Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

ARTICLE IN PRESS

Am J of Geriatric Psychiatry &&:&& (2021) &&−&&

Available online at www.sciencedirect.com

ScienceDirect
journal homepage: www.ajgponline.org

Regular Research Article

Adverse Impacts of Declining


Financial and Health Literacy
in Old Age
Lei Yu, Ph.D., Gary Mottola, Ph.D., David A. Bennett, M.D.,
Patricia A. Boyle, Ph.D.

ARTICLE INFO ABSTRACT

Article history: Objectives: Inadequate financial and health literacy presents a formidable
Received November, 23 2020 public health and economic challenge in old age. This study investigated
Revised February, 6 2021 declining financial and health literacy in relation to decision making perfor-
Accepted February, 7 2021 mance, scam susceptibility and psychological wellbeing. Design: Longitudinal
study. Setting: A community-based cohort in Northeastern Illinois, USA.
Key Words: Participants: One thousand fourty-six older adults who were free of dementia
Domain-specific literacy at baseline and underwent annual clinical and literacy assessments.
decision making Measurements: Financial and health literacy, decision making, scam sus-
scam susceptibility ceptibility, and psychological wellbeing were assessed using validated
psychological wellbeing instruments. Linear mixed effects models estimated person-specific rates of
change in financial and health literacy, and multivariable regression analy-
ses examined the associations of declining literacy with subsequent levels of
decision making, scam susceptibility, and psychological wellbeing.
Results: The mean age was 81 years and 76% were female. Over up to
10 years of annual follow-ups, the average financial and health literacy score
dropped 1 percentage point a year. Substantial variability in decline was
observed between participants. Faster decline in financial and health literacy
was associated with poorer decision making, higher scam susceptibility, and
lower psychological wellbeing. Notably, these associations were above and
beyond the baseline literacy level and persisted even after controlling for cog-
nition. Conclusions: Most community-dwelling older adults experience
decline in financial and health literacy over time, but decline is not inevitable.
Declining literacy is related to poorer decision making, greater scam suscepti-

From the Rush Alzheimer’s Disease Center, Rush University Medical Center (LY, DAB, PAB), Chicago, IL, USA; Department of Neuro-
logical Sciences, Rush University Medical Center (LY, DAB), Chicago, IL, USA; FINRA Investor Education Foundation (GM), Wash-
ington DC, USA; and the Department of Behavioral Sciences, Rush University Medical Center (PAB), Chicago, IL, USA. Send
correspondence and reprint requests to Lei Yu, Ph.D., Rush Alzheimer’s Disease Center, 1750 W Harrison Street, Suite 1000, Chicago, IL
60612. e-mail: Lei_Yu@rush.edu
This study is supported by the National Institute on Aging and the FINRA Investor Education Foundation.
© 2021 American Association for Geriatric Psychiatry. Published by Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jagp.2021.02.042

Am J Geriatr Psychiatry &&:&&, && 2021 1


ARTICLE IN PRESS
Adverse Impacts of Declining Financial and Health Literacy

bility and lower wellbeing. These findings suggest that efforts to mitigate
declining financial and health literacy may promote independence and well-
being in old age. (Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2021; &&:&&−&&)

Highlights
 What is the primary question addressed by this study?
What is the relationship of declining literacy with subsequent adverse outcomes?
 What is the main finding of this study?
Declining literacy impacts on decision making, scam susceptibility, and wellbeing.
 What is the meaning of the finding?
Efforts to mitigate declining literacy promote independence and wellbeing in old age.

Low financial and health literacy have major impli-


cations for the overall wellbeing of older adults. In
particular, older persons with poor financial and
OBJECTIVE
health literacy tend to make suboptimal financial and
healthcare decisions8−11 and are more susceptible to
F inancial and health literacy, the acquisition, proc-
essing, and utilization of financial and health
information to facilitate effective decisions,1 is of par-
financial scams.12,13 Further, low financial and health
literacy is also associated with poor mental health.
ticular importance to successful aging in modern soci- Prior studies reported that individuals with inade-
ety. Every day, thousands of older Americans are quate health literacy have worse mental health func-
confronted with a series of financial and healthcare tioning, and that older adults with lower financial
challenges ranging from planning for retirement, and health literacy tend to have more depressive
making investment decisions, to choosing Medicare symptoms and are more emotionally isolated.14,15
coverage and signing up for healthcare services. Notably, research on the impacts of financial and
Alarming data have shown that older adults are health literacy to date has relied almost exclusively
highly vulnerable to poor financial and health liter- on cross-sectional data. Emerging evidence suggests
acy.2 In a nationally representative survey of older that financial and health literacy is not static and may
Americans, a significant proportion of respondents change across the lifespan. One study reported that
failed to understand basic concepts of compound financial literacy deteriorates after age 6016 and a
interest, inflation or mutual funds.3 Older adults study on the life-cycle pattern of financial mistakes
also lack financial sophistication in areas such as suggests that financial literacy and decision making
risk diversification, asset valuation, portfolio choice likely follow an inverse U-shape pattern that peaks in
and investment fees.4 Similarly, many older adults middle-age.17 Decline in health literacy among aging
lack the fundamental health literacy necessary to populations has been reported as well.18 With finan-
maneuver the current healthcare environment. The cial and health literacy declining in old age, it raises
high prevalence of inadequate health literacy is not an important question on whether the consequences
only confined to older patients in public hospital of inadequate financial and health literacy in old age
settings,5 but is also widely observed among older are driven primarily by starting levels of literacy, age-
community residents.6 In a previous American related decline in literacy, or both.
Medical Association report,7 approximately a third To address this knowledge gap, we present a con-
of English-speaking and separately over a half of ceptual model of declining financial and health liter-
Spanish-speaking Medicare enrollees in managed acy and its impacts in old age (Supplementary
care facilities had inadequate or only marginal Materials). Briefly, we hypothesize that starting levels
health literacy. (i.e., financial and health literacy accumulated

