Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

Article

Journal of Marketing Research


1-11
Customization in Luxury Brands: ª American Marketing Association 2020
Article reuse guidelines:

Can Valentino Get Personal? sagepub.com/journals-permissions


DOI: 10.1177/0022243720943191
journals.sagepub.com/home/mrj

C. Page Moreau, Emanuela Prandelli, Martin Schreier, and Silke Hieke

Abstract
Luxury brands have started to offer consumers the opportunity to customize their exclusive products by making certain aesthetic
decisions, such as the color, fabric, or cut of their products. A robust finding in the marketing literature is that consumers place a
greater value on customized than on standard products because these unique products better fit and communicate their tastes,
preferences, and identity. However, the majority of focal products in these studies fall outside the luxury segment. The authors
demonstrate that consumers’ customization preferences differ between mainstream and luxury brands. In the luxury segment,
consumers pay a premium for the designer’s expertise and the status that it can convey. As such, the consumers’ desire for self-
expression can potentially erode the product’s signaling value. Through a series of four experiments, the authors demonstrate
that luxury brands can benefit from customization but they can also take customization too far. Their findings suggest that brand
managers should allow consumers to make fewer design decisions for luxury versus mainstream brands to preserve the signal
value created by the designer.

Keywords
crowdsourcing, fashion, luxury, mass customization, self-design, signaling, status

The market for personal luxury goods is expanding rapidly, allows consumers to design made-to-order bags, and Prada
with expected growth of 3%–5% per year through 2025, for invites consumers to create unique fragrances. The impact of
a total value increase from €320 billion to €365 billion these customization initiatives on brand equity, however, is not
(D’Arpizio et al. 2019). The composition of the market is shift- well understood. Can luxury firms keep or even enhance the
ing in important ways as well. The United States, for example, “dream value” derived from their exclusivity by engaging with
has experienced a “changing of the generational guard” as the the masses using one of these online tools (Chandon, Laurent,
growing population of affluent consumers (those with incomes and Valette-Florence 2016, p. 301)? While customization
over $100,000) is now comprised of more Generation Xers than might enhance the uniqueness of their luxury offerings, it may
Baby Boomers (skynewsinternational.com). The middle class also simultaneously harm the value of these products by
is also becoming a significant force in this market as some decreasing their recognizability. Through a series of four stud-
luxury brands shift from prestige to masstige (Chandon, Laur- ies, our research demonstrates that luxury firms can include
ent, and Valette-Florence 2016). As a result, luxury brands are customization in their business models if they are thoughtful
facing the paradoxical challenge of maintaining an aura of about the type and number of design decisions offered to
exclusivity while also targeting the “emerging aspirational consumers.
masses” (Chandon, Laurent, and Valette-Florence 2016,
p. 299; Kahn 2014).
C. Page Moreau is John R. Nevin Professor of Marketing, Wisconsin School of
To maintain their prestige while simultaneously engaging
Business, University of Wisconsin (email: page.moreau@wisc.edu). Emanuela
with a broader audience, some notable brands are leveraging Prandelli is LVMH Associate Professor of Fashion and Luxury Management,
technology to offer consumers a custom experience. Burberry, Department of Management and Technology, ICRIOS, Bocconi University
for example, has offered web-optimized customization to (email: emanuela.prandelli@unibocconi.it). Martin Schreier is Professor of
enable consumers to design their own trench coat. For a sig- Marketing, Department of Marketing, WU Vienna University of Economics
and Business (email: martin.schreier@wu.ac.at). Silke Hieke is Senior
nificant premium, consumers can go online to Burberry Research and Customer Experience Consultant, Digitalberatung GmbH and
Bespoke and choose the color, fabric, cut, and lining to create former Research and Teaching Associate, Department of Marketing, WU
a one-of-a-kind product (Kahn 2014). Similarly, Louis Vuitton Vienna University of Economics and Business (email: sh@digitalberatung.at).
2 Journal of Marketing Research XX(X)

Research by Fuchs et al. (2013) suggests that luxury brands consumption of luxury brands can also signal important infor-
should be wary when inviting consumers into the design pro- mation about the identity of the user, including the groups to
cess. Their research shows that consumer demand for luxury which the consumer belongs and those from which the con-
fashion brands fell substantially when the products were known sumer dissociates (Belk 1988; Escalas and Bettman 2003;
to be crowdsourced rather than designed in-house by the firm’s Kleine, Kleine, and Allen 1995; Richins 1999; Wang, John,
professional designers. Consumers in their studies believed that and Griskevicius 2015; White and Dahl 2006, 2007). In doing
the crowdsourced products—products designed by other con- so, these brands can enhance consumers’ self-esteem (Chan-
sumers—were of inferior quality and were less capable of con- don, Laurent, and Valette-Florence 2016; Han et al. 2010;
veying status than their professionally designed counterparts Vickers and Renand 2003; Wang and Griskevicius 2014) and
(Fuchs et al. 2013). This finding follows from the fact that offer them dreams of a better life (Hagtvedt and Patrick 2009;
professional designers are often perceived to hold a significant Pozharliev et al. 2015).
skill advantage over consumer–designer because of their train- Luxury brands, compared with their more mainstream coun-
ing, experience, and knowledge (Moreau and Herd 2010). terparts, provide these psychological benefits to consumers
Customization platforms, in contrast, enable consumers to because they are perceived to carry a premium price and to
modify professionally designed products by making certain be higher in quality, more exclusive, and rare (Bruce and Kratz
design decisions using an online toolkit. Importantly, the firm 2007; Dubois, Laurent, and Czellar 2001; Fuchs et al. 2013; Ko
subsequently produces and sells that product only to the et al. 2019; Wiedmann, Hennigs, and Siebels 2009). How, then,
consumer who designed it (Franke, Schreier, and Kaiser are these psychological benefits affected when luxury brands
2010). Considering that this customization enables firms to offer allow consumers to customize their offerings? The answer to
professionally designed yet individualized, one-of-a-kind prod- this question is not a straightforward one, as we highlight in the
ucts to consumers, luxury brands may benefit from embracing following section.
this approach. Essentially, customization may further enhance
the perceived exclusivity of the luxury product by making it
even more unique. However, before addressing our primary
Luxury Brands and Customization
research question of whether and how luxury brands can benefit To understand the effects of customization on luxury brands, it is
from customized offerings, we must first define what we mean first important to distinguish between “Design-It-Yourself” and
by luxury brands and consumers’ motivations for buying them. “Do-It-Yourself” (Kahn 2014). The luxury brands that offer cus-
tomization to their consumers still maintain control over all of the
production process. As such, the objective quality of the materials
Luxury Brands and craftsmanship should be unaffected by the availability of
online customization. The issue, however, is that luxury brands
In keeping with prior work, we define luxury brands as those
often maintain their design equity by distancing themselves from
that bestow prestige on consumers who simply display and/or
their end consumers and adopting the stance of a pompous expert
use them (Han, Nunes, and Drèze 2010; Wang and Griskevi-
(Fuchs et al. 2013; Kapferer and Bastien 2009). Because custo-
cius 2014; Wiedmann, Hennigs, and Siebels 2009). Luxury
mization gives consumers control over some aspects of the prod-
brands are considered “emotionally significant stimuli” and
uct’s design, the influence of the designer’s expertise is
have the ability to evoke hedonic responses from consumers
necessarily diminished when consumers substitute their judgment
who connect with them (Hagtvedt and Patrick 2009; Pozharliev
for that of the professional. Consequently, the consumer may
et al. 2015).
place a lower value on a luxury product that they have customized
A considerable amount of research has examined why con-
as compared with its professionally designed counterpart. Such a
sumers buy or aspire to buy luxury brands (see Chandon, Laur-
finding would run counter to nearly all of the existing research on
ent, and Valette-Florence 2016, pp. 300–301). A recent review
customization (Franke and Piller 2004; Franke and Schreier 2010;
article on luxury brands provides an overview of the five pri-
Townsend, Kaiser, and Schreier 2015).
mary theories that have been used to explain consumers’ luxury
Conversely, customization may enhance the value a con-
brand consumption (Ko, Costello, and Taylor 2019). Dating
sumer places on a luxury product because the customization
back to Veblen’s (1899) theory of conspicuous consumption,
process yields a genuinely exclusive product that will be sold
all five theories are largely social in nature and describe con-
solely to them. Not only is the product exclusive to that con-
sumers’ desire to signal information to both themselves and
sumer; it is also likely to be unique in its design given the vast
others through the acquisition and consumption of luxury prod-
choice space offered by customization. Luis Vuitton, for exam-
ucts.1 Specifically, the consumption of these prestigious prod-
ple, reduces the probability than any two customers will design
ucts can signal status, wealth, power, class, taste, and
the same bag by letting them choose among eight types of
accomplishment to the appropriate audience, including oneself
leather, five different shapes, 26 colors, and two sizes (moder-
(Berger and Ward 2010; Han et al. 2010; Ko et al. 2019). The
nwearing.com 2013). Guerlain allows consumers to choose
among more than 1,200 different combinations to make their
1
The other four theories are self-concept theory, social comparison theory, the own customized lipstick and paired case. The number of
theory of the extended self, and the theory of uniqueness (see Ko et al. 2019). options ensures the uniqueness of each offering, a trait valued
Moreau et al. 3

