Rabelo 2014

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

Psychol Rec (2014) 64:71–77

DOI 10.1007/s40732-014-0006-2

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Dolls are for Girls and Not for Boys: Evaluating


the Appropriateness of the Implicit Relational Assessment
Procedure for School-Age Children
Laura Z. Rabelo & Renato Bortoloti & Debora H. Souza

Published online: 29 March 2014


# Association of Behavior Analysis International 2014

Abstract The Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure A common bias found in psychological interviews and ques-
(IRAP) has been used for assessing implicit attitudes across tionnaires is the general tendency for participants to respond in
different domains. The current study aimed to investigate a manner that they consider to be socially desirable, especially
whether the IRAP is an adequate measure to assess children’s when the questions asked concern controversial matters
implicit gender-based attitudes toward toys (dolls and toy cars). (Barnes-Holmes et al. 2010a). For instance, someone may
Although the software parameters used were taken from previ- believe that women are bad drivers but state that women are
ous adult studies, children performed successfully in the task. excellent drivers in a self-report questionnaire in order to
The strongest IRAP effect found was for relations between avoid being regarded as sexist. The Implicit Association Test
dolls and girls (i.e., participants responded faster when relating (IAT) was designed as an alternative to explicit measures
dolls-girls-match than dolls-boys-match). Additionally, chil- which are known to be susceptible to social desirability effects
dren’s response latencies were longer for pairings of dolls and (Greenwald et al. 1998).
boys than for pairings of toy cars and girls. These findings seem The IAT consists of simultaneous presentation of two
to confirm that the IRAP is sensitive to gender-stereotyped attributive concepts and a target stimulus on a computer
relations established in children and may be a useful tool in screen. Participants are required to respond by pressing one
providing additional information to complement explicit mea- of two keys on the keyboard, each associated with one of
sures in children’s attitudes. the two concepts. The investigator can work with multiple
target stimuli that are alternated across trials, and the main
variable of interest is the speed of responding. Greenwald
Keywords IRAP . Children . Gender-related toys et al. (1998), for example, used different names of flowers
and insects as target stimuli and two evaluation attributes:
“pleasant” and “unpleasant.” In one block of trials, partic-
The first author was supported by FAPESP during the development of ipants were asked to relate the different flowers to the
this research (#2009/04736-6) and by CAPES during the preparation of
the manuscript. Renato Bortoloti was supported by FAPESP through a attribute “pleasant” and the different insects to “unpleas-
Young Investigator grant (#2011/50561-3). This study is part of the ant”; in another block, participants had to associate the
research program of Instituto Nacional de Ciência e Tecnologia sobre different flowers with “unpleasant” and the different in-
Comportamento, Cognição e Ensino (Deisy G. de Souza, Chairperson), sects with “pleasant.” The difference in mean latency of
supported by grants from the Brazilian National Research Council–CNPq
(Grant #573972/2008-7) and FAPESP (Grant #08/57705-8). We thank responding between these two blocks of trials is the IAT
Julio C. de Rose for his ongoing and invaluable support. Finally, we thank effect. The underlying assumption is that participants re-
Dr. Dermot Barnes-Holmes and two anonymous reviewers for their spond faster in consistent trials, that is, trials that involve
helpful suggestions. more probable associations. In Greenwald et al.’s (1998)
L. Z. Rabelo (*) : R. Bortoloti : D. H. Souza study, the concepts of flower and pleasantness were ex-
Universidade Federal de São Carlos, and Instituto Nacional de pected to be more readily associated than flower-
Ciência e Tecnologia sobre Comportamento, Cognição e Ensino,
unpleasant, and the concept of insect-unpleasant was ex-
Rodovia Washington Luís, Km 235, São Carlos,
SP 13565-905, Brazil pected to be more readily associated than insect-pleasant.
e-mail: laurazrabelo@gmail.com The data did support this hypothesis: Participants responded
72 Psychol Rec (2014) 64:71–77