2 Am J Geriatr Psychiatry &&:&&, && 2021


ARTICLE IN PRESS
Yu et al.

through earlier life experiences) and rates of decline (i. over time, the analyses were restricted to participants
e., age-related deterioration in literacy) vary across with at least 2 literacy assessments (N = 1,088). We
individuals, and both are important determinants of further excluded participants who were demented by
adverse outcomes. Older adults with lower level of the baseline literacy assessment (N = 42), resulting in
literacy and/or faster declining literacy tend to make an analytic sample size of 1,046. Participants were fol-
poorer financial and health decisions, are more vul- lowed annually for up to 10 years (Mean: 4.8 years,
nerable to financial fraud and scams, and have lower SD: 2.9). In examining the associations of declining lit-
wellbeing. Importantly, this model was built upon eracy with adverse outcomes, approximately 5% of
overwhelming cross-sectional findings that link poor the total sample were excluded from the regression
financial and health literacy to adverse outcomes as analyses due to incomplete data for income or out-
well as prior evidence of declining literacy in old age. come measure.
The relationships presented in the model are in gen- Both the MAP parent study and the decision mak-
eral consistent with and complementary to prior theo- ing substudy were approved by an institutional
retical frameworks on various causal pathways that review board of the Rush University Medical Center.
link literacy to financial and health outcomes.19−22 In Participants signed informed consents and agreed to
the current work, we test our conceptual model by annual home visits that include detailed clinical eval-
leveraging data from over 1,000 community-dwelling uation as well as a full decision making battery.
older adults with an average 80 years of age. Specifi-
cally, we investigate the extent to which declining
Financial and health literacy
financial and health literacy are associated with sub-
sequent decision making performance, scam suscepti- Details on financial and health literacy assessment
bility, and psychological wellbeing, above and were described previously.24 Financial literacy was
beyond the effects of starting level of literacy. assessed via 23 items adapted from the Health and
Retirement Study. In brief, 12 items assessed financial
and institutional knowledge, nine items assessed
numeracy, and the remaining three items assessed
METHODS the skill that combines both financial knowledge and
numeracy in the context of investment returns. The
Study participants
financial literacy score was the percentage of the 23
The present study included 1,046 older persons items that were answered correctly.
from the Rush Memory and Aging Project (MAP). Health literacy was assessed via nine items that
MAP, started in 1997, is an ongoing cohort study of measure knowledge of health information and con-
common chronic conditions of aging that include (but cepts. The questions were tailored to aging popula-
are not limited to) Alzheimer’s and other dementias, tion and focused specifically on Medicare, following
motor impairment and Parkinsonism, as well as phys- prescription instruction, flu vaccination, leading
ical frailty and disability.23 The study recruits older causes of heart disease and stroke, and determining
residents throughout northeastern Illinois of the drug risk. Similar to financial literacy, the health liter-
United States, and participants primarily come from acy score was the percentage of the nine items that
continuous care retirement communities and subsi- were answered correctly. The present study used a
dized housing facilities. A decision making substudy composite score to examine the change in financial
was introduced in 2010. As of May 31, 2020, 1,698 and health literacy over time, which is the average of
MAP participants were alive and active since the start financial and health literacy scores (Supplementary
of the decision making substudy. Two hundred four- Methods). Higher scores indicate higher financial and
teen were ineligible for the substudy due to reasons health literacy.
such as moving out of the geographical area, lan-
guage barriers, or severe sensory impairment. Of the
Financial and health decision making
remaining 1,484, 1,277 (86.1%) had completed the
baseline literacy assessment. In order to estimate per- A 12-item version of the Decision Making Assess-
son-specific change in financial and health literacy ment Tool25 assessed financial and health decision