by luxury consumers (Wiedmann, Hennigs, and Siebels 2009). Study 1: Adidas versus Hermès Sneakers
As Stylist Magazine put it (2010), “Owning a Prada bag in the
first instance is an honour, but owning one personalised to you Method
is what dreams are made of.” With luxury brands becoming In Study 1, we recruited 317 students (Mage ¼ 24.54 years)
more ubiquitous, perceptions of rarity, exclusivity, and unique- from a major European university to participate in a study in
ness take on more importance. These perceptions seem to be exchange for the chance to win Amazon coupons. Each respon-
especially relevant for millennials, who place less weight on dent was randomly assigned to one of six conditions in a 2
mere exclusivity and more on a search for true uniqueness (brand: mainstream vs. luxury)  3 (design freedom: none
(DeLeon 2018). vs. low vs. high) between-participants design experiment admi-
This discussion highlights the tension that customization nistered in a lab setting on a computer. Hermès served as the
creates for luxury brands. Consumers who choose to customize luxury brand, and Adidas represented the mainstream fashion
luxury products can access their own, unique self-knowledge to industry; sneakers were used as the focal product category.
inform their decisions during the design process. Leveraging At the outset of the experiment, participants in all conditions
this knowledge helps consumers create a truly unique product received a short introduction informing them that [Hermès/
that better fits their tastes and preferences than an off-the-shelf Adidas] would be offering a new sneakers model as part of its
equivalent (Franke and Piller 2004; Salvador, De Holan, and upcoming Fall/Winter collection. This new model, which was
Piller 2009; Schreier 2006; Von Hippel and Katz 2002). identical across conditions, was depicted from different angles
Indeed, uniqueness and preference fit are among the factors in a series of color pictures (the model was pretested to be
that explain the premium that consumers are willing to pay for consistent with both the Hermès and Adidas brands). Partici-
a customized product (Franke and Schreier 2008). Such pre- pants then indicated on a seven-point scale the extent to which
miums have been identified in categories as diverse as watches,
they liked this product (1 ¼ “dislike,” and 7 ¼ “like”).
sports shoes, skis, granola, backpacks, and apparel (cf. Town-
Participants in the customization conditions (high vs. low
send, Kaiser, and Schreier 2015). However, none of the focal
design freedom), were also informed that they could customize
products in these studies would be considered members of the
the sneakers. For those in the high design freedom condition,
luxury segment. Customization in this segment requires con-
participants learned that [Hermès/Adidas] provides customers
sumers to substitute their own design judgment for that of the
with many degrees of design freedom to personalize almost all
professional designers employed by firms who have carefully
features of the sneakers according to their taste and prefer-
cultivated their design equity. This difference between main-
ences. In particular, customers were informed that they could
stream and luxury brands may have significant implications for
select the color and material of the laces and of the heel tab of
the design of customization toolkits.
the shoes, and any other modular element of the upper shoe. In
Toolkits in the real world differ in the extent to which con-
sumers can depart from the professionally designed model. The addition, customers could change the color, size, shape, and
more product attributes that can be customized and the more material of the sole, and they could even define a different form
design options available for each attribute, the higher the poten- of the shoe’s overall shape. They could also put their own
tial solution space and, thus, the design freedom granted to the initials on the tongue inside the sneakers. Participants learned
consumer–designer (Franke, Schreier, and Kaiser 2010; Salva- that all these customizations would be implemented through a
dor, De Holan, and Piller 2009; Von Hippel and Katz 2002). For user-friendly online toolkit (easy and intuitive to use for any
mainstream brands, design freedom has been shown to increase customer).
purchase intentions monotonically (Franke, Schreier, and Kaiser Participants in the low design freedom condition were told
2010). However, in the luxury market, we propose that the rela- that [Hermès/Adidas] provides customers with a few degrees of
tionship between design freedom and purchase intentions may design freedom to personalize selected features of the sneakers
become more complex as the trade-off between a consumer’s according to their taste and preferences, while maintaining the
own preferences and those of the professional designer become basic core product design of the specific [Hermès/Adidas]
more pronounced. While a consumer may value the ability to sneakers shown to them. In particular, customers were
make certain design decisions, even in the luxury market, too informed that they could select the color and material of the
much design freedom may be viewed as a detriment to design laces and of the heel tab of the shoes. They could also put their
equity. As a result, we hypothesize the following: own initials on the tongue inside the sneakers. Again, partici-
pants learned that all these customizations would be implemen-
H1a: For mainstream customized products, the relationship ted through a user-friendly online toolkit (easy and intuitive to
between design freedom and purchase intentions is positive use for any customer).
and linear. Participants in the control condition (no design freedom), in
H1b: For luxury customized products, the relationship contrast, learned that they could purchase exactly the design
between design freedom and purchase intentions is non- shown in the picture (with no possibility to change the snea-
linear, following an inverted U-shaped pattern. kers’ design). Participants in all conditions then responded to
two dependent measures capturing purchase intentions for the
We test this hypothesis in Study 1. respective sneakers: “How likely is it that you would purchase
4 Journal of Marketing Research XX(X)

