interval of 0 ◦ C
to − 2◦ C. Heatwave is included as number of days, and is defined as at least three consecutive days where daily temperature is above 90th percentile of historical
temperatures. Cold-spell is included as number of days, and is defined as at least three consecutive days where daily temperature is below 10th percentile of historical
temperatures. Standard errors (not shown in table due to space constraints) are clustered at city-level. *** p < 0.01, ** p < 0.05, * p < 0.1.

7
J. Li et al. Global Environmental Change 81 (2023) 102698

Table 4
Climate change information seeking by topics.
Dep. var. Climate Climate Climate Climate Climate Climate mitigation
knowledge adaptation mitigation knowledge adaptation
(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6)

Main explanatory var. Avg temperature Temperature intervals

Average temperature − 0.001 − 0.002 0.003**


(0.001) (0.002) (0.001)
Temperature-square 0.001*** 0.001*** − 0.000**
(0.000) (0.000) (0.000)

Binary temperature 5 ◦ C to 10 ◦ C − 0.039*** − 0.093*** 0.009


intervals (0.010) (0.010) (0.010)
10 ◦ C to 15 ◦ C − 0.055*** − 0.173*** 0.011
(0.013) (0.014) (0.013)
15 ◦ C to 20 ◦ C − 0.062*** − 0.172*** 0.007
(0.016) (0.017) (0.016)
20 ◦ C to 25 ◦ C − 0.052*** 0.036* 0.002
(0.019) (0.021) (0.018)
25 ◦ C to 30 ◦ C − 0.001 0.404*** − 0.005
(0.022) (0.027) (0.020)
>30 ◦ C 0.250*** 0.739*** 0.038
(0.029) (0.034) (0.024)

Constant 4.497 4.854 8.154*** 5.970** 8.321* 9.207***


(2.752) (4.602) (2.459) (2.727) (4.326) (2.411)

Observations 1,155,870 1,159,156 1,159,156 1,155,870 1,159,156 1,159,156


Adj. R-squared 0.593 0.674 0.636 0.593 0.674 0.636

Notes: All regressions are estimated with calendar date fixed effects, city-year-month fixed effects, and weather controls (precipitation, sea level pressure, relative
humidity, wind speed, hours of sunlight, and all of their quadratics). All coefficients in Columns (4) to (6) should be interpreted with respect to the excluded tem­
perature interval of below 5 ◦ C. Standard errors in parentheses, and are clustered at city-level. *** p < 0.01, ** p < 0.05, * p < 0.1.

paths, in turn, this also means that it is likely that local weather changes 30 ◦ C.
will induce public interest in different types of climate change topics. Third, the results presented until now consistently show that public
The average daily temperature is around 14.2 ◦ C, reflecting China’s interest in climate change topics is generally positively associated with
mostly temperate climatic conditions. However, similar to search vol­ temperature. However, China is a large country with highly varied cli­
ume, there is large variation in daily temperature as they range from matic zones (Lam et al., 2005). As such, it is unlikely that the entire
− 39 ◦ C to 36 ◦ C. Not surprisingly, distribution of temperature anomalies country shares a similar temperature-outcome profile. For instance,
is U-shaped as more than 50% of days fall between − 2 ◦ C below ex­ daily averages of 30 ◦ C are commonplace in the subtropical Southern
pected to 2 ◦ C above expected. However, we detect signs of global parts of China whereas it is considered an unusually warm day in the
warming as more days fall on the right-side of the temperature anom­ Northern parts of the country. To account for regional heterogeneity in
alies distribution (i.e., hotter-than-expected) compared to the left-side baseline climates, we now express daily temperature as temperature
(i.e., colder-than-expected). anomalies (i.e., deviation from historical average temperature). The
results in Column (3) of Table 3 show a U-shaped outcome where search
4. Results volume on climate change is highest on extreme ends of temperature
anomalies. However, the effects of unexpected cold and hot tempera­
4.1. Overall searches tures are uneven, with the latter generating much more information
seeking. Specifically, relative to a day of no temperature anomalies, a
We test Hypothesis 1 by examining the relationship between daily highly anomalous cold day (>8 ◦ C colder than expected) will elicit
ambient temperature and public interest in the average search volume additional 11% searches on climate change. On the other end of the
across all climate change-related topics. spectrum, a highly anomalous warm day (>8 ◦ C hotter than expected)
First, we include temperature as a continuous variable up until its also causes more interest in climate change at around 21% more
quadratic form. The results in Column (1) of Table 3 show that search searches.
volume for climate change increases at an increasing rate with tem­ Fourth, it is possible that extended periods of extreme or anomalous
perature. For instance, a 1 ◦ C increase in China’s annual mean temper­ temperatures have accumulative impacts on public interest in climate
ature of 14 ◦ C is associated with a 2% increase in search volume whereas change. To test this assertion, we define periods of heatwaves and cold-
a 1 ◦ C increase at a temperature of 30 ◦ C is associated with a 4.3% in­ spells as at least three continuous days where the average temperature is
crease in search volume. respectively above the 90th percentile and below the 10th percentile.
Second, to allow for a more flexible relationship between tempera­ Toward this end, we find that heatwaves not only increase search vol­
ture and search activities, we include temperature as intervals (Table 3, ume on climate change, but the effect is amplified as the event prolongs
Column (2)). Compared to the excluded interval of under 5 ◦ C, search (Table 3, Column (4)). Specifically, searches increase by 2.9% on the
volume on climate change decreases on days where average temperature first day of a heatwave, followed by 5.8% on the second, and so on.
is between 5 ◦ C and 25 ◦ C. However, China’s public interest in climate While we also recover statistically significant results for cold-spells, the
change surges exponentially on days with average temperature beyond effects are much smaller. Searches on climate change increase by 0.03%
25 ◦ C. Specifically, there are large increases of 18.3% in searches for on the first day of a cold-spell, followed by 0.06% on the second day, and
climate change on days between 25 and 30 ◦ C, and 50.4% on days above so on.

8
J. Li et al. Global Environmental Change 81 (2023) 102698

Table 5
Climate change information seeking by topics.
Dep. var. Climate Climate Climate Climate Climate Climate
knowledge adaptation mitigation knowledge adaptation mitigation
(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6)

Main explanatory var. Temperature anomalies intervals Heatwaves and cold-spells

Binary temperature anomalies <− 8 ◦ C 0.170*** 0.168*** − 0.017


intervals (0.024) (0.025) (0.023)
− 6 ◦ C to − 8 0.137*** 0.075*** − 0.015

C
(0.016) (0.015) (0.014)
− 4 ◦ C to − 6 0.072*** 0.030*** − 0.011

C
(0.009) (0.009) (0.008)
− 2 ◦ C to − 4 0.023*** − 0.014** − 0.012**

C
(0.006) (0.005) (0.005)
0 ◦ C to 2 ◦ C 0.001 0.050*** − 0.002
(0.004) (0.005) (0.004)
2 ◦ C to 4 ◦ C 0.033*** 0.136*** 0.000
(0.006) (0.007) (0.006)
4 ◦ C to 6 ◦ C 0.091*** 0.201*** − 0.003
(0.009) (0.009) (0.008)
6 ◦ C to 8 ◦ C 0.152*** 0.267*** − 0.006
(0.014) (0.013) (0.012)
>8 ◦ C 0.219*** 0.314*** 0.006
(0.022) (0.021) (0.019)
Heatwave 0.035*** 0.036*** 0.006***
(0.004) (0.004) (0.002)
Cold-spell 0.001*** 0.000 0.000
(0.000) (0.000) (0.000)
Constant 3.884 -2.554 8.836*** 4.410 3.339 9.194***
(2.717) (4.376) (2.425) (2.710) (4.436) (2.408)
Observations 1,155,870 1,159,156 1,159,156 1,155,870 1,159,156 1,159,156
Adj. R-squared 0.593 0.673 0.636 0.593 0.672 0.636