faster in the consistent condition (i.e., flower-pleasant and toward work in North Americans and Irish individuals
insect-unpleasant) than in the inconsistent one (i.e., flower- (Chan et al. 2009); self-esteem (Timko et al. 2010); and,
unpleasant and insect-pleasant). more recently, fear of spiders (Nicholson and Barnes-
Despite its usefulness in a wide range of situations, the Holmes 2012). The IRAP has also proved to be a useful
IAT procedure has some limitations (e.g., Barnes-Holmes instrument for evaluating the relatedness of equivalent
et al. 2006). In particular, the IAT provides an assessment of stimuli (Bortoloti and de Rose 2012).
associative strength, which does not provide enough infor- Similar to the IAT effect, the IRAP effect refers to the
mation regarding participants’ implicit attitudes or beliefs. difference in the mean response latency between consistent
For example, if participants respond faster to pairings of and inconsistent trials, and is interpreted as evidence of atti-
pictures of White people and positive attributes (as well as tude bias. Thus, shorter response latencies for obese-negative
to associations of Black people and negative words) than relations compared to longer response latencies for obese-
when these associations are switched, more than one inter- positive during IRAP is taken to indicate that participants
pretation is possible. It cannot be known if Black and White agree with the former relations. The IRAP effect indicating
people are both liked but White people are preferred over bias has been demonstrated and replicated across different
Black, or if they are both disliked and Black people are domains, and the IRAP has shown predictive validity compa-
more disliked than White. In sum, the IAT effect fails to rable to the IAT (e.g., Barnes-Holmes et al. 2009, 2010b;
indicate to what degree the stimuli are liked or disliked Roddy et al. 2010). The IAT has been used successfully with
individually (Barnes-Holmes et al. 2010a). children (e.g., Baron and Banaji 2006; Cvencek et al. 2011;
The IRAP aims to overcome this and other limitations by Dunham et al. 2006). Scanlon has unpublished data on the use
treating implicit cognition as a relational rather than an asso- of the IRAP to assess self-esteem in children aged 8 to 14 years
ciative process. In essence, it is a computer-based procedure with and without ADHD (as cited in O’Toole et al. 2009).
that measures the latency of relational responses when the However, to the extent of our knowledge, there is still no
participant is under pressure to respond quickly and accurate- published work reporting IRAP use with children; data on
ly. Another advantage of the IRAP is that it involves four trial children’s implicit attitudes and beliefs may be especially
types (Barnes-Holmes et al. 2010a). More specifically, each relevant to research gender and race stereotypes. Moreover,
trial involves the simultaneous presentation of one label IRAP studies might contribute to a better understanding of the
stimulus, one target item, and two relational terms. Its development of intergroup biases in children and adolescents
underlying rationale is that participants’ responses will which, in turn, represents the theoretical foundation for inter-
have shorter mean response latencies when asked to vention programs designed to prevent discrimination and
confirm consistent relations than when asked to confirm prejudice early in childhood (Brown and Bigler 2005;
inconsistent relations, as the former present relations that Brown et al. 2011; Leaper 2011; Pahlke et al. 2012).
may accord with participants’ preexperimental learning histo- Therefore, the present study aimed to evaluate the adequacy
ries (e.g., flower-pleasant-true may be considered consistent, of the IRAP for studies with school-age children. In particular,
insect-pleasant-true may be considered false; Barnes-Holmes we were interested in assessing children’s implicit attitudes
et al. 2010a). toward toys that may be traditionally perceived as gender
The IRAP has been used successfully to assess implicit specific in the wider social community, for example, dolls
attitudes toward sensitive social matters. One of the strengths for girls and cars for boys. The IRAP was used to measure
of the IRAP is that it appears difficult to fake, even if partic- children’s attitudes toward toys, that is, to assess whether they
ipants are asked to perform the task after receiving an expla- display gender stereotyped attitudes by responding faster
nation of how the software works (Mckenna et al. 2007). In when asked to relate specific toys traditionally associated with
recent years, the IRAP has been used to investigate several either girls or boys.
matters, such as preference for national groups (Power et al.
2009); attitudes toward BDSM (bondage, discipline/
dominance, sadism, and masochism) and fetish in groups of Method
students and individuals with BDSM/fetish interest
(Stockwell et al. 2010); pro-White and anti-Black stereotyped Participants
attitudes in public and private contexts in Irish individuals
(Barnes-Holmes et al. 2010a); attitudes toward autism in Ten children, six boys and four girls, ranging in age from 7 to
professionals who have worked with individuals with au- 10 years (M=8.7, SD=1.24) participated in the present study.
tism for less than 6 months, more than 6 months, and people They had previously participated in other experiments
who have never worked with them (Barnes-Holmes et al. (on stimulus equivalence or do-say correspondence) conduct-
2006); attitudes toward meat and vegetables in meat-eaters ed at the laboratory where the two first authors work. All of
and vegetarians (Barnes-Holmes et al. 2010b); attitudes them were enrolled in public elementary schools in a town
Psychol Rec (2014) 64:71–77 73