Hermès sneakers (MAdidas ¼ 4.54 vs. MHermès ¼ 4.07, F(1,311) ¼


Purchase Intent
(1 = "low," and 7 = "high") 6.85, p < .01). Given that brands influence consumers’ price
7 expectations, this main effect is not surprising if one assumes a
6 5.43 downward sloping demand curve. Second, we find a main
5
4.81 4.82 effect of the design freedom factor (F (2,311) ¼ 43.88, p <
4.1
4 3.29
.001). In particular, participants indicated a higher purchase
3.18
3 intent for customizable versus standard off-the-shelf sneakers
2 (Mhigh ¼ 4.81 vs. Mlow ¼ 4.82 vs. Mnone ¼ 3.23). While this
1 effect emerged in both the high and low design freedom con-
Mainstream brand: Adidas Luxury brand: Hermès ditions (ps < .001), the two customization conditions them-
No customizaon Low customizaon Full customizaon selves did not differ significantly between each other (p ¼ .76).
Most importantly, for our test of H1, the results also revealed
Figure 1. How self-customization affects purchase intent for a a significant interaction between design freedom and brand
product in the context of a luxury versus a mainstream brand (F(2,311) ¼ 9.24, p < .001). As predicted by H1a, design freedom
(Study 1). positively influenced purchase intentions for Adidas sneakers,
with those in the high design freedom condition reporting
these sneakers for the next season?” and “How probable is it greater purchase intentions than those in the low design free-
that you would purchase these sneakers for the next season?” dom condition (Mhigh ¼ 5.43 vs. Mlow ¼ 4.81, p ¼ .018) and
Answers were expressed on a Likert scale (1 ¼ “unlikely/ with those in the low design freedom condition reporting
improbable,” and 7 ¼ “likely/probable”) and averaged for fur- greater purchase intentions than those in the control condition
ther analysis (r ¼ .85). (Mnone ¼ 3.18, p < .001). Consistent with H1b, however, we
A series of control and manipulation checks followed: found a different pattern of effects for the Hermès brand: while
(1) “How would you describe the brand [Hermès/Adidas]?” participants in the low design freedom condition indicated
(1 ¼ “not at all luxurious,” and 7 ¼ “very luxurious”), greater purchase intentions than those in the control condition
(2) “According to the above description of the product, how (Mlow ¼ 4.82 vs. Mnone ¼ 3.29, p < .001), moving from low to
customizable are the sneakers presented to you? That is, how high design freedom significantly reduced participants’ pur-
much of the sneaker design can you change before pur- chase intent for Hermès sneakers (Mhigh ¼ 4.10, p < .01).
chasing?” (1 ¼ “not at all customizable. Nothing can be
changed,” 4 ¼ “somewhat customizable. A few things can
be changed,” and 7 ¼ “totally customizable. A lot can be Discussion
changed”). Finally, respondents reported their age and gender. The results from Study 1 demonstrate that luxury brands can
take the customization process too far. By giving consumers
too much freedom in the design process, these companies
Findings appear to erode some of the design equity they had created.
Manipulation checks. A series of 2 (brand: mainstream vs. lux- How does design freedom do so? To answer this question, it is
ury)  3 (design freedom: none vs. low vs. high) ANOVAs important to specify—at a more granular level—what it
confirmed the effectiveness of the manipulations. For the item means to be a luxury brand. The measures proposed by Ko
capturing perceptions of brand luxury, the main effect of the et al. (2019) are helpful in this regard. According to their
brand was significant (MAdidas ¼ 3.65 vs. MHermès ¼ 6.31; specification, luxury brands offer greater value to consumers
F(1,311) ¼ 482.27, p < .001). For the item capturing perceptions the more that they remain true to the brand’s “espoused
of design freedom, the predicted main effect of the design values,” “reflect a timeless design,” serve “as a symbol of
freedom manipulation emerged (F(2,311) ¼ 213.85, p < .001). prestige,” and make consumers proud to have others know
In particular, participants felt they had more design freedom (1) that they use the brand (see Ko et al. 2019, p. 408, for their
in the high versus low design freedom conditions (Mhigh ¼ 5.66 complete set of measures).
vs. Mlow ¼ 4.39, p < .001) and (2) in the low versus none Customization threatens value creation on all four of these
design freedom conditions (Mnone ¼ 2.19, p < .001). Finally, dimensions when the consumer’s idiosyncratic taste is substi-
participants did not differ in their liking of the focal brands tuted for that of the designer. It does so because consumers may
(MAdidas ¼ 5.65 vs. MHermès ¼ 5.50; F(1,311) ¼ 1.63, p ¼ .20). not understand the brand’s visual signature, the “subtle cues”
that help identify a brand, “even in the absence of an explicit
Purchase intent for the respective sneakers. We used a 2 (brand: logo or brand name” (Han et al. 2010, p. 27). If a consumer
mainstream vs. luxury)  3 (design freedom: none vs. low vs. inadvertently removes or negates one of those subtle cues dur-
high) ANOVA to test whether design freedom differentially ing the customization process, the product will likely stray
influenced consumers’ purchase intentions for luxury and further from the brand’s value and timelessness and, impor-
mainstream brands (see Figure 1). The results first revealed a tantly, may lose some or all of its signaling value. Thus, brands
main effect of the brand manipulation. with participants report- must be quite thoughtful in the amount and type of customiza-
ing higher purchase intentions for Adidas sneakers than for tion they allow.
Moreau et al. 5