Am J Geriatr Psychiatry &&:&&, && 2021 3


ARTICLE IN PRESS
Adverse Impacts of Declining Financial and Health Literacy

making in older adults, as previously described.26 The Cognition


instrument is designed to simulate real world situa-
Cognition was assessed using 19 tests which cover
tions in choosing mutual funds (financial decision
five primary domains of episodic memory, sematic
making) and HMO plans (health decision making).
memory, working memory, perceptual speed and
Specifically, for financial decision making, tables with
visuospatial ability.30,31 To reduce random variability
information about different mutual funds were pre-
and potential floor and ceiling artifacts in individual
sented to the participants, followed by six questions
tests, raw scores of each test were first standardized
that range from simple understanding of the informa-
using baseline mean and SD of the MAP cohort, and
tion presented to more challenging problems that
then averaged across the 19 tests to obtain a global
require integrating multiple information. The assess-
cognitive score. Higher scores indicate higher cogni-
ment of health decision making follows a similar
tion. Additional information on the cognitive tests,
structure. Tables with information about various
as well as clinical diagnoses of dementia and mild
HMO plans were presented, and participants were
cognitive impairment are provided (Supplementary
asked six questions of varying difficulty level. Each
Materials).
correct answer is scored 1 point, and the number of
correct answers was tallied to obtain a total financial
and health decision making score. The total score can
Statistical analysis
range between 0 and 12, and higher scores indicate
higher financial and health decision making ability. To investigate longitudinal change in financial and
health literacy, we fit a linear mixed effects model
with annual financial and health literacy scores as the
Scam susceptibility
longitudinal outcome. Time in years since baseline
Scam susceptibility was assessed by averaging par- was the predictor, and the corresponding coefficient
ticipants’ responses to five statements that indicate estimates the mean annual rate of change in financial
vulnerability to scams according to findings from the and health literacy (i.e., the mean slope). The random
AARP and the Financial Industry Regulatory Author- slopes from the model estimated the deviation of indi-
ity Risk Meter.27 Specifically, participants rated, on a vidual slopes of change from the mean slope. As a
7-point Likert scale, their likelihood of 1) answering result, these random slopes represent adjusted per-
and 2) ending a phone call from a stranger/telemar- son-specific rates of change in financial and health
keter; 3) listening to sales pitches from a telemarketer; literacy.
and whether they agree on the statement that 4) if To examine the influence of change in financial and
something sounds too good to be true, it usually is, health literacy on the measures of decision making,
and 5) older persons are often targeted by scammers. scam susceptibility and psychological wellbeing, we
Higher average ratings across the five items indicate fit a series of linear regression models. In these mod-
greater scam susceptibility. els, the slopes of change in financial and health liter-
acy, estimated using the data from baseline up until
the last decision making assessment, was the primary
Psychological wellbeing
predictor. The measures of financial and health deci-
An 18-item instrument was adapted from Ryff’s sion making, scam susceptibility and psychological
Scales of Psychological Well Being,28 as previously wellbeing at the last assessment were the continuous
described.29 Participants rated, on a 7-point Likert outcomes. Each outcome was modeled separately. All
scale, their agreement to each item. Six different the regression models were controlled for age, sex,
aspects of psychological wellbeing were assessed, education, income, as well as the baseline level of
which include self-acceptance, autonomy, environ- financial and health literacy. For ease of comparison,
mental mastery, purpose in life, positive relations the magnitude of the association was contextualized
with others, and personal growth. Item-specific rat- as the difference in SD of an outcome measure with
ings were averaged to obtain an overall wellbeing every 1SD increase or decrease of a predictor.
measure, and higher scores indicate higher psycho- Statistical analyses were conducted using SAS soft-
logical wellbeing. ware 9.4 on the Linux platform. As the analyses

4 Am J Geriatr Psychiatry &&:&&, && 2021


ARTICLE IN PRESS
Yu et al.