Notes: All regressions are estimated with calendar date fixed effects, city-year-month fixed effects, and weather controls (precipitation, sea level pressure, relative
humidity, wind speed, hours of sunlight, and all of their quadratics). Temperature anomalies is defined as the difference between current temperature on date t and the
average temperature for date t across the past 30 years for each city. All coefficients in Columns (1) to (3) should be interpreted with respect to the excluded tem­
perature anomalies interval of 0 ◦ C to − 2◦ C. Heatwave is included as number of days, and is defined as at least three consecutive days where daily temperature is above
90th percentile of historical temperatures. Cold-spell is included as number of days, and is defined as at least three consecutive days where daily temperature is below
10th percentile of historical temperatures. Standard errors in parentheses, and are clustered at city-level. *** p < 0.01, ** p < 0.05, * p < 0.1.

Our findings so far are largely consistent with existing literature. both increase by large amounts – 28.4% and 109.3% respectively. In
That is, we affirm Hypothesis 1 as Chinese residents’ interests in climate contrast, searches for M increase by only 3.9% and are not statistically
change-related information are positively correlated with variation in significant even at the 10% level.
local daily temperature. While these effects are observed for both hot Third, we now express daily temperature in terms of anomalies in­
and cold days, the results are especially strong for the former. tervals (Table 5, Columns (1) to (3)). We find that search volume for A
increases by 18% on a day that is more than 8 ◦ C below expected tem­
perature. A hotter-than-expected day of more than 8 ◦ C above expected
4.2. Results by topics temperature elicits much more searches at around 37% increase. In­
formation seeking for K is also positively associated with temperature
Even though climate change is a highly diverse and multi-faceted anomalies at around 19% for an anomalously cold day of more than 8 ◦ C
topic, earlier studies in this literature mostly did not distinguish be­ below the expected temperature. However, an interesting dichotomy
tween different topics in climate change. More importantly, as seen from earlier results is that search volume for K increases by a similar
earlier, information seeking in the three related but distinct climate magnitude of 24% for anomalously hot days of more than 8 ◦ C above
topics of i) knowledge (K), ii) adaptation (A), and iii) mitigation (M), are expected temperature. Consistent with Hypothesis 2 and earlier results,
not correlated over time. we find that information seeking in M is not triggered by temperature
In this sub-section, we now test the set of Hypothesis 2 by repeating anomalies.
the earlier empirical analyses on each of the three climate change topics. Fourth, we examine how searches in each of these three topics are
First, when temperature is included as quadratic terms, we see that affected by heatwaves and cold-spells. We first focus on heatwaves.
search volume for A and K both increase at an increasing rate with Public interest in A and K are most sensitive toward heatwaves as the
respect to daily temperature (Table 4, Columns (1) to (3)). For instance, first day of a heatwave increase search volume by around 3.5%. In
the marginal impact of temperature on A’s search volume is 4.36% at 14 comparison, the effects on M are modest at around 0.6%. On the other

C. However, this impact increases to 9.4% at 30 ◦ C. While searches on K hand, cold-spells either do not have a sizable or statistically significant
also grow at an increasing rate with temperature, the rate of increase is impact on searches (Table 5, Columns (4) to (6)).
much slower. In contrast, search volume on M has an entirely different Lastly, one plausible explanation for the relative lack of public in­
relationship as marginal impact decreases as temperature increases. terest on M could be due to short attention span on climate-related topics
Second, we include temperature as intervals. For brevity sake, we (Sisco et al., 2017a). We test this possibility by constructing three-day
will only discuss the impact on days where average temperature is above
30 ◦ C (Table 4, Columns (4) to (6)). Relative to a temperature-below-5

C day, search volumes for K and A on a temperature-above-30 ◦ C day

9
J. Li et al. Global Environmental Change 81 (2023) 102698

Fig. 3. Temperature coefficients for sub-


categories of climate knowledge and
climate adaptation keywords. Notes: All co­
efficients in Panel (A) should be interpreted
with respect to the excluded temperature
interval of below 5 ◦ C. Temperature anoma­
lies is defined as the difference between
current temperature on date t and the
average temperature for date t across the
past 30 years for each city. All coefficients in
Panel (B) should be interpreted with respect
to the excluded temperature anomalies in­
terval of 0 ◦ C to − 2◦ C. The vertical lines
indicate 95% confidence intervals.

Fig. 4. Climate classification zones across China. Notes: The blank areas in this figure are cities not represented in our dataset. These are mostly remote areas with
sparse population. The cities in our dataset account for around 96% of the China’s population.

10
J. Li et al. Global Environmental Change 81 (2023) 102698

Fig. 5. Temperature coefficients for each climate topic across different climatic zones. Notes: All coefficients in Panel (A) should be interpreted with respect to the
excluded temperature interval of below 5 ◦ C. Temperature anomalies is defined as the difference between current temperature on date t and the average temperature
for date t across the past 30 years for each city. All coefficients in Panel (B) should be interpreted with respect to the excluded temperature anomalies interval of 0 ◦ C
to − 2◦ C. The vertical lines indicate 95% confidence intervals.

Fig. A1. Temperature coefficients for each climate topic across different time frequencies. Notes: All coefficients should be interpreted with respect to the excluded
temperature interval of below 5 ◦ C. The vertical lines indicate 95% confidence intervals.

11
J. Li et al. Global Environmental Change 81 (2023) 102698

Table A1
Climate change information seeking by sub-topics.
Dep. var. Knowledge: Global Knowledge: Other climate Knowledge: Other Adaptation: Adaptation: Adaptation: Extreme
warming impacts pollution Heat General events

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6)


Average 0.002*** − 0.003*** − 0.000 − 0.002 0.001 − 0.000
temperature
(0.001) (0.001) (0.001) (0.002) (0.001) (0.000)
Temperature- 0.000*** 0.000*** 0.000 0.001*** 0.000*** 0.000
square
(0.000) (0.000) (0.000) (0.000) (0.000) (0.000)
Constant 3.117** 2.735 2.531 4.505 − 10.795*** 0.153
(1.338) (1.688) (1.549) (3.041) (1.796) (0.659)
Observations 1,155,870 1,155,870 1,155,870 1,159,156 1,159,156 1,159,156
Adj. R-squared 0.600 0.572 0.491 0.683 0.608 0.258

Notes: All regressions are estimated with calendar date fixed effects, city-year-month fixed effects, and weather controls (precipitation, sea level pressure, relative
humidity, wind speed, hours of sunlight, and all of their quadratics). Standard errors in parentheses, and are clustered at city-level. *** p < 0.01, ** p < 0.05, * p < 0.1.