located in the state of São Paulo, Brazil. Their parents signed Procedure
an informed consent form prior to their participation. They
were randomly assigned to one of two experimental condi- Each child was asked to sit in front of the computer. The
tions: Condition A presented gender-stereotype-consistent tri- experimenter sat beside the child and told him/her the follow-
al blocks first and Condition B presented gender-stereotype- ing short story and provided the following instructions:
inconsistent blocks first.
Once upon a time, there were two siblings: John and
Mary. They used to play together a lot, but after each
Setting time they played, they would put their toys away in a
chest. You are going to take part in a game during which
The experiment was conducted in a quiet room in the you will tell me which toys from the chest match each
Department of Psychology at Universidade Federal de São one of the siblings. Pay close attention to what I’m
Carlos. Participants completed the IRAP individually on a asking you to do. First, we are going to play the game
desktop computer, but the researcher stayed in the room together, so you can learn the rules; then, you will play it
throughout the entire session, giving instructions at every by yourself. A name (John or Mary) is going to appear
phase of the experiment. on the computer screen along with the picture of a toy
(a doll or a toy car). If the screen shows Mary’s name and
the picture of a doll, press the green key in order to say
Apparatus and Materials that the doll matches Mary. You will do the same thing
when John’s name and the picture of a toy car appear
The IRAP software (an updated version is available at www. together. If you see Mary’s name and a toy car, press the
irapresearch.org) was used to present the stimuli and record red key to indicate the car does not match her. You will do
participants’ responses. Each trial displayed two stimuli and two the same if the screen shows John’s name and a doll.
response options on a white background. Participants responded Even if you think differently, please, do as I say.
by pressing one of two keys on the computer keyboard.
The response options were: “It matches” and “It doesn’t The instructions were read to participants in Condition A;
match” (translated from the Brazilian Portuguese expressions the children assigned to Condition B received similar instruc-
“Combina” and “Não combina”). The stimuli were two tions, except for the last segment: “If the screen shows Mary
names, Maria and João (Mary and John in English), and and the picture of a doll, you press the red key to indicate that
colored pictures of two toy cars and two dolls (see Fig. 1 for the doll does not match Mary. You will do the same if it shows
a pictorial representation of the task). There was a green John’s name and a car.”
sticker on top of the d key on the keyboard and a red one on The adopted parameters were very similar to those used in
top of the k key. adult studies utilizing the IRAP. Children were presented with

Fig. 1 The four IRAP trial types.