involve three primary outcomes, statistical significance decline (Random slopes variance: 1.70, SE: 0.20, Z:
for the primary analyses was determined at a level of 8.55, p <0.001). It is evident from the data that, while
0.015 after correction for multiple testing. most participants (N = 873, 83.5%) experienced
decline in financial and health literacy, others
(N = 173, 16.5%) managed to maintain their literacy
level during the follow-up (Fig. 1).
RESULTS Older participants (Spearman r: 0.37, N = 1,046, p
<0.001) with lower income (Spearman r: 0.23,
Financial and health literacy declines in old age
N = 1,009, p <0.001) or fewer years of education
Characteristics of the study participants are (Spearman r: 0.24, N = 1,046, p <0.001) showed faster
described (Table 1). At baseline, approximately 70% decline in financial and health literacy. Female partici-
of the financial and health questions were answered pants declined faster than male participants (t456.6:
correctly on average. We examined the longitudinal 4.89, p <0.001), as did participants with mild cogni-
trajectory of financial and health literacy using annual tive impairment at baseline (t535.2: 11.02, p <0.001).
literacy scores from baseline up until the last assess-
ment. Consistent with previous reports,32,33 we Declining financial and health literacy and
observed a decline in financial and health literacy decision making performance
over time. On average, the financial and health liter-
acy score dropped about 1 percentage point each year To examine the association of declining literacy
(Mean slope estimate: 0.82, Standard error [SE]: with financial and health decision making, we
0.07, t1045: 12.03, p <0.001). Notably, the model regressed the total decision making scores on the
revealed substantial person-to-person variation in slopes of preceding change in the financial and health
literacy score. In the model adjusted for demo-
graphics (i.e., age, sex, education, and income), lower
TABLE 1. Baseline Characteristics of Study Participants
baseline level of financial and health literacy was
Age (Years)a 81.1 (7.4, 59.0−100.2) associated with poorer decision making years later
Education (Years)b 15.6 (3.0)
Femalec 798 (76.3%) (Table 2). In the same model, faster decline in finan-
Non-Hispanic Whitec 959 (91.7%) cial and health literacy was also associated with
Incomed 8 (6−9) poorer decision making. Specifically, with 1SD of
Financial and health literacy score 69.2 (14.2)
(% correct)a additional decline in the financial and health literacy
Financial and health decision making score 8.1 (2.6) score, the decision making score was 0.33SD lower
(# correct)a
(Fig. 2A). By comparison, 1SD lower in the baseline
Susceptibility to scamsa 2.7 (0.8)
Psychological wellbeinga 5.6 (0.6) level of financial and health literacy score (i.e.,
Cognitiona 0.22 (0.52) approximate 14 percentage points) corresponds to
Mild cognitive impairmentc 317 (30.3%)
0.21SD lower in the decision making score; and 1SD
Income: 1: $0−$4,999, 2: $5,000−$9,999, 3: $10,000−$14,999, 4: of additional years of age (i.e., approximate 7 years)
$15,000−$19,999, 5: $20,000−$24,999, 6: $25,000−$29,999, 7: corresponds to 0.24SD lower in the decision making
$30,000−$34,999, 8: $35,000−$49,999, 9: $50,000−$74,999, 10:
$75,000 and over. score. Together, these results suggest that the strength
Cognitive score is the average of individual scores for 19 cognitive of association of declining literacy with the subse-
tests after standardization. A score of 0 represents the mean baseline quent level of decision making is stronger than base-
cognition of the cohort, and each unit approximates 1 standard devia-
tion of the baseline cognition of the cohort. line literacy or even age. The domain-specific results
Mild cognitive impairment: each participant underwent annual were similar (Supplementary Results).
clinical evaluations including detailed cognitive assessments and neu-
Cognitive health is strongly associated with both
rologic examinations. A diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment is ren-
dered if a person has cognitive impairment as determined by financial and health literacy as well as decision mak-
neuropsychological testing but does not meet the criteria for demen- ing.34−37 Thus, to assess whether the association of
tia diagnosis by the clinician.
a
Mean (SD, Min-Max).
declining literacy and decision making is robust
b
Mean (SD). against traditionally measured cognition, we aug-
c
N (%). mented the regression model by further controlling
d
Median (Interquartile range).
for the global cognitive score derived from 19

Am J Geriatr Psychiatry &&:&&, && 2021 5


ARTICLE IN PRESS
Adverse Impacts of Declining Financial and Health Literacy

FIGURE 1. Illustrates the longitudinal profiles of change in financial and health literacy for 100 (approximate 10%) older adults
randomly selected from our study sample. The left panel is a spaghetti plot that shows the actual data, where each wiggly line traces
annual total literacy scores (y-axis) observed over the years since baseline (x-axis) for a particular participant. For this same group
of participants, the right panel shows individual slopes of change in total literacy scores (gray) as well as the mean slope (black),
estimated from a linear mixed effects model. Figure 1 reveals 2 key features of change in financial and health literacy. A negative
mean slope suggests that on average literacy declines over time in old age. The difference in person-specific slopes suggests that
rates of change vary and decline is not inevitable. Some older adults show faster decline, some slower decline, while a small pro-
portion managed to maintain their literacy level over the years.

cognitive tests. As expected, higher global cognitive Declining financial and health literacy and scam
score was associated with higher decision making susceptibility
score. Importantly, however, the association of declin-
Next, we examined declining financial and health
ing literacy with decision making persisted even after
controlling for cognition, suggesting that the impact literacy in association with scam susceptibility. Multi-
variable regression analysis suggested that partici-
of declining literacy is relatively independent of cog-
nition (Table 2). pants with lower baseline level of financial and health

TABLE 2. Association of Declining Literacy with Financial and Health Decision Making

Model 1 Model 2
Variable B SE p B SE p
Age 0.097 0.010 <0.001 0.075 0.011 <0.001
Male sex 0.061 0.177 0.729 0.364 0.178 0.041
Education 0.081 0.028 0.004 0.065 0.028 0.019
Income 0.115 0.034 <0.001 0.102 0.033 0.002
Cognition - - - 1.078 0.149 <0.001
Baseline literacy 0.045 0.006 <0.001 0.032 0.007 <0.001
Decline in literacy 1.147 0.099 <0.001 0.609 0.123 <0.001

B: unstandardized regression coefficient; SE: standard error.


Statistical inference for model 1 was based on t-statistic with 979 degree of freedom, and statistical inference for model 2 was based on t-statistic
with 961 degree of freedom.

6 Am J Geriatr Psychiatry &&:&&, && 2021


ARTICLE IN PRESS
Yu et al.