Table A2
Climate change information seeking by sub-topics.
Dep. var. Knowledge: Global Knowledge: Other climate Knowledge: Other Adaptation: Adaptation: Adaptation: Extreme
warming impacts pollution Heat General events

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6)


Heatwave 0.028*** 0.019*** 0.000 0.035*** 0.005*** − 0.000
(0.003) (0.002) (0.002) (0.003) (0.002) (0.001)
Cold-spell 0.000 0.000*** − 0.000 0.000** − 0.000** − 0.000
(0.000) (0.000) (0.000) (0.000) (0.000) (0.000)
Constant 4.011*** 1.626 2.361 3.004 − 10.751*** 0.060
(1.310) (1.649) (1.529) (2.843) (1.764) (0.645)
Observations 1,155,870 1,155,870 1,155,870 1,159,156 1,159,156 1,159,156
Adj. R- 0.600 0.572 0.491 0.681 0.608 0.258
squared

Notes: All regressions are estimated with calendar date fixed effects, city-year-month fixed effects, and weather controls (precipitation, sea level pressure, relative
humidity, wind speed, hours of sunlight, and all of their quadratics). Heatwave is included as number of days, and is defined as at least three consecutive days where
daily temperature is above 90th percentile of historical temperatures. Cold-spell is included as number of days, and is defined as at least three consecutive days where
daily temperature is below 10th percentile of historical temperatures. Standard errors in parentheses, and are clustered at city-level. *** p < 0.01, ** p < 0.05, * p < 0.1.

and five-day moving averages of search volume for each topic.5 In this 4.3. Results by search keywords
regard, by comparing the coefficients for temperature bins across
different time frequencies, we can discern how attention-span changes. While earlier studies conclude that exposure to local weather
The results in Fig. A1 show that attention-span weakens (decrease in changes will prompt information seeking on climate change, we expand
coefficients’ magnitude) as we move from daily (or one-day moving upon this literature to show that the Chinese public seeks information in
average) to three- and five-day moving averages. The decrease in a highly purposeful manner. That is, individuals are much more likely to
magnitude is especially obvious at high temperature intervals. When seek out climate knowledge information that allows them to assess
compared across topics, we find that attention-span erodes at the fastest current and future susceptibility, and climate adaptation information
rate for M (at the hottest temperature interval), followed by K, and A. that offers direct protection or alleviation against adverse weather. In
These results tentatively supports Sisco et al. (2017a) as we may indeed this sub-section, we further segregate keywords from each topic into
observe extreme temperature having statistically significant and posi­ sub-categories based on their relevance to the local temperature
tive impacts on M if higher-frequency data is available (e.g., hourly). conditions.
However, the clear disparity in coefficients’ size indicates that even at If individuals are indeed searching for information to appraise
more granular time periods, the impact of temperature on search volume threats and coping responses according to the PMT, then it should also
will still be largest for A, followed by K and M topics. be the case that the information they are looking out for is directly
To summarize, this set of results confirms Hypothesis 2. Due to dif­ related to the local weather. On the other hand, if we observe that the
ferences in their coping effectiveness, we see that public interest is not Chinese public is also searching for information that is tangentially
evenly distributed across the three climate change topics. Specifically, related to climate change, then we would be less sure of PMT being an
we consistently find that local weather changes have much larger im­ adequate framework to examine how local weather affects climate
pacts on information seeking on climate adaptation, and climate change-related behaviors.
knowledge. On the other hand, there are little to no effects on climate To test Hypothesis 3a, we first separate the keywords in the topic of
mitigation behaviors. Knowledge into three sub-categories of i) global warming and temper­
ature changes, ii) secondary climate impacts, and iii) other atmospheric
pollution. Similarly, keywords in Adaptation are sorted into three sub-
categories of i) adaptation to high heat, ii) general adaptation to tem­
perature, and iii) adaptation to extreme events (see Table 1 for the
5
The rationale is that the daily-level analyses presented so far show the keywords in each sub-category). These keywords are grouped in a
immediate response of temperature on public interests on climate change. descending manner where the higher-ordered categories contain words
However, when a three-day moving average is used as the dependent variable, or phrases that are much more related to local temperature.
the model will now capture longer-term effects of temperature on, say, 1st For the sake of brevity, we will only present and discuss results using
January, on search volume from 1st January to 3rd January.

12
J. Li et al. Global Environmental Change 81 (2023) 102698

Table A3
Climate change information seeking by climatic zones.
Dep. var. Severe cold Cold Mild Hot summers and cold winters Hot summers and mild winters

Panel A. Climate knowledge


Average temperature 0.003 0.001 − 0.007 − 0.021*** − 0.014
(0.002) (0.003) (0.009) (0.005) (0.012)
Temperature-square 0.000* 0.000*** 0.001*** 0.001*** 0.000
(0.000) (0.000) (0.000) (0.000) (0.000)
Constant 1.596 − 1.884 − 14.968 22.407 − 20.737
(5.908) (3.080) (31.579) (16.597) (15.130)
Observations 176,295 334,787 84,436 398,217 162,135
Adj. R-squared 0.571 0.636 0.540 0.535 0.617

Panel B. Climate adaptation


Average temperature 0.011*** − 0.016*** − 0.131*** − 0.077*** − 0.146***
(0.002) (0.003) (0.018) (0.005) (0.016)
Temperature-square 0.001*** 0.002*** 0.006*** 0.003*** 0.005***
(0.000) (0.000) (0.001) (0.000) (0.000)
Constant − 0.464 1.853 − 7.655 39.051* − 13.360
(7.956) (5.713) (20.036) (21.921) (26.961)
Observations 176,295 334,787 84,436 398,217 162,135
Adj. R-squared 0.593 0.754 0.580 0.565 0.670

Panel C. Climate mitigation


Average temperature − 0.001 0.003 0.022** − 0.001 0.015
(0.002) (0.002) (0.010) (0.004) (0.009)
Temperature-square − 0.000 − 0.000* − 0.000* 0.000 − 0.000*
(0.000) (0.000) (0.000) (0.000) (0.000)
Constant 1.153 7.996** 9.954 21.656 6.343
(4.475) (3.521) (29.823) (14.856) (13.041)
Observations 176,295 334,787 84,436 398,217 162,135
Adj. R-squared 0.578 0.721 0.571 0.564 0.635

Notes: All regressions are estimated with calendar date fixed effects, city-year-month fixed effects, and weather controls (precipitation, sea level pressure, relative
humidity, wind speed, hours of sunlight, and all of their quadratics).