The ideal balance between the consumer’s and the that [Chanel/Zara] would be offering a new bag—bauletto—
designer’s input into the product’s design likely varies by cus- as part of its upcoming Fall/Winter collection. All participants
tomer segment. In particular, fashion-conscious consumers were also informed that they could customize the bag accord-
may be more averse to replacing a designer’s judgment with ing to their own preferences. For those in the high design
his or her own because of a greater premium placed on the freedom condition, participants were told that [Chanel/Zara]
product’s signal value. While the fashion-consciousness con- was planning to give customers the opportunity to customize
struct is multifaceted, it has been positively associated with many features of the bag, including the material for the hand-
consumers’ prestige sensitivity, their self-confidence, and the bag’s body, bottom, and handle, the shape of the bag, and the
attention they pay to the image of luxury brands (Mulyanegara adornments for the bag. Participants in the low design freedom
2011; Summers 1970; Wan, Youn, and Fang 2001). Consumers condition were told that [Chanel/Zara] was planning to give
who score higher on fashion-consciousness also tend to be customers the opportunity to customize a few features of the
more involved with clothing styles and with their own appear- bag, including the color of the material for the bag’s base and
ance (Nam et al. 2007; Wan et al. 2001). the selection of several predesigned adornments.
As such, we expect consumers’ fashion-consciousness to Participants then responded to the primary dependent mea-
amplify the effects we observed in Study 1, and thus, we pre- sures capturing purchase intentions similar to those used in
dict a three-way interaction among fashion-consciousness, Study 2. Specifically, participants indicated how likely they
brand, and design freedom. Specifically, at high levels of would be to purchase that type of customized bauletto for next
design freedom, fashion-consciousness should be negatively season and how probable that purchase would be (1 ¼
related to consumers’ purchase intensions for luxury brands “extremely unlikely/improbable,” and 7 ¼ “extremely likely/
because these consumers would likely place a greater amount probable,” r ¼ .95). The two items were averaged to form an
of weight on the status that the design could convey. Conver- indicator of purchase intentions. Two manipulation checks fol-
sely, at these same high levels of design freedom, fashion- lowed: (1) “How would you describe the brand?” (1 ¼ “not at
consciousness would likely be positively related to consumers’ all luxurious,” and 7 ¼ “very luxurious”) and (2) “When you
purchase intensions for mainstream brands because these con- think about the customization options described to you in this
sumers have greater confidence in their own sense of style and study, how much design freedom were you given?” (1 ¼ “very
would welcome the opportunity to express that style through little,” and 7 ¼ “a great deal”). Finally, respondents reported
more extensive customization. More formally, we advance the their age and their fashion consciousness by indicating the
following hypothesis: extent to which they agreed with the following four statements:
“My friends would describe me as fashion–conscious,” “My
H2a: At high levels of design freedom, fashion-consciousness friends would tell you that I have a good style,” “My friends
negatively influences purchase intentions for customized lux- would say that I know what looks good on me,” and “My
ury products. friends would tell you that I know how to mix and match
H2b: At high levels of design freedom, fashion-consciousness different fashion items” (alpha ¼ .92). The items were aver-
positively influences purchase intentions for customized aged to form an indicator of fashion consciousness.
mainstream products.

The following study, therefore, is designed to test H2 and to Findings


generalize the effects demonstrated in Study 1 to different Manipulation checks. Two 2 (design freedom: low vs. high)  2
stimuli and to a different location: the heart of luxury fashion, (brand: mainstream vs. luxury) ANOVAs confirmed the effec-
Milan’s downtown shopping district. tiveness of the manipulations. For the item capturing percep-
tions of brand luxury, the main effect of the brand was
Study 2: Chanel versus Zara significant (MChanel ¼ 6.24 vs. MZara ¼ 2.87; F(1, 293) ¼
392.9, p < .001). For the item capturing perceptions of design
In Study 2, we recruited 297 women aged between 18 and 30 freedom, the predicted main effect of the design freedom
years (Mage ¼ 25.40 years) as they were shopping in the Quad- manipulation emerged (M high ¼ 4.64 vs. M low ¼ 3.73,
rilatero della Moda (“The Four Streets of Fashion”) in down- F(1, 293) ¼ 18.9, p < .001).
town Milan. They knew they were contributing to a relevant
university research project, without getting any monetary com- Purchase intent for the customized bag. We used a 2 (design
pensation for their participation. Each respondent was ran- freedom: low vs. high)  2 (brand: mainstream vs. luxury)
domly assigned to one of four conditions in a 2 (brand: ANOVA to test whether design freedom differentially influ-
mainstream vs. luxury)  2 (design freedom: high vs. low) enced consumers’ purchase intentions for luxury and main-
between-participants design experiment. Chanel served as the stream brands (see Figure 2). The results revealed a main
luxury brand, and Zara represented the mainstream fashion effect of the brand manipulation with participants reporting
industry. higher purchase intentions for the Zara bauletto than for the
Upon agreeing to participate in the study, women in all of Chanel version (MZara ¼ 3.91 vs. MChanel ¼ 3.44, F(1, 293) ¼
the conditions received a short introduction informing them 4.40, p < .05). This finding is unsurprising given that the Zara
6 Journal of Marketing Research XX(X)

Purchase Intent 3 2.59


2.37
(1 = "low," and 7 = "high") 2.04
2 1.71
7 1.27

6 1
4.86
5 4.51
0
4
2.85 -1
3 2.45 -0.76
2 -1.4
-2
1
-1.88
Mainstream brand: Zara Luxury brand: Chanel -3 -2.36
-2.68
Design freedom: low Design freedom: high Mainstream brand (Zara) Luxury brand (Chanel)