FIGURE 2. Illustrates the associations of declining financial and health literacy with financial and health decision making, scam
susceptibility and psychological wellbeing, after controlling for demographics and the baseline literacy level. Each panel is a par-
tial residual plot with corresponding regression line and 95% confidence band. Blue circles are adjusted person-specific slopes of
change in the total literacy score plotted against total decision making score (A), scam susceptibility rating (B) and overall psycho-
logical wellbeing score (C). The covariates of age, sex, education, income and the baseline total literacy score are regressed out.

literacy and separately faster decline in literacy were Similar to the result for financial and health deci-
more susceptible to scams (Table 3). With 1SD of sion making, the association of declining literacy with
additional decline in the financial and health literacy scam susceptibility persisted after the model was con-
score, the scam susceptibility score was 0.34SD higher trolled for cognition. Further, during the annual fol-
(Fig. 2B). By contrast, the strength of association for low-ups into the decision making substudy,
baseline literacy level was approximately half of that approximately 13% of the participants had developed
for declining literacy, such that 1SD lower in the base- dementia. To account for potential recall bias in the
line literacy score corresponds to 0.17SD higher in the scam susceptibility measure among the demented
scam susceptibility score, again suggesting that the people, we reanalyzed the data by restricting to par-
association was stronger for declining literacy than ticipants who were free of dementia throughout the
level. study period. In this subsample (N = 906), declining

TABLE 3. Association of Declining Literacy with Susceptibility to Scams

Model 1 Model 2
Variable B SE p B SE p
Age 0.028 0.003 <0.001 0.021 0.003 <0.001
Male sex 0.265 0.056 <0.001 0.196 0.057 <0.001
Education 0.007 0.009 0.433 0.008 0.009 0.343
Income 0.031 0.011 0.004 0.027 0.011 0.011
Cognition - - - 0.315 0.048 <0.001
Baseline literacy 0.011 0.002 <0.001 0.007 0.002 0.001
Decline in literacy 0.365 0.032 <0.001 0.219 0.039 <0.001

B: unstandardized regression coefficient; SE: standard error.


Statistical inference for model 1 was based on t-statistic with 981 degree of freedom, and statistical inference for model 2 was based on t-statistic
with 965 degree of freedom.

Am J Geriatr Psychiatry &&:&&, && 2021 7


ARTICLE IN PRESS
Adverse Impacts of Declining Financial and Health Literacy

TABLE 4. Association of Declining Literacy With Psychological Wellbeing

Model 1 Model 2
Variable B SE p B SE p
Age 0.019 0.003 <0.001 0.016 0.003 <0.001
Male sex 0.114 0.043 0.008 0.082 0.044 0.060
Education 0.032 0.007 <0.001 0.031 0.007 <0.001
Income 0.019 0.008 0.020 0.017 0.008 0.037
Cognition - - - 0.140 0.037 <0.001
Baseline literacy 0.003 0.002 0.070 0.001 0.002 0.545
Decline in literacy 0.137 0.024 <0.001 0.065 0.030 0.030

B: unstandardized regression coefficient; SE: standard error.


Statistical inference for model 1 was based on t-statistic with 985 degree of freedom, and statistical inference for model 2 was based on t-statistic
with 968 degree of freedom.

literacy remained associated with greater scam sus- financial and health literacy with cognition, disability,
ceptibility (B: 0.341, SE: 0.039, t862: 8.86, p <0.001). mental health,15 as well as decision making
ability.24,38 Financial and health literacy is also an
important correlate of scam susceptibility.12 Of note,
Declining financial and health literacy and
these studies have not examined the degree to which
psychological wellbeing
declining literacy may have longer term impacts on
Finally, we examined the association of declining these outcomes. In the present study, we first demon-
financial and health literacy with psychological well- strated that financial and health literacy declines over
being. After controlling for demographics, we did not time among community-dwelling older adults. Sec-
observe an association of baseline literacy level with ond, we presented for the first time evidence that
psychological wellbeing; however, participants with declining financial and health literacy is associated
faster decline in financial and health literacy exhibited with subsequent decision making performance, scam
lower psychological wellbeing (Table 4). With 1SD of susceptibility and psychological wellbeing. These
additional decline in the literacy score, the overall new findings extend prior research on the impact of
psychological wellbeing score was 0.19SD lower financial and health literacy in several important
(Fig. 2C). In a model that was further controlled for ways.
the global cognitive score, the regression coefficient First, our finding greatly extends a very limited lit-
for declining literacy was attenuated by approxi- erature on change in financial and health literacy in
mately a half but remained nominally significant. A old age. It is hypothesized that domain-specific liter-
similar association was observed in the subset of par- acy may improve with educational programs or
ticipants who had not developed dementia since the decline with progression of age-related pathologic
decision making baseline (B: 0.088, SE: 0.029, t863: processes.39 Yet prior evidence of change in literacy
3.02, p = 0.003). Separate analysis on each of the relies heavily on either cross-sectional data on persons
6 domains of psychological wellbeing suggested that of different age groups or differences in performance
declining literacy was strongly associated with all between only two time points.2,18 Repeated measures
domains except purpose in life (Supplementary of financial and health literacy assessment on the
Results). same individuals are required to robustly estimate
longitudinal change in financial and health literacy
and document whether there is truly an age-related
decline in literacy. The present study leveraged data
CONCLUSIONS
from over 1,000 older residents with up to 11 (and a
Financial and health literacy is an important deter- mean of 5) annual literacy assessments. Two patterns
minant of independence and wellbeing across the life- of change emerged. An average older person declined
span but is particularly critical in old age. Cross- 1 percentage point a year in the total literacy score,
sectional studies have reported associations of and this result depicts a general trend of decline in