temperature intervals and temperature anomalies.6 4.4. Results by underlying climatic conditions
First, we plot the coefficients for each temperature interval in Fig. 3
to provide an overall view of how information seeking is affected across Another dimension in which we can show that information seeking
all temperature bins. The left-hand-side graph on Panel A shows the on climate change is dependent on local conditions is by exploiting
results for keywords related to climate knowledge. Just as the earlier spatial variation in climatic conditions. For instance, it is less likely for
results, we observe a lot more information seeking on hot days. How­ warm days to prompt notions of climate change and searches for coping
ever, it is clear that most of these searches are prompted by climate responses if residents in that location are already accustomed to high
change keywords that are most related to high temperature. On the temperature.
other hand, other climate knowledge-related keywords that have lesser In this regard, we test Hypothesis 3b by first categorizing Chinese
connections to high temperature or temperature changes are not as cities into one of five climate classification zones as defined by the
frequently searched by Chinese residents. country’s official building code (Ministry of Housing and Urban-Rural
Second, the right-hand-side graph on Panel A shows similar out­ Development of the People’s Republic of China, 2016). The Chinese
comes for keywords related to climate adaptation. As predicted by the government uses these classifications to determine appropriate building
PMT, we see that Chinese residents’ information-seeking behaviors are codes for each region. The entire country is segregated into five climatic
mostly motivated by the need for coping with high temperatures, which zones based on the average weather conditions they experience in each
means that keywords more related to heat adaptation are much more season: i) severe cold, ii) cold, iii) mild, iv) cold winters and warm
commonly searched for. For instance, on a temperature-above-30 ◦ C summers, and v) mild winters and warm summers (Fig. 4).
day, keywords on heat adaptation increased by more than 100%. On the Based on these classifications, Hypothesis 3b predicts that residents
other hand, adaptation keywords on general weather adaptation from the colder or less warm regions, i.e., climatic zones (i), (ii), and
increased by only 16%. (iii), are much more likely to seek information on climate change on
Third, the results for temperature anomalies again reflect the earlier warm days. On the other hand, residents from the hotter zones (iv) and
findings (Fig. 3, Panel B). We observe a U-shaped relationship between (v) are less likely to require additional information to appraise suscep­
search volume and temperature anomalies where the extreme ends elicit tibility and coping responses for warm days due to their underlying
more information-seeking behaviors. Importantly, we see that keywords climatic conditions.
most related to high temperature or temperature changes have the Similar to the earlier section, we will only present results using
largest coefficients. Not surprisingly, this is especially true for positive temperature intervals and temperature anomalies.7 Panel A of Fig. 5
temperature anomalies or “hotter-than-expected” weather. plots temperature coefficients for each interval across five climatic zones
and three climate-related topics. As hypothesized, we see a very clear
pattern where search activities for climate knowledge and adaptation
have the largest coefficients on warm days in climatic zones where hot

6 7
The other results obtained using continuous temperature, and heatwaves The other results obtained using continuous temperature, and heatwaves
and cold-spells offer similar conclusions, and are available in Tables A1 and A2. and cold-spells offer similar conclusions, and are available in Tables A3 and A4.

13
J. Li et al. Global Environmental Change 81 (2023) 102698

Table A4 researchers explored if objective and observable factors can better


Climate change information seeking by climatic zones. persuade the public into believing the phenomenon of climate change,
Dep. var. Severe Cold Mild Hot Hot thereby increasing support for mitigation and adaptation policies. Due
cold summers summers to its saliency, one of the most commonly studied objective factors is
and cold and mild local weather trends. The rationale is that since climate is inherently
winters winters
related to short-term weather, personal experiences of fluctuations in
Panel A. the latter should remind the public of climate change (Whitmarsh,
Climate 2008). Indeed, there are many studies affirming that public support and
knowledge
Heatwave 0.134*** 0.105*** − 0.002 0.019*** 0.028***
interest in climate change increases during occurrences of anomalous
(0.041) (0.021) (0.017) (0.004) (0.007) temperature and/or extreme weather events (e.g., Lang, 2014a; Sisco
Cold-spell − 0.000 0.000 − 0.003 − 0.001 0.021 et al., 2017b). However, despite record-breaking temperatures across
(0.000) (0.000) (0.003) (0.002) (0.049) the world and increased frequencies of hazardous weather events over
Constant 3.392 − 0.693 − 8.537 34.389** − 20.498
the past decades, we still do not observe a translation of climate change
(5.938) (2.991) (30.183) (16.284) (15.854)
Observations 176,295 334,787 84,436 398,217 162,135 awareness into stronger political support for climate mitigation policies
Adj. R- 0.571 0.636 0.540 0.535 0.617 or reinforcement of individuals’ mitigation efforts (Stoddard et al.,
squared 2021).
In this regard, the main objective of this study is to examine the
Panel B. conundrum of why changing weather patterns fail to translate into
Climate greater public support for climate mitigation by taking a closer and more
adaptation
structured examination of how local weather prompts information-
Heatwave 0.131*** 0.046*** 0.079*** 0.016*** 0.053***
(0.039) (0.009) (0.027) (0.004) (0.012)
seeking behaviors in climate change.
Cold-spell − 0.000 0.000 0.006* 0.003** 0.049* Using the Protection Motivation Theory (PMT) introduced by Rogers
(0.000) (0.000) (0.003) (0.001) (0.028) (1975), we hypothesize that while changes to local weather would
Constant 13.047 − 0.497 − 36.540 77.776*** − 28.438 prompt higher public interest in climate change, it is more precise to say
(8.371) (6.124) (22.569) (23.591) (34.495)
that the public is seeking out information to appraise threats of climate
Observations 176,295 334,787 84,436 398,217 162,135
Adj. R- change and cost-effectiveness of coping responses.
squared In this regard, two important questions remain: what information
does the public seek, and what does it tell us about their eventual
Panel C. behaviors?
Climate To examine the relationship between local weather and public in­
mitigation terest in climate change, we follow established literature by using daily-
Heatwave 0.036 0.001 0.254*** 0.001 0.006
level internet search volume from the most widely-used search engine in
(0.037) (0.014) (0.017) (0.003) (0.005)
Cold-spell − 0.000 0.000 − 0.005 − 0.002 − 0.008 China – Baidu – as the proxy for information seeking or public interest.
(0.000) (0.000) (0.003) (0.002) (0.022) We first find that, similar to other studies in this literature, internet
Constant − 0.002 8.647** 20.769 23.810 6.050 search volume on keywords related to climate change increases on days
(4.362) (3.409) (27.345) (14.650) (13.171) of abnormally high or low temperatures, and during heatwaves and cold
Observations 176,295 334,787 84,436 398,217 162,135
Adj. R- 0.578 0.721 0.571 0.564 0.635
spells. However, it needs to be emphasized that higher temperatures
squared elicit much more information-seeking activities on climate change
compared to days with lower temperatures.
Notes: All regressions are estimated with calendar date fixed effects, city-year-
Second, we find that attention or public interest is not equally
month fixed effects, and weather controls (precipitation, sea level pressure,
relative humidity, wind speed, hours of sunlight, and all of their quadratics).
dispersed when we categorize climate change-related keywords into
Heatwave is included as number of days, and is defined as at least three three disparate topics: i) knowledge, ii) mitigation, and iii) adaptation.
consecutive days where daily temperature is above 90th percentile of historical Consistent with the hypotheses formed using PMT, we find that on days
temperatures. Cold-spell is included as number of days, and is defined as at least with anomalous temperatures, the Chinese public is much more likely to
three consecutive days where daily temperature is below 10th percentile of seek out information related to climate adaptation, followed by climate
historical temperatures. Standard errors in parentheses, and are clustered at city- change knowledge. In contrast, there is little to no interest in seeking
level. *** p < 0.01, ** p < 0.05, * p < 0.1. information on climate mitigation behaviors. A plausible explanation is
that information on climate knowledge and adaptation could help in­
weather is least experienced. Similar to earlier findings and consistent dividuals appraise environmental threats and coping measures, while
with the PMT, we still do not detect any statistically significant effects information on climate mitigation could not.
for information seeking on climate mitigation regardless of climatic Third, we further refine our findings to show that the climate change-
zones. related information sought by the public is conditional on specific
The results for temperature anomalies are however different (Panel B weather patterns and climatic conditions of residence. For example,
of Fig. 5). While we still observe the same U-shaped relationship be­ Chinese residents are much more likely to search for heat-related effects
tween anomalies and search volume, there are no discernible differences of climate change on a hot day, rather than tangential information such
between climatic zones. A persuasive explanation is that the tempera­ as glaciers retreat as information on the latter does not help individuals
ture anomalies have essentially eliminated or controlled for the inherent to assess realistic threats. Similarly, residents from cooler parts of China
climatic differences between different regions. As such, there is no are much more likely to search for climate change-related information
reason to expect why a 2 ◦ C anomalies is different for an individual in a on a hot day, compared to their counterparts from warmer regions. Our
cool climate compared to someone from a warmer region. interpretation of these results is that residents from the latter are already
acclimatized to such weather conditions.
5. Conclusions and discussions In summary, our findings offer the following policy and research
implications.
Compared to other environmental problems, issues surrounding First, many studies concluded that extreme temperatures or severe
climate change have always been politically and scientifically contro­ weather events will prompt public belief in climate change, thus leading
versial (Boykoff and Boykoff, 2004; Corbett and Durfee, 2004). In turn, to a window of opportunity for policy outreach (e.g., Egan and Mullin,