Figure 2. How design freedom differentially affects purchase intent Figure 3. How fashion consciousness differentially affects the effect
for a self-customized product in the context of a luxury versus a of design freedom per brand tier (Study 2).
mainstream brand (Study 2).
This effect is particularly strong for those with high fashion
bag would be expected to carry a significantly smaller price tag consciousness who, importantly, are the likeliest target market
than the Chanel bag. Importantly, the results also revealed a for these (customized) luxury brands. This finding further
significant interaction between design freedom and brand pre- emphasizes the importance of distinguishing between main-
dicted by H1 (F(1, 293) ¼ 128.3, p < .001). As expected, greater stream and luxury brands in both the customization literature
design freedom yielded higher purchase intentions for the Zara and practice.
bag (Mhigh ¼ 4.86 vs. Mlow ¼ 2.85, F(1, 293) ¼ 60.5, p < .001), While purchase intentions are clearly an important factor
while the opposite was true for the Chanel bag (Mhigh ¼ 2.45 predicting the likelihood that the consumer will indeed pur-
vs. Mlow ¼ 4.51, F(1, 293) ¼ 68.0, p < .001). chase the customized product, the measures used to capture
To test H2, we then added fashion consciousness to the purchase intentions presume that the consumer will success-
model, which revealed a negative main effect of fashion con- fully complete the customization process. This presumption,
sciousness (b ¼ 2.64, t ¼ 2.89, p < .01) as well as the however, likely overinflates the likelihood that a consumer will
predicted three-way interaction among design freedom, brand, actually purchase the product because there is a significant
and fashion consciousness (b ¼ 1.08, t ¼ 3.18, p < .01). chance that the consumer will abandon the customization pro-
Overall, the model fit improved significantly with the addition of cess before it actually ends. Online retailers, such as Nike, face
the three-way interaction (change in F(1, 289) ¼ 10.1, p < .01). A significant challenges with cart abandonment. Even when con-
floodlight analysis using the Johnson–Neyman technique (Hayes
sumers are purchasing standard, off-the-rack products, an aver-
2013) indicates that the interaction between design freedom and
age of 69.1% of them leave items in their cart without
brand is enhanced for those higher in fashion consciousness.
completing a purchase (https://blog.salecycle.com/post/the-
Specifically, we find that the interaction between brand and
remarketing-report-q1-2018/). For those engaged in the custo-
design freedom is significant for participants scoring 3.33 or
mization process, the percentage is likely much higher.
higher on the fashion consciousness scale. Further, the effect
A number of factors may lead consumers to halt the custo-
becomes stronger as respondents’ fashion consciousness
mization process prior to its completion, including frustration
increases (e.g., b25th percentile of fashion consciousness [4.75] ¼ 3.11,
experienced during the design process (Metz, Franke, and Mor-
t ¼ 7.25, p < .001 vs. b75th percentile of fashion consciousness [6.25] ¼
eau 2018). This frustration could be a function of learning how
4.73, t ¼ 10.69, p < .001). In support of H2a, the purchase
intentions of the Milanese fashionistas were significantly lower to use the toolkit itself; however, it may also result from having
for the Chanel brand than were those of their less fashion- too many choices (i.e., choice overload: Iyengar and Lepper
conscious counterparts at high levels of design freedom (see 2000; Schwartz 2004). Firms, therefore, should consider the
Figure 3). The opposite was true for the Zara brand, thereby amount of choice they offer to consumers in terms of the num-
supporting H2b. ber of customization decisions the consumer must make as well
as the number of options available for each of those decisions.
In our final two studies, we therefore shift our focus away
Discussion from consumers’ purchase intentions for customized products
The results from this study offer additional support for our and, instead, focus on consumers’ preferences for the amount of
basic proposition that luxury brands need to use caution when design freedom they would like to have during the design pro-
offering customization options to the consumer. When the lux- cess. In doing so, we aim to offer further evidence of the ten-
ury brand’s equity is based on the cachet, expertise, and artistry sion between consumers’ desire for a unique, personalized
of its designers, offering consumers too many customization product and their desire to maintain the signal value offered
options can diminish rather than enhance purchase intentions as by luxury products. If our theoretical argument is correct, con-
the designer’s judgment is replaced by that of the consumer. sumers should prefer less design freedom when they customize
Moreau et al. 7

luxury versus mainstream products because of their greater sunglasses (1 ¼ “minimal solution space,” and 7 ¼ “maximum
desire to preserve the product’s ability to signal its brand solution space”).
identity. As a manipulation check, participants were asked to indicate
One overt way to signal a luxury product’s brand identity is the extent to which they agreed on the item “The [BRAND]-
through the use of a logo (Berger and Ward 2010; Han et al. logo will be clearly visible on the sunglasses” on a seven-point
2010). We propose that when a brand logo is prominently dis- scale (1 ¼ “strongly disagree,” and 7 ¼ “agree”). They next
played on a customized product, its presence and visibility will responded to an item to capture the perceived brand positioning
give the consumer license to develop a more unique and idio- (1 ¼ “mainstream,” and 7 ¼ “luxury”). Finally, participants
syncratic product that better fits their personal design prefer- indicated their fashion confidence and their age and were then
ences. In short, a prominent brand logo could increase a debriefed.
consumer’s desire for design freedom during the customization
process for a luxury brand because it preserves the value of the
signal while enabling greater personalization. For mainstream Findings
products, the prominence of the brand logo will have less of an Manipulation checks. We used two 2 (brand: mainstream vs.
effect on consumers’ desire for design freedom because the luxury)  2 (brand logo prominence: low vs. high) ANOVAs
brand, by definition, has less ability to signal status information to confirm that the brand and the brand prominence manip-
in the first place. As such, consumers are less concerned that ulations worked as intended. Both models revealed the
their customization decisions will erode the brand’s signal anticipated main effect of the manipulation. Specifically,
value. More formally: Dior was perceived as more luxurious than Benetton
(MDior ¼ 5.63 vs. MBenetton ¼ 3.58, F(1, 290) ¼ 135.27,
H3: Brand type (luxury vs. mainstream) interacts with brand p < .001), and in the high brand prominence condition, the
logo prominence to influence the desired level of design brand was, indeed, considered higher in prominence than in
freedom such that brand logo prominence increases desired the low brand prominence condition (M high ¼ 4.00 vs.
design freedom for luxury, but not mainstream, products. Mlow ¼ 2.85, F(1, 290) ¼ 26.78, p < .001).

We test this hypothesis in the following study. Design freedom. A 2 (brand: mainstream vs. luxury)  2 (brand
logo prominence: low vs. high) ANOVA revealed a significant
main effect of brand. Consistent with our prior results, partici-
pants desired greater design freedom when they were customiz-
Study 3: The Moderating Role of Brand ing a mainstream versus a luxury brand (MBenetton ¼ 4.94 vs.
Prominence MDior ¼ 4.32, F(1, 290) ¼ 12.00, p ¼ .001). The main effect of
Participants in Study 3 were 294 male students recruited at a brand logo prominence was not significant (F(1, 290) ¼ .59,
major European university who participated in exchange for p ¼ .44), but importantly, the interaction hypothesized in H3
course credit or monetary compensation in a series of unrelated was significant (F(1, 290) ¼ 4.75, p ¼ .03). As predicted, parti-
studies (Mage ¼ 22.94 years; female respondents were busy cipants in the luxury brand condition desired greater design
with another, unrelated study). Upon entering the lab, partici- freedom when the brand logo was highly prominent rather than
pants were randomly assigned to one of four conditions in a 2 when it was absent (Mhigh ¼ 4.58, Mlow ¼ 4.04, F(1, 290) ¼ 4.40,
(brand type: mainstream vs. luxury)  2 (brand logo promi- p ¼ .04). For participants in the mainstream brand condition,
nence: low vs. high) between-participants experiment. Partici- brand logo prominence had little effect on their desired design
pants in all conditions were shown a picture of a new model of freedom (Mhigh ¼ 4.81, Mlow ¼ 5.07, F(1, 290) ¼ .99, p ¼ .32;
sunglasses, portrayed to come from either the upcoming col- see Figure 4).
lection of Dior (luxury) or Benetton (mainstream). In addition,
participants in all conditions received a short introduction
informing them that the focal brand offers these sunglasses as
Discussion
a product that can be self-customized. Participants were then The findings from this study highlight the importance of both
told that the brand’s well-known logo would either be dis- the brand and its prominence in consumers’ preferences for
played on the outer side of the sunglasses’ arms (high brand design freedom in a customization task. While consumers still
prominence) or would not be visible to an observer (low brand prefer less design freedom when customizing luxury products,
prominence). All participants then read a description of the they are more open to a broader range of options when the
customization process, which explained that, in general, the brand is prominently featured on these high-status items.
solution space can range from minimal (e.g., changing the color Essentially, the “badge” of the designer can give the consumer
of the lenses) to extensive (e.g., changing the color of the cover to experiment with a wider range of options in the cus-
frames, the shape of the sunglasses, the size of the arms) (orig- tomization task.
inal stimuli were administered in German). The focal depen- The operationalization of brand prominence in this study
dent variable in this study was how much design freedom they was quite overt: the logo either appeared or did not appear
would like to have if they were going to customize these on the outside of the sunglasses. Thus, the brand prominence
8 Journal of Marketing Research XX(X)