8 Am J Geriatr Psychiatry &&:&&, && 2021


ARTICLE IN PRESS
Yu et al.

financial and health literacy among community-living the level of performance at a single point in time.
older adults. Separately, the rate of change varied Thus, the wide individual differences observed in
substantially from person to person. Not everyone financial and health literacy could simply be reflective
experienced declining financial and health literacy of a longstanding and preexisting discrepancy
and a small proportion were able to maintain their lit- between different demographics, rather than an age-
eracy level over the course of follow-ups. Older age, related deterioration. However, with aging, human
female, lower income, fewer years of education, and beings are increasingly exposed to various diseases
impaired cognition at baseline were correlated with (e.g., Alzheimer’s disease and stroke) that degrade
faster decline in financial and health literacy. These many functional abilities including financial and
results are consistent with a previous report from a health literacy. Of note, the capacity for tolerating
smaller sample with less follow-ups.32 Future studies these disease insults differs between persons. Varying
are needed to identify modifiable determinants of abilities of tolerating disease pathology may result in
declining financial and health literacy. person-specific rates of decline in literacy and further
Second, the present study expands prior work on contribute to individual differences in financial and
the impact of domain-specific literacy to include a health literacy in late life. Indeed, our results show
critically important mental health outcome, psycho- that the association of declining financial and health lit-
logical wellbeing. Wellbeing is a multidimensional eracy with subsequent outcomes was above and
construct that refers to the sense that one is striving, beyond the starting level of literacy. More importantly,
emotionally connected and balanced, and generally judging by the regression coefficients, the strength of
functioning well mentally. Wellbeing is related to associations with all three outcomes (i.e., financial and
multiple psychological and health outcomes in old health decision making, scam susceptibility and psy-
age yet has not been a focus of literacy studies.40 chological wellbeing) are stronger for decline than
Here, we showed that declining literacy is not only level, suggesting that it is actually the aging-related
related to lower wellbeing in general, but specifically decline in literacy that has the most potent impact on
the tendency to have lower self-acceptance, lack of key outcomes. These results highlight the importance
self-determination, feeling easily overwhelmed, hav- of capturing and possibly reversing age-related decline
ing difficulty maintaining positive relationships, and in financial and health literacy.
limited focus on personal growth. This greatly Inadequate financial and health literacy in old age
expands our understanding of the health consequen- presents a formidable economic and public health
ces of declining literacy and suggests that it affects challenge. Boomers are the most vulnerable age
domains of functioning not previously considered. group that can suffer from unrecoverable financial
Older adults already face many challenges to their loss and severe health consequences due to poor liter-
psychological health (e.g., disease-related, loss of acy and decision making. While possessing the vast
friends and family), and we show that declining liter- majority of financial resources, older adults unfortu-
acy presents an additional source of distress. Fortu- nately lack the flexibility to bounce back from finan-
nately, unlike many other challenges, literacy is cial setbacks and are least protected by financial
modifiable. Thus, efforts to prevent or reduce declin- regulations.17 Various diseases common in old age
ing literacy in old age may confer meaningful psycho- impair cognitive and physical functions and further
logical benefit. worsen the situation health-wise. By showing that
Third, this study disentangles the influence of lon- declining financial and health literacy impacts a vari-
gitudinal decline in financial and health literacy from ety of adverse outcomes, yet that decline is not ubiq-
that of starting level of literacy. While cross-sectional uitous and potentially preventable or reversible, the
data are crucial in evaluating the impact of financial present study has several important real-world impli-
and health literacy on important outcomes, an impor- cations. First, it demonstrates that the impact of finan-
tant limitation is that the level and change are con- cial and health literacy is not confined to the starting
founded. Financial and health literacy is strongly level accumulated throughout the life-span; rather,
influenced by life experience factors such as educa- change in literacy over time is a more important
tion, occupation and contextual factors like systemic determinant of decision making and other wellbeing
racism and sexism, and these factors greatly influence outcomes in old age. Second, the associations of