14
J. Li et al. Global Environmental Change 81 (2023) 102698

2012; Kim et al., 2021; Zaval et al., 2014). We deviate from this opti­ intentions behind the searches. In this regard, more detailed data
mistic outlook as we show that, while local weather will indeed prompt sources with information on actual behaviors will further our under­
interest in climate change, the public is not randomly seeking out all standing of how the public reacts toward climate change.
information related to climate change or even generally interested in Second, while the PMT predicts that local weather changes do not
this subject. Instead, as predicted by the Protection Motivation Theory prompt higher interest in climate mitigation behaviors, it does not mean
(PMT), in the face of local weather changes, increased search volumes that the Chinese public is not interested in reducing or mitigating their
are mostly driven by climate adaptation topics as residents seek out GHG emissions. To be sure, the data shows significant search activities
ways to cope with current and future extreme temperatures. Conse­ for climate mitigation behaviors. However, it is beyond the scope of this
quently, GHG emissions may instead increase resource usage since study to examine in detail the environmental, social, or economic factor
climate adaptation behaviors often have large carbon or environmental (s) contributing to public interest in climate mitigation. As successful
footprint (Qin et al., 2022; Zhao et al., 2015) or even lead individuals to mitigation of GHG emissions is the key to alleviating climate change,
acclimatize to new temperature baselines (Moore et al., 2019). Our future research should take a closer look into the trigger points of in­
findings suggest a footrace between climate adaptation and mitigation, dividuals’ and households’ mitigation behaviors.
and underscore the urgent need for efforts in the latter (Stoddard et al., Third, it is apparent that the PMT with its appraisal of risk and coping
2021). Similarly, given these results, it is likely that policymakers and responses is a highly rational and cognitive process (Rogers, 1983).
the mass media cannot rely on attention-grabbing weather abnormal­ However, it is also true that there are other predictors of responses to­
ities to generate public interest in climate mitigation efforts. Instead, ward climate change which are more affective in nature – e.g., fear,
tailored policies are needed to encourage the public to adopt less carbon- anxiety (van Valkengoed and Steg, 2019). More generally, Loewenstein
intensive behaviors (Dubois et al., 2019). et al. (2001) expounded the “risk-as-feeling” framework where risk
Second, our findings also need to be situated within existing studies perceptions, encompassing both cognitive and affective components,
which found that support for climate mitigation policies is positively exert significant influence on decision-making. They further predict that
associated with experience with extreme weather (e.g., Constantino behaviors driven by the latter are likely to be more extreme than the
et al., 2022; Osberghaus and Demski, 2019; Sisco and Weber, 2022). former, especially when risks are not within control of the individual
Rather than being mutually exclusive, the contrast between this (Loewenstein et al., 2001). While our dataset does not allow us to
study and others can be attributed to the distinct characteristics of two separately identify the cognitive and affective components behind
categories of climate change mitigation behaviors: individual-level and climate change information seeking behaviors, surveys that contain
collective-level behaviors (Lacroix et al., 2022). We focus on the former, detailed socioeconomic characteristics, locational history, and various
such as recycling, adhering to a vegetarian diet, practicing energy con­ climate change perceptions and behaviors can better isolate these
servation whereas the other studies focused on latter, such as supporting different aspects of decision-making.
renewable energy policies and voting for climate-friendly parties. While
not explicitly investigated here or in other studies, it is plausible that CRediT authorship contribution statement
personal experiences with extreme weather have different impacts on
public interest on climate mitigation policies and direct behavioral Jun Li: Data curation, Methodology, Formal analysis, Writing –
changes to reduce one’s carbon footprint. As both types of mitigation original draft, Writing – review & editing. Ping Qin: Conceptualization,
efforts are needed to reduce GHG emissions (IPCC, 2022; Newell et al., Supervision, Writing – review & editing. Yifei Quan: Methodology,
2022; Stoddard et al., 2021), a direct and needed extension of this line of Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing. Jie-Sheng Tan-Soo:
work is to examine if there are different drivers behind individual viz-a- Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Supervision, Writing – review &
viz collective mitigation efforts. editing, Writing – review & editing.
Third, despite the large volume of existing studies, this is surprisingly
one of the few conducted in developing countries’ settings. It is obvious
that global decarbonization cannot take place without cooperation from Declaration of Competing Interest
developing countries, hence it is important to understand public senti­
ments and beliefs toward climate change from these parts of the world. The authors declare that they have no known competing financial
In this study, we demonstrate the viability of using readily available “big interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence
data” sources, as hypotheses formed using PMT are verified and exam­ the work reported in this paper.
ined by using information on internet search volume. While our findings
are generalizable to China, we need to carefully consider if they are Data availability
applicable to other parts of the world. For instance, Jakučionytė-
Skodienė and Liobikienė (2021) found that risk perception, cost- Data will be made available on request.
effectiveness, and response efficacy appraisals of climate change vary
among individuals with different levels of economic development, cul­ Acknowledgements
tural values, and ideological backgrounds. As such, while the PMT
framework can also be applied in similar manners to other countries, the We are grateful to two anonymous referees for providing helpful
generated hypotheses are likely to be different across places. For comments, and to PCC@RUC for providing computing support.
instance, in lesser-developed economies, it is possible that cost-effective Generous financial support was given by the National Natural Science
adaptation measures are dominated by low-cost options such as electric Foundation of China (grant.: 72173126, 72134006) [to Q.P.], and by the
fans or frequenting public air-conditioner spaces. As such, to generate a Singapore Ministry of Education Tier 1 FRC grant (grant: A-8000736-00-
broader knowledge base, our study approach can be readily applied to 00) [to J.S.T.S].
data-scarce countries, which are mostly located in developing parts of
the world. Appendix A
Lastly, this is study is not without limitations. For one thing, a
common but deserved criticism of internet-sourced data (e.g., search
engines or social media) is that we do not observe actual behaviors or
intentions. In the context of our findings, while we find that the Chinese
public is seeking out information on climate adaptation, it is unclear the
extent to which they translate into actual adoption behaviors or their