prominence, participants were told that they would not find any
Desired Design Freedom
logo on the exterior of the backpack.
7
We manipulated brand signature prominence as follows: in
6
5.07
the high signature prominence condition, participants were
4.81
5 4.58 informed that the brand’s designers considered the backpack
4.04
4 a central piece in this season’s [Valentino/Benetton] collection
3
and that the backpack would be considered a “signature
product,” meaning that its design would strongly identify the
2
brand and its current collection. In the low prominence condi-
1
Mainstream Brand: Beneon Luxury Brand: Dior
tion, participants learned that the brand’s designers considered
Low brand prominence High brand prominence
the backpack a peripheral piece in this season’s [Valentino/
Benetton] collection and that the backpack would not be con-
Figure 4. How brand prominence (logo manipulation) affects one’s
sidered a “signature product,” meaning that its design would
desire for design freedom in the context of a luxury versus a main- not strongly identify the brand and its current collection.
stream brand (Study 3). In addition, participants in all conditions learned that the
focal brand had also decided to offer its customers the option
of customizing this specific backpack. Next, participants read a
manipulation in this case directly affects a consumer’s ability
description of the customization process, which explained that,
to signal to others. However, as we have noted, the brand’s
in general, the solution space could range from minimal (e.g.,
visual signature can also be used to communicate a brand’s
the choice of the backpack’s color, material) to extensive (e.g.,
identity, albeit in a more subtle way (Han et al. 2010). In our
the choice of the backpack’s color, material, shape, size, dec-
final study, we use this much more subtle manipulation of
orations). All participants learned that customers would be
brand prominence and expect it to reduce consumers’ desire
using an intuitive, easy-to-use online software to customize the
for design freedom when customizing luxury products. We
product and that the brand would subsequently produce and
make such a prediction because, unlike the signal sent by the
deliver the backpacks to the customers.
brand logo manipulation in Study 3, the signal sent by the brand
As in Study 3, the focal dependent variable in this study was
signature can be overridden by consumers’ customization deci-
how much design freedom they would like to have if they were
sions. More formally:
going to customize these backpacks (1 ¼ “minimal solution
H4: Brand type (luxury versus mainstream) interacts with space,” and 7 ¼ “maximum solution space”). We further cap-
brand signature prominence to influence the desired level of tured participants’ brand perceptions (1 ¼ “mainstream brand,”
design freedom such that brand signature prominence and 7 ¼ “luxury brand”) and fashion-consciousness (as mea-
decreases desired design freedom for luxury, but not main- sured in Study 2, alpha ¼ .82). Finally, participants indicated
stream, products. their age and gender and were debriefed.

We test this hypothesis in the following study.


Findings
Manipulation checks. A 2 (brand type: mainstream vs. luxury) 
Study 4: The Moderating Effect of a Brand 2 (brand signature prominence: low vs. high) ANOVA on the
Signature brand perception measure confirmed the effectiveness of the
Method manipulation. Participants perceived Valentino to be more lux-
urious than Benetton (MValentino ¼ 5.93 vs. MBenetton ¼ 2.74,
Participants in Study 4 were 249 female students recruited F(1,244) ¼ 663,41, p < .001).
from several leading European universities in exchange for
course credit (Mage ¼ 22.71 years). At the outset of the experi- Design freedom. A 2 (brand type: mainstream vs. luxury)  2
ment, participants were randomly assigned to one of four (brand signature prominence: low vs. high) ANOVA on parti-
conditions in a 2 (brand type: mainstream vs. luxury)  2 cipants’ desired level of design freedom revealed a significant
(brand signature prominence: low vs. high) between- effect of the brand type manipulation, such that participants in
participants experiment. Valentino served as the luxury brand the mainstream (Benetton) condition desired more design free-
and Benetton as the mainstream brand; backpacks were the dom than those in the luxury (Valentino) condition (MBenetton ¼
focal product category. 5.05 vs. MValentino ¼ 3.48, F(1,245) ¼ 80.89, p < .001). This
Participants in all conditions first received a short introduc- main effect supports our theorizing by demonstrating that
tion that informed them that the study was about a new back- brand tier influences consumers’ desired level of design free-
pack from [Valentino/Benetton], which would be launched in dom. Second, the ANOVA revealed a main effect of brand
the next Fall/Winter season. The backpack was described as signature prominence such that participants desired less design
cool and versatile, large enough to carry some books but small freedom when the brand signature was high rather than low in
enough to replace a classic handbag. To control for brand logo prominence (Mhigh ¼ 4.08 vs. Mlow ¼ 4.44, F(1,245) ¼ 4.07,
Moreau et al. 9