Names (John or Mary), toy
John John
pictures, and response options
were presented all together on
each trial. Arrows and text boxes
were not presented on screen Consistent Inconsistent Inconsistent Consistent

It matches It doesn’t match It matches It doesn’t match

Mary Mary

Consistent Inconsistent Inconsistent Consistent

It matches It doesn’t match It matches It doesn’t match


74 Psychol Rec (2014) 64:71–77

eight blocks of 24 trials, with each toy picture presented four car-matches, Mary-toy car- does not match, John-doll-does not
times with each of the names (John and Mary) in the trials. match) and in the inconsistent blocks (Mary-toy car-matches,
The trials were presented in a quasirandom sequence—two John-doll-matches, Mary-doll-does not match, John-toy
identical trials could not be presented consecutively. In adult car-does not match).
studies, the position of the response options (at the bottom
right and bottom left corners of the screen) is frequently varied
across trials in a counterbalanced manner. In the present study, Results
the response options were fixed in order to facilitate children’s
learning of the task. Nine children were able to perform the task successfully. One
Two blocks were assigned as practice (one of consistent boy (P9) did not reach the practice criteria after three training
trials and one of inconsistent trials, following the procedure blocks and was thus excluded from the study.
used in adult studies), and the remaining six were test blocks. Individual response latency data were transformed into D-
At the beginning of the session, the participants were informed IRAP scores using the D-IRAP algorithm provided by the
that they were going to learn how to “play the game.” The IRAP software. Latencies for the gender-stereotyped-
experimenter told them to pay attention to what they were inconsistent blocks were subtracted from latencies for the
doing, and that making mistakes at the beginning was all right. consistent blocks. Thus, positive D-IRAP scores indicate that
Participants had to reach a minimum criterion of 70 % correct the participants responded more rapidly in the consistent
answers within a mean latency of 3,000 ms. If they did not blocks, whereas negative D-IRAP scores indicate more rapid
reach the criteria on the first practice block, they were exposed responding in the inconsistent blocks. The overall mean D-
up to twice more to that block. If they still could not reach IRAP score was positive (0.162) and significantly different
criteria, they were told that the game was over and that their from zero (t=2.33, p<0.05), indicating an overall D-IRAP
participation had ended. The experimenter then gave the child effect that was consistent with a gender-stereotyped effect for
a sticker and thanked him/her for participating. dolls and cars. Mean IRAP scores for each of the four trial-
During test blocks, the children were told they would have types are presented in Fig. 2.
to do the same as before, but from that moment on, they had to Participants were faster to respond that dolls matched Mary
“go fast” and try not to make mistakes. After the first testing (the mean D-IRAP score was 0.32 and different from zero, t=
block, the experimenter told the child that they were going to 2.02, p<0.05) and that dolls did not match John (the mean D-
play the game differently next, because they would have to IRAP score was 0.22 and different from zero, t=2.06, p<0.05)
give opposite answers in each trial. If they had pressed the than when the opposite responses were required. The trials
green key when seeing “Mary” and the picture of a doll during involving toy cars produced relatively small and nonsignifi-
the previous trials, now they would have to press the red key cant IRAP effects (the mean D-IRAP score for cars and Mary
to state that they do not match. The experimenter described in was 0.05, t=0.29, p>0.05, and for cars and John, it was 0.06,
detail how the child should proceed in each of the four t=1.02, p>0.05), suggesting that the children responded with
different trial types (John-matches/does not match-toy car; roughly equal speed when asked if cars matched or did not
John-matches/does not match-doll; Mary-matches/does not match John/Mary.
match-doll; Mary-matches/does not match-toy car, see
Fig. 1) and after giving the instructions, she asked the partic-
ipant to tell her with his/her own words what she/he was Discussion
supposed to do next.
For participants in Condition A, the gender-stereotyped- As predicted, participants in the current study (n=9) showed
consistent blocks were Numbers 1, 3, and 5. Condition B gender-stereotyped attitudes toward toys, and the effect was
participants started with an inconsistent block, and the greater for the Mary-doll-matches and John-doll-does not
gender-stereotyped-consistent ones were Numbers 2, 4, and match trial types. In spite of the fact that a plethora of research
6. When the experiment was finished, participants were has been produced recently, we could not find any published
thanked and received a sticker for their participation. studies that utilized the IRAP to investigate implicit attitudes
in children. In the same direction as Scanlon’s unpublished
Data Analysis doctoral work (as cited in O’Toole et al. 2009), however, our
findings suggest that this is a ripe area for research enquiry.
Response latency was measured by the time in milliseconds To set up the experimental tasks employed in the current
that elapsed between the onset of the trial and the emission of study, we considered evidence suggesting gender differences
the response. Latency data were processed using the D-IRAP in children’s play behavior and toy preference (Blakemore and
algorithm, available in the IRAP software for each trial Centers 2005; see Leaper and Bigler 2011 and Smith 2010, for
type in the consistent blocks (Mary-doll-matches, John-toy reviews). It is still not known, however, to what extent
Psychol Rec (2014) 64:71–77 75