15
J. Li et al. Global Environmental Change 81 (2023) 102698

References Huddart Kennedy, E., Krahn, H., Krogman, N.T., 2015. Are we counting what counts? A
closer look at environmental concern, pro-environmental behaviour, and carbon
footprint. Local Environ. 20 (2), 220–236.
Akram, R., Chen, F., Khalid, F., Ye, Z., Majeed, M.T., 2020. Heterogeneous effects of
Hughes, L., Konisky, D.M., Potter, S., 2020. Extreme weather and climate opinion:
energy efficiency and renewable energy on carbon emissions: Evidence from
evidence from Australia. Clim. Change 163 (2), 723–743.
developing countries. J. Clean. Prod. 247, 119122.
IPCC. (2014). Climate Change 2014: Impacts, Adaptation, and Vulnerability. Part B: Regional
Auffhammer, M., 2014. Cooling China: The weather dependence of air conditioner
Aspects. Contribution of Working Group II to the Fifth Assessment Report of the
adoption. Front. Econ. China 9 (1), 70–84.
Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change: Cambridge University Press, Cambridge,
Barreca, A., Clay, K., Deschenes, O., Greenstone, M., Shapiro, J.S., 2016. Adapting to
United Kingdom and New York, NY, USA.
climate change: The remarkable decline in the US temperature-mortality
IPCC. (2022). Climate change 2022: impacts, adaptation and vulnerability - summary for
relationship over the twentieth century. J. Polit. Econ. 124 (1), 105–159.
policymakers: Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change.
Beck, K.H., Feldman, R.H., 1983. Information seeking among safety and health
Jakučionytė-Skodienė, M., Liobikienė, G., 2021. Climate change concern, personal
managers. J. Psychol. 115 (1), 23–31.
responsibility and actions related to climate change mitigation in EU countries:
Bergquist, M., Nilsson, A., Schultz, P., 2019. Experiencing a severe weather event
Cross-cultural analysis. J. Clean. Prod. 281, 125189.
increases concern about climate change. Front. Psychol. 10, 220.
Janmaimool, P., 2017. Application of protection motivation theory to investigate
Biesbroek, G.R., Swart, R.J., Van der Knaap, W.G., 2009. The mitigation–adaptation
sustainable waste management behaviors. Sustainability 9 (7), 1079.
dichotomy and the role of spatial planning. Habitat Int. 33 (3), 230–237.
Kim, J.H., Seo, M.H., Sinclair, B., 2021. Local weather effects: Perception of climate
Birkland, T.A., 1997. After Disaster: Agenda Setting, Public Policy, and Focusing Events.
change and public support for government intervention. Soc. Sci. Q. 102 (2),
Georgetown University Press.
881–896.
Birkland, T.A., 2017. Agenda setting in public policy. In: Handbook of Public Policy
Kirilenko, A.P., Molodtsova, T., Stepchenkova, S.O., 2015. People as sensors: Mass media
Analysis. Routledge, pp. 89–104.
and local temperature influence climate change discussion on Twitter. Glob.
Bockarjova, M., Steg, L., 2014. Can Protection Motivation Theory predict pro-
Environ. Chang. 30, 92–100.
environmental behavior? Explaining the adoption of electric vehicles in the
Konisky, D.M., Hughes, L., Kaylor, C.H., 2016. Extreme weather events and climate
Netherlands. Glob. Environ. Chang. 28, 276–288.
change concern. Clim. Change 134 (4), 533–547.
Boykoff, M.T., Boykoff, J.M., 2004. Balance as bias: Global warming and the US prestige
Lacroix, K., Carman, J.P., Goldberg, M.H., Gustafson, A., Rosenthal, S.A., Leiserowitz, A.,
press. Glob. Environ. Chang. 14 (2), 125–136.
2022. Does personal climate change mitigation behavior influence collective
Bradley, G.L., Babutsidze, Z., Chai, A., Reser, J.P., 2020. The role of climate change risk
behavior? Experimental evidence of no spillover in the United States. Energy Res.
perception, response efficacy, and psychological adaptation in pro-environmental
Soc. Sci. 94, 102875.
behavior: A two nation study. J. Environ. Psychol. 68, 101410.
Lam, C.K.C., Gallant, A.J., Tapper, N.J., 2019. Short-term changes in thermal perception
Brulle, R.J., Carmichael, J., Jenkins, J.C., 2012. Shifting public opinion on climate
associated with heatwave conditions in Melbourne, Australia. Theor. Appl. Climatol.
change: an empirical assessment of factors influencing concern over climate change
136 (1), 651–660.
in the US, 2002–2010. Clim. Change 114 (2), 169–188.
Lam, J.C., Tsang, C., Yang, L., Li, D.H., 2005. Weather data analysis and design
Cavanagh, P., Lang, C., Li, X., Miao, H., Ryder, J.D., 2014. Searching for the determinants
implications for different climatic zones in China. Build. Environ. 40 (2), 277–296.
of climate change interest. Geography Journal 2014, 1–11.
Landauer, M., Juhola, S., Klein, J., 2019. The role of scale in integrating climate change
Champ, P.A., Boyle, K.J., Brown, T.C., Peterson, L.G., 2003. A Primer on Nonmarket
adaptation and mitigation in cities. J. Environ. Plan. Manage. 62 (5), 741–765.
Valuation, Vol. 3. Springer.
Lang, C., 2014a. Do weather fluctuations cause people to seek information about climate
Chen, M.-F., 2020. Moral extension of the protection motivation theory model to predict
change? Clim. Change 125 (3), 291–303.
climate change mitigation behavioral intentions in Taiwan. Environ. Sci. Pollut. Res.
Lang, C., 2014b. Do weather fluctuations cause people to seek information about climate
27 (12), 13714–13725.
change? Clim. Change 125 (3–4), 291–303.
Choi, D., Gao, Z., Jiang, W. (2020). Attention to global warming. Rev. Finan. Stud., 33(3),
Li, Y., Johnson, E.J., Zaval, L., 2011. Local warming: Daily temperature change
1112-1145.
influences belief in global warming. Psychol. Sci. 22 (4), 454–459.
Constantino, S. M., Cooperman, A. D., Keohane, R. O., Weber, E. U. (2022). Personal
Li, Y., Pizer, W.A., Wu, L., 2019. Climate change and residential electricity consumption
hardship narrows the partisan gap in COVID-19 and climate change responses. Proc.
in the Yangtze River Delta, China. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 116 (2), 472–477.
Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A., 119(46), e2120653119.
Li, W., Yang, G., Li, X., 2021. Correlation between PM2. 5 pollution and its public