customization: they perceive greater value in the customized


Desired Design Freedom
7
product relative to its off-the-shelf counterparts (Franke, Schre-
6
ier, and Kaiser 2010). However, at high levels of customiza-
5
4.98 5.12 tion, too much of the designer’s judgment can be displaced by
4
3.89 that of the consumer, thereby diminishing the value of the
3
3.05 luxury product. By taking the customization process too far,
luxury brands are in danger of eroding much of the designer
2
equity they have created. This designer equity is crucial to
1
Beneon (Mainstream Brand) Valenno (Luxury Brand) brand equity because it contributes to the perceived exclusivity
Nonsignature product Signature product of the brand. Thus, luxury brand managers must carefully cali-
brate the extent of customization offered. Critically, managers
Figure 5. How brand signature affects one’s desire for design freedom must identify the sweet spot on the design freedom continuum
in the context of a luxury versus a mainstream brand (Study 4). that allows the consumer–designer to imbue the product with
enough self-essence while ensuring that enough designer
p ¼ .045). Importantly, we also found support for the signifi- essence is maintained.
cant interaction described in H4 (F(1,245) ¼ 7.66, p < .01). As Study 2 reinforces the importance of this lesson for luxury
predicted, for the luxury backpack, participants desired less brands by demonstrating that too much design freedom is par-
design freedom when brand signature was high rather than low ticularly damaging for those in their most attractive target seg-
in prominence (Mhigh ¼ 3.05 vs. Mlow ¼ 3.89, p ¼ .001). ment: consumers high in fashion-consciousness. In doing so,
However, for the mainstream backpack, brand signature pro- this study sheds additional light on the process underlying the
minence had no significant effect on desired design freedom inverted-U effect. Consumers high in fashion-consciousness
(Mhigh ¼ 5.12 vs. Mlow ¼ 4.98, p ¼ .60; see Figure 5). tend to be more concerned with their own appearance, more
sensitive to prestige, and more aware of the image associated
with luxury brands (Mulyanegara 2011; Nam et al. 2007; Sum-
General Discussion mers 1970; Wan et al. 2001). As such, the more pronounced
According to a recent report by Bain & Company, the online effect of design freedom on these consumers suggests that
luxury market has grown more than tenfold since 2004, reach- customization can erode the perceived prestige and signal value
ing 10% of the total market by the end of 2018 (D’Arpizio et al. offered by luxury brands.
2019). While this growth has been fueled, in part, by the suc- Study 3, however, suggests that luxury brands can protect
cess of customization in categories as diverse as women’s fra- their ability to convey status by making the brand more promi-
grances to men’s ready-to-wear clothing, customization has not nent through overt means (e.g., via the obvious display of brand
been an important source of growth in all luxury categories. logos). By assuring the luxury consumer that others can receive
Indeed, some luxury firms whose business model depended on the status signal that they are sending, these brands can give
customized offerings have failed to survive (e.g., Tinker Tai- their consumers greater license to express themselves through a
lor). D’Arpizio et al. (2019) highlight the need for a better more extensive set of customization decisions. However, Study
understanding of customization in the market for luxury goods. 4 warns that when the brand signature, rather than the brand
Our research helps fill this gap and explain why some luxury logo, is what creates the signal, less design freedom is desired
brands have failed to fully monetize customization: when prod- for luxury brands because the more one customizes, the more
ucts have a signaling role, giving customers too many degrees the brand signature is lost. In addition to the substantive gui-
of design freedom can backfire. dance, these insights contribute to the customization literature
What distinguishes luxury brands from their more main- by demonstrating that the type of customized attribute matters
stream counterparts is the air of exclusivity and quality that in a customization task.
they exude. These elite perceptions are founded, in part, on
consumers’ perceptions of the expertise contributed by the
products’ designers (Fuchs et al 2013; Moreau and Herd
Future Research
2010). This “design equity” is often cultivated by creating a The four studies presented herein demonstrate the importance
perceived gulf between the designers and the consumers they of brand type (luxury versus mainstream) in understanding the
serve. Customization, by its very nature, erodes that gulf by potential value of customization. Whereas prior research has
allowing consumers to join with designers in contributing to a focused on the value of self-expression to explain consumers’
product’s design. preference for customized products, we show that the value of
Our research highlights the inherent tension between con- the designer’s expertise and status should also be given con-
sumers’ desire for self-expression and their desire to signal siderable weight when designing customization opportunities
status in the market for customized luxury goods. Study 1 in the luxury market. Luxury brand managers must carefully
demonstrates that when luxury brands offer low and moderate consider the balance between consumers’ contributions to the
levels of customization, consumers respond in much the same design process and that of the professional designers. The opti-
way as they do when mainstream brands offer similar levels of mal balance for each luxury brand likely depends on several
10 Journal of Marketing Research XX(X)

factors, including the product category, the traits of the target Tomorrow to Understand Today,” (accessed January 20, 2020)
consumers, and the culture of the market. [available at http://www.bain.com].
It is important to note that we have only examined fashion DeLeon, Jian (2018), “The New Luxury: Buying in Is the New Selling
brands in our work, making it important for future research to Out,” Highsnobiety Whitepaper, Berlin (accessed February 5,
assess whether our findings extend to other types of luxury 2020) [available at https://www.highsnobiety.com/p/the-new-lux
goods that tend to carry few brand identifiers (e.g., jewelry). ury-whitepaper-highsnobiety/].
It could be that the materials themselves (e.g., the metal and the Dubois, Bernard, Gilles Laurent, and Sandor Czellar (2001), Con-
stones) are capable of signaling status on their own, making sumer Rapport to Luxury: Analyzing Complex and Ambivalent
concerns about design freedom less relevant in these cate- Attitudes. Paris: HEC Paris.
gories. Thus, the more general insight is that luxury brand Escalas, Jennifer Edson and James R. Bettman (2003), “You Are What
managers will need to test and experiment with their own cus- They Eat: The Influence of Reference Groups on Consumers’ Con-
tomization initiatives to find the point where both consumers nections to Brands,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 13 (3),
and designers can add value to the design of exclusive products. 339–48.
Doing so will enable managers to offer additional guidance to Franke, Nikolaus and Frank Piller (2004), “Value Creation by
consumers in the customization task, particularly if consumers Toolkits for User Innovation and Design: The Case of the
are tempted to exploit all of the customization options Watch Market,” Journal of Product Innovation Management,
provided. 21 (6), 401–15.
Franke, Nikolaus and Martin Schreier (2008), “Product Uniqueness as
a Driver of Customer Utility in Mass Customization,” Marketing
Acknowledgments
Letters, 19 (2), 93–107.
The authors thank the editorial team for their truly exceptional gui-
Franke, Nikolaus and Martin Schreier (2010), “Why Customers Value
dance; the reviewers for their constructive feedback; Maria Beatrice
Casarosa, Rachele Ciulli, Elena Di Muzio, Francesca Mandrini, Self-Designed Products: The Importance of Process Effort and
Niccolò Suriano, and Lidia Tinacci for their help with the empirical Enjoyment,” Journal of Product Innovation Management, 27 (7),
work, and Joe Nunes for his feedback on earlier studies for this 1020–31.
research. Franke, Nikolaus, Martin Schreier, and Ulrike Kaiser (2010), “The ‘I
Designed It Myself’ Effect in Mass Customization,” Management
Science, 56 (1), 125–40.
Associate Editor
Fuchs, Christoph, Emanuela Prandelli, Martin Schreier, and Darren
Vladas Griskevicius
W. Dahl (2013), “All That Is Users Might Not Be Gold: How
Labeling Products as User Designed Backfires in the Context of
Declaration of Conflicting Interests Luxury Fashion Brands,” Journal of Marketing, 77 (5), 75–91.
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to Hagtvedt, Henrik and Vannessa M. Patrick (2009), “The Broad
the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. Embrace of Luxury: Hedonic Potential as a Driver of Brand Exten-
dibility,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 4 (19), 608–18.
Funding Han, Young Jee, Joseph C. Nunes, and Xavier Dr èze (2010),
“Signaling Status with Luxury Goods: The Role of Brand
The author(s) received no financial support for the research, author-
Prominence,” Journal of Marketing, 74 (4), 15–30.
ship, and/or publication of this article.
Hayes, Andrew F. (2013), Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and
Conditional Process Analysis. New York: Guilford Press.
References Iyengar, S. Sheena and Mark R. Lepper (2000), “When Choice Is
Belk, Russel W. (1988), “Possessions and the Extended Self,” Journal Demotivating: Can One Desire Too Much of a Good Thing?”
of Consumer Research, 15 (2), 139–68. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79 (6),
Berger, Jonah and Morgan Ward (2010), “Subtle Signals of Inconspic- 995–1006.
uous Consumption,” Journal of Consumer Research, 37 (4), Kahn, Mattie (2014), “The Luxury Market Comes Up Close and Gets
555–69. Very, Very Personal,” (accessed January 21, 2017), [available at
Bruce, Margaret and Christine Kratz (2007), “Competitive Marketing http://abcnews.go.com].
Strategies of Luxury Fashion Companies,” in Fashion Marketing: Kapferer, Jean-Noël and Vincent Bastien (2009), “The Specificity of
Contemporary Issues, Tony Hines and Margaret Bruce, eds. Luxury Management: Turning Marketing Upside Down,” Journal
Oxford: Elsevier, 130–50. of Brand Management, 16 (5-6), 311–22.
Chandon, Jean-Louis, Gilles Laurent, and Pierre Valette-Florence Kleine, Susan Schultz, Robert E. Kleine, and Chris T. Allen (1995),
(2016), “Pursuing the Concept of Luxury: Introduction to the JBR “How Is a Possession “Me” or “Not Me”? Characterizing Types
Special Issue on “Luxury Marketing from Tradition to and an Antecedent of Material Possession Attachment,” Journal of
Innovation,” Journal of Business Research, 69 (1), 299–303. Consumer Research, 22 (3), 327–43.
D’Arpizio, Claudia, Federica Levato, Filippo Prete, Elisa Del Fabbro, Ko, Eunju, John P. Costello, and Charles R. Taylor (2019), “What Is a
and Joëlle de Montgolfier (2019), “Luxury Goods Worldwide Mar- Luxury Brand? A New Definition and Review of the Literature,”
ket Study, Fall–Winter 2018: The Future of Luxury: A Look into Journal of Business Research, 99, 405–13.
Moreau et al. 11