Am J Geriatr Psychiatry &&:&&, && 2021 9


ARTICLE IN PRESS
Adverse Impacts of Declining Financial and Health Literacy

declining literacy persisted even after controlling for that MAP is a voluntary cohort which limits the gen-
cognition, which supports the concept that domain- eralizability of our findings. Over 90% of the partici-
specific literacy is a higher order brain function dis- pants in the present study are non-Latino whites.
tinct from memory or other traditionally measured Annual decision making assessments have been
cognitive abilities. In this context, declining financial expanded to a separate cohort of older African Ameri-
and health literacy may represent a novel harbinger cans in 2017. As data mature, we will be able to exam-
of adverse outcomes, and regular monitoring of ine whether the same associations can be replicated in
financial and health literacy could serve as a useful the minority population.
tool to identify individuals at risk of impending
impairment in decision making and other abilities.
The result further suggests that interventions aimed
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
to maintain or improve literacy may be beneficial for
behavioral economic and psychological outcomes LY, GM and PAB contributed to the conception
regardless of individuals’ level of cognitive perfor- and design of the work; DAB and PAB contributed to
mance. Third, the present study highlights the poten- the acquisition of data for the work; LY contributed to
tial of health and financial literacy as a primary target the analysis of data for the work; all authors contrib-
of prevention against poor decision making or loss of uted to the interpretation of data for the work. LY
overall wellbeing, both of which have critical adverse drafted the work and all authors contributed to revis-
effects. ing it critically for important intellectual content.
The present study underscores the critical need for
efforts to maintain or even improve financial and
health literacy among older persons. Evidence
DISCLOSURE
suggests that educational programs (e.g., eHealth lit-
eracy intervention) that are designed to boost under- Nothing to disclose.
standing of financial and healthcare concepts are well
received by recipients and have positive effects on
real-life financial and healthcare actions taken by
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
older adults.41 Importantly, our findings suggest that
the benefits of such interventions also may extend to Funding for this study comes from the National Insti-
the psychological domain. Thus, targeted efforts to tute on Aging (R01AG17917, R01AG33678, and
increase both the availability and usability of literacy R01AG34374) and the FINRA Investor Education Foun-
interventions for older adults hold the potential to dation. All results, interpretations, and conclusions
offer considerable public health benefit and should be expressed are those of the research team alone, and do not
prioritized. necessarily represent the views of the National Institute of
Strengths and limitations are discussed. By embed- Aging or of the FINRA Investor Education Foundation or
ding annual decision making assessments into an any of its affiliated companies. This study would not have
ongoing epidemiologic cohort study of well-charac- been possible without the contributions of the participants
terized community-dwelling older persons that has a from the Rush Memory and Aging Project, as well as
high rate of follow-up, it allows us to robustly esti- investigators and staff at Rush Alzheimer’s Disease Center
mate the longitudinal change in financial and health (RADC). Data used in this study can be requested for
literacy. The analytic approach specifically targets the research purpose through the RADC Research Resource
rate of change in financial and health literacy that pre- Sharing Hub at https://www.radc.rush.edu.
cedes the outcomes of interest. This temporal relation-
ship between the predictor and the outcomes
facilitates the investigation on the consequences of
SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIALS
declining literacy. The instruments used for testing
financial and health literacy, decision making, scam Supplementary material associated with this article
susceptibility and psychological wellbeing have been can be found, in the online version, at https://doi.
validated in various publications. Limitations include org/10.1016/j.jagp.2021.02.042.

10 Am J Geriatr Psychiatry &&:&&, && 2021


ARTICLE IN PRESS
Yu et al.