Corbett, J.B., Durfee, J.L., 2004. Testing public (un) certainty of science: Media
concern in China: Evidence from Baidu Index. J. Clean. Prod. 293, 126091.
representations of global warming. Sci. Commun. 26 (2), 129–151.
Liu, T., He, G., Lau, A., 2018. Avoidance behavior against air pollution: evidence from
Currie, J., Neidell, M., 2005. Air pollution and infant health: what can we learn from
online search indices for anti-PM2. 5 masks and air filters in Chinese cities. Environ.
California’s recent experience? Q. J. Econ. 120 (3), 1003–1030.
Econ. Policy Stud. 20 (2), 325–363.
Dasgupta, S., van Maanen, N., Gosling, S. N., Piontek, F., Otto, C., Schleussner, C.-F.
Loewenstein, G.F., Weber, E.U., Hsee, C.K., Welch, N., 2001. Risk as feelings. Psychol.
(2021). Effects of climate change on combined labour productivity and supply: an
Bull. 127 (2), 267.
empirical, multi-model study. Lancet Planet. Health, 5(7), e455-e465.
Lorenzoni, I., Nicholson-Cole, S., Whitmarsh, L., 2007. Barriers perceived to engaging
Deschenes, O., Greenstone, M., 2007. The economic impacts of climate change: evidence
with climate change among the UK public and their policy implications. Glob.
from agricultural output and random fluctuations in weather. Am. Econ. Rev. 97 (1),
Environ. Chang. 17 (3–4), 445–459.
354–385.
Marlon, J.R., Wang, X., Mildenberger, M., Bergquist, P., Swain, S., Hayhoe, K., Howe, P.
Dong, D., Xu, X., Xu, W., Xie, J., 2019. The relationship between the actual level of air
D., Maibach, E., Leiserowitz, A., 2021. Hot dry days increase perceived experience
pollution and residents’ concern about air pollution: evidence from Shanghai, China.
with global warming. Glob. Environ. Chang. 68, 102247.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 16 (23), 4784.
McDonald, R.I., Chai, H.Y., Newell, B.R., 2015. Personal experience and the
Donner, S.D., McDaniels, J., 2013. The influence of national temperature fluctuations on
‘psychological distance’of climate change: An integrative review. J. Environ.
opinions about climate change in the US since 1990. Clim. Change 118 (3), 537–550.
Psychol. 44, 109–118.
Drummond, A., Hall, L.C., Sauer, J.D., Palmer, M.A., 2018. Is public awareness and
Mertz, O., Halsnæs, K., Olesen, J.E., Rasmussen, K., 2009. Adaptation to climate change
perceived threat of climate change associated with governmental mitigation targets?
in developing countries. Environ. Manage. 43 (5), 743–752.
Clim. Change 149 (2), 159–171.
Ministry of Housing and Urban-Rural Development of the People’s Republic of China.
Dubois, G., Sovacool, B., Aall, C., Nilsson, M., Barbier, C., Herrmann, A., Bruyère, S.,
(2016). Thermal Design Code for Civil Building. In (Vol. GB50176-2016). Beijing:
Andersson, C., Skold, B., Nadaud, F., Dorner, F., Moberg, K.R., Ceron, J.P.,
China Architecture & Building Press.
Fischer, H., Amelung, D., Baltruszewicz, M., Fischer, J., Benevise, F., Louis, V.R.,
Moore, F.C., Obradovich, N., Lehner, F., Baylis, P., 2019. Rapidly declining remarkability
Sauerborn, R., 2019. It starts at home? Climate policies targeting household
of temperature anomalies may obscure public perception of climate change. Proc.
consumption and behavioral decisions are key to low-carbon futures. Energy Res.
Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 116 (11), 4905–4910.
Soc. Sci. 52, 144–158.
Nejat, P., Jomehzadeh, F., Mahdi, M., Gohari, M., 2015. A global review of energy
Egan, P.J., Mullin, M., 2012. Turning personal experience into political attitudes: The
consumption, CO2 emissions and policy in the residential sector (with an overview
effect of local weather on Americans’ perceptions about global warming. J. Polit. 74
of the top ten CO2 emitting countries). Renew. Sustain. Energy Rev. 43, 843–862.
(3), 796–809.
Newell, P., Daley, F., Twena, M., 2022. Changing Our Ways: Behaviour Change and the
Guo, Y., Gasparrini, A., Armstrong, B., Li, S., Tawatsupa, B., Tobias, A., Lavigne, E., de
Climate Crisis. Cambridge University Press.
Sousa Zanotti Stagliorio Coelho, M., Leone, M., Pan, X., Tong, S., Tian, L., Kim, H.o.,
Newell, R.G., Prest, B.C., Sexton, S.E., 2021. The GDP-temperature relationship:
Hashizume, M., Honda, Y., Guo, Y.-L., Wu, C.-F., Punnasiri, K., Yi, S.-M.,
implications for climate change damages. J. Environ. Econ. Manag. 108, 102445.
Michelozzi, P., Saldiva, P.H.N., Williams, G., 2014. Global variation in the effects of
Nguyen, Q.A., Hens, L., Nguyen, N., MacAlister, C., Lebel, L., 2020. Explaining intentions
ambient temperature on mortality: a systematic evaluation. Epidimiology 25 (6),
by vietnamese schoolchildren to adopt pro-environmental behaviors in response to
781–789.
climate change using theories of persuasive communication. Environ. Manag. 66 (5),
Halsnæs, K., Verhagen, J., 2007. Development based climate change adaptation and
845–857.
mitigation—conceptual issues and lessons learned in studies in developing countries.
NOAA, 2021. State of the Climate: Global Climate Report for Annual 2020. Retrieved
Mitig. Adapt. Strat. Glob. Chang. 12 (5), 665–684.
from. https://www.ncdc.noaa.gov/sotc/global/202013.
He, G., Chen, Y., Chen, B., Wang, H., Shen, L.i., Liu, L., Suolang, D., Zhang, B., Ju, G.,
Obradovich, N., Migliorini, R., Mednick, S.C., Fowler, J.H., 2017. Nighttime temperature
Zhang, L., Du, S., Jiang, X., Pan, Y.u., Min, Z., 2018. Using the Baidu search index to
and human sleep loss in a changing climate. Sci. Adv. 3 (5), e1601555.
predict the incidence of HIV/AIDS in China. Sci. Rep. 8 (1).
Osberghaus, D., Demski, C., 2019. The causal effect of flood experience on climate
Howe, P.D., Markowitz, E.M., Lee, T.M., Ko, C.-Y., Leiserowitz, A., 2013. Global
engagement: evidence from search requests for green electricity. Clim. Change 156
perceptions of local temperature change. Nat. Clim. Chang. 3 (4), 352–356.
(1), 191–207.
Huang, X., Zhang, L., Ding, Y., 2017. The Baidu Index: Uses in predicting tourism
flows–A case study of the Forbidden City. Tour. Manag. 58, 301–306.