Fig. 2 Participant’s mean


D-IRAP scores across four
different IRAP trial types

phylogenetic, ontogenetic, and/or cultural factors may explain instructions given, making sure that they had understood the
these differences. Nevertheless, the notion of gender-specific nature of the task. Nonetheless, future studies should evaluate
toys is widely spread in the Western world: dolls are for girls the IRAP effect for other types of toys that are representative
and not for boys. Moreover, there is evidence that parents of children’s preferences, such as a dollhouse for girls and a
reinforce gender-stereotypic play and toy preferences tool kit for boys, avoiding, therefore, a possible confound.
(Langlois and Downs 1980; Lytton and Romney 1991). It is important to note that our findings are consistent with
Even parents with gender-egalitarian attitudes and beliefs previous research showing that boys and girls receive differ-
frequently act in gender-stereotyped ways with their children ential treatment with regard to their toy and play preferences.
(Friedman et al. 2007). Similarly, the present research showed More specifically, boys are more severely punished than girls
that children respond faster to gender typical associations when playing with cross-sex toys (Langlois and Downs
(Mary matches dolls and John does not match dolls) than to 1980), and they suffer more peer pressure than girls to behave
gender atypical associations (Mary does not match dolls and in gender-normative ways (e.g., Carter and Mccloskey 1984).
John matches dolls). More specifically, as can be seen in This differential treatment might explain why we did not find
Fig. 2, the IRAP effect was stronger for the following rela- a large IRAP effect in the trials involving toy cars. That is, the
tions: Mary-matches-dolls; John-does not match-dolls. IRAP effects for John and toy car trials was similar to the one
One question might be raised concerning the pictorial found for Mary and toy car trials. Therefore, girls may have
stimuli used in the present experiment: Girl dolls and girl more flexibility than boys with regard to their toy choices. The
names may be more easily matched than girl dolls and boys social message they receive seems to be: “It is all right for girls
names, and this difference may be unrelated to gender to play with cars, but it is not okay for boys to play with dolls.”
stereotyping. However, the choice of stimuli (girl dolls and The present research also suggests that the IRAP may be an
toy cars) was based on previous studies investigating gender adequate procedure for use with children, since the majority of
differences on Brazilian children’s toy preferences participants were able to complete the sessions successfully.
(Wanderlind et al. 2006). More specifically, girl dolls are still However, considering that one child did not reach the practice
rated as a favorite toy among girls. Boys, on the other hand, criteria and thus was unable to proceed to the testing blocks,
present a preference for toys from the technical world, such as possible intervening variables should be further investigated.
vehicles and robots. As the current study was the first to test a In this particular case, the child’s inability to advance to test
possible IRAP effect on children’s preferences for toys tradi- blocks might be related to his poor reading skills (as observed
tionally perceived as gender specific in Brazil, we aimed to in previous studies in which the child participated). Indeed, it
find stimuli that should be prototypically related with each may be possible to use the IRAP with children even younger
gender in the Brazilian culture. Moreover, before running than those employed in the current study, but one constraint
trials, the experimenter asked the child to repeat the may be reading ability, particularly when written words are
76 Psychol Rec (2014) 64:71–77