References
1. Kindig DA, Panzer AM, Nielsen-Bohlman L: Health Literacy: a 21. Paasche-Orlow MK, Wolf MS: The causal pathways linking health
Prescription to End Confusion. National Academies Press, literacy to health outcomes. Am J Health Behav 2007; 31(Suppl
2004 1):S19–S26
2. Baker DW, Gazmararian JA, Sudano J, et al: The association 22. von Wagner C, Steptoe A, Wolf MS, et al: Health literacy and
between age and health literacy among elderly persons. J Geron- health actions: a review and a framework from health psychol-
tol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2000; 55:S368–S374 ogy. Health education & behavior: the official publication of the
3. Lusardi A, Mitchell OS: Financial Literacy and Planning: Implica- Society for. Pub Health Educ 2009; 36:860–877
tions for Retirement Wellbeing. National Bureau of Economic 23. Bennett DA, Schneider JA, Buchman AS, et al: Overview and find-
Research, 2011 ings from the Rush Memory and Aging Project. Curr Alzheimer
4. Lusardi A, Mitchell OS, Curto V: Financial literacy and financial Res 2012; 9:646–663
sophistication in the older population. J Pension Economics 24. James BD, Boyle PA, Bennett JS, et al: The impact of health and
Finance 2014; 13:347–366 financial literacy on decision making in community-based older
5. Williams MV, Parker RM, Baker DW, et al: Inadequate functional adults. Gerontology 2012; 58:531–539
health literacy among patients at two public hospitals. JAMA 25. Finucane ML, Gullion CM: Developing a tool for measuring the
1995; 274:1677–1682 decision-making competence of older adults. Psychol Aging
6. Sudore RL, Mehta KM, Simonsick EM, et al: Limited literacy in 2010; 25:271–288
older people and disparities in health and healthcare access. 26. Han SD, Boyle PA, James BD, et al: Mild cognitive impairment is
J Am Geriatr Soc 2006; 54:770–776 associated with poorer decision-making in community-based
7. Gazmararian JA, Baker DW, Williams MV, et al: Health literacy older persons. J Am Geriatr Soc 2015; 63:676–683
among Medicare enrollees in a managed care organization. JAMA 27. Boyle PA, Yu L, Schneider JA, et al: Scam awareness related to
1999; 281:545–551 incident alzheimer dementia and mild cognitive impairment: a
8. Scott TL, Gazmararian JA, Williams MV, et al: Health prospective cohort study. Ann Intern Med 2019; 170:702–709
literacy and preventive health care use among Medicare 28. Ryff CD, Keyes CL: The structure of psychological well-being
enrollees in a managed care organization. Med Care 2002; revisited. J Pers Soc Psychol 1995; 69:719–727
40:395–404 29. Wilson RS, Boyle PA, Segawa E, et al: The influence of cognitive
9. Wood S, Hanoch Y, Barnes A, et al: Numeracy and Medicare Part decline on well-being in old age. Psychol Aging 2013; 28:304–313
D: the importance of choice and literacy for numbers in optimiz- 30. Wilson RS, Boyle PA, Yang J, et al: Early life instruction in foreign
ing decision making for Medicare’s prescription drug program. language and music and incidence of mild cognitive impairment.
Psychol Aging 2011; 26:295–307 Neuropsychology 2015; 29:292–302
10. Braun RT, Barnes AJ, Hanoch Y, et al: Health literacy and plan 31. Wilson RS, Barnes LL, Krueger KR, et al: Early and late life cogni-
choice: implications for medicare managed care. Health Literacy tive activity and cognitive systems in old age. J Int Neuro Soc
Res Pract 2018; 2:e40–e54 2005; 11:400–407
11. Lusardi A, Mitchell OS: Baby boomer retirement security: the 32. Yu L, Wilson RS, Han SD, et al: Decline in literacy and incident
roles of planning, financial literacy, and housing wealth. J Mone- AD dementia among community-dwelling older persons. J Aging
tary Econ 2007; 54:205–224 Health 2017, 0898264317716361
12. James BD, Boyle PA, Bennett DA: Correlates of susceptibility to 33. Angrisani M, Burke JR, Lusardi A, et al: The stability and predic-
scams in older adults without dementia. J Elder Abuse Neglect tive power of financial literacy: evidence from longitudinal data.
2014; 26:107–122 Manag Sci 2020, under review
13. Engels C, Kumar K, Philip D: Financial literacy and fraud detec- 34. Wilson RS, Yu L, James BD, et al: Association of financial and
tion. Eur J Finance 2020; 26:420–442 health literacy with cognitive health in old age. Neuro Develop
14. Wolf MS, Gazmararian JA, Baker DW: Health literacy and func- Cognit 2017; 24:186–197
tional health status among older adults. Arch Intern Med 2005; 35. Boyle PA, Yu L, Wilson RS, et al: Cognitive decline impairs finan-
165:1946–1952 cial and health literacy among community-based older persons
15. Bennett JS, Boyle PA, James BD, et al: Correlates of health and without dementia. Psychol Aging 2013; 28:614–624
financial literacy in older adults without dementia. BMC Geriat- 36. Wang Y, Ruhe G: The cognitive process of decision making. Int J
rics 2012; 12:30 Cognit Infor Natural Intell (IJCINI) 2007; 1:73–85
16. Finke MS, Howe JS, Huston SJ: Old age and the decline in finan- 37. Federman AD, Sano M, Wolf MS, et al: Health literacy and cogni-
cial literacy. Manage Sci 2017; 63:213–230 tive performance in older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc 2009;
17. Agarwal S, Driscoll JC, Gabaix X, et al: The age of reason: finan- 57:1475–1480
cial decisions over the life cycle and implications for regulation. 38. Stewart CC, Yu L, Wilson RS, et al: Correlates of healthcare and
Brookings Pap Econ Activity 2009; 2009:51–117 financial decision making among older adults without dementia.
18. Kobayashi LC, Wardle J, Wolf MS, et al: Cognitive function and Health Psychol 2018; 37:618–626
health literacy decline in a cohort of aging English adults. J Gen 39. Baker DW: The meaning and the measure of health literacy.
Intern Med 2015; 30:958–964 J Gen Intern Med 2006; 21:878–883
19. Gillen M, Yang H, Kim H: Health literacy and difference in cur- 40. Hernandez R, Bassett SM, Boughton SW, et al: Psychological
rent wealth among middle-aged and older adults. J Family Econ well-being and physical health: Associations, mechanisms, and
Issues 2020; 41:281–299 future directions. Emotion Rev 2018; 10:18–29
20. Lusardi A, Mitchell OS: The economic importance of financial lit- 41. Xie B: Effects of an eHealth literacy intervention for older adults.
eracy: Theory and evidence. J Econ Lit 2014; 52:5–44 J Med Intern Re 2011; 13:e90

Am J Geriatr Psychiatry &&:&&, && 2021 11

You might also like