Metz, Franziska, Nikolaus Franke, and C. Page Moreau (2018), Summers, John O. (1970), “The Identity of Women’s Clothing Fash-
“Affect During the Customization Process: A Longitudinal Ana- ion Opinion Leaders,” Journal of Marketing Research, 7 (2),
lysis,” working paper. 178–85.
Modernwearing.com (2013), “Customization Is the New Luxury,” Townsend, Claudia, Ulrike Kaiser, and Martin Schreier (2015), “User
(accessed January 19, 2017), [available at http://www.modernwear Design through Self-Customization,” in The Cambridge Handbook
ing.com]. of Consumer Psychology, Michael I. Norton, Derek D. Rucker, and
Moreau, C. Page, and Kelly B. Herd (2010), “To Each His Own? How Cait Lamberton, eds. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University
Comparisons with Others Influence Consumers’ Evaluations of Press, 233–54.
Their Self-Designed Products,” Journal of Consumer Research, Veblen, Thorstein (1899), The Theory of the Leisure Class: An Eco-
36 (5), 806–19. nomic Study in the Evolution of Institutions. New York: The Mac-
Mulyanegara, Riza (2011), “The Effects of Consumer Personality on millan Company.
Fashion Consciousness and Prestige Sensitivity,” in AP - Asia- Vickers, Jonathan S., and Franck Renand (2003), “The Marketing of
Pacific Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 9, Zhihong Yi, Luxury Goods: An Exploratory Study—Three Conceptual
Jing Jian Xiao, June Cotte and Linda Price, eds. Duluth, MN: Dimensions,” The Marketing Review, 3 (4), 459–78.
Association for Consumer Research, 336–39. Von Hippel, Eric, and Ralph Katz (2002), “Shifting Innovation to
Nam, Jinhee, Reagan Hamlin, Hae Jin Gam, Hye Kang Ji, Jiyoung Kim, Users via Toolkits,” Management Science, 48 (7), 821–33.
Pimpawan Kumphai, Cathy Starr, and Lynne Richards (2007), “The Wan, Fang, Seounmi Youn, and Tammy Fang (2001), “Passionate
Fashion-Conscious Behaviours of Mature Female Consumers,” Surfers in Image-Driven Consumer Culture: Fashion-Conscious,
International Journal of Consumer Studies, 31 (1), 102–08. Appearance-Savvy People and Their Way of Life,” in
Pozharliev, Rumen, Willem J. Verbeke, Jan W. Van Strien, and NA-Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 28. Mary C. Gilly and
Richard P. Bagozzi (2015), “Merely Being with You Increases Joan Meyers-Levy, eds. Valdosta, GA: Association for Consumer
My Attention to Luxury Products: Using EEG to Understand Con- Research, 266–74.
sumers’ Emotional Experience with Luxury Branded Products,” Wang, Yajin, Deborah Roedder John, and Vladas Griskevicius,
Journal of Marketing Research, 52 (4), 546–58. (2015), “The Devil Wears Prada? How Luxury Consumption Influ-
Richins, Marsha L. (1999), “Possessions, Materialism, and Other- ences Social Behaviors,” in NA - Advances in Consumer Research,
Directedness in the Expression of Self,” in Consumer Value: A Vol. 43. Kristin Diehl and Carolyn Yoon, eds. Duluth, MN: Asso-
framework for Analysis and Research, Morris B. Holbrook, ed. ciation for Consumer Research, 142–46.
New York: Routledge, 85–104. Wang, Yajin and Vladas Griskevicius (2014), “Conspicuous Con-
Salvador, Fabrizio, Pablo Martin De Holan, and Frank. T. Piller sumption, Relationships, and Rivals: Women’s Luxury Products
(2009), “Cracking the Code of Mass Customization,” MIT Sloan as Signals to Other Women,” Journal of Consumer Research, 40
Management Review, 50 (3), 70–9. (5), 834–54.
Schreier, Martin (2006), “The Value Increment of Mass-Customized White, Kate, and Darren W. Dahl (2006), “To Be or Not Be?
Products: An Empirical Assessment,” Journal of Consumer Beha- The Influence of Dissociative Reference Groups on Con-
viour, 5 (4), 317–27. sumer Preferences,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 16
Schwartz, Barry (2004), The Paradox of Choice: Why More Is Less. (4), 404–14.
New York: Harper Perennial. White, Kate, and Darren W. Dahl (2007), “Are All Out-Groups Cre-
Skynewsinternational.com (2016), “IPSOS Affluent Survey Europe ated Equal? Consumer Identity and Dissociative Influence,” Jour-
2015,” (accessed January 23, 2017), http://www.skynewsinterna nal of Consumer Research, 34 (4), 525–36.
tional.com. Wiedmann, Klaus-Peter, Nadine Hennigs, and Astrid Siebels (2009),
Stylist Magazine (2010), “Prada Gets Personal,” (accessed January 19, “Value-Based Segmentation of Luxury Consumption Behavior,”
2017), http://www.stylist.co.uk. Psychology & Marketing, 26 (7), 625–51.

You might also like