used as stimuli in the IRAP. Additionally, it would be inter- open many possibilities to explore aspects of children’s cog-
esting to test the IRAP procedure using only pictures as nition that cannot be assessed by explicit measures, especially
stimuli when testing children who do not have good reading when they do not have sufficiently sophisticated verbal reper-
skills. However, the software version used in the current toires to express their views on a particular topic.
study did not allow for programming trials with pictorial
stimuli only.
The current study was exploratory in that there are no References
recommended parameters for children and it is not known
how they affect IRAP responses in this population. We used
Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Power, P., Hayden, E., Milne, R.,
software parameters commonly used in adult studies.
& Stewart, I. (2006). Do you really know what you believe?
Although participants were able to perform the task success- Developing the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP)
fully, it is important to address optimum experimental param- as a direct measure of implicit beliefs. The Irish Psychologist, 32(7),
eters for children in future studies. One aspect that should be 169–177.
Barnes-Holmes, D., Waldron, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Stewart, I.
taken into consideration is the latency constraint. Barnes-
(2009). Testing the validity of the Implicit Relational Assessment
Holmes et al. (2010a) found that when a relatively short Procedure and the Implicit Association Test: measuring attitudes
latency constraint was employed (i.e. 3 s instead of 2 s), toward Dublin and country life in Ireland. The Psychological
measures of pro-White and anti-Black racial stereotyping Record, 59, 389–406.
Barnes-Holmes, D., Murphy, A., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Stewart, I.
observed with the IRAP increased among White Irish individ-
(2010a). The Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure: exploring
uals. The role of shorter latencies on children’s performance is the impact of private versus public contexts and the response latency
not known and should be investigated. Another aspect of the criterion on pro-white and anti-black stereotyping among white Irish
task that needs to be addressed in the future is whether having individuals. The Psychological Record, 60, 43–66.
Barnes-Holmes, D., Murtagh, L., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Stewart, I.
the response options always displayed at the same position
(2010b). Using the implicit association test and the implicit relation-
(“It matches” at the bottom right corner of the screen and “It al assessment procedure to measure attitudes toward meat and
doesn’t match” at the bottom left corner) has an effect on vegetables in vegetarians and meat-eaters. The Psychological
participants’ responses. Record, 60, 287–306.
Baron, A. S., & Banaji, M. R. (2006). The development of implicit
The current findings should be interpreted with caution,
attitudes evidence of race evaluations from ages 6 and 10 and
given the limited number of participants, which precluded us adulthood. Psychological Science, 17(1), 53–58. doi:10.1111/j.
from testing possible effects of order of block presentation 1467-9280.2005.01664.x.
(consistent vs. inconsistent), as well as possible effects of Blakemore, J. E. O., & Centers, R. E. (2005). Characteristics of boys’ and
participant’s gender. The data from adult studies, however, girls’ toys. Sex Roles, 53, 619–633.
Bortoloti, R., & de Rose, J. C. (2012). Equivalent stimuli are more
do not show effects of order of block presentation. strongly related after training with delayed matching than after
Nonetheless, the fact that an IRAP effect was found in such simultaneous matching: a study using the Implicit Relational
a small sample is impressive and suggests that this is a Assessment Procedure (IRAP). The Psychological Record,
promising line of investigation. 62, 41–54.
Brown, C. S., & Bigler, R. S. (2005). Children’s perceptions of discrim-
Future studies should also include explicit measures to ination: a developmental model. Child Development, 76(3),
check whether they match the results obtained with the 533–553.
IRAP. In the current study, we asked all participants to indicate Brown, C. S., Alabi, B. O., Huynh, V. W., & Masten, C. L. (2011).
(using a 5-point Likert scale) whether the toy was only for Ethnicity and gender in late childhood and early adolescence: group
identity and awareness of bias. Developmental Psychology,
boys, more for boys, for both, more for girls, only for girls, but 47(2), 463.
we found two main problems with this explicit measure: first, Carter, D. B., & Mccloskey, L. A. (1984). Peers and the maintenance of
because the words for boy and girl in Portuguese are very sex-typed behavior: the development of children’s conceptions of
similar (there is only a change in the last letter—menino vs. cross-gender behavior in their peers. Social Cognition, 2, 294–314.
Chan, G., Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Stewart, I. (2009).
menina), children may have misheard or misread the options; Implicit attitudes to work and leisure among North American and
second, we did not use pictorial stimuli to represent each point Irish individuals: a preliminary study. International Journal of
of the scale, as done in previous studies. As a result, we Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 9(3), 317–334.
decided not to include the data. Cvencek, D., Meltzoff, A. N., & Greenwald, A. G. (2011). Math–gender
stereotypes in elementary school children. Child Development,
In conclusion, our attempt to adapt the IRAP to children 82(3), 766–779. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01529.x.
was successful. Nonetheless, some procedural variables Dunham, Y., Baron, A. S., & Banaji, M. R. (2006). From American city to
which may influence participants’ responses remain un- Japanese village: a cross-cultural investigation of implicit race atti-
known. In particular, the IRAP may not be suitable for use tudes. Child Development, 77(5), 1268–1281. doi:10.1111/j.1467-
8624.2006.00933.x.
with children with limited reading ability, although this hy- Friedman, C. K., Leaper, C., & Bigler, R. (2007). Do mothers’ gender
pothesis should be tested by further investigation. The appro- related attitudes or comments predict young children’s gender be-
priateness of the IRAP for use with school-age children might liefs? Parenting: Science and Practice, 7, 1–15.
Psychol Rec (2014) 64:71–77 77