16
J. Li et al. Global Environmental Change 81 (2023) 102698

Paton, D., Kerstholt, J., Skinner, I. (2017). Hazard readiness and resilience. Disaster Sisco, M.R., Weber, E.U., 2022. Local temperature anomalies increase climate policy
Resilience: An Integrated Approach, 2nd ed.; Paton, D., Johnston, DM, Eds, 114-137. interest and support: Analysis of internet searches and US congressional vote shares.
Pianta, S., Sisco, M.R., 2020. A hot topic in hot times: how media coverage of climate Glob. Environ. Chang. 76, 102572.
change is affected by temperature abnormalities. Environ. Res. Lett. 15 (11), Stern, N., Stiglitz, J., 2022. The economics of immense risk, urgent action and radical
114038. change: towards new approaches to the economics of climate change. J. Econ.
Qin, P., Chen, S., Tan-Soo, J.-S., Zhang, X.-B., 2022. Urban household water usage in Methodol. 29 (3), 181–216.
adaptation to climate change: Evidence from China. Environ. Sci. Policy 136, Stern, N. (2021). A time for action on climate change and a time for change in economics.
486–496. Retrieved from.
Qin, Y., Zhu, H., 2018. Run away? Air pollution and emigration interests in China. Stoddard, I., Anderson, K., Capstick, S., Carton, W., Depledge, J., Facer, K., Gough, C.,
J. Popul. Econ. 31 (1), 235–266. Hache, F., Hoolohan, C., Hultman, M., Hällström, N., Kartha, S., Klinsky, S.,
Rahimi, M., 2020. Public awareness: what climate change scientists should consider. Kuchler, M., Lövbrand, E., Nasiritousi, N., Newell, P., Peters, G.P., Sokona, Y.,
Sustainability 12 (20), 8369. Stirling, A., Stilwell, M., Spash, C.L., Williams, M., 2021. Three decades of climate
Ray, A., Hughes, L., Konisky, D.M., Kaylor, C., 2017. Extreme weather exposure and mitigation: why haven’t we bent the global emissions curve? Annu. Rev. Env.
support for climate change adaptation. Glob. Environ. Chang. 46, 104–113. Resour. 46 (1), 653–689.
Rippetoe, P.A., Rogers, R.W., 1987. Effects of components of protection-motivation Tan-Soo, J.-S., Li, J., Qin, P., 2022. Individuals’ and households’ climate adaptation and
theory on adaptive and maladaptive coping with a health threat. J. Pers. Soc. mitigation behaviors: A systematic review. China Econ. Rev. 101879.
Psychol. 52 (3), 596. Taylor, A., de Bruin, W.B., Dessai, S., 2014. Climate change beliefs and perceptions of
Rode, A., Carleton, T., Delgado, M., Greenstone, M., Houser, T., Hsiang, S., McCusker, K. weather-related changes in the United Kingdom. Risk Anal. 34 (11), 1995–2004.
E., 2021. Estimating a social cost of carbon for global energy consumption. Nature Thøgersen, J., Crompton, T., 2009. Simple and painless? The limitations of spillover in
598 (7880), 308–314. environmental campaigning. J. Consum. Policy 32 (2), 141–163.
Rogers, R.W., 1975. A protection motivation theory of fear appeals and attitude change1. van Valkengoed, A.M., Steg, L., 2019. Meta-analyses of factors motivating climate
J. Psychol. 91 (1), 93–114. change adaptation behaviour. Nat. Clim. Chang. 9 (2), 158–163.
Rogers, R.W., 1983. Cognitive and psychological processes in fear appeals and attitude Wang, J., Obradovich, N., Zheng, S., 2020. A 43-million-person investigation into
change: A revised theory of protection motivation. Soc. Psychophysiol. 153–176. weather and expressed sentiment in a changing climate. One Earth 2 (6), 568–577.
Scannell, L., Gifford, R., 2013. Personally relevant climate change: The role of place Whitmarsh, L., 2008. Are flood victims more concerned about climate change than other
attachment and local versus global message framing in engagement. Environ. Behav. people? The role of direct experience in risk perception and behavioural response.
45 (1), 60–85. J. Risk Res. 11 (3), 351–374.
Schlenker, W., Walker, W.R., 2015. Airports, air pollution, and contemporaneous health. Yang, X., Pan, B., Evans, J.A., Lv, B., 2015. Forecasting Chinese tourist volume with
Rev. Econ. Stud. 83 (2), 768–809. search engine data. Tour. Manag. 46, 386–397.
Semenza, J.C., Wilson, D.J., Parra, J., Bontempo, B.D., Hart, M., Sailor, D.J., George, L. Zanocco, C., Boudet, H., Nilson, R., Flora, J., 2019. Personal harm and support for
A., 2008. Public perception and behavior change in relationship to hot weather and climate change mitigation policies: Evidence from 10 US communities impacted by
air pollution. Environ. Res. 107 (3), 401–411. extreme weather. Glob. Environ. Chang. 59, 101984.
Sharifi, A., 2020. Trade-offs and conflicts between urban climate change mitigation and Zaval, L., Keenan, E.A., Johnson, E.J., Weber, E.U., 2014. How warm days increase belief
adaptation measures: A literature review. J. Clean. Prod. 276, 122813. in global warming. Nat. Clim. Chang. 4 (2), 143–147.
Sisco, M.R., Bosetti, V., Weber, E.U., 2017a. When do extreme weather events generate Zhao, L., Zeng, W., Yuan, Z., 2015. Reduction of potential greenhouse gas emissions of
attention to climate change? Clim. Change 143 (1–2), 227–241. room air-conditioner refrigerants: a life cycle carbon footprint analysis. J. Clean.
Sisco, M.R., Bosetti, V., Weber, E.U., 2017b. When do extreme weather events generate Prod. 100, 262–268.
attention to climate change? Clim. Change 143 (1), 227–241. Zhong, S., Yu, Z., Zhu, W., 2019. Study of the effects of air pollutants on human health
Sisco, M.R., Constantino, S.M., Gao, Y.u., Tavoni, M., Cooperman, A.D., Bosetti, V., based on Baidu indices of disease symptoms and air quality monitoring data in
Weber, E.U., 2023. Examining evidence for the Finite Pool of Worry and Finite Pool Beijing, China. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 16 (6), 1014.
of Attention hypotheses. Glob. Environ. Chang. 78, 102622.

17

You might also like