Greenwald, A. G., Mcghee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Pahlke, E., Bigler, R. S., & Suizzo, M. A. (2012). Relations between
Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the Implicit colorblind socialization and children’s racial bias: evidence from
Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, European American mothers and their preschool children. Child
1464–1480. Development, 83, 1164–1179. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01770.x.
Langlois, J. H., & Downs, A. C. (1980). Mothers, fathers, and peers as Power, P., Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Stewart, I. (2009).
socialization agents of sex-typed play behaviors in young children. The Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP) as a measure
Child Development, 51, 1217–1247. of implicit relative preferences: a first study. The Psychological
Leaper, C. (2011). Research in developmental psychology on gender and Record, 59, 621–640.
relationships: Reflections on the past and looking into the future. Roddy, S., Stewart, I., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2010). Anti-fat, pro-slim, or
British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 29(2), 347–356. both? Using two reaction time based measures to assess implicit
Leaper, C., & Bigler, R. S. (2011). Gender. In M. K. Underwood & L. S. attitudes to the slim and overweight. Journal of Health Psychology,
Rosen (Eds.), Social development: Relationships in infancy, child- 15, 416–425.
hood, and adolescence (pp. 289–315). New York: Guilford Press. Smith, P. (2010). Children and play: Understanding children’s worlds.
Lytton, H., & Romney, D. M. (1991). Parents’ differential socialization of Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.
boys and girls: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 2, 267–296. Stockwell, F. M., Walker, D. J., & Eshleman, J. W. (2010). Measures of
Mckenna, I., Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Stewart, I. implicit and explicit attitudes toward mainstream and BDSM sexual
(2007). Testing the fake-ability of the Implicit Relational terms using the IRAP and questionnaire with BDSM/fetish and
Assessment Procedure (IRAP): the first study. International student participants. The Psychological Record, 60, 307–324.
Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 7(2), 123–138. Timko, C. A., England, E. L., Herbert, J. D., & Forman, E. M. (2010).
Nicholson, E., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2012). The Implicit Relational The Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure as a measure of self-
Assessment Procedure (IRAP) as a measure of spider fear. esteem. The Psychological Record, 60, 679–698.
Psychological Record, 62, 263–278. Wanderlind, F., Martins, G. D. F., Hansen, J., Macarini, S. M., & Vieira,
O’Toole, C., Barnes-Holmes, D., Murphy, C., O’Connor, J., & Barnes- M. L. (2006). Diferenças de gênero no brincar de crianças pré-
Holmes, Y. (2009). Relational flexibility and human intelligence: escolares e escolares na brinquedoteca [Gender differences in play
extending the remit of Skinner’s Verbal Behavior. International of prescholar and scholar’s children at toy-library]. Paidéia, 16(34),
Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 9, 1–17. 263–273.

You